Abstract
Auditory activity plays an important role in the development of the auditory system. Decreased activity can result from conductive hearing loss (CHL) associated with otitis media, which may lead to long-term perceptual deficits. The effects of CHL have been mainly studied at later stages of the auditory pathway, but early stages remain less examined. However, changes in early stages could be important because they would affect how information about sounds is conveyed to higher-order areas for further processing and localization. We examined the effects of CHL at auditory nerve synapses onto bushy cells in the mouse anteroventral cochlear nucleus following occlusion of the ear canal. These synapses, called endbulbs of Held, normally show strong depression in voltage-clamp recordings in brain slices. After 1 week of CHL, endbulbs showed even greater depression, reflecting higher release probability. We observed no differences in quantal size between control and occluded mice. We confirmed these observations using mean-variance analysis and the integration method, which also revealed that the number of release sites decreased after occlusion. Consistent with this, synaptic puncta immunopositive for VGLUT1 decreased in area after occlusion. The level of depression and number of release sites both showed recovery after returning to normal conditions. Finally, bushy cells fired fewer action potentials in response to evoked synaptic activity after occlusion, likely because of increased depression and decreased input resistance. These effects appear to reflect a homeostatic, adaptive response of auditory nerve synapses to reduced activity. These effects may have important implications for perceptual changes following CHL.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Normal hearing is important to everyday life, but abnormal auditory experience during development can lead to processing disorders. For example, otitis media reduces sound to the ear, which can cause long-lasting deficits in language skills and verbal production, but the location of the problem is unknown. Here, we show that occluding the ear causes synapses at the very first stage of the auditory pathway to modify their properties, by decreasing in size and increasing the likelihood of releasing neurotransmitter. This causes synapses to deplete faster, which reduces fidelity at central targets of the auditory nerve, which could affect perception. Temporary hearing loss could cause similar changes at later stages of the auditory pathway, which could contribute to disorders in behavior.
Introduction
Auditory experience can cause changes in nervous system function, which may have adaptive, but also pathological, impacts (Kopp-Scheinpflug and Tempel, 2015). Processing disorders can result from otitis media (OM), which is common during childhood (Holm and Kunze, 1969; Gravel et al., 2006). It has been suggested that the primary risk factor is actually the degree of conductive hearing loss (CHL), rather than status of OM itself (Gravel and Wallace, 2000; Whitton and Polley, 2011). However, it is not known how reduction in auditory activity causes deficits, nor what the cellular origin of deficits is.
Most studies of the effects of CHL have focused on high-order neurons, such as those in the inferior colliculus or auditory cortex (Wang et al., 2002; Xu et al., 2007; Popescu and Polley, 2010; Takesian et al., 2010; de Villers-Sidani and Merzenich, 2011). However, it is important to understand the effects of experience on early parts of the auditory pathway because they could partially underlie changes observed at later levels. Furthermore, changes in early cells and synapses could be magnified in downstream auditory processing, and thereby lead to disorders in behavior.
One important early stage is the anteroventral cochlear nucleus (AVCN). Within the AVCN, auditory nerve fibers terminate on bushy cells (BCs), forming large and powerful synapses called “endbulbs of Held” (Lorente de Nó, 1981; Ryugo and Fekete, 1982). BCs relay precise timing information from the auditory nerve to brain areas involved in sound localization (Smith et al., 1993; Oertel, 1999; Cant and Benson, 2003).
Some studies have examined the effects of reduced activity on endbulbs, by using strains of mice that are congenitally deaf, have early-onset hearing loss, or carry mutations in proteins important to cochlear or synaptic function (Oleskevich and Walmsley, 2002; Wang and Manis, 2005; Mendoza Schulz et al., 2014; Wright et al., 2014). There is also extensive work studying multiple deafening approaches in the chick (Tucci and Rubel, 1985; Tucci et al., 1987; Kuba et al., 2010, 2014). These deafness models have been linked to synaptic changes in release probability (Pr), quantal size (Q), the size of the readily releasable pool of vesicles (N), and general synaptic morphology, which is likely related to N. Different models can have different results, which has made it difficult to determine whether these changes are caused by auditory nerve activity, by a direct effect of the mutated protein, or by nonspecific differences in mouse strains on synaptic transmission. For example, age-related deafness led to decreased Q and Pr (Wang and Manis, 2005), whereas mutation of the presynaptic scaffold bassoon, which also causes hearing loss, resulted in increased Q and Pr (Mendoza Schulz et al., 2014). Changes in Q are suggestive of synaptic scaling, which is a well-established form of activity-dependent homeostasis (O'Brien et al., 1998; Turrigiano et al., 1998; Turrigiano and Nelson, 2004; Keck et al., 2013). However, the opposite effects of these two manipulations on Pr and Q are difficult to reconcile.
To address such issues, we used a milder and reversible manipulation to reduce auditory activity, bilateral occlusion of the ear canal. We examined presynaptic and postsynaptic changes after occlusion using electrophysiology and immunohistochemistry at endbulbs of Held. Endbulbs showed more depression after occlusion, and partially recovered after returning to normal conditions. There was no change in quantal size (Q), but a decrease in the number of release sites (N). In addition, occlusion led to lower firing probability in BCs. These effects suggest the existence of a homeostatic and adaptive mechanism to optimize the efficiency of synapses during low activity. These effects may be important for understanding how CHL induces changes at the very first stage of the auditory system.
Materials and Methods
All experiments were performed with the approval of the University at Buffalo's Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Experimental subjects were CBA/CaJ mice of either sex, ranging in age from postnatal day 15 (P15) to P33.
Bilateral ear occlusion.
Ears were bilaterally occluded by injecting a small volume of silicone elastomer (Kwik-Cast, WPI) into the auditory canal or by ligating the auditory canal at P14 (Popescu and Polley, 2010). Mice were anesthetized with 200 mg/kg ketamine plus 10 mg/kg xylazine. An incision was made beside the pinna, and a portion of the ear canal was exposed and ligated with surgical silk (Harvard Apparatus). The incision was closed using suturing and Vetbond. Mice were restored to their home cages until brain slice electrophysiology recordings. In a subset of mice, ears were ligated at P21 and the ligatures were removed at P24.
Auditory brainstem responses (ABRs).
ABRs were recorded to assess hearing thresholds after ear occlusion and after ear occlusion removal. Mice were anesthetized with 200 mg/kg ketamine plus 10 mg/kg xylazine. ABRs were recorded with a vertex electrode, an electrode inserted behind the pinna ipsilateral to the stimulated ear, and a ground electrode inserted contralateral to the stimulated ear. Clicks (100 μs) were presented through a speaker placed into the ear canal. ABR threshold was obtained for each animal by reducing the stimulus intensity in 10 dB steps and then 5 dB steps to identify the lowest intensity that elicited a response. ABRs were recorded in a sound-proof room with auditory-evoked response averaging system controlled by TDT software (BioSig) (Sun et al., 2009).
Brain slice electrophysiology.
Parasagittal slices of AVCN were cut from the auditory brainstem of P15-P33 mice as described previously (Yang and Xu-Friedman, 2008; Yang and Xu-Friedman, 2015). Briefly, slices (142 μm) were cut in an ice-cold sucrose-based solution containing the following (in mm): 76 NaCl, 75 sucrose, 25 NaHCO3, 25 glucose, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 7 MgCl2, and 0.5 CaCl2. Then slices were transferred and incubated at 30°C for 20 min in standard recording solution containing the following (in mm): 125 NaCl, 26 NaHCO3, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 20 glucose, 1.5 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, 4 Na-l-lactate, 2 Na-pyruvate, 0.4 Na-l-ascorbate, bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2. Slices were then maintained at room temperature until recording. Mean-variance analysis experiments were conducted in different external calcium concentrations (Cae) by replacing 1.5:1.0 CaCl2:MgCl2 with 3:0 or 1:1.5 CaCl2:MgCl2. Strychnine (5 μm) was present during all recordings to block spontaneous glycinergic IPSCs. TTX was not used because blocking action potentials is unnecessary for measuring endbulb mEPSCs. Action potential-evoked EPSCs are easily distinguished from spontaneous quantal release because of the >10-fold difference in amplitude, and auditory nerve fibers do not fire spikes without electrical stimulation. Thus, all small spontaneous synaptic currents reflect quantal release.
BCs were patched under an Olympus BX51WI microscope with a Multiclamp 700B (Molecular Devices) controlled by an ITC-18 interface (Instrutech), driven by custom-written software (mafPC) running in Igor (WaveMetrics). The bath was perfused at 3–4 ml/min using a pump (403U/VM2; Watson-Marlow), with saline running through an inline heater to maintain the temperature at 34°C (SH-27B with TC-324B controller; Warner Instruments).
Pipettes were pulled from borosilicate glass (OD: 1.5 mm, ID: 0.86 mm; Sutter Instruments) to a resistance of 1–2 MΩ and were filled with internal solution containing the following (in mm): for voltage clamp, 35 CsF, 100 CsCl, 10 EGTA, 10 HEPES, and 1 QX-314; or for current clamp, 130 KMeSO3, 10 NaCl, 2 MgCl2, 0.16 CaCl2, 0.5 EGTA, 10 HEPES, 2 Na2ATP, 0.4 NaGTP, and 14 Tris-CrPhos (pH 7.3, 305 mOsm).
For voltage-clamp experiments in Figures 2⇓⇓–5, BCs were held at −70 mV, with access resistance 5–15 MΩ compensated to 70%. Cells were identified by EPSCs having rapid decay kinetics (τ < 0.2 ms) and half-width ≤ 0.5 ms (Chanda and Xu-Friedman, 2010). For current-clamp experiments in Figure 7, BCs were maintained at −60 mV and were identified by their 1 to 2 undershooting spikes in response to depolarizing current pulses (Wu and Oertel, 1984). Single fibers were stimulated using a glass microelectrode placed 30–50 μm away from the soma with currents of 5–22 μA through a stimulus isolator (WPI, A360). Single or paired pulses were applied every 10 s. Train stimuli were applied every 30 s. For the integration method, trains of 45 ESPCs were evoked, in the presence of 1 mm kynurenate to prevent postsynaptic receptor desensitization and saturation.
Immunohistochemistry.
Mice were anesthetized with 200 mg/kg ketamine and 10 mg/kg xylazine, then perfused transcardially with 0.9% saline followed by 4% buffered PFA. Brains were removed and postfixed (room temperature, 2 h) before cryoprotecting in 20% sucrose (4°C, overnight). Sagittal sections were cut frozen at 50 μm thickness on a sliding microtome (American Optical). Slices were treated with 0.5% Triton X-100 in 0.2 m PBS for 10 min. The slices were then blocked in 1% normal goat serum for 1 h at room temperature, washed three times in 0.2 m PBS solution, and incubated overnight at 4°C in primary antibody solution containing the following: anti-VGLUT-1 (Invitrogen) at 1:500, and 0.2 m PBS with 0.5% Triton X-100. The sections were then washed three times in 0.2 m PBS with 0.5% Triton X-10 and incubated for 2 h with 1:200 FITC-conjugated goat anti-rabbit antibody in 0.2 m PBS, 0.5% Triton X-100. The sections were then mounted on glass slides using Fluoromount G with DAPI (Southern Biotechnology).
Confocal images were acquired using an Olympus confocal microscope. All images were taken with similar settings for both control and occlusion conditions. BCs were identified by clear labeling of VGLUT1-positive puncta around the soma, and 3 or 4 well-separated BCs from each mouse were chosen for analysis. BCs without recognizable puncta would not be included in this analysis. All of the quantification was performed blind to rearing conditions by randomly naming image files. To quantify the average number and area of puncta, the optical section at the midline of each cell was selected and Yen's threshold algorithm (Yen et al., 1995) plug-in in ImageJ was applied to identify puncta. The automated analyze particles command in ImageJ was used to scan the threshold image and find the edges of puncta. Outlines of the particles were verified against the original image to confirm the detection of puncta. The puncta not belonging to the cell were excluded, and particles that were part of one puncta were merged.
Data analysis.
Average results are reported throughout as mean ± SEM. All statistical tests are unpaired Student's t tests, except where otherwise noted.
Results
Verifying ear occlusion and removal
To generate bilateral CHL, we occluded ears reversibly, using either an earplug or ligature of the ear canal. We validated successful ear occlusion and occlusion removal by measuring hearing threshold with ABRs. In the example of Figure 1A (left), a control mouse at P20 had a hearing threshold of 30 dB SPL. For a mouse with ear canal ligated at P14, the threshold was 80 dB SPL measured at P19 (Fig. 1A, middle), reflecting a decrease in sensitivity of ∼50 dB SPL. In the example shown in Figure 1A (right), the mouse was ligated at P21, then unligated at P24, and by P27, the threshold recovered to 30 dB SPL, indicating that these shifts were reversible. We found similar results in all ears tested. The average threshold of control mice was 33.0 ± 1.2 dB SPL (5 ears). Following occlusion, threshold was significantly elevated to 80.0 ± 1.1 dB SPL (8 ears; p < 0.01), and after removing occlusion, threshold was back to 36.0 ± 2.0 dB SPL (7 ears; p = 0.28) (Fig. 1B). This indicates that the methods to create occluded and recovered ears are valid and reliable.
Occlusion affects endbulb function
We studied how bilateral occlusion affects synaptic function by characterizing endbulbs in terms of release probability (Pr), quantal size (Q), and number of release sites (N). We first assessed Pr by quantifying the level of synaptic depression. We recorded pairs of EPSCs at different intervals (Δt) from BCs in whole-cell voltage clamp, and calculated the paired-pulse ratio (PPR), which is the amplitude of the second EPSC normalized to the first (PPR = EPSC2/EPSC1). Endbulbs normally show depression (Fig. 2A, left) and small PPR (Fig. 2B, open circles) at all intervals, indicating they have high Pr. Endbulbs from occluded mice showed even stronger depression (Fig. 2A, right) and smaller PPR (Fig. 2B, closed circles), indicating they have higher Pr. We found similar results across multiple recordings. Average PPR for Δt = 3 ms was 0.50 ± 0.03 (13 cells) in control mice versus 0.31 ± 0.02 (22 cells) in occluded mice, which is significantly lower (p < 0.01). PPRs at other intervals were also significantly lower following occlusion (Fig. 2C; p < 0.05), indicating that endbulbs had higher Pr after occlusion.
In theory, if all other synaptic properties remain equal, an increase in Pr should lead to larger initial EPSCs. However, we found that the amplitudes of first EPSC (EPSC1) in control versus occluded mice were not significantly different (Fig. 2D; control: 28 cells; occlusion: 45 cells; p = 0.34, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test). This indicates that other changes must be taking place to offset the increase in Pr.
In addition, it is important to understand how quickly Pr changes following occlusion. To address this, we occluded ears at P14 (the onset of hearing) and assessed PPR for Δt = 3 ms starting from P15 (Fig. 2E). We detected a significant decrease after 1 week of occlusion, starting from P21 (Fig. 2F).
Quantal size remains unchanged after occlusion
An important form of activity-dependent homeostasis is synaptic scaling, in which decreased activity leads to an increase in Q (O'Brien et al., 1998; Turrigiano et al., 1998; Turrigiano and Nelson, 2004; Keck et al., 2013). We tested for synaptic scaling in endbulbs by recording spontaneous mEPSCs in voltage clamp from occluded and control mice (Fig. 3A). The average frequency and amplitude were calculated by recording 70–250 mEPSCs from each cell. Spontaneous mEPSC frequency in occluded mice was similar to that in control mice (control: 12 cells; occlusion: 13 cells; p = 0.60, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test; Fig. 3B). Spontaneous average mEPSC amplitudes in occluded mice were also similar to those in control mice (control: 12 cells; occlusion: 13 cells; p = 0.56, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test) (Fig. 3C). The mEPSC amplitudes were also analyzed by taking 100 mEPSCs from cells with sufficient number of mESPCs and merging into two overall distributions for control versus occluded conditions. This approach also showed no difference between control and occluded mESPC amplitudes (control: 8 cells; occlusion: 6 cells; p = 0.12, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test; Fig. 3D). Thus, the mEPSCs were not affected by occlusion, suggesting that synaptic scaling does not occur under our conditions. Instead, a separate mechanism appears to be involved in the changes we observed in response to reduced activity.
Quantification of Pr, N, and Q
Our direct measurements of PPRs and mEPSCs suggested that Pr increased and Q did not change after CHL. To verify these changes and also to quantify N, we used mean-variance analysis and the integration method. Mean-variance analysis is based on considering neurotransmitter release as a binomial random process, so the mean (μ) and variance (σ2) of the EPSC amplitude are related according to σ2 = Qμ − (μ2/N), when Pr is varied by changing external calcium concentrations (Cae) (Silver, 2003; Foster and Regehr, 2004). We measured EPSC amplitude in different Cae (1, 1.5, and 3 mm) in BCs from control (Fig. 4A) and occluded mice (Fig. 4C). Mean-variance plots were fitted with the equation above to estimate N and Q, and then Pr at normal Cae (1.5 mm) could be calculated by Pr = EPSC/QN (Fig. 4B,D). Figure 4 shows examples of a control endbulb (Fig. 4A,B) and an occluded endbulb (Fig. 4C,D). We made similar measurements in a number of BCs, which indicated that after occlusion N decreased significantly (Fig. 4E; control: 98 ± 8, 9 cells; occlusion: 70 ± 9, 16 cells; p = 0.03), and Pr increased significantly (Fig. 4F; control: 0.43 ± 0.04, 9 cells; occlusion: 0.55 ± 0.03, 16 cells; p = 0.03). No significant difference was detected between control and occluded mice in Q (Fig. 4G; control: 124 ± 12, 9 cells; occlusion: 151 ± 15, 16 cells; p = 0.17).
In addition to the mean-variance analysis, we also used an independent method based on long trains (Schneggenburger et al., 1999; Thanawala and Regehr, 2013, 2016). EPSCs (100 Hz, 45 pulses) were evoked from control (Fig. 4H) and occluded BCs (Fig. 4J), in the presence of 1 mm kynurenate to prevent postsynaptic receptor desensitization and saturation. The EPSCs were integrated, and the rate of vesicle recovery was estimated by a straight-line fit to the last 20 pulses of the cumulative EPSC (Fig. 4I,K). The y-intercept was proportional to N, and dividing the first EPSC by N yields Pr. We did similar experiments in a number of control and occluded BCs. The results indicated that N significantly decreased (Fig. 4L; control: 5.2 ± 0.6 nA, 11 cells; occlusion: 3.7 ± 0.4 nA, 10 cells; p = 0.04) and Pr significantly increased (Fig. 4M; control: 0.38 ± 0.03, 11 cells; occlusion: 0.47 ± 0.03, 10 cells; p = 0.04) after occlusion. Thus, both methods showed that occlusion led to increased Pr, and mean-variance analysis also showed no change in Q, consistent with our measurements of PPR and mEPSCs. In addition, both methods indicated N decreased after ear occlusion, which may account for the unchanged EPSC1 amplitude we observed in Figure 2D.
Recovery of Pr and N
We also tested whether the changes in Pr and N we observed in Figure 4 recover after returning to normal sound conditions. We found that, for ears ligated at very early ages (P14), it was difficult to surgically reopen the ear canal. Therefore, for this set of experiments, we ligated the ear canal at P21 and unligated at P24 (Fig. 5A). We first used the integration method to confirm that 3 d of ligation was enough to affect N and Pr (Fig. 5A, “Occ”). We found that, by P24, N decreased compared with age-matched controls (Fig. 5A, “Ctrlearly”) (control: 5.5 ± 0.7 nA, 10 cells; occlusion: 3.6 ± 0.6 nA, 11 cells; p = 0.04; Fig. 5B) and Pr increased (control: 0.39 ± 0.03, 10 cells; occlusion: 0.54 ± 0.04, 11 cells; p = 0.009; Fig. 5C). Then in another group of mice, we unligated at P24 and assessed recovery of N and Pr after 1 week (Fig. 5A, “Rec”). In these animals, N returned to near age-matched controls (Fig. 5A, “Ctrllate”) (control: 5.2 ± 0.9 nA, 13 cells; recovery: 5.0 ± 0.8 nA, 15 cells; p = 0.83; Fig. 5B) and Pr decreased toward control levels (control: 0.37 ± 0.05, 13 cells; recovery: 0.45 ± 0.02, 15 cells; p = 0.13; Fig. 5C). Thus, Pr does show recovery after hearing is restored, but recovery does not appear to be complete after 1 week, while N seems to fully recover.
We were concerned that the mathematical analyses associated with the integration method could introduce additional variability into the estimate of Pr. Therefore, we also assessed recovery using the PPR at Δt = 3 ms, which is very sensitive to changes in Pr. PPR decreased significantly from 0.55 ± 0.04 (Ctrlearly, 13 cells) to 0.41 ± 0.03 (Occ, 10 cells) after 3 d of occlusion (p = 0.01), reflecting an increase in Pr. In another set of mice, we occluded ears at P21 and unoccluded them at P24, then measured PPR after P30. PPR in these animals was 0.55 ± 0.04 (Rec, 9 cells), which was significantly greater than the occluded cells (p = 0.03), but not significantly different from PPR measured in mice after P30 that were never occluded (Ctrllate, PPR = 0.53 ± 0.04, 10 cells, p = 0.85). Thus, PPR appeared to recover completely after occlusion was removed.
Endbulb structural changes after occlusion
The physiological experiments using mean-variance analysis and the integration method indicated that ear occlusion caused a decrease in the number of release sites or vesicles (Fig. 4E,L). Deafening in mice and cats has been linked to changes in endbulb structure (Limb and Ryugo, 2000; O'Neil et al., 2011; Connelly et al., 2016), so we examined whether ear occlusion had similar effects by using VGLUT1 immunohistochemistry, which labels glutamatergic vesicles around multiple release sites (Lauer et al., 2013; Ngodup et al., 2015). Figure 6A shows examples of VGLUT1-labeled endbulbs from control (left) and occluded mice (right), which contained strong punctate labeling surrounding the BC somata. We quantified the number of puncta around the cell body in single optical sections in the middle of the cell. We used an automated detection and measurement algorithm (Yen et al., 1995) in ImageJ and did the analysis blind to treatment conditions. Figure 6B shows outlines of puncta around the BC cell body from example control (Fig. 6B, left) and occluded (Fig. 6B, right) mice. We did the same analysis on multiple cells from control and occluded mice. There was no significant change in the number of puncta after occlusion (Fig. 6C; control: 12.3 ± 1.1, 10 cells; occlusion: 12.8 ± 0.9, 10 cells; p = 0.72). By contrast, the average area of puncta shrank after occlusion (Fig. 6D; control: 2.6 ± 0.3 μm2, 10 cells; occlusion: 1.6 ± 0.2 μm2, 14 cells; p = 0.01). This indicates that endbulb structure changes following occlusion and is consistent with the decrease in the number of release sites found using mean-variance analysis and the integration method.
Decreasing excitability of BCs after occlusion
In addition to synaptic properties, acoustic environment and activity can also affect postsynaptic intrinsic properties, such as by modulating channels (Song et al., 2005). We characterized the intrinsic electrical properties in current-clamp recordings by injecting current pulses into BCs from control and occluded mice. As in control (Oertel, 1983), BCs from occluded mice fired one undershooting action potential in response to depolarizing current injection (Fig. 7A). There was no significant difference in firing thresholds between BCs of control and occluded mice (Fig. 7B; control: −46.2 ± 1.2 mV, 8 cells; occlusion: −46.6 ± 0.7 mV, 24 cells; p = 0.77). We also used these data to characterize the input resistance (Rin). Figure 7C shows the average membrane voltage as a function of current injection for control and occluded BCs. The shallower slope in Figure 7C indicated that Rin decreased after occlusion. We calculated Rin around resting membrane potential (−60 mV), which was significantly higher in control compared with occluded mice (Fig. 7D; control: 66.3 ± 9.2 MΩ, 8 cells; occlusion: 38.8 ± 6.7 MΩ, 24 cells; p = 0.017). This may indicate that the expression level of potassium channels at postsynaptic BCs increased following occlusion (Barnes-Davies et al., 2004; Lu et al., 2004).
The deeper depression and lower Rin would both be expected to influence synaptically evoked spiking in BCs. We tested this by stimulating a single auditory nerve fiber 20 times at 100 or 200 Hz. After occlusion, the BCs fired fewer action potentials at both 100 (Fig. 7E) and 200 Hz (Fig. 7F). We quantified firing probability during the second half of the train (pulses 11–20) when EPSPs are near steady state. Firing probability was significantly lower in occluded mice than in control mice at both 100 Hz (Fig. 7G; control: 0.84 ± 0.10, 5 cells; occlusion: 0.39 ± 0.12, 9 cells; p = 0.013) and 200 Hz (Fig. 7H; control: 0.50 ± 0.10, 5 cells; occlusion: 0.12 ± 0.05, 9 cells; p = 0.014). Therefore, it appears that the excitability of BCs decreased following occlusion.
Discussion
We have shown that CHL changes fundamental properties of endbulbs and BCs in the AVCN. Pr increased after occlusion and reverted when normal conditions were restored. Moreover, N decreased, appearing to compensate for increased Pr to maintain a constant EPSC1. Q remained constant in response to occlusion. These results indicate that endbulbs respond homeostatically to CHL, but not through synaptic scaling. These as well as intrinsic changes in Rin led to decreased postsynaptic firing in BCs.
These cellular and synaptic changes appear to optimize cellular properties in response to reduced auditory activity. High Pr causes synaptic depletion, but when the activity is low, there is increased opportunity for recovery, so the consequences of high Pr are minimal. By contrast, maintaining a large endbulb with many unused vesicles may be inefficient. Therefore, dropping N and raising Pr could make endbulbs more efficient when auditory activity is low.
Consequences of CHL
These synaptic and cellular effects may have relevance for the long-lasting effects of OM, which is common among young children (Gates, 1996; Zhou et al., 2008). OM can have long-term behavioral consequences, including shifts in sensitivity to interaural level and timing differences and also in FM detection thresholds (Thornton et al., 2012; Buran et al., 2014; Caras and Sanes, 2015). However, the cellular basis is not well understood. Our studies showed that Pr increased at endbulbs during CHL, which may initially yield little change in spike fidelity at endbulbs when activity is reduced. However, when normal hearing is restored and activity increases, synaptic depletion could become significant, which would reduce spike fidelity and possibly impair perception.
Importantly, we observed changes at the start of the auditory pathway, and similar mechanisms likely exist throughout the pathway. These changes may have negative consequences, particularly if CHL resolves and the synapses do not recover accordingly. Early CHL can engender susceptibility to audiogenic seizures (Henry, 1967; Sun et al., 2011). In auditory cortex, pyramidal neurons and inhibitory synapses onto them were permanently altered when CHL occurred before P18 (Mowery et al., 2015), which may underlie changes in auditory behavior following CHL (Popescu and Polley, 2010; Polley et al., 2013; Caras and Sanes, 2015). We found that Pr at endbulbs largely recovered after normal sound conditions were restored (Fig. 5), indicating that endbulbs adapt to acoustic conditions even after early development. Such flexibility may be needed because the auditory periphery continues to change throughout life (Schmiedt et al., 1996; Skoe et al., 2015). Therefore, it may be useful to study the cellular mechanisms underlying CHL-dependent changes in endbulbs, to learn how to reverse nonadaptive changes at other stages of the auditory pathway that impede normal processing.
It will also be important to establish how other synapses in the auditory pathway are affected by CHL. This has been considered at the calyx of Held in the medial nucleus of the trapezoid body using unilateral CHL. Calyces carrying information from the occluded ear showed no change in Pr, but calyces from the nonoccluded ear showed reduced Pr (Grande et al., 2014). Also, unilateral CHL led to changes in AMPAR expression (Wang et al., 2011; Clarkson et al., 2016). Such receptor changes might affect mEPSCs, but we observed no changes in mEPSC amplitude or decay with bilateral occlusion. It is difficult to tell whether these effects reflect a fundamentally different underlying phenomenon or whether changes in activity are more complicated following unilateral occlusion, compared with the bilateral occlusion.
We found that both juvenile and mature synapses in mice were sensitive to CHL, suggesting that human endbulbs could be too, although there are significant differences between the auditory systems of mice and humans. To understand this better, it will be important to investigate further the time course and age dependence of recovery, as well as the perceptual consequences.
Other models of hearing loss
Our results may relate to additional models of reduced activity. For example, mutations of bassoon, a presynaptic scaffolding protein, and otoferlin, the putative calcium sensor at hair cell synapses, both reduced afferent activity and increased Pr at endbulbs (Mendoza Schulz et al., 2014; Wright et al., 2014), consistent with our findings. Bassoon and otoferlin mutations also led to larger Q, likely reflecting homeostatic synaptic scaling (Turrigiano et al., 1998; Keck et al., 2013), which we did not observe after occlusion (Figs. 3, 4). This may be because ligation preserves much of ear function, including spontaneous activity and receptivity to self-generated sounds. Mutations of bassoon and otoferlin may have had a more severe impact on auditory activity, which could recruit additional homeostatic mechanisms, such as synaptic scaling. This would imply that there are multiple signaling pathways with different thresholds for activity that induce changes in Pr and N vs Q.
By contrast, early-onset hearing loss in DBA mice led to smaller Pr and Q (Wang and Manis, 2005), which is not consistent with the effects we observed after occlusion. To reconcile these findings, it will be important to understand how cochlear degeneration affects acoustically driven and spontaneous activity, as well as the activity-dependent signaling pathways that regulate endbulb properties.
Furthermore, many studies have examined how deafening affects the avian auditory system using such manipulations as middle and inner ear damage (Tucci and Rubel, 1985; Tucci et al., 1987; Harris and Rubel, 2006; Kuba et al., 2010, 2014). Such manipulations have dramatic effects on cell structure and excitability in the homologous nucleus magnocellularis. However, the consequences for chick endbulb physiology are not well known. It will be interesting to examine whether similar manipulations have similar effects in mammalian and avian systems.
Quantification of N, Pr, and Q
We assessed synaptic parameters using mean-variance analysis and the integration method. It is important to interpret the values of N, Pr, and Q appropriately. Endbulb AMPARs show significant saturation (Chanda and Xu-Friedman, 2010), so for mean-variance analysis, the synaptic parameters we measured in the absence of a low-affinity antagonist likely relate to release sites rather than individual vesicles. Our mean-variance estimate of N in control was ∼100, which is on the same order as previous estimates of the number of release sites (∼126) and is smaller than the estimated vesicle pool (∼300) (Nicol and Walmsley, 2002; Mendoza Schulz et al., 2014). In addition, the estimate of Q from mean-variance analysis (∼120 pA) was larger than the mEPSC measured in voltage clamp (∼100 pA). This is consistent with the mean-variance Q reflecting the average quantal size of an entire release site, rather than the receptors activated by a single mEPSC.
The integration method also yields quantitative estimates of N and Pr, but these values must be interpreted differently. Kynurenate was used in these experiments to prevent postsynaptic receptor saturation, so the value of Pr should relate to individual vesicles rather than release sites. Also, because kynurenate blocks the majority of AMPA receptors, Q is difficult to measure and, therefore, N cannot be converted to the number of releasable vesicles.
Bidirectional, experience-dependent, homeostatic mechanism at endbulbs
The effects of occlusion at endbulbs are likely related to the effects of exposure to constant noise (Ngodup et al., 2015). After noise exposure, endbulbs decreased Pr and increased N, and BC spiking was enhanced. Furthermore, neither occlusion nor noise-exposure led to changes in EPSC1 or Q. In addition, Pr recovered to near-normal after noise-exposure and occlusion ceased. These parallels suggest that endbulbs optimize synaptic properties over a wide range of sound conditions using a single activity-dependent, homeostatic mechanism. This presynaptic mechanism could be related to activity-dependent changes in Pr seen in hippocampal cell culture and the Drosophila neuromuscular junction (Frank et al., 2006; Branco et al., 2008). However, in those systems, changes take place within a few hours, which is much faster than at endbulbs.
Postsynaptic effects by contrast do not appear to be subject to a single mechanism. After occlusion, spike threshold remained constant and Rin decreased, whereas after noise-rearing, spike threshold decreased and Rin remained constant (Ngodup et al., 2015). A change in spike threshold could arise from changes in the axon initial segment (Grubb and Burrone, 2010; Kuba et al., 2010). A change in Rin could occur through an increase in K channel expression (Barnes-Davies et al., 2004; Lu et al., 2004). Thus, changes in postsynaptic excitability may result from two separate, activity-dependent mechanisms.
Accounting for normal variability in Pr
Our results may shed light on the regulation of Pr under normal sound conditions. In vitro, the amount of depression among endbulbs varies considerably (Yang and Xu-Friedman, 2009), and in vivo, auditory nerve fibers vary in their amount of activity (Kiang and Watanabe, 1965; Liberman, 1978). Experience-dependent changes of Pr may connect diversity in firing rate to Pr: low firing rate fibers would have higher Pr, whereas high firing rate fibers would have lower Pr. Spontaneous rate of auditory nerve fibers has been linked to synaptic morphology (Ryugo and Rouiller, 1988), with high-rate fibers having larger and more intricate structures. Furthermore, synaptic morphology correlates with Pr at the calyx of Held (Grande and Wang, 2011). This mechanism may also account for the fact that auditory nerve fibers that converge on the same BC have similar Pr, but onto different BCs do not (Yang and Xu-Friedman, 2009, 2012), because fibers with similar firing rate appear to converge on the same BCs (Ryugo and Sento, 1991). Finally, our results add more evidence that Pr is functionally important, because it is difficult to understand why Pr would change in different activity conditions if it were not. Additional work will be needed to test how changes in Pr and N at endbulbs support auditory processing.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Science Foundation Grant 1208131 to M.A.X.-F. We thank H. Yang, Y. Yang, T. Ngodup, N. Wong, and G. Si for helpful comments on the manuscript.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Matthew A. Xu-Friedman, Department of Biological Sciences, University at Buffalo, State University of New York, Buffalo, NY 14260. mx{at}buffalo.edu