Abstract
Binge alcohol drinking, a behavior characterized by rapid repeated alcohol intake, is most prevalent in young adults and is a risk factor for excessive alcohol consumption and alcohol dependence. Although the alteration of synaptic plasticity is thought to contribute to this behavior, there is currently little evidence that this is the case. We used drinking in the dark (DID) as a model of binge alcohol drinking to assess its effects on spike timing-dependent plasticity (STDP) in medium spiny neurons (MSNs) of the core nucleus accumbens (NAc) by combining patch-clamp recordings with calcium imaging and optogenetics. After 2 weeks of daily alcohol binges, synaptic plasticity was profoundly altered. STDP in MSNs expressing dopamine D1 receptors shifted from spike-timing-dependent long-term depression (tLTD), the predominant form of plasticity in naive male mice, to spike-timing-dependent long-term potentiation (tLTP) in DID mice, an effect that was totally reversed in the presence of 4 μm SCH23390, a dopamine D1 receptor antagonist. In MSNs presumably expressing dopamine D2 receptors, tLTP, the main form of plasticity in naive mice, was inhibited in DID mice. Interestingly, 1 μm sulpiride, a D2 receptor antagonist, restored tLTP. Although we observed no alterations of AMPA and NMDA receptor properties, we found that the AMPA/NMDA ratio increased at cortical and amygdaloid inputs but not at hippocampal inputs. Also, DID effects on STDP were accompanied by lower dendritic calcium transients. These data suggest that the role of dopamine in mediating the effects of binge alcohol drinking on synaptic plasticity of NAc MSNs differs markedly whether these neurons belong to the direct or indirect pathways.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT We examined the relationship between binge alcohol drinking and spike timing-dependent plasticity in nucleus accumbens (NAc) neurons. We found that repeated drinking bouts modulate differently synaptic plasticity in medium spiny neurons of the accumbens direct and indirect pathways. While timing-dependent long-term depression switches to long-term potentiation (LTP) in the former, timing-dependent LTP is inhibited in the latter. These effects are not accompanied by changes in AMPA and NMDA receptor properties at cortical, amygdaloid, and hippocampal synapses. Interestingly, dopamine D1 and D2 receptor antagonists have opposite effects on plasticity. Our data show that whether core NAc medium spiny neurons belong to the direct or indirect pathways determines the form of spike timing-dependent plasticity (STDP), the manner by which STDP responds to binge alcohol drinking, and its sensitivity to dopamine receptor antagonists.
Introduction
As originally described by Tomsovic (1974), binge alcohol drinking is defined as repeated periods of heavy drinking, followed by periods of abstinence. In a recent attempt to standardize the study of binge alcohol drinking, an updated definition stipulates that binge alcohol drinking brings blood alcohol levels to ≥0.08%, which typically corresponds to consuming five or more drinks (male) or four or more drinks (female) in ∼2 h (National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 2004). This drinking pattern has been studied mostly in young adult populations (e.g., college students) as they represent by far the largest group affected by this behavior (Naimi et al., 2003). Not only does heavy binge alcohol drinking potentially lead to destructive behaviors like suicide, insomnia, drunk driving (Wechsler et al., 1994), and cognitive deficits (Duka et al., 2004), but it is also recognized as being the precursor to long-term alcohol-related problems like sleep disorders, stroke, and social anxiety (Townshend and Duka, 2002; Weissenborn and Duka, 2003; Hartley et al., 2004). Importantly, binge drinkers, contrary to light drinkers, typically display a strong response to early euphoric effects but are less sensitive to the sedative effects of alcohol, indicative of a predisposition for the development of alcohol addiction (Schuckit, 1994; Schuckit et al., 2008).
While binge alcohol drinking has been studied extensively from a behavioral and epidemiological perspective, there is comparatively very little known about how it impacts neurological processes. To date, a number of studies have shown that binge alcohol drinking alters the function of a number of ionotropic (e.g., AMPA) and metabotropic (e.g., mGluR1 and 5) glutamate receptors (Cozzoli et al., 2009; Lum et al., 2014; Agoglia et al., 2015; Silvestre de Ferron et al., 2015), possibly through protein kinases (Gibb et al., 2011; Cozzoli et al., 2016). This has led some to propose that synaptic plasticity may be one of the binge alcohol drinking targets (Stephens and Duka, 2008). In a recent study (Silvestre de Ferron et al., 2015), two consecutive ethanol “binges” in the hippocampi of adolescent rats reversibly abolished long-term depression (LTD) and evoked cognitive deficits via a short-lasting, repeated blockade of NMDA receptors, seemingly contradicting two earlier reports (Roberto et al., 2002; Stephens et al., 2005) showing an inhibitory effect of binge alcohol drinking on long-term potentiation (LTP) in the same brain region. Yet, whether and how binge alcohol drinking modulates synaptic plasticity, and by extension memory formation in other brain regions targeted by drugs of abuse, remains unknown. To address this problem, we examined synaptic plasticity in medium spiny neurons (MSNs) of the core NAc, a brain region known for its central role in mediating the rewarding properties of all drugs of abuse, including alcohol (Koob, 1996). In light of a recent report showing that ethanol blocked conventional LTD in NAc MSNs in a different model of chronic alcohol exposure [i.e., chronic intermittent ethanol exposure (CIE); Renteria et al., 2017], we wondered whether drinking in the dark (DID), a model of shorter ethanol exposure, also regulates spike timing-dependent plasticity (STDP). Additionally, Shen et al. (2008), having reported that dopamine receptors differentially modulate STDP of the dorsal striatum direct and indirect pathways, we hypothesize that they also differentially alter the response of these pathways to binge alcohol drinking in core NAc MSNs. Our data show that whether core NAc MSNs belong to the direct or indirect pathways determines the form of STDP, the manner by which STDP responds to binge alcohol drinking, and its sensitivity to dopamine receptor antagonists.
Materials and Methods
All experiments were performed on male C57BL/6J and D1r.Td-tomato mice. All mice were handled according to the American Association for the Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care guidelines. The protocol was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Massachusetts Medical School. Mice were maintained at constant temperature and humidity with a 12 h light/dark cycle. Water and food were provided ad libitum.
Mice surgeries.
We injected 21- to 24-d-old (10–15 g) C57BL/6J mice with adeno-associated virus containing pAAV9-CaMKIIa-hChR2(H134R)-EYFP (a gift from Dr. K. Deisseroth, Stanford University, Palo Alto, CA) bilaterally (0.8 μl in each side) using a Hamilton syringe with a 2-inch-long 26 ga needle stereotaxically placed into the prelimbic PFCx (anteroposterior, +1.85 mm from bregma; mediolateral, +0.3 mm from bregma; ventral, −2.9 mm from skull surface), the amygdala basal nucleus (anteroposterior, −0.9 mm from bregma; mediolateral, +2.8 mm from bregma; ventral, −5 mm from skull surface), or the ventral hippocampus (anteroposterior, −3.3 mm from bregma; mediolateral, 2.8 mm from bregma; ventral, −4.8 mm from skull surface). Virus injections in the PFCx, amygdala, and hippocampus were performed on different mice. The injector was left in place for 5 min, raised 1 mm, and left for an additional 5 min before being removed. We returned mice to their home cages for 21 d before performing electrophysiological experiments.
Drinking in the dark.
Two days following brain viral injections, mice were allowed to adapt to the reverse light/dark cycle (12 h cycle, off at 8:00 A.M., on at 8:00 P.M.) for 1 week, at which point they were single housed. They were given water bottles with sipper tubes before the experiment to habituate them and reduce the novelty effect once the ethanol bottle, containing a similar sipper tube, was presented. The total habituation time for the reverse cycle was 2 weeks before the experiment began. The experiment started 2 h into the dark phase of the cycle and lasted for 2 h (Rhodes et al., 2005; Hendrickson et al., 2009). At the start of the experiment, each water bottle was removed and replaced with a preweighed 50 ml conical tube containing 20% ethanol (or 10% sucrose for control mice) with a rubber stopper and double-ball bearing sipper tube. The mice were allowed to drink for 2 h, and then the ethanol bottles were removed, weighed, and the water bottles were replaced. Ethanol consumed was measured as grams of ethanol divided by mouse body weight in kilograms. This protocol was repeated 5 d/week with 2 d off (water only) after each 5 d span. Drip controls were used to account for evaporation and dripping, and experimental bottle weights were corrected using the control values. On average, mice steadily increased their consumption of alcohol before reaching a maximum ∼7 g/kg 20% EtOH per drinking session at the end of a 2 week period (Fig. 1D; n = 15). All electrophysiological recordings were performed 24 h after the last drinking bout during the third week.
Slice preparation.
To prepare coronal slices from fresh brain tissue, we rapidly removed and transferred the brain in a cold (∼0.5°C), oxygenated (95% O2 and 5% CO2) solution with the following composition (in mm): 95 N-methyl-d-glucamine (NMDG), 2 thiourea, 5 Na+-ascorbate, and 3 Na+-pyruvate into cut slices (300 μm) with a Vibroslicer (VT1200, Leica MicroInstrutments). Slices were immediately transferred in an incubation chamber and left to recuperate in a NMDG-based solution for 22 min at 32°C before being moved into a chamber containing an artificial CSF (ACSF) as follows (in mm): 126 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, NaH2PO4.H2O, 1 MgCl2, 2 CaCl2, 26 NaHCO3, and 10 d-glucose, at room temperature. Slices were left in this chamber for at least 1 h before being placed in a recording chamber and perfused with ACSF at a constant rate of 2–3 ml/min at room temperature (∼21°C). We visualized neurons in infrared differential interference contrast (60×, infrared differential interference contrast) videomicroscopy using a fully motorized upright microscope (Scientifica).
Electrophysiology.
We performed whole-cell patch-clamp recordings in the absence of GABA receptor antagonists as described previously (Ji et al., 2015). Briefly, we filled borosilicate glass electrodes (outer diameter, 1.5 mm; resistance, 4–6 MΩ) with an internal solution containing the following (in mm): 120 K-methanesulfonate; 20 KCl; 10 HEPES; 2 K2ATP, 2 K2GTP, and 12 phosphocreatine. We replaced K-methanesulfonate with Cs-methanesulfonate and added 1 μm QX-314 in the pipette solution when recording AMPA and NMDA EPSCs. Following seal rupture, series resistance (Rs), typically ranging between 10 and 20 MΩ, was fully compensated in current-clamp recording mode, and periodically monitored throughout recording sessions. Recordings with Rs changes >20% were rejected. We acquired voltage and current traces in whole-cell patch clamp with an EPC10 amplifier (HEKA Elektronik). We sampled and filtered voltage and current traces acquired with PatchMaster version 2.15 software (HEKA Elektronik) at 10 kHz (20 kHz during induction of plasticity) and 2 kHz, respectively. We subsequently analyzed all traces off-line using FitMaster 2.15 software (HEKA Electronik). We evoked EPSPs by placing a concentric bipolar stimulating electrode in proximity to the recording patch pipette. To generate synaptic plasticity, we paired postsynaptic action potential (AP; evoked with a 5 ms/700–800 pA depolarizing pulse) and electrically- and optogenetically-driven EPSPs with a 20 ms interval at a rate of 1 Hz for 90 s, as described previously (Ji et al., 2015). For optogenetic experiments, we evoked EPSPs-EPSCs by flashing 1-ms-long 470 nm blue light through the light path of a microscope 60× objective using a high-powered LED (pE-100, CooLED) under the control of the acquisition software (PatchMaster, HEKA Electronik). When examining AMPA/NMDA ratio in voltage-clamp mode, we evoked AMPA-EPSCs at −70 mV in the presence of 15 μm bicuculline (BIC). After blocking AMPA-EPSPs for 3–4 min after 5 μm NBQX perfusion, we held membrane potential at +40 mV to evoke NMDA-EPSCs. We purchased QX-314 from Ascent Scientific, and BIC, ethanol, and NBQX from Sigma-Aldrich.
Two-photon laser scanning calcium imaging.
We filled recording pipettes with Fluo5F (20 μm) and Alexa Fluor 549 (30 μm; Invitrogen). Dendritic locations were determined based on the loading of Alexa Fluor 594 after breaking into the cell (15–25 min). A custom-made two-photon laser scanner (Scientifica) with a Mai Tai HP Ti:Sapphire (Spectra-Physics) laser providing pulses at <100 fs at 820 nm was used for imaging. Linescans along each dendritic location were performed using a 60× (1.0 numerical aperture) objective (Olympus). We recorded activity at various dendritic locations from the soma with clear branch points (1° = primary, 2° = secondary, 3° = tertiary). Calcium transients were acquired using a standardized protocol controlled by ScanImage (Vidrio) that allowed the laser beam to be “parked” between each image capture. We used PatchMaster version 2.15 to evoke theta burst firing patterns in response to 800 pA current injections to drive Ca2+ transients in MSNs. Only cells with a stable resting membrane potential between −82 and −90 mV were selected for recordings. All experiments were conducted at 32–34°C. The protocol consisted of three consecutive potentials at a frequency of 5 Hz, with 10 s intervals, and at 42 frames/s. Fluorescence ratios were calculated based on ΔF = F/F0, where the baseline fluorescence (F0) was the average of the first 20 frames. Three responses were averaged and then smoothed using Savitzky–Golay (first-order polynomial, 15 point) filtering to acquire the individual calcium traces for each dendritic location. Slope of the rise of the signal was calculated as shown in Figure 5C. Correlative comparisons between dendritic locations were calculated based on the percentage of the difference between EtOH-naive dendrites and DID dendrite/DID dendrite. Maximum amplitude of the signal was captured for each calcium trace, within a limited response window of 15 frames following the baseline fluorescence. All analysis was conducted using custom-written code in MATLAB.
Analysis of timing-dependent LTP and timing-dependent LTD.
We compared the maximum amplitude of EPSPs measured in a 20 ms time window 10 ms before and after the onset of the stimulus. We performed this measurement on 30 consecutive EPSPs before and 20 min after AP-EPSP pairing to smooth out the natural variability in the amplitude of EPSPs. We expressed the difference of EPSP amplitude before and after induction as a percentage of control (100%). Statistical analysis of EPSC and action potential properties in naive and DID mice was performed with a two-tailed t test and of calcium transients with the Mann–Whitney test in Prism 7 (GraphPad). For experiments comparing STDP in various alcohol exposures, we used a one-way ANOVA and Bartlett's post hoc test. For experiments comparing STDP in dopamine D1 receptors [D1R(+)] and dopamine D2 receptors [D1R(−)] MSNs, we used a two-way ANOVA and a post hoc Bonferroni's multiple-comparison analysis to test statistical significance when comparing the magnitude of STDP in multifactorial experimental conditions. All averaged results are expressed as the mean ± SEM. In all tests, p values <0.05 were considered to be statistically significant.
Results
Binge alcohol drinking affects both timing-dependent LTP and timing-dependent LTD in core accumbens medium spiny neurons
In naive mice, pairing of AP and electrically-driven EPSP (eEPSP) evoked tLTP (Fig. 1A,E, Naive, red symbol) and tLTD (Fig. 1E, black symbol) in equal proportion, confirming previous results (Ji and Martin, 2012). Figure 1A shows representative tLTP in a naive mouse MSN. In fresh slices of tissue from naive mice acutely exposed to 20 mm EtOH, a concentration typically reached during binge alcohol drinking (Hwa et al., 2011), not only was tLTP nearly totally inhibited but EtOH now favored tLTD as the dominant form of synaptic plasticity [acute EtOH (aEtOH); Fig. 1B,E], as previously reported in the same brain region (Ji et al., 2015). To test whether a history of binge alcohol drinking affected synaptic plasticity in core NAc MSNs, we exposed mice to ethanol daily for 2 weeks using the DID protocol. During this period, ethanol consumption escalated during the first week before reaching a plateau of ∼7 g/kg ethanol (Fig. 1D). In contrast to our observations in naive mice, 24 h following the last drinking bout, we recorded tLTP in a majority of MSNs (Fig. 1C,E, DID). Interestingly, when slices from DID mice were acutely exposed to 20 mm aEtOH, tLTP was still observed in most MSNs (Fig. 1E, DID + aEtOH), although its amplitude appeared smaller compared with that of naive mice. Averaging values in these various experimental conditions confirmed these initial observations (F(3,32) = 5.58; p = 0.0034). Thus, in naive mice, nearly 50% of all MSNs tested evoked tLTP with an average amplitude of 144.4 ± 8.9% of control (n = 11; Fig. 1E, Naive) while tLTD was induced in the other half (n = 9, Fig. 1E, Naive) with an amplitude of 81.2 ± 2.4% of control (Fig. 1E). In the presence of acute EtOH in naive mice, tLTP was nearly totally inhibited while tLTD remained unaffected (Fig. 1E, aEtOH). In DID mice, while the amplitude of tLTD (78.9 ± 5.8%; n = 5) was comparable (p = 0.73) to that of naive mice, LTP (117.6 ± 4.3; n = 13) was significantly smaller than that of naive mice (p = 0.026; Fig. 1E). In DID mice, cells re-exposed to 20 mm acute EtOH did not demonstrate inhibition of tLTP as it did in naive mice (Fig. 1E), which is indicative of the development of tolerance. Although it did moderately reduce tLTP amplitude (115.3 ± 3.4%; n = 10) compared with that of DID mice, the difference was not significant. Finally, to test whether the pharmacological or rewarding properties of EtOH were responsible for this effect, in the DID behavioral paradigm we replaced EtOH with sucrose, another naturally occurring rewarding drug (Schneider, 1989; Conover and Shizgal, 1994). Our results indicate that 2–3 weeks of drinking a sucrose-based solution did not alter tLTP/tLTD parity (n = 15; Fig. 1E,F), mimicking results obtained in naive mice. To quantify the apparent shift toward tLTP in DID mice, we calculated the ratio of the number of MSNs expressing tLTP over tLTD. Figure 1F shows that in naive and sucrose-drinking mice, the ratio was close to 1, pointing to an equal probability of evoking tLTP and tLTD. However, in DID mice, the ratio shifted markedly toward tLTP, indicating that the parity between tLTP and tLTD observed in naive mice now favored tLTP (Fig. 1F). This result suggests that the shift of tLTP/tLTD ratio is likely due to the pharmacological properties of EtOH rather than to its rewarding properties. This set of experiments demonstrates that STDP adapts to binge alcohol drinking by shifting plasticity to a form of tLTP that developed tolerance to the drug.
The switch to tLTP in DID mice occurred at all glutamatergic afferents
In a recent study, we showed that plasticity at glutamatergic inputs to core NAc MSNs in naive mice presented key differences based on their origin (Ji et al., 2015). While both tLTP and tLTD were present at amygdaloid and hippocampal inputs, only tLTD was found at cortical synapses. We tested whether the shift in synaptic plasticity observed with EPSPs evoked with electrical stimulation in DID mice presented differences based on the origin of the inputs. We injected pAAV9-CaMKIIa-hChR2(H134R)-EYFP into the PFCx, amygdaloid, and hippocampal regions separately (Fig. 2A, top panels). As with STDP electrically evoked, the tLTP/tLTD ratio at all synapses increased markedly: 5.3 and 5.9 at amygdaloid and hippocampal inputs, respectively (Fig. 2C), which represented a dramatic shift from the ratios recorded at the same synapses in naive mice (Fig. 2C, solid red circles; Ji et al., 2015). At PFCx synapses, while no tLTP was observed in naive mice (Ji et al., 2015), only this form of plasticity was recorded in DID mice (n = 7; Fig. 2A, PFCx). A two-tailed t test comparison of the optogenetically driven STDP amplitude in naive (Ji et al., 2015) and DID mice at cortical and hippocampal synapses showed a significant shift toward tLTP (two-tailed t test, t(13) = 6.72, p < 0.0001 and t(17) = 2.44, p = 0.033, respectively). Although we observed a similar trend at amygdala synapses in DID mice compared with naive mice, the difference was not significant (p = 0.08). In addition to this shift, tLTP amplitude was reduced compared with that of naive mice, ranging from 123.4 ± 4.6%, to 128.1 ± 6%, to 138.6 ± 6.3% of control at hippocampal, amygdaloid, and cortical inputs, respectively (Fig. 2A), although this reduction was not significant. Finally, in DID mice, the magnitude of light-driven tLTP evoked is very similar to the electrically driven tLTP (Fig. 2B, Elec Stim), suggesting that the optogenetic approach did not introduce a bias.
To test whether changes in the basic properties of the glutamatergic synaptic transmission were associated with the shift of STDP, we first examined the probability of glutamate release using the paired-pulse ratio (PPR) protocol by measuring paired AMPA currents evoked at different intervals at a holding potential of −70 mV and calculating the EPSCs2/EPSCs1 ratio (P2/P1). As in naive mice, the ratio increased steadily with increasing intervals across all synapses. However, at amygdaloid inputs (Fig. 3A, red traces) and hippocampal inputs (Fig. 3A, blue traces), the PPR was significantly smaller in DID mice compared with naive mice (Fig. 4B, open symbols). Interestingly, we observed no clear changes at cortical inputs (Fig. 3A, right). To determine whether binge alcohol drinking altered AMPA and NMDA receptor subunit composition, we studied their deactivation kinetics. Figure 3C presents representative AMPA-EPSCs evoked at cortical, amygdaloid, and hippocampal inputs, whose decay was best fitted with a single exponential (red lines). Comparison of the current deactivation kinetics between naive and DID mice revealed no significant changes (Fig. 3D). Similarly, we found that the deactivation rates of NMDA currents were statistically unchanged in DID mice compared with naive mice (Fig. 3E,F) at all three synapses.
Because an increase in the number of AMPA receptors is thought to underlie tLTP (Huganir and Nicoll, 2013), we measured the AMPA/NMDA ratio, an index of changing AMPA receptor expression, at all three glutamatergic inputs. Figure 4A shows representative AMPA and NMDA currents recorded at the three inputs in DID mice. In naive mice, the AMPA/NMDA ratio was 2.6 ± 0.28 (n = 22), 2.51 ± 0.13 (n = 11), and 4.25 ± 1.04 (n = 12), at PFCx, amygdaloid, and hippocampal synapses, respectively. In DID mice, it increased markedly at the first two inputs to 6.4 ± 1.26 (n = 15) and 5.3 ± 0.54 (n = 18), respectively. Surprisingly, it decreased modestly at hippocampal synapses (3.6 ± 0.5; n = 14). Statistical analysis showed that the ratio increase observed at PFCx and amygdaloid inputs was significant (two-tailed t test, t(35) = 3.51, p = 0.0012 and t(27) = 3.98, p = 0.0005, respectively), unlike the small drop at hippocampal inputs (p = 0.46). These results suggest that alterations of synaptic plasticity in DID mice are unlikely to be due to changes in the properties of the glutamatergic synaptic transmission.
Binge alcohol drinking attenuates calcium transients in dendrites of NAc MSNs
While our data suggest that binge alcohol drinking controls tLTD induction, how the former influences the latter was unclear. Having previously shown that AP-driven calcium-induced calcium release during synaptic induction is necessary for tLTD (Ji and Martin, 2012), one putative path connecting EtOH, APs, and tLTD is calcium. We hypothesized that the ability of APs to drive dendritic calcium transients was altered in DID mice. Combining two-photon laser-scanning microscopy with patch-clamp recordings, we measured calcium signals, driven by action potentials evoked somatically (Fig. 5B), along the dendritic arbor in first-order (1°), second-order (2°), and third-order (3°) dendrites (Fig. 5A). Dendritic calcium signals, a proxy for measuring the strength of back-propagating APs (BAPs), were characterized by a rapid rise followed by a slow decay (Fig. 5C). In naive mice, the amplitude of calcium transients remained relatively stable, increasing slightly to 112.1 ± 10.37% in 2° dendrites relative to 1° processes (Fig. 5D, red symbols). However, we observed a significant decline in 3° dendrites as the calcium signal dropped to 57.4 27 ± 7.08% (Fig. 5D, green symbols). Statistical analysis revealed a significant difference between 2° and 3° dendrites (Mann–Whitney U test = 54, z = 1.998 p = 0.0228) and 1° and 3° dendrites (Mann–Whitney U test = 60, z = 2.091, p = 0.0182) only. In DID mice, although the calcium signal similarly decreased between the 1° and 3° dendrites, the drop of amplitude was already detectable in 2° dendrites compared with 1° dendrites (65.94 ± 2.04%; Fig. 5E), an effect that was significant (Mann–Whitney U test = 150, z = 4.517, p < 0.0001), unlike in naive mice (Mann–Whitney U test = 120, z = 0.681, p = 0.247). In comparing these peak calcium transients, we found the values of the 1° EtOH-naive dendritic response to be 16.4% greater than that of DID mice, while the EtOH-naive 2° and 3° indicated a more prominent difference [2°, 23.3% greater (p = 0.098); 3°, 35.7% greater (p = 0.081)]. The amplitude of calcium transients further declined in 3° processes (51.34 ± 10.47%; Fig. 5E, green symbols; p < 0.0001). The slope of the mean decay [(ΔF/F)/Δt] in EtOH-naive mice were found to also exceed that of the DID where the values are as follows: naive (1°, 2.83 ± 0.64 s; 2°, 1.81 ± 0.32 s; 3°, 0.96 ± 0.22 s); DID (1°, 2.41 ± 0.31 s; 2°, 1.46 ± 0.15 s; 3°, 0.71 ± 0.11 s). Interestingly, attenuation of calcium transients (Fig. 5F, gray area) in 2° dendrites (Fig. 6F, red line) in DID mice was mostly observed in MSNs, where calcium transients were stronger in 1° dendrites (gray area). Also, the latency between APs and calcium signal onsets in naive and DID mice indicates that changes in the amplitude of calcium signals are unlikely to be the result of transients being recorded at different distances from the soma (data not shown). These data indicate that a faster decay of the dendritic calcium signal may be one of the cellular mechanisms responsible for the loss of tLTD in binge alcohol-drinking mice.
Dopamine receptors modulate STDP differently in MSNs from the direct and indirect pathways
NAc MSNs are typically classified as belonging to the direct or indirect pathways based on whether they express dopamine D1 or D2 receptors, respectively (Alexander and Crutcher, 1990; Gerfen et al., 1995; Gerfen and Surmeier, 2011). The idea that a drug of abuse like cocaine exerts different effects on the two MSN subpopulations has been supported by a number of studies (Volkow et al., 2009; Ersche et al., 2012; Bock et al., 2013; Creed et al., 2016). However, whether binge alcohol drinking also differentially affects D1R(+) and D1R(−) MSNs is unknown. To test this idea, we electrically evoked EPSPs and measured STDP in both D1R(+) and putative D2R (D1(−)) MSNs from naive and DID D1r.Td-tomato mice. Two-way ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of D1 antagonist (F(1,38) = 22.38, p < 0.0001), but not of chronic alcohol ingestion (i.e., DID), and a significant D1R antagonist × alcohol interaction (F(1,38) = 15.26; p = 0.0004). Surprisingly, in naive mice, the AP-EPSP pairing evoked tLTD in a majority of neurons (i.e., 9 of 13 neurons; Fig. 6A, Naive) with a mean amplitude of 91.5 ± 5.1% of control (n = 13), an effect that was not significantly affected (p > 0.99) by the presence of 4 μm SCH23390, a specific D1R(+) antagonist. Indeed, in this condition, we still recorded tLTD in a majority of MSNs (9 of 11 MSNs; Fig. 6A, Naive + SCH) with a mean amplitude of 87.1 ± 6.6% (n = 11). However, 2 weeks of binge alcohol drinking significantly altered the response of D1R(+) MSNs to the pairing induction protocol (p = 0.025). Thus, most MSNs switched to tLTP (Fig. 6A, DID; 111.2 ± 5.6%; n = 11). Additionally, the shift to tLTP in DID mice was totally and significantly (p = 0.0001) reversed in the presence of SCH23390 (Fig. 6A, DID + SCH) as all MSNs reverted to a robust tLTD (66.05 ± 2.3% of control; n = 7). The graph in Figure 6B illustrates the shift of the tLTP/tLTD ratio in various experimental conditions. In D1R(−) MSNs, a similar two-way ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of the alcohol treatment (F(1,36) = 5.42, p < 0.0425), but not of D2R antagonist, and a significant D2R antagonist × alcohol interaction (F(1,38) = 22.1; p = 0.0001). In naive mice, AP–EPSP pairing evoked tLTP in 9 of 11 MSNs (Fig. 6C, Naive). The mean STDP amplitude measured by pooling all MSNs (n = 11) was 123.3 ± 7.5% of control, an effect that was totally and significantly (p = 0.0016) reversed by 1 μm sulpiride, a specific D2R antagonist (Fig. 6C, Naive + Sulp; 81.7 ± 4.4% of control; n = 10). In DID mice, tLTP was blocked but no shift to tLTD was observed, with an overall effect showing no significant change of the synaptic strength compared with preinduction levels (102 ± 4.4% of control; n = 8; Fig. 6C, DID; p = 0.45). Exposing slices from DID mice to sulpiride restored tLTP (128.7 ± 9.5% of control; n = 10; Fig. 6C, DID + Sulp). However, this effect was not significant compared with STDP in DID alone (p = 0.071). The graph in Figure 6D illustrates the shift of tLTP/tLTD ratio in various experimental conditions in DID mice. These results highlight the different roles of D1R and D2R in regulating the effects of binge alcohol drinking on the core accumbens synaptic plasticity.
Having previously shown that tLTD is a form of plasticity that is controlled by BAPs (Ji and Martin, 2012), and in light of the faster decay of transient calcium signals in dendrites of DID mice (Fig. 5), we wondered whether the switch to tLTP in D1R(+) MSNs was caused by the changing of action potential properties by repeated alcohol exposure. To address this question, we compared the amplitude, width, threshold, and afterhyperpolarization (AHP) of the first action potential of a train evoked by the injection of a 300 pA and 800-ms-long depolarizing current pulse in D1R(+) and D1R(−) MSNs from naive and DID mice (Fig. 7A). Thus, the overshoots of D1R(+) MSNs in naive and DID mice were 35.5 ± 1.8 mV (n = 21) and 32.4 ± 1.6 mV (n = 18; Fig. 7B), respectively. The thresholds were 41 ± 3.2 and 44 ± 3.3 mV (Fig. 7B, bottom graph), while the widths were 4 ± 0.8 and 3.5 ± 0.8 ms in naive and DID mice, respectively (Fig. 7C). Finally, the action potential AHPs were 10.3 ± 1.2 and 10.7 ± 0.9 mV (Fig. 7C, bottom graph). Although we observed overall no clear changes in these parameters in DR1(−) MSNs between naive and DID mice, we did find a small significant increase of the overshoot (two-tailed t test: t(47) = 2.33; p = 0.023) from 33.1 ± 1.4 mV (n = 27) in naive mice to 37.9 ± 1.6 mV (n = 21) in DID mice. These data suggest that the effects of DID on tLTD are unlikely to be due to the direct action of ethanol on somatic action potentials.
Discussion
D1R(+)- and D1R(−)-dependent synaptic plasticity in NAc MSNs
Research on synaptic plasticity in the nucleus accumbens over the past decade using tetanic stimulation, which reliably evokes LTP at the single-cell level in a great number of brain regions including the dorsal striatum (Kombian and Malenka, 1994; Calabresi et al., 1998; Centonze et al., 2001), failed to achieve the same effect in the NAc. This may stem from the low (i.e., <4 Hz) basal firing rates of all glutamatergic neurons projecting to NAc MSNs (Rosenkranz and Grace, 1999; Margrie et al., 2002; Puig et al., 2003), possibly reflecting biophysical properties that may not be conducive to using high-frequency tetanic stimulation. This has shifted the focus on LTD when studying the cellular and molecular mechanisms responsible for the modulation of synaptic plasticity by drugs of abuse, leaving a number of unanswered questions about LTP and its sensitivity to drugs of abuse, including alcohol. Here we confirmed our initial observation that the same induction paradigm induces both tLTP and tLTD (Ji and Martin, 2012), a surprising result in light of STDP data from the dorsal striatum, a structure having comparable anatomical organization with the NAc. For instance, D1R and D2R MSNs represent 95% of the total neuronal population and belong to the direct and indirect pathways, respectively (Meredith and Totterdell, 1999). In this region, Fino et al. (2005) showed that STDP with a negative timing (AP-EPSP) induced tLTP, while reversing the pairing order (i.e., EPSP-AP, positive timing) evoked tLTD. Interestingly, other studies reported the opposite results with positive and negative timings evoking tLTP and tLTD, respectively (Pawlak and Kerr, 2008; Shen et al., 2008). Our study in the NAc reveals a different dichotomy in naive mice, with the polarity of plasticity (tLTD vs tLTP) based on whether MSNs are part of the direct versus indirect pathways. The different effects of dopamine on STDP in the dorsal and ventral striatum suggest that this neurotransmitter may be responsible for the unique features of NAc STDP. While the D1R antagonist SCH23390 promotes tLTD in both the dorsal striatum (Shen et al., 2008) and NAc in naive mice, the D2R antagonist sulpiride appears to have opposite effects by inducing tLTP in the former and tLTD in the latter. Our data show that the ratio of electrically evoked tLTP/tLTD in unidentified MSNs (Fig. 2F) and identified MSNs (Fig. 6B,D) is the same (i.e., 2.5), a value that is smaller than that of optogenetically evoked STDP. The inputs that are recruited when evoking eEPSPs may explain the discrepancy between optogenetically and electrically driven STDP. Indeed, Britt et al. (2012) showed that the hippocampal pathway to accumbens shell MSNs could evoke significantly larger EPSCs compared with the amygdaloid and cortical inputs, suggesting that all inputs are not equal, a finding confirmed recently by MacAskill et al. (2014). In light of these observations, it is possible that we preferentially recruited amygdaloid and hippocampal inputs when evoking relatively small electrically driven EPSPs. A different and possibly complementary explanation may stem from another observation by MacAskill et al. (2014), who reported a marked difference between MSNs from the direct and indirect pathways, with the former being less sensitive to ventral hippocampal inputs than the latter. Therefore, an involuntary bias toward one subpopulation of MSNs while recording eEPSPs in unidentified MSNs may compound the discrepancy in the tLTP/tLTD ratio that was observed in our study.
Modulation of STDP in DID mice
One of the main findings of our study is the differential response of STDP to binge alcohol drinking in D1R(+) and D1R(−) MSNs. While tLTD switched to tLTP in D1R(+) MSNs, tLTP was inhibited in D1R(−) MSNs. The switch to tLTP in D1R(+) MSNs is reminiscent of the effects of alcohol on MSNs in the shell NAc (Jeanes et al., 2011) in a different model of ethanol exposure (i.e., CIE) that is characterized by longer daily bouts of EtOH vapors (i.e., 16 vs 2 h for the DID model). In a subsequent study, this group showed that the switch from LTP to LTD was limited to D1R(+) (Renteria et al., 2017). However, a meaningful comparison with our own data is limited for a number of reasons. First, in this study LTD was evoked using a different induction protocol (i.e., pairing EPSPs with a 10-min-long strong depolarization) and was NMDA receptor dependent. This contrasts with tLTD in the core NAc MSN that is NMDA receptor independent and is controlled by BAPs (Ji and Martin, 2012). Also, while Renteria et al. (2017) reported no changes of synaptic strength in D1R(−) MSNs in the shell NAc of naive mice, we found that tLTP was the predominant form of plasticity in DID mice. Therefore, it is likely that LTD and tLTD in naive mice, and their regulation in CIE and DID mice, mobilizes different cellular and molecular mechanisms.
The switch from tLTD to tLTP in D1R(+) MSNs was somewhat surprising considering that tLTP was totally inhibited while tLTD was insensitive to acute EtOH in naive mice (Ji and Martin, 2012). Although input-specific changes in the properties of the glutamatergic synaptic transmission may account for this shift, as seen with cocaine in the accumbens (Britt et al., 2012; MacAskill et al., 2014), our data do not seem to support this hypothesis. Alternatively, tLTP may result from a loss of tLTD, resulting from smaller dendritic calcium transients evoked by BAPs in DID mice. However, our data showing no difference in the properties of APs between naive and DID mice rule out the possibility that DID alters somatic action potentials, the electrical event that is at the inception of BAPs. It is more likely that short daily bouts of alcohol regulate the expression of ion channels in dendrites that contribute to action potential backpropagation. There is a large body of evidence showing that action potential backpropagation is an active process that relies on sodium, calcium, and potassium channels (Frick and Johnston, 2005), and that their expression impacts backpropagation and calcium transients (Frick et al., 2004). Therefore, alterations of the expression of dendritic ion channels, their modulation by dopamine, or both in DID mice may account for the attenuated dendritic calcium signals and, by extension, to the loss of tLTD in these mice. It is important to note that changes in dendritic calcium transients are only correlative and do not necessarily fully account for the loss of tLTD in DID mice. Experiments based on the tetanic stimulation paradigm led to the generally accepted view that LTP is associated with fast and large rises of calcium, while smaller and slower calcium transients are thought to be responsible for LTD (Bienenstock et al., 1982; Lisman, 1989; Hansel et al., 1997). As such, one would expect to see a reduction in dendritic calcium transients to target tLTP. Yet, there are potentially two main caveats to this idea. First, it is not clear whether this rule applies to all neuronal types, including the nucleus accumbens MSNs. Second, STDP differs substantially from the more classical tetanic stimulation paradigm as it relies on the precise timing of EPSPs and BAPs, the latter being essential to tLTD induction (Ji and Martin, 2012). Therefore, alterations of BAP-mediated dendritic calcium are likely to impact directly the probability of MSNs to evoke tLTD, as shown here. Additionally, measures of dendritic calcium provide only a partial view of its role in shaping plasticity, a phenomenon that ultimately occurs mostly in spines, and it would be imprudent to draw a direct line between dendritic calcium transients and altered tLTD. Further studies are needed to fully establish the relationship between weaker tLTD and smaller dendritic calcium transients.
In contrast to D1R(+) MSNs, the failure of D1R(−) MSNs to evoke tLTP in DID mice suggests that these neurons adapt very differently to repeated EtOH exposures. Although it is unclear what cellular underpinning accounts for the loss of tLTP, it is likely to require an inhibition of calcium signals, the key messenger underlying LTP formation (Raymond and Redman, 2002). Although NMDA receptors may be considered a prime target considering their high permeability to calcium, this is unlikely since the Ca2+ permeabilities of heteromeric NMDA receptor channels are comparable among the four kinds of heteromeric channels (Burnashev et al., 1992; Monyer et al., 1994). Alternatively, the loss of tLTP may stem from AMPA receptors undergoing changes in their subunit composition. Indeed, they are also permeable to calcium, albeit to a lesser extent than NMDA receptors. Thus, DID may cause a switch of the expression of AMPA receptor from the GluR1, a highly calcium-permeable subunit that is expressed in the NAc (Martin et al., 1993; Kondo et al., 2000), to the GluR2 subunits that is nearly impermeable to this cation (Geiger et al., 1995). The decreased expression of GluR1 in the amygdala (Jin et al., 2014b) and the increased expression of GluR2 in the cortical region of individuals with alcoholism (Jin et al., 2014a) attest to the sensitivity of these AMPA receptor subunits to alcohol.
The functional relevance of an overrepresentation of tLTP in D1R(+) MSNs from DID mice remains unclear. However, studies of behavioral sensitization showed that this mouse model for addiction (Wolf, 1998; Robinson and Berridge, 2000) was linked to LTP (Ungless et al., 2001; Saal et al., 2003), the leading candidate mediating neuronal plasticity. In naive mice, Creed et al. (2016) recently reported that the ventral pallidum integrates converging information from the direct and indirect pathways in the accumbens. In the ventral pallidum, the same stimulation paradigm induces LTP and LTD at GABAergic synapses from NAc direct and indirect pathway, respectively, a reversed image of what we report here at glutamatergic inputs. Importantly, while D1Rs MSN projections to the ventral pallidum are responsible for the reinforcing effects of cocaine, D2R-MSN projections carry their anhedonic properties. This finding further underscores the idea that D1R MSNs and D2R MSNs, possibly through LTP and LTD, encode different facets of addiction. Although EtOH self-administration does not equate with drug sensitization, the parallel with our data suggests that tLTP may be necessary for the development of alcohol drinking.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Addiction Grants AA020501 (G.E.M), AA017656 (A.R.T), and AA022567 (M.G.); and by the Integrative Neuroscience Initiative on Alcoholism (INIA-West; G.E.M.).
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Gilles E. Martin, University of Massachusetts Medical School, The Brudnick Neuropsychiatric Research Institute, Department of Psychiatry; 303 Belmont Street, Worcester, MA 01604. gilles.martin{at}umassmed.edu