Abstract
Multiple components have been identified that exhibit different stabilities for aversive olfactory memory in Drosophila. These components have been defined by behavioral and genetic studies and genes specifically required for a specific component have also been identified. Intermediate-term memory generated after single cycle conditioning is divided into anesthesia-sensitive memory (ASM) and anesthesia-resistant memory (ARM), with the latter being more stable. We determined that the ASM and ARM pathways converged on the Rgk1 small GTPase and that the N-terminal domain-deleted Rgk1 was sufficient for ASM formation, whereas the full-length form was required for ARM formation. Rgk1 is specifically accumulated at the synaptic site of the Kenyon cells (KCs), the intrinsic neurons of the mushroom bodies, which play a pivotal role in olfactory memory formation. A higher than normal Rgk1 level enhanced memory retention, which is consistent with the result that Rgk1 suppressed Rac-dependent memory decay; these findings suggest that rgk1 bolsters ASM via the suppression of forgetting. We propose that Rgk1 plays a pivotal role in the regulation of memory stabilization by serving as a molecular node that resides at KC synapses, where the ASM and ARM pathway may interact.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Memory consists of multiple components. Drosophila olfactory memory serves as a fundamental model with which to investigate the mechanisms that underlie memory formation and has provided genetic and molecular means to identify the components of memory, namely short-term, intermediate-term, and long-term memory, depending on how long the memory lasts. Intermediate memory is further divided into anesthesia-sensitive memory (ASM) and anesthesia-resistant memory (ARM), with the latter being more stable. We have identified a small GTPase in Drosophila, Rgk1, which plays a pivotal role in the regulation of olfactory memory stability. Rgk1 is required for both ASM and ARM. Moreover, N-terminal domain-deleted Rgk1 was sufficient for ASM formation, whereas the full-length form was required for ARM formation.
Introduction
Drosophila olfactory learning and memory, in which an odor is associated with stimuli that induce innate responses such as aversion (Quinn et al., 1974; Tully and Quinn, 1985), has served as a useful model with which to elucidate the molecular basis of memory (Dudai, 1985; Davis, 1993, 1996; Waddell and Quinn, 2001; Heisenberg, 2003; Davis, 2005; Margulies et al., 2005; McGuire et al., 2005; Keene and Waddell, 2007). Olfactory memory is divided into several temporal components (Quinn and Dudai, 1976; Folkers et al., 1993; Tully et al., 1994; Heisenberg, 2003; Isabel et al., 2004; Trannoy et al., 2011; Plaçais et al., 2012; Bouzaiane et al., 2015) and the intermediate-term memory (ITM) generated after single cycle conditioning is further classified into two distinct phases, anesthesia-sensitive memory (ASM) and anesthesia-resistant memory (ARM) (Quinn and Dudai, 1976). Evidence has suggested that ASM and ARM are distinctly regulated at the neuronal level (Lee et al., 2011; Wu et al., 2013; Zhang et al., 2013; Bouzaiane et al., 2015) and at the molecular level (Dudai, 1988; Folkers et al., 1993; Schwaerzel et al., 2007; Knapek et al., 2010; Knapek et al., 2011; Scheunemann et al., 2012).
Mushroom bodies (MBs) represent the principal mediator of olfactory memory (Dubnau et al., 2003; Heisenberg, 2003; McGuire et al., 2005; Busto et al., 2010; Davis, 2011; Guven-Ozkan and Davis, 2014; Hige et al., 2015a; Owald and Waddell, 2015; Barnstedt et al., 2016). Kenyon cells (KCs) are the intrinsic neurons of MBs, which are bilaterally located clusters of neurons that project anteriorly to form characteristic lobe structures and are a platform of MB-extrinsic neurons that project onto or out of the MBs (Tanaka et al., 2008; Aso et al., 2014). To elucidate the molecular mechanisms that underlie olfactory memory, screenings for MB-expressing genes have been a useful strategy (Han et al., 1992; Skoulakis et al., 1993). A technique used to examine gene expression in a small amount of tissue samples has enabled the investigation of the expression profile in MBs with a substantial dynamic range of expression levels and high sensitivity (Tang et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2009; Wang and Navin, 2015), thereby representing a promising approach with which to identify novel genes responsible for memory. We deep sequenced RNA isolated from adult MBs and identified rgk1 as a KC-specific gene.
The RGK protein family, for which Drosophila Rgk1 exhibits significant protein homology, belongs to the Ras-related small GTPase subfamily, which is composed of Kir/Gem, Rad, Rem, and Rem2. Their roles include the regulation of Ca2+ channel activity (Béguin et al., 2001; Finlin et al., 2003) and the reorganization of cytoskeleton (Pan et al., 2000; Leone et al., 2001; Piddini et al., 2001; Ward et al., 2002). Notably, mammalian REM2 is expressed in the brain (Finlin et al., 2000) and has been shown to be important for synaptogenesis (Ghiretti and Paradis, 2011; Moore et al., 2013), as well as activity-dependent dendritic complexity (Ghiretti and Paradis, 2014; Ghiretti et al., 2014). These findings raise the possibility that RGK proteins may have a role in the synaptic plasticity that underlies memory formation. Drosophila has several genes that encode proteins homologous to the RGK family, including rgk1 (Puhl et al., 2014). Therefore, based on the ample resources available in Drosophila for the investigation of neuronal morphology and functions (Keene and Waddell, 2007), Drosophila Rgk proteins will provide a good opportunity to elucidate the function of RGK family proteins.
Here, we describe the analysis of Drosophila rgk1, which exhibited specific expression in KCs. Rgk1 accumulated at synaptic sites and was required for olfactory aversive memory, making the current study the first to demonstrate the role of an RGK family protein in behavioral plasticity. Our data suggest that Rgk1 supports ASM via the suppression of Rac-dependent memory decay, whereas the N-terminal domain has a specific role in ARM formation. Together, these findings indicated that Rgk1 functions as a critical synaptic component that modulates the stability of olfactory memory.
Materials and Methods
Expression analysis of MBs with RNA-seq.
Expression screening was designed to identify genes that are enriched in adult MBs. The RNA-seq analysis (subsequently described in detail) yielded a list of genes that were expressed in the MBs. We selected candidate genes for subsequent analysis with the following criteria: (1) genes highly expressed in the MBs, (2) genes that had not been reported previously to function in memory, and (3) genes that encode proteins that are homologous to mammalian proteins reported to regulate neuronal functions.
cDNA preparation for RNA-seq.
Female flies that expressed green fluorescent protein (GFP) in KCs with OK107-Gal4 were maintained at 25°C before dissection. The flies were dissected under a fluorescent microscope in ice-cold PBS–BSA solution to isolate the MBs with tweezers. The isolated MBs were immediately lysed with XB buffer (PicoPure RNA isolation Kit; Thermo Fisher Scientific) at 42°C for 30 min and were subsequently maintained at −80°C until RNA purification. After total RNA extraction and DNaseI treatment, the RNA quality was assessed with the Experion automated electrophoresis system (Bio-Rad). cDNA was generated from these RNA samples with a Superscript III kit (Invitrogen) and amplified following a previously described protocol (Kurimoto et al., 2007; Tang et al., 2009). The qualities of the final products were determined via qPCR with primers for gapdh, damb, and staufen.
RNA-seq analysis.
cDNA samples were processed for RNA-seq analysis according to the manufacture's standard protocol (Applied Biosystems SOLiD Library Preparation protocol) and were sequenced on Applied Biosystems SOLiD platforms (SOLiD4 and 5500) to generate single-end 50 bp reads. Sequenced RNA reads were aligned to FlyBase mRNA database using Bowtie version 0.12.5 with the “−v3 −m10” option, which allows three mismatches in the first 28 bases and discards reads having >10 reportable alignments. Multiply aligned reads were divided equally among all locations (N-times matched reads were weighted as 1/N reads) and aligned transcript reads were merged for a single gene. The expression level of each gene was calculated in reads per kilobase per million reads. RNA-seq data have been deposited to Sequence Read Archive with the accession number SRP093518.
cDNA preparation for qPCR.
Female flies were anesthetized with CO2 and, after decapitation, they were dissected in ice-cold PBS buffer that contained BSA to isolate the brains. After total RNA extraction, cDNA was synthesized with a Superscript III kit (Invitrogen).
qPCR analysis.
qPCR analysis was conducted with SYBR Green (Roche) on a Light Cycler 480 (Roche). For the quantification of rgk1 isoforms in the brain, cDNAs of wild-type CS10 brains were used and qPCR assay was conducted with the following primers: 5′-TGATTAGCAGCGTCTCGACTG and 5′-TCTACAAGCGCATCTGCCG for the RA and RC isoforms and 5′-AGCAGCGTCTCGACTGTATTG and 5′-ATCAACGTGACCGCGAATCC for the RB isoform.
FISH.
DIG-labeled RNA probes were used for the FISH assay. The target sequence of the RNA probe spanned several rgk1 exons (from 4175 to 4782 bp in the rgk1-RB isoform). A fragment of the rgk1 gene was amplified with the primers 5′-TGTCTGCCCCAGCAGAGATCCA and 5′-TGCCTTCTGGGCGATGTTCTGA using ExTaq (Takara) from the adult female brain cDNA and cloned into a Topo cloning vector (Takara). After a sequence check, the in vitro translation was conducted using Sp6 or T7 RNA polymerase (Roche) and a DIG-labeling kit (Roche). The resultant probe was purified using QuickSpin columns for RNA (Roche). Fluorescent signals were generated using HNPP and FastRed (Roche). The signal recording was conducted using an LSM710 confocal microscope (Zeiss). The antibody for Dachshund (Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank; DSHB) was used to label KC bodies.
Antiserum generation.
A rabbit polyclonal antibody for Rgk1-PB was created by injecting rabbits with an HPLC-purified synthetic peptide, PGGTATTRSRGARA, which represented a portion of the N-terminal region that is only present in Rgk1-PB (74–87). Another rabbit polyclonal anti-Rgk1 (anti-Rgk1-C) was generated using two HPLC-purified peptides, GKELVARKRNSQQL (925–938) and PGSAQSSPRKYRGS (1334–1348), which correspond to a C-terminal region of the Rgk1 proteins. We confirmed that the sera recognized exogenously expressed Rgk1 in the adult brain.
Generation of transgenic flies (UAS-rgk1-RB).
The entire length of the rgk1-RB was amplified using PrimeStar (Takara) from cDNA, which was synthesized from total RNA extracted from female CS10 brains. The following primers were used: 5′-CACAGATCTGCTTGGTCTGCATGACTGCCGATCCCAT ATCGTTGTGC and 5′-CACTCTAGAGGATAATCTCTGTGCTTAGAGTACATG CAGATTCTCG. The resultant fragment was cloned into the pUAST vector using the BglII and XbaI sites. Ligation was conducted with T4 ligase (Takara). The entire construct was verified by sequencing. Injections were applied onto a CS10 background by BestGene using standard P-element-mediated germline transformation.
Generation of transgenic flies (UAS-rgk1-RB fused with GFP).
GFP-fused UAS-Rgk1 constructs, full-length or truncated forms, were generated using the in-fusion technique (In-Fusion HD Cloning Kit; Clontech). The ΔC construct lacks the sequence from tyrosine 1121 through the end of Rgk1-PB, which encompasses the entire GTPase domain. The ΔN construct lacks the protein sequence from A.A.161 to 797, including the entire DUF2967 domain. Briefly, the pAcGFP fragment and each rgk1-RB fragment (full, ΔN, or ΔC) were amplified using primers with linker sequences and linked using In-Fusion. GFPs were fused to the N-terminal of the Rgk1 constructs. The pAcGFP1 vector (Clontech) and UAS-rgk1-RB (previously described) were used as templates for the amplifications. The resultant GFP-fused Rgk1 fragments were cloned using XhoI and XbaI sites into pJFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP (Addgene) after it was cut with XhoI and XbaI to remove mCD8::GFP and provide room for the fragments. The entire construct was checked via sequencing. Injections were performed by BestGene in flies that possessed the attP site on the second chromosome (attP40).
Generation of transgenic flies (UAS-rgk1-S1134N).
UAS-GFP-rgk1-S1134N was generated using UAS-GFP-rgk1-RB as a template. A point mutation (AGT to AAT conversion) was introduced at serine 1134 to convert it to asparagine using In-Fusion with the following primer sets: 5′-CGGCCGCGGCTC GAGCAACATGACTGCCGATCCCATATCG and 5′-GAGCGAATTCTTGCCCAC CGCAGGACCT; 5′-GGCAAGAATTCGCTCGTCTCGCAGTTCAT and 5′-ACAAA GATCCTCTAGATTACTTGTACAGCTCATCCATGCCG. The entire constructs were checked via sequencing. Injections were performed by BestGene in flies with the attB site on the second chromosome (attP40).
Generation of the deletion allele.
The P element was excised by crossing the KG00183 allele with the Δ2–3 strain, which has a P transposase (Robertson et al., 1988). The occurrences of imprecise excision and resultant deletions were determined via PCR with the following primers: 5′-ATGCGCCTCGCCTGTTTCTCGGGAAAATCTCCA and 5′-AAGCTAAGACCGA GGGAGCGTGACCCCAACCAC. The deletion of exons of the RA and RC isoforms was checked via PCR with the following primers: 5′-CGTTATATGTCCGTAAGGCACCGC and 5′-ATAGAGAGCCATCCGAAAGCAAA GC. For rgk1-RB check: 5′-TCCTCAATGGTGAGCGTGTTG and 5′-CATCGCGC AAGAACTCCAAG. For gapdh check: 5′-ACGCCAAGGCTGGCATTTCG and 5′-AGGTCGATGACGCGGTTGGAG (see Fig. 2A). To determine the deleted gene region in the Δrgk1 allele, a part of rgk1-RB was amplified from the female brain cDNA of Δrgk1 and sequence analysis indicated a frameshift in rgk1-RB at 3405 bp, which causes a change in the resultant translated peptide, that is, QYV (765–767) to HPR followed by a stop codon. This change resulted in an open reading frame that encodes a truncated Rgk1 devoid of the entire homologous GTPase domain.
Generation of UAS-rgk1-sh.
rgk1-short hairpin (rgk1-sh) was achieved following the online protocol available from the TRip website of Harvard Medical School (http://fgr.hms.harvard.edu/files/fly/files/2ndgenprotocol.pdf). The following target oligos were designed using an online design tool DSIR (http://biodev.extra.cea.fr/DSIR/DSIR.html) (Vert et al., 2006). The top strand was oligo 5′-CTAGCAGTGACCATGGATACACCGAAATGTAGTTATATTCAAGCATACA TTTCGGTGTATCCATGGTCGCG-3′; the bottom strand was oligo 5′-AATTCGCGA CCATGGATACACCGAAATGTATGCTTGAATATAACTACATTTCGGTGTATCCATGGTCACTG-3′. Oligos were synthesized (outsourced to operon) with NheI and EcoRI sites on each side and inserted into the VALIUM20 vector. After the sequence check, vectors were injected into the attP40 sites (BestGene). The insertion of the construct in the genome was checked by amplifying the construct from the extracted genome and sequencing it.
Immunohistochemistry.
Adult female flies were anesthetized with CO2. Brains were dissected in ice-cold PBS solutions and fixed in 4% formaldehyde/PBS solutions for 20 min at room temperature, with the exception of the experiments that used anti-N-Rgk1-PB or anti-Rgk1-C, which required a shorter (8 min) room temperature fixation time to obtain signals. After 30 min of blocking with 5% normal donkey serum (NDS; Jackson Laboratories) in PBS solution with 0.15% Triton X-100 (PBT), the brains were incubated at 4°C in a PBT solution that contained primary antibodies together with 1% NDS overnight. After the wash in PBT solution, the samples were incubated with a secondary antibody for 1 d at 4°C. The antibody concentrations were as follows: anti-Rgk1-PB-N 1:200, anti-Rgk1-C 1:200, anti-Trio (9.4A; DSHB) 1:20, anti-Bruchpilot (Brp) (nc82; DSHB) 1:20, anti-Dlg (4F3; DSHB) 1:20, anti-Dachshund (mAbdac2-3; DSHB) 1:20, rat-anti-GFP (1A5, sc-101536; Santa Cruz Biotechnology) 1:50, goat anti-rat IgG-FITC (sc-2011) 1:200, rabbit-Cy3 1:200, and mouse-Cy3 1:200.
Imaging.
Images were acquired with an LSM710 confocal microscope (Zeiss) using a 40× objective and were processed with ImageJ software. The images were adjusted for the brightness and contrast with Adobe Photoshop.
Fly stocks.
Flies were maintained under a 12/12 h light-dark cycle on standard food at 25°C and 50–60% humidity. The strains used for the memory assays were outcrossed to CS10 at least six times with the exception of dnc1, which was outcrossed to the Canton-S strain at least six times. When the white gene was not available as a marker to verify outcrossing, we checked the presence of mutations or transgenes via PCR or the Gal4 presence by crossing them with UAS-GFP and determining the GFP expression in each outcrossing cycle. The fly stocks included the following: c739-Gal4 (O'Dell et al., 1995), mb247-Gal4 (Zars et al., 2000), elav[c155]-Gal4 (Lin et al., 1994), rutabaga2080, dnc1. NP5225-Gal4 (Hayashi et al., 2002), KG00183 (Bellen et al., 2004). CS10, hsflp;AYGal4 (Ito et al., 1997), tubGal80ts (McGuire et al., 2004). OK107-Gal4 (Connolly et al., 1996), Df(2R)BSC26 (Parks et al., 2004), and UAS-racV12 (Luo et al., 1994). The Gal4 lines 1993-Gal4 and 2765-Gal4 were originally generated and identified as MB-specific drivers in our laboratory (Abe et al., 2014).
Behavioral assays.
Mixed populations of males and females were used for the assay unless otherwise noted. The CS10 strain was used as a control for the memory assay. Single sessions of olfactory aversive conditioning and the calculations of performance index were conducted according to previously described protocols (Tully and Quinn, 1985) using a semiautomated conditioning device (Murakami et al., 2010) with a modification that replaced odor cups with 25 ml × 2 cups, which had been originally designed for 10 ml odor. Memory tests were performed using a T-maze paradigm (Tully and Quinn, 1985). For all conditioning assays and tests, a 4-methylcyclohexanol (Fluka) dilution of 1:1000 in mineral oil and a 3-octanol (Fluka) dilution of 1:1000 in mineral oil were used. In a session of olfactory aversive conditioning, electric shock pulses of 1.5 s duration were applied 12 times with 5 s interpulse intervals. The conditioned flies were immediately tested or placed in empty vials to rest for 2 or 3 h before the memory test. To correct for a bias toward an odor, two groups of independent populations were reciprocally conditioned and tested to compensate for the innate preference to one of the odors.
Odor and shock avoidance.
Olfactory acuity and shock reactivity were assayed in untrained flies using methods described previously (Berry et al., 2012). For odor avoidance, the avoidance index was calculated as the fraction of flies that avoided the odor minus the fraction of flies that did not. For shock avoidance, the avoidance index was calculated as the fraction of flies that avoided the electrified grid minus the fraction of flies that did not. The side with the odor or the side that is electrified was alternated to correct for side bias of the T maze.
Cold shock application.
To anesthetize flies by cold shock, empty vials that contained flies were immersed in ice-cold water for 2 min, followed by a rest at 25°C until the test. Cold shocks were applied 2 h after the conditioning and the flies were tested after resting for 1 h at 25°C.
Statistical analysis.
One-way ANOVA and Student's t test analyses were performed with Kareidagraph (Synergy Software) and Excel (Microsoft). Post hoc tests were conducted with Fisher's least significant difference (LSD) tests after ANOVA to determine pairs of significance. The sample sizes are indicated on each bar in the graphs. All data are presented as the mean ± SEM.
Results
Rgk1 was exclusively expressed in the adult KCs and accumulated at KC synaptic sites
We identified rgk1 in a screening to identify genes that are highly expressed in the KCs of the MBs (refer to details in the Materials and Methods). Among the candidate genes, putative regulatory genes of neuronal functions were further examined for KC expression using an RNA in situ hybridization assay. Twenty-six genes were examined and eight genes exhibited a preferential expression in the adult KCs, including the mushroom-body expressed (mub) and retinal degeneration C (rdgC) genes, which have been reported previously to be expressed preferentially in the MBs (Steele et al., 1992; Grams and Korge, 1998).
rgk1 encodes three isoforms, RA, RB, and RC (Fig. 1A), according to the flybase (http://flybase.org/reports/FBgn0264753.html). The RB isoform differs from the other two isoforms in that it encodes a protein that has an additional N-terminal sequence, as well as a common region that contains the putative GTPase domain (Fig. 1B); RB was the most abundant isoform in the brain (Fig. 1C).
KCs are localized to the posterior side of the brain and extends axons anteriorly to form lobe structures (Fig. 1D). Among the identified genes in the screening, rgk1 exhibited strong and nearly exclusive expression in the adult KCs (Fig. 1E).
To examine the expression of Rgk1 protein in the brain, polyclonal antibodies were raised against a region near the N-terminal of Rgk1-PB or two sites in the C-terminal region of Rgk1 (Fig. 1B). The anti-Rgk1-PB-N recognizes only Rgk1-PB. Both sera exclusively stained the MBs (Fig. 1F,G). These MB signals were virtually absent in the flies that expressed rgk1-specific miRNA in the KCs (Fig. 1F′,G′), which suggests that the staining signals reflected Rgk1 protein expression. Rgk1 exhibited a polarized localization pattern in the KCs, including strong signals in the lobes and weaker signals in the calyx and KC bodies, as is evident when viewed from a dorsal perspective (Fig. 1H,H′). Rgk1-PB exhibited a cell-type specificity in the KCs, which are classified into three major populations, including α/β, α′/β′, and γ neurons; the expression was strong in the α/β and γ neurons and weak in the α′/β′ KCs (Fig. 1I,I′). We also determined that the transgenic rgk1 induction with pan-neural elav-Gal4 did not increase signals outside of the MBs, which suggests a regulatory mechanism that restricts Rgk1 to the KCs (data not shown). Overall, these findings indicate that Rgk1 is specifically expressed in KCs.
We subsequently determined whether Rgk1 is present at KC output synapses, a site that is thought to be central in olfactory memory formation (Dubnau et al., 2001; McGuire et al., 2001; Schwaerzel et al., 2003; Barnstedt et al., 2016). We examined the distribution of Rgk1 in the γ lobes with the Rgk1 antibody, as well as anti-Brp, an active zone marker of synapses (Wagh et al., 2006). Rgk1 formed puncta that were intermingled with the Brp signal (Fig. 1J). Moreover, Rgk1 puncta were colocalized with the Brp signal or resided next to the signal (Fig. 1K–K″). However, MB lobes consist not only of intrinsic KCs but also extrinsic neurons, so it is difficult to determine whether Rgk1 puncta localize to KC synapses in these immunostaining experiments. We subsequently expressed GFP-fused Rgk1 stochastically and sparsely using hsflp and AYGal4 (Ito et al., 1997) in KCs, as well as a synaptic marker HA-tagged synaptotagmin (Robinson et al., 2002), to examine the Rgk1 localization in a single KC. We again identified two-tiered localization patterns: Rgk1 colocalized with syt-HA signals or next to syt-HA signals (Fig. 1L). We conclude that the localization of Rgk1 is closely associated with the presynapses of KCs.
miRNA-mediated knockdown of rgk1 in KCs caused memory defects
To examine the role of the rgk1 gene in olfactory learning and memory, we used an RNAi technique to knock down rgk1 function in KCs, which is also useful to determine the neurons in which rgk1 acts. An rgk1-specific miRNA was generated according to the TRiP protocol (Ni et al., 2011) and was placed under the control of an upstream activating sequence (UAS) for target expression (Brand and Perrimon, 1993). We confirmed that the Rgk1 expression in the KCs was inhibited when UAS-rgk1-sh was crossed with OK107-Gal4, which is a KC-specific driver (Connolly et al., 1996; Fig. 1F′,G′). Memory performance was measured with an olfactory aversive conditioning assay (Tully and Quinn, 1985; Murakami et al., 2010). We determined that 2 h memory was severely affected in the rgk1-sh animals (Fig. 2A). Five-minute memory was also mildly affected in the rgk1-sh animals (Fig. 2A). The olfactory acuity and responses to electric shocks were intact in the rgk1-sh animals (Table 1), as well as the morphology of the MBs (data not shown). Therefore, these findings suggest that the KC-expressing rgk1 gene was required for olfactory memory.
To further determine the subset of KCs that require rgk1, UAS-rgk1-sh was crossed with MB-Gal4 drivers expressed in a subset of KCs (Fig. 2B–E). All Gal4s with the exception of 1993-Gal4, which is specifically expressed in α′/β′, exhibited impairments in memory performance (Fig. 2B), which suggests that rgk1 is required in α/β and γ neurons, but not in α′/β′ neurons. This finding is consistent with the expression pattern of Rgk1-PB protein (Fig. 1I,I′) in the adult MBs in which α′/β′ neurons exhibited weaker Rgk1-PB expression. This expression pattern was also identified in the expression of NP5225-Gal4 (Hayashi et al., 2002; Fig. 2E), which has a gal4-reporter insertion 14 bp upstream of the transcription start site of the rgk1 gene.
To determine whether rgk1 is required for the development or physiological function of KCs, rgk1-sh was conditionally induced only during the adult stage using the TARGET system (McGuire et al., 2004). A temporarily restricted induction of the hairpin in the adult KCs caused a memory defect (Fig. 2F), which suggests that rgk1 is required at the adult stage, likely at the time when memory is formed. To gain further insights into the transient nature of rgk1 function, rgk-sh induction was terminated after 3 d and memory performance was measured. The memory defect was alleviated after the cessation of the hairpin induction (Fig. 2G), which suggests that rgk1 loss does not cause unrecoverable damage to neurons. Therefore, rgk1 may act as a signaling molecule that regulates transient changes in neurons.
Genomic disruption of rgk1 causes memory defects
To obtain an independent genetic confirmation that the rgk1 gene is required for olfactory memory, a genomic deletion allele was generated by imprecisely excising the P-element from the KG00183 allele that has a P insertion in the rgk1 gene locus (Bellen et al., 2004; Fig. 3A). The excision removed the first exons of the RA and RC isoforms (Fig. 3B) and partially deleted an exon of the RB isoform (Fig. 3C). The homozygote of the resultant Δrgk1 allele was devoid of the Rgk1 signal in the MB, which was confirmed with a polyclonal antibody that targeted the C-terminal region of Rgk1 (Fig. 3D,D′). The target region of the antibody is present in all rgk1 isoforms, so this finding indicates that Δrgk1 is devoid of products of all rgk1 isoforms. The morphology of the MBs was intact in the deletion allele. The Δrgk1 homozygous mutants were viable, which enabled us to measure their memory performance at the adult stage. The original line KG00183 did not exhibit memory defects, whereas the Δrgk1 homozygous mutants exhibited an impairment in 2 h memory, as well as slight decreases in the scores 5 min after the olfactory aversive training (Fig. 3E,F), thereby reproducing the results of the miRNA-mediated knockdown experiment. The memory defect of Δrgk1 was not complemented by the deletion allele Df(2R)BSC26, which lacks the entire rgk1 gene (Fig. 3G). The olfactory acuity and responsiveness to electric shock were indistinguishable between the wild-type and Δrgk1 (Table 1). Therefore, these findings indicate that the rgk1 gene is necessary for olfactory memory.
Δrgk1 memory phenotype was rescued by rgk1 transgene induction in KCs
To further confirm the role of KC-expressing rgk1 in olfactory memory, UAS-rgk1-RB was generated to determine whether exogenous expression of the rgk1 transgene in KCs rescues the Δrgk1 memory phenotype. We confirmed that the expression of Rgk1 was restored in the KCs in flies that possessed the UAS-rgk1-RB and KC-Gal4 drivers (Fig. 4A and data not shown). The memory performance was rescued by transgene induction with OK107-Gal4 (Fig. 4B) or by conditionally expressing the rgk1-RB transgene at the adult stage with the TARGET system that used OK107-Gal4 (Fig. 4C). Therefore, rgk1 is required in KCs for olfactory memory at the adult stage, which is compatible with the results of the adult-specific induction of rgk1-miRNA (Fig. 2E).
We identified a memory enhancement in rescued flies (Fig. 4B,C). The 2 h memory score was significantly increased in flies that expressed transgenic rgk1 in the MBs (F(3,28) = 24.72, p < 0.0001. Fisher's LSD test, rescued flies vs wild-type flies **p = 0.0074, Fig. 4B; F(3,32) = 37.56, p < 0.0001; Fisher's LSD test, rescued flies vs wild-type flies 29°C **p = 0.0058; Fig. 4C). Therefore, we aimed to determine whether rgk1 overexpression could enhance memory in the wild-type flies. When rgk1-RB was overexpressed in the KCs of wild-type, 2 h memory was enhanced (Fig. 4D). The enhancement was observed with two independent UAS-rgk1-RB lines, but was not observed in short-term memory (Fig. 4E), which suggests that rgk1 has a critical role in ITM and potentially in memory retention.
The rescue of the memory performance was also observed with c739-Gal4 or the pan-neuronal elav-Gal4 driver (Fig. 4F,G), but the enhancement was not obvious with these Gal4 lines. This might be due to the difference in the strength and/or the cell-type specificity of the Gal4 expression. Experiments with more Gal4 drivers will be required to test this possibility.
C- and N-terminal regions of Rgk1 have distinct roles in memory
rgk1-RB encodes a putative GTPase domain that exhibits homology to mammalian Ras and RGK proteins, as well as a domain specific to Drosophila (DUF2967), the function of which is unknown (Fig. 5A). Considering the conserved nature of the GTPase domain, it is of particular interest whether the putative GTPase domain is indispensable for the function of rgk1 in memory. Therefore, to gain insights regarding the function of these domains, partially deleted rgk1 constructs were generated (Fig. 5A). GFP was attached to the N-terminus of the constructs. To minimize the possible negative effect of tagging the protein with GFP (Hanson and Ziegler, 2004), we avoided the C-terminal of Rgk1 as the GFP-fusion site because the C-terminal motif is highly conserved among RGK family proteins and is unique to RGK proteins (Puhl et al., 2014), implicating the importance of this region. We found no obvious differences between Rgk1 and GFP-Rgk1 in the distribution within the KCs or the localization to synaptic sites (data not shown). The constructs were tested using an olfactory aversive memory assay for their ability to rescue the Δrgk1 memory phenotype. The results presented so far indicated that Rgk1 is important for the modulation of the ITM. We sought to focus the subsequent analysis on ITM, especially on the roles of Rgk1 in ASM and ARM. Because previous studies typically measured ITM 3 h after the training (Shuai et al., 2010; Knapek et al., 2011; Bouzaiane et al., 2015; Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2016), we decided to use 3 h memory in the subsequent ITM examinations. The full-length and ΔN constructs (Δ160–797) rescued the 3 h memory defect, in contrast to the ΔC construct (Δ1120–1380 in Rgk1-PB; Fig. 5B); these findings indicate that the C-terminal region, which includes a putative GTPase domain, is critical for memory. The substitution of serine 17 for asparagine in RasN17 inhibits the activation of Ras (Farnsworth and Feig, 1991) and has been used as a dominant-negative form of Ras (Feig and Cooper, 1988). The sequence near Serine 17 of Ras is highly conserved in Drosophila Rgk1. To determine whether the homologous GTPase domain in Rgk1 is important for memory, we generated a transgene that mimicked RasN17, which has a single amino acid substitution (S1134 to N) at the corresponding position to that of RasN17. We determined that UAS-rgk1-S1134N was not able to rescue the Δrgk1 memory phenotype (Fig. 5C), which suggests the importance of the GTPase domain for memory.
In rescue experiments using the deletion constructs, we aimed to determine whether Rgk1 domains have specific roles in ASM and ARM. The full-length Rgk1, but not the ΔN construct, rescued the Δrgk1 memory defect after the cold shock application (Fig. 5D), which suggests that the full-length Rgk1 is required to express both ASM and ARM, whereas Rgk1ΔN is sufficient to express ASM but not ARM. It is intriguing that the rescue with the Rgk1ΔN construct apparently restored ITM as well as the full-length construct (Fig. 5B) despite the fact that it failed to support ARM (Fig. 5D). These findings suggest that the Rgk1ΔN overexpression may somehow enhance ASM and compensate for its inability to form ARM.
Genetic analysis suggests that rgk1 is required for both ASM and ARM
The previously described results suggest that rgk1 maintains memory through both ASM and ARM. To further confirm the notion that rgk1 acts for both ASM and ARM, we compared the phenotype of rgk1 with known genes specific for ASM or ARM: rutabaga and dunce have been shown to be specifically required for ASM and ARM, respectively (Scheunemann et al., 2012). Three-hour memory was affected to similar degrees in the rgk1, rut, and dnc mutants (Fig. 6A). However, after the cold shock application that eliminates only ASM, a significant difference in the memory scores was observed between rut and dnc as reported previously (Scheunemann et al., 2012), but not between rgk1 and each of the two mutants (Fig. 6A); these findings suggest that rgk1 does not act in the rut or dnc pathway to modulate ASM or ARM specifically. Instead, rgk1 is required for both ASM and ARM. The further removal of the rgk1 function from the rut or dnc mutant also confirmed this finding; the removal of rgk1 function deprived the residual memory component that remained in the rut or dnc mutant; that is, the ASM component from dnc and the ARM component from rut (Fig. 6B,C).
Rgk1 suppressed Rac-dependent memory decay
A small GTPase, Rac, has been established as the facilitator of forgetting in olfactory memory. Rac acts on ASM and the inhibition of Rac activity leads to memory enhancement (Shuai et al., 2010). In addition, both Rac and RGK proteins regulate cytoskeletal remodeling (Luo, 2000; Etienne-Manneville and Hall, 2002). Therefore, we aimed to assess the potential interplay between Rgk1 and Rac by examining whether rgk1 overexpression suppresses the Rac-dependent memory decay. We determined that the memory defect caused by adult-specific induction of RacV12 was suppressed by coexpression of Rgk1 transgenes (Fig. 7A). The suppressive effect was identified with the Rgk1ΔN construct as well as the full-length construct. In contrast, the ΔC construct did not suppress the memory defect, which suggests that the C-terminal region of Rgk1-PB (1120–1380) is necessary to counteract Rac-dependent memory decay. We also investigated whether endogenous rgk1 has a role in the suppression of Rac-dependent memory decay. When one copy of rgk1 was removed, the RacV12-dependent memory decay was enhanced (Fig. 7B,C), which suggests that rgk1 suppresses RacV12 activity. These findings indicate that Rgk1 functions, at least in part, through the suppression of Rac-dependent forgetting to maintain memory.
Discussion
Our genetic analysis demonstrates that rgk1 plays a pivotal role in Drosophila olfactory aversive memory. We propose that the ITM is genetically divided into three components: the rut-, dnc-, and rgk1-dependent pathways. The rut and dnc pathway act specifically for ASM and ARM, respectively, whereas rgk1 acts for both ASM and ARM, albeit partially. Consistent with this notion, it is noteworthy that the ASM and ARM pathways converge on Rgk1, yet the functional domains may be dissected; the full-length form of Rgk1 is required for ARM, whereas the molecule that lacks the N-terminal domain is capable of generating ASM, which suggests that the protein(s) required for ARM formation may interact with the N-terminal domain of Rgk1.
Our data suggested that Rgk1 acts for both ASM and ARM, whereas the rgk1 deletion mutant, which was shown to be a protein null, exhibited only a partial reduction in ITM; these findings imply that Rgk1 regulates an aspect of each memory component. This idea may be explained by the expression pattern of Rgk1. Rgk1 exhibited exclusive expression and cell-type specificity in the KCs, whereas the memory components have been shown to be regulated by the neuronal network spread outside of the MBs and are encoded by multiple neuronal populations (Berry et al., 2008; Scheunemann et al., 2012; Scholz-Kornehl and Schwarzel, 2016). For example, two parallel pathways exist for ARM (Lee et al., 2011; Wu et al., 2013) and ASM is modulated, not only by MB-extrinsic neurons (Waddell et al., 2000; Keene et al., 2006), but also by the ellipsoid body that localizes outside of the MBs (Zhang et al., 2013). dnc-dependent ARM requires antennal lobe local neurons (Scheunemann et al., 2012) and octopamine-dependent ARM requires α′/β′ KCs (Wu et al., 2013), in neither of which was Rgk1 detected. Therefore, Rgk1 may support a specific part of memory components that exists in a subset of KCs.
The specific expression of Rgk1 in KCs suggests its dedicated role in MB function. Rgk1 exhibited cell-type specificity in KCs from anatomical and functional points of view. Rgk1 is strongly expressed in α/β and γ KCs and weakly expressed in α′/β′ KCs and the expression of the rgk1-sh transgene in α/β and γ KCs was sufficient to disrupt memory. Several genes required for memory formation have been shown to be expressed preferentially in the KCs and the notable genes include dunce, rutabaga, and DC0 (Nighorn et al., 1991; Han et al., 1992; Skoulakis et al., 1993; Davis, 2005; Keene and Waddell, 2007). Although a recent study in KC dendrites showed that the modulation of neurotransmission into the KCs affects memory strength (Gai et al., 2016), KC synapses are thought to be the site in which memory is formed and stored (Dubnau et al., 2001; McGuire et al., 2001; Schwaerzel et al., 2003; Hige et al., 2015b; Barnstedt et al., 2016). Our analyses with immunostaining and GFP fusion transgenes indicated that Rgk1 is localized to synaptic sites of the KC axons, which raises the possibility that Rgk1 may regulate the synaptic plasticity that underlies olfactory memory. Among the RGK family proteins, Rem2 is highly expressed in the CNS and regulates synapse development through interactions with 14-3-3 proteins (Finlin et al., 2000; Ghiretti and Paradis, 2011), which have been shown to be localized to synapses (Zhou et al., 1999) and are required for hippocampal long-term potentiation and associative learning and memory (Qiao et al., 2014). In Drosophila, 14-3-3ζ is enriched in the MBs and is required for olfactory memory (Philip et al., 2001; Messaritou et al., 2009). In addition, the C-terminal region of Drosophila Rgk1 contains serine and threonine residues that exhibit homology to binding sites for 14-3-3 proteins in mammalian RGK proteins (Puhl et al., 2014). Therefore, Rgk1 and 14-3-3ζ may act together in the synaptic plasticity that underlies olfactory memory.
The roles of RGK family proteins in neuronal functions have been investigated extensively. Our data, when combined with the accumulated data on the function of RGK family proteins, provide novel insights into the mechanism that governs two distinct intermediate-term memories, ASM and ARM. Regarding the regulation of ASM, our data showed that Rgk1 suppressed the forgetting that was facilitated by Rac. Rac is a major regulator of cytoskeletal remodeling (Luo, 2000; Etienne-Manneville and Hall, 2002). Similarly, mammalian RGK proteins participate in the regulation of cell shape through the regulation of actin and microtubule remodeling (Piddini et al., 2001; Ward et al., 2002). Rgk1 may affect Rac activity indirectly by sharing an event in which Rac also participates because there have been no reports showing that RGK proteins regulate Rac activity directly; further, we determined that rgk1 transgene expression did not affect the projection defect of KC axons caused by RacV12 induction during development (data not shown). Therefore, we suggest that Rgk1 signaling and Rac signaling may merge at the level of downstream effectors in the regulation of forgetting. A member of the mammalian RGK1 proteins, Gem, has been shown to regulate Rho GTPase signaling (Correll et al., 2008) through interactions with Ezrin, Gimp, and Rho kinase (Aresta et al., 2002; Ward et al., 2002; Hatzoglou et al., 2007). Rho kinase is a central effector for Rho GTPases (Van Aelst and D'Souza-Schorey, 1997; Hall, 1998; Mackay and Hall, 1998; Kaibuchi et al., 1999) and has been shown to phosphorylate LIM-kinase (Maekawa et al., 1999). In Drosophila, the Rho-kinase ortholog DRok has been shown to interact with LIM-kinase (Verdier et al., 2006). Furthermore, Rac regulates actin reorganization through LIM kinase and cofilin (Yang et al., 1998) and the PAK/LIM-kinase/cofilin pathway has been postulated to be critical in the regulation of memory decay by Rac (Shuai et al., 2010). It was shown recently that Scribble scaffolds a signalosome consisting of Rac, Pak3, and Cofilin, which also regulates memory decay (Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2016). Therefore, Rgk1 may counteract the consequence of Rac activity (i.e., memory decay) through the suppression of the Rho-kinase/LIM-kinase pathway. DRok is a potential candidate for further investigation of the molecular mechanism in which Rgk1 acts to regulate memory decay.
Our data indicated that Rgk1 is required for ARM in addition to ASM. It has been shown that Synapsin and Brp specifically regulate ASM and ARM, respectively (Knapek et al., 2010; Knapek et al., 2011). The functions of Synapsin and Brp may be differentiated in a synapse by regulating distinct modes of neurotransmission (Knapek et al., 2011). The exact mechanism has not been identified for this hypothesis; however, the regulation of voltage-gated calcium channels may be one of the key factors that modulate the neurotransmission (Catterall and Few, 2008; Nakamura et al., 2015). Voltage-gated calcium channels are activated by membrane depolarization and the subsequent Ca2+ increase triggers synaptic vesicle release (Catterall and Few, 2008). The regulation of voltage-gated calcium channels has been shown to be important in memory; a β-subunit of voltage-dependent Ca2+ channels, Cavβ3, negatively regulates memory in rodents (Jeon et al., 2008). Importantly, Brp regulates the clustering of Ca2+ channels at the active zone (Kittel et al., 2006). Moreover, it has been demonstrated extensively that mammalian RGK family proteins regulate voltage-gated calcium channels. Kir/Gem and Rem2 interact with the Ca2+ channel β-subunit and regulate Ca2+ channel activity (Béguin et al., 2001; Finlin et al., 2003; Yang and Colecraft, 2013). In addition, the ability to regulate Ca2+ channels has been shown to be conserved in Drosophila Rgk1 (Puhl et al., 2014). Therefore, both Brp and Rgk1 may regulate ARM through the regulation of calcium channels, the former through the regulation of their assembly and the latter through the direct regulation of their activity. Our finding that Rgk1 localized to the synaptic site and colocalized with Brp lends plausibility to the scenario that Rgk1 regulates voltage-gated calcium channels at the active zone.
Several memory genes identified in Drosophila, including rutabaga, PKA-R, and CREB, have homologous genes that have been shown to regulate behavioral plasticity in other species (Wu et al., 1995; Abel et al., 1997; Davis, 2005; Kida and Serita, 2014). The identification of Drosophila rgk1 as a novel memory gene raises the possibility for another conserved mechanism that governs memory. There is limited research regarding the role of RGK proteins at the behavioral level in other species; however, the extensively documented functions of RGK proteins with respect to the regulation of neuronal functions, combined with our data presented here regarding Drosophila Rgk1, raise the possibility of an evolutionally conserved function for RGK family proteins in memory.
Note Added in Proof: During the proof processing, a close scrutiny of the entire data revealed several errors in posting during the statistical analysis (Fig. 2A, Fig. 4C–F, Fig. 6A), as well as in a brain image processing (Fig. 3D). These errors have been corrected appropriately. None of them changed any conclusions.
Footnotes
This work was supported by grants-in-aid for Scientific Research from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology (MEXT) of Japan (Grants 20115005 and 25115008 to T.T. and S.M.), Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (Grant 15K06700 to S.M.), the Takeda Science Foundation (T.T.), the Platform Project for Supporting in Drug Discovery and Life Science Research (Platform for Drug Discovery, Informatics, and Structural Life Science) from MEXT (K.S.), and the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (K.S.). We thank M. Saitoe, M. Heisenberg, H. Tanimoto, Y. Aso, A. Sugie, and T. Miyashita for providing fly strains and reagents; the Kyoto Drosophila Genetic Resource Center (DGRC), the National Institute of Genetics, the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center (BDSC), the Berkeley Drosophila Genome Project, the Exelixis Collection, and the Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank for reagents; H. Tarui and K. Kurimoto for help with the RNA experiments; and members of Tabata laboratory for valuable comments and discussions.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Tetsuya Tabata, Institute of Molecular and Cellular Biosciences, University of Tokyo, Yayoi 1-1-1, Tokyo 113-0032, Japan. ttabata{at}iam.u-tokyo.ac.jp