Abstract
The identification of genes related to encoding, storage, and retrieval of memories is a major interest in neuroscience. In the current study, we analyzed the temporal gene expression changes in a neuronal mRNA pool during an olfactory long-term associative memory (LTAM) in Caenorhabditis elegans hermaphrodites. Here, we identified a core set of 712 (538 upregulated and 174 downregulated) genes that follows three distinct temporal peaks demonstrating multiple gene regulation waves in LTAM. Compared with the previously published positive LTAM gene set (Lakhina et al., 2015), 50% of the identified upregulated genes here overlap with the previous dataset, possibly representing stimulus-independent memory-related genes. On the other hand, the remaining genes were not previously identified in positive associative memory and may specifically regulate aversive LTAM. Our results suggest a multistep gene activation process during the formation and retrieval of long-term memory and define general memory-implicated genes as well as conditioning-type-dependent gene sets.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The identification of genes regulating different steps of memory is of major interest in neuroscience. Identification of common memory genes across different learning paradigms and the temporal activation of the genes are poorly studied. Here, we investigated the temporal aspects of Caenorhabditis elegans gene expression changes using aversive olfactory associative long-term memory (LTAM) and identified three major gene activation waves. Like in previous studies, aversive LTAM is also CREB dependent, and CREB activity is necessary immediately after training. Finally, we define a list of memory paradigm-independent core gene sets as well as conditioning-dependent genes.
Introduction
The identification of genes involved in encoding, storage, and retrieval of memories is of paramount scientific interest in neuroscience because of its implications for basic and medical research. So far, several microarray studies were performed to identify genes that contribute to learning and memory, long-term potentiation, and long-term plasticity (Cavallaro et al., 2002; Paratore et al., 2006; Burger et al., 2008; Benoit et al., 2011). For example, expression changes of neuronal genes were analyzed in vertebrates such as rats and in mice to detect genes that are differentially expressed after induction of long-term potentiation (Park et al., 2006; Ryan et al., 2011). Moreover, the memory consolidation process in rats, mice, and rabbits was studied at the transcriptional level after different learning paradigms, such as passive avoidance learning, fear conditioning, T-maze learning, Morris water maze training, and classical eye-blink conditioning (for review, see Paratore et al., 2006). Importantly, many early microarray studies in vertebrates were performed using arrays limited to a selected subset of known neuronal genes, thus reducing the consistency of gene expression findings across the different memory assays.
The relatively simple nervous system of Caenorhabditis elegans allows associative learning between a variety of volatile or soluble odorants and food. Several olfactory learning assays of long-term associative memory (LTAM) have been established, such as positive olfactory conditioning, in which repeated presentation of 2-butanone as an odorant in association with food leads to long-term attraction to the odorant (Kauffman et al., 2011). Alternatively, under aversive olfactory conditioning, pairing of diacetyl (DA) as an attractant with starvation results in long-term avoidance to DA (Vukojevic et al., 2012). Previously, tests using different memory assays in C. elegans have already led to the discovery of several genes critical for long-term associative learning (for review, see Ardiel and Rankin, 2010).
Like in mammals, long-term memory requires transcriptional gene activation also in C. elegans (Kauffman et al., 2011; Vukojevic et al., 2012). Recently, transcriptome-wide changes directly after olfactory LTAM training have been investigated in C. elegans (Lakhina et al., 2015). Combining positive olfactory training together with genome-wide expression analysis of wild-type and CREB mutant animals, Lakhina et al. (2015) identified CREB-dependent or -independent LTAM-induced genes. However, previous studies in C. elegans have not investigated the temporal aspects of gene activation during LTAM.
Here, we performed a microarray-based time course assay with an enriched neuronal mRNA pool, after aversive olfactory associative conditioning in C. elegans. We identified a core set of 712 (538 upregulated and 174 downregulated) genes that was reproducibly regulated with different temporal kinetics during LTAM. The gene regulation followed three different temporal activation patterns suggesting multiple gene activation waves during consolidation of LTAM. Two of these activation peaks coincided with the memory inhibitory effect of actinomycin D that showed a biphasic inhibition of the aversive LTAM. Fifty percent of the identified upregulated genes were previously identified during positive olfactory LTAM (Lakhina et al., 2015) and might therefore, represent stimulus-independent memory-regulated genes. On the other hand, the remaining 50% of the genes were not related to positive LTAM and may specifically regulate aversive olfactory LTAM. Furthermore, we showed that aversive LTAM is CREB dependent, that CREB phosphorylation increases after conditioning, and that CREB activity is necessary in the first 2 h after training. In summary, our results suggest the existence of a multistep gene activation process during LTAM and define general memory-implicated genes as well as conditioning-type-specific gene sets.
Materials and Methods
Reagents, C. elegans growth conditions, and strains used
All reagents used were from Sigma-Aldrich, unless indicated. Worms were maintained using standard methods and grown at 20°C (Brenner, 1974). For all the experiments, a synchronized population of young adult hermaphrodite worms was used. Strains used were the wild-type C. elegans Bristol strain, variety N2, gaIs153[pF25B3.3::FLAG::pab-1;sur-5::GFP] (Von Stetina et al., 2007), gaIs146[myo-3::FLAG::pab-1;sur-5::GFP] (Roy et al., 2002), syg-1(ky652), ppt-1(gk139), rig-5(utr2), ras-2(ok682), eps-8(by160); Ex[opt-2p::EPS-8A::GFP] (Croce et al., 2004), and rpm-1(ju41), syd-2(ok217) ncx-3(utr4), prkl-1(ok3182), ttx-1(oy26) oyIs17, crh-1(tz2), crh-1(tz2); utrEx80[hsp16.41p::crh-1::FLAG; sur-5::dsred; myo-3::rfp].
Conditioning of C. elegans and behavior assays
Long-term conditioning was performed as described previously (Vukojevic et al., 2012), and three independent biological replicates were tested. Briefly, well fed synchronized young adults were starved in the presence of 2 μl of DA for 1 h, and the conditioning was repeated separated by a 30 min recovery phase on seeded plates without DA. In the case of the temporal profiling assay (first experiment), the worms were either collected immediately or transferred on nematode growth medium (NGM) plates with abundant food and collected 0.5, 1, 2, 4, 8, 16, and 24 h after conditioning. As controls, worms were exposed for 1 h to starvation alone (S) or to DA in the presence of abundant food (A). For the second expression analysis, worms were collected 4 or 8 h after conditioning, starvation, or DA treatment in the presence of food. Pulse treatment with actinomycin D was performed by soaking worms for 5 min in M9 supplemented with DMSO alone or 100 μg/ml actinomycin D in DMSO at different times after DA conditioning. After treatment, worms were washed in M9 and placed on seeded NGM plates for additional time, and chemotaxis toward DA of the treated worms was tested 24 h after the initial DA conditioning. Chemotaxis was assessed by washing well fed young adult worms of different genotypes with CTX buffer (25 mm K-phosphate, pH 6.0, 1 mm CaCl2, and 1 mm MgSO4) and tested their attraction to 0.1% DA on 10 cm CTX agar plates. For the LTAM behavioral assays, well fed young adult worms were tested for their attraction to 0.1% DA before conditioning (naive) or exposed to 2 μl of DA for 1 h in the absence of food. Conditioning was repeated separated by a 30 min recovery phase on seeded plates without DA. After conditioning, worms were tested for their preference to DA immediately (0 h) or worms were washed in M9 and placed on seeded NGM plates for additional time, and their chemotaxis toward DA was tested 24 h after the initial DA conditioning. The attraction to diacetyl was visualized either as chemotaxis index (CI; Bargmann et al., 1993) or as memory index (MI = CInaive − CIdelay/CInaive; Stetak et al., 2009).
Real-time qPCR
Total RNA was isolated from synchronized adult worms using the Direct-zol RNA MiniPrep kit (Zymo Research Cooperation) with DNase treatment. A total of 400 ng of RNA was supplemented with 20 ng of Phe spike RNA, and the mix was reverse-transcribed using a mix of random decamers (Ambion) and anchored oligo(dT)20 primers (Invitrogen). Real-time PCR was performed using the SyBr Fast kit (Kapa Biosystems) according to the manufacturer's recommendations in a Rotor Gene-6000 instrument (Corbett Research). Expression levels of tba-1 were normalized to the Phe spike RNA and compared with the untreated tba-1 expression level. Fold differences were calculated using the ΔΔCt method (Pfaffl, 2001).
Western blot analysis
Fifty to 100 worms were lysed directly in Laemmli buffer and subjected to SDS-PAGE, transferred onto PVDF membranes, blocked with 5% nonfat dry milk in Tris-buffered saline containing 0.05% Tween 20, and incubated with primary antibodies as indicated. Antibodies used were rabbit CREB Ser133 (Cell Signaling Technology, 1:1000 dilution), mouse actin (Millipore, 1:2000 dilution), and mouse FLAG (Sigma-Aldrich, 1:1000 dilution). Primary antibodies were detected with appropriate HRP-conjugated secondary antibodies (Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories, 1:5000 dilution).
Tissue-specific mRNA isolation
Neuronal mRNA was isolated as described previously (Von Stetina et al., 2007) with some modifications. After the different treatments, polyA-binding protein (PAB-1::FLAG)-expressing worms were collected and washed, and RNA–protein complexes were stabilized by incubating worms in 5 ml of 0.5% formaldehyde in CTX buffer (25 mm K-phosphate, pH 6.0, 1 mm CaCl2, and 1 mm MgSO4) for 1 h at 4°C, washed once with CTX, washed twice with 700 μl of homogenization buffer [HB; 300 mm NaCl, 50 mm HEPES buffer, pH 7.6, 10 mm MgCl2, 1 mm EGTA, 30 mm EDTA, 0.2 mg/ml heparin, 10% glycerol, 1 mm dithiothreitol, 8 mm vanadyl ribonucleoside complex, 50 U/ml Rnasin ribonuclease inhibitor (Promega), and EDTA-free protease inhibitor mixture (Roche)], and shock-frozen in liquid nitrogen. The homogenization was done in a TissueLyzer MM 301 Ball Mill Homogenizer (Retsch) three times for 30 s at 30 Hz. The homogenate was cleaned with centrifugation, and immunoprecipitation was done with 30 μl of anti-FLAG M2 agarose bead slurry (Sigma-Aldrich) equilibrated in HB. Precipitated mRNA–protein complexes were eluted in elution buffer (50 mm Tris-HCl, pH 8.0, 10 mm EDTA, 1.3% SDS, and 160 U/ml Rnasin ribonuclease inhibitor), and RNA was isolated using the Direct-zol RNA MiniPrep kit (Zymo Research Cooperation) according to the manufacturer's protocol. The RNA was eluted and concentrated using the RNA Clean & Concentrator-5 kit (Zymo Research Cooperation) without DNase treatment according to the manufacturer's recommendations.
RNA amplification for the microarrays
Seventy-five nanograms of target RNA were processed using the GeneChip 3′ IVT Express kit (Affymetrix) according to the manufacturer's recommendations. Briefly, the RNA was reverse transcribed using oligo(dT) primer. After second-strand cDNA synthesis, the biotin-labeled RNA was purified using magnetic beads; fragmented and hybridized on a GeneChip C. elegans Genome Array (Affymetrix); stained and washed with the FS450_000 fluidics protocol on the GeneChipFluidics Station 450, using the GeneChip Hybridization, Wash, and Stain kit (Affymetrix); and scanned using the GeneChipScanner (Affymetrix). The replicates were processed in three independent batches.
Microarray data analysis
Preprocessing.
Expression data were processed using the affy R package (Gautier et al., 2004). Expression intensities were normalized using the RMA algorithm. Above background expression filtering was applied using the Mas5Calls algorithm: probe sets called as absent (cutoff, p ≥ 0.05) in all arrays of an experiment were excluded, yielding a total of 15,671 and 14,979 probe sets for the temporal profiling study and second experiment, respectively.
Temporal profiling.
To characterize temporal profiles after conditioning, probe sets were selected based on differential expression (DE) analysis between naive state and conditioning time points. DE analysis was performed using linear models, as implemented in the limma R package (Ritchie et al., 2015), considering batch (3 levels) and condition (11 levels corresponding to the naive, starved, adapted, and eight time points after conditioning). From these models, contrasts were first built to compare each conditioning time point with the naive state; p values were corrected for multiple testing (global correction for all probe sets and eight contrasts) using Benjamini and Hochberg false discovery rate (FDR). A probe set was considered as differentially expressed after aversive conditioning if at least one significant contrast was detected (FDR <0.05). To further limit the number of probe sets entering clustering analysis, a fold change (FC) criteria ≥|1.8| was applied that corresponded to the median |FC| observed for all significant contrasts. To circumvent overly conservative multiple correction for the control conditions, change after starvation or adaptation (SA) response compared with the naive state was based on FC criteria solely (≥|1.8|); thus a total of 736 probe sets were identified as responding after SA. Among those, 639 probe sets simultaneously showed differential expression after conditioning and were referred to as SA set.
Before clustering analysis, expression intensities were adjusted for batch effects using R package PamR (Hastie et al., 2014). Hierarchical clustering (average linkage with Pearson's correlation distance) was applied to per-probe mean intensities centered across the 11 states (N, S, A, 0 h, 0.5 h, 1 h, 2 h, 4 h, 8 h, 16 h, 24 h).
Cluster assignment was obtained using the dynamic tree cut algorithm implemented in the dynamicTreeCut R package (Langfelder et al., 2008), with a minimum cluster size of 200.
Second differential expression analysis.
Statistical analysis was based on limma linear models including batch (three levels) and condition (seven levels: naive, starved 4 h, starved 8 h, adapted 4 h, adapted 8 h, conditioned 4 h, conditioned 8 h). To increase statistical power, differential expression contrasts were built considering combined time points within each state (4 h + 8 h ÷ 2). Thus, the following three contrasts were evaluated: naive versus conditioning (4 h + 8 h), starvation (4 h + 8 h) versus conditioning (4 h + 8 h), and adaptation (4 h + 8 h) versus conditioning (4 h + 8 h). Association p values were adjusted for multiple testing using the Benjamini and Hochberg FDR method (global correction for all probe sets and all contrasts).
Enrichment analyses.
Affymetrix probe set gene annotation was performed using WormBase (www.wormbase.org) database WS245 (affy oligo mapping information). A total of 12,999 mapped genes were thus used as background in all enrichment analyses.
Gene ontology (GO) enrichment analysis was performed using the DAVID functional annotation on-line tool (https://david.ncifcrf.gov/; Huang da et al., 2009). Positive olfactory gene lists corresponding to CREB-dependent upregulated, CREB-dependent downregulated, CREB-independent upregulated, and CREB-independent downregulated categories were obtained from published materials in the study by Lakhina et al. (2015; genes identified by SAM analysis reported in Tables S4 and S5 from original report) and annotated using WormBase database WS245. Genes overlapping with our background genes list (n = 12,999) were considered for analysis. Given an observed overlap between the previously defined gene categories, we further restricted the analysis to genes exclusively found within one single category, yielding the following nonoverlapping gene lists: CREB-dependent upregulated (n = 538), CREB-dependent downregulated (n = 86), CREB-independent upregulated (n = 1195), and CREB-independent downregulated (n = 851). Enrichment analyses for positive olfactory assay gene lists were performed using Fisher's exact test.
Results
Temporal transcriptomic profiling after aversive conditioning
Previously, we demonstrated that both transcription and translation are required to maintain LTAM after aversive olfactory conditioning (Vukojevic et al., 2012). To gain insight on the gene regulation process during LTAM, we first performed a time assay to define the time points of gene activation related to aversive olfactory LTAM. Therefore, we performed actinomycin D pulse treatment of DA-conditioned worms at different times after training and tested the LTAM of these animals 24 h after conditioning (Fig. 1a). To define the duration of the pulse treatment, we analyzed the relative amount of tubulin mRNA using real-time PCR. We found that a 5 min actinomycin D treatment inhibited transcription for 1 h starting 20 min after treatment (Fig. 1b). Inhibition of transcription using short actinomycin D treatment immediately after conditioning very efficiently blocked LTAM, whereas application of actinomycin D between 45 min and 2 h after conditioning was not effective. We observed a second peak of inhibition 3 h after conditioning that corresponds to a wave of gene activation between 3 and 4 h after conditioning. Thus, our results show that aversive associative LTAM induces a biphasic gene activation with a peak corresponding to immediate genes and a second peak 4 h after conditioning.
To identify neuronal genes regulated by aversive LTAM in a temporal manner, we used the previously established and characterized pan-neuronal expressed PAB-1::FLAG system (Von Stetina et al., 2007). First, we tested the enrichment of neuronal genes using PAB-1::FLAG–RNA complex purification and tested enrichment of specific genes over total RNA. We tested the levels of known neuronal, or as a control, muscle expressed gene with qPCR in the purified PAB-1-bound RNA pool from pan-neuronal expressed PAB-1::FLAG (Von Stetina et al., 2007) or as a control from myo-3-driven PAB-1::FLAG (Roy et al., 2002). We found that PAB-1::FLAG precipitation enriched the tested RNAs according to their expected tissue specificity (Fig. 1c).
Thus for the microarray study, we collected neuronal enriched mRNA pools using the pan-neuronal promoter-driven FLAG::PAB-1 protein (Von Stetina et al., 2007) at different time points after aversive conditioning (Fig. 2a). In addition, we assessed gene expression in untrained worms or animals exposed to starvation or to DA alone. Gene expression profiling for each treatment was performed in three independent biological replicates. First, we analyzed the global gene expression intensity profile using principal component analysis that showed a clear separation between the naive and control states and the different conditioning time points displayed in a regular and chronologically consistent pattern (Fig. 2b). Using the microarray results, we also tested the global enrichment of neuronal genes in the PAB-1::FLAG immunoprecipitations by comparing the detected genes with the recent results of Kaletsky et al. (2016) reporting a set of ∼11,000 genes expressed in C. elegans adult neurons. Using this gene expression annotation, we observed a significant increase in the proportion of neuronal expressed genes among genes expressed above background intensity in our microarray study (72.8%) compared with the entire set of genes mapped by the array (59%; proportion test, p < 2.2e−16). Although we could show neuronal gene enrichment using pan-neuronal expressed PAB-1 over the total RNA pool, neither the qPCR experiments nor the global enrichment analysis fully rule out alterations in the PAB-1 expression across different neuronal cells.
Next, to characterize transcriptomic patterns induced by conditioning, we first assessed differential expression between each conditioning time point and naive state (see Materials and Methods). Using limma-moderated t tests, we selected a total of 3563 transcripts exhibiting at least one time point with significant contrast (FDR, <0.05; fold change, ≥|1.8|; Fig. 2c).
Among the 3563 identified transcripts, 639 probe sets also showed change between naive state and adaptation [DA only, conditioned stimulus (CS)] or starvation [food withdrawal only, unconditioned stimulus (US); fold change, ≥|1.8|] (Fig. 2c). These probe sets were referred to as SA-regulated probe sets.
To gain insight on the temporal patterns of expression after conditioning, we performed hierarchical clustering on expression intensities of the 3563 identified transcripts across the 11 states (see Materials and Methods) and thus identified six distinct patterns of regulation.
Three of the identified clusters (clusters 1–3) corresponded to transcripts upregulated after conditioning (Fig. 2d). These clusters encompassed only a minor fraction of SA probe sets (Fig. 3a), suggesting that the observed transcriptional patterns may not reflect early adaptation- or starvation-dependent transcriptional responses.
Among those, clusters 1 and 2 showed temporal patterns that closely coincided with kinetics of gene activation during LTAM determined after actinomycin D treatment (Fig. 1). Interestingly, both clusters 1 and 2 were characterized by activation immediately after conditioning (Fig. 2d), which in the case of cluster 1 remained elevated for at least 4 h after conditioning. On the other hand, genes in cluster 2 showed a biphasic activation pattern with an immediate minor activation peak followed by a dominant upregulation peak culminating ∼4 h after conditioning (Fig. 2d), which coincide with the second wave of transcription required for LTAM formation (Fig. 1). GO analysis (see Materials and Methods) revealed enrichment for transcriptional activity-related terms (FDRB-H, <0.05) in clusters 1 and 2 (Fig. 2e). Cluster 2 also showed over-representation of genes linked to neuronal development and neuronal function (Fig. 2e). Finally, the last upregulation cluster 3 contained genes reaching the maximum activation ∼8 h after conditioning. However, no specific GO term enrichment was observed in this cluster. The three remaining clusters (clusters 4–6) contain a substantial fraction of genes that are characterized by an early or a delayed downregulation pattern after conditioning (Fig. 3b–d). These gene clusters showed significant enrichment (FDRB-H, <0.05) for GO categories broadly related to metabolic activity (Table 1). In addition, two of these clusters (clusters 4 and 5) encompassed a majority of the SA probe sets (Fig. 3a), indicating that the observed transcriptional patterns may primarily coincide with adaptation- or starvation-related transcriptomic response. Interestingly, among the clusters containing downregulation genes, cluster 5 showed a significant over-representation of genes related to neuropeptide signaling pathways (p = 5.6e−15) and notably contained 17 FMRF amide (Phe-Met-Arg-Phe-NH2)-like peptides, 13 nlp peptides, and 10 insulin-like peptides (Table 2). Finally, in the case of cluster c6, we observed only a minor fraction of the early SA probe sets (Fig. 3a), suggesting that starvation and adaptation response may also induce delayed transcriptional downregulation change. Altogether, temporal kinetic analysis of LTAM-regulated genes identified three waves of upregulation events, with peaks occurring at 1, 4, and 8 h after conditioning. Similarly, nearly half of the identified genes were found to be downregulated with kinetic patterns similar to the upregulated ones. However, these genes are more likely linked to metabolic processes, suggesting that they may reflect gene expression changes attributable to general homeostatic changes.
In a second independent experiment, we included additional conditional filters to differentiate between memory-regulated and starvation- or adaptation-related genes. We focused on genes differentially expressed at 4 and 8 h after conditioning as they represent ideal time points to cover most LTAM genes (Fig. 2). We assessed specificity of aversive LTAM-regulated transcriptional change (4 and 8 h) compared with adaptation- and starvation-regulated delayed responses (Fig. 4a). Transcriptomic profiles were obtained from neuronal enriched RNA extracted from naive worms, 4 and 8 h after conditioning, starvation (US only), and adaptation (CS only). Differential expression was subsequently evaluated between naive and conditioning states, starvation and conditioning states, and adaptation and conditioning states using limma-moderated t tests. To increase statistical power in differential expression analysis, contrasts were built considering the combined time points at 4 and 8 h within each state (see Materials and Methods).
In this second approach, we identified 1039 probe sets (706 upregulated, 333 downregulated) that simultaneously showed significant expression change between conditioning versus naive state, adaptation, and starvation (FDR, <0.05). Thus, these probe sets may represent specific memory-regulated transcripts. Absolute fold changes observed were higher for naive differential expression (median absolute FC, 1.9 across 1039 probe sets) than for adaptation and starvation (median absolute FC, 1.5 and 1.4, respectively).
We next examined the overlap between these memory-regulated probe sets and the set of 3563 probes identified in the first transcriptomic profiling study. This revealed that 82.4% of the 706 upregulated memory-regulated probe sets were previously identified as being differentially expressed after conditioning and had a similar direction of regulation, whereas only a minor fraction (2.8%) showed immediate change after adaptation or starvation (SA set). In contrast, only 55.6% of the 333 downregulated memory-regulated probe sets were consistently identified in the temporal profiling experiment without showing simultaneous early change in response to starvation or adaptation, whereas 24.9% showed early change in response to starvation or adaptation (SA set). Thus, this result further indicates that downregulated genes are likely related to starvation or adaptation response.
Finally, we compiled results from both microarray experiments (Fig. 4b) to generate a specific core set of probes meeting the following criteria: (1) differential expression after conditioning in the temporal profiling study; (2) not included in early SA set; and (3) identified as a memory-regulated gene in the second experimental setup with a direction of regulation consistent with the one observed in the temporal profiling study. Applying these filters, we defined a core set of aversive olfactory LTAM-regulated transcripts, which included a total of 582 upregulated and 185 downregulated probe sets, corresponding to 538 and 174 genes, respectively (Fig. 4c). As expected from our previous results, a majority of this core set was encompassed in cluster 2 (48%), which also showed the highest specificity toward adaptation and starvation (Fig. 4d). In summary, we defined a core long-term memory gene set that is mostly upregulated during aversive LTAM.
Different long-term memory paradigms affect transcriptional regulation of mostly identical genes
By combining positive conditioning assay with genome-wide transcriptional analysis of CREB mutants, Lakhina et al. (2015) recently reported sets of CREB-dependent and -independent LTAM-regulated genes in C. elegans. In this positive olfactory assay, the odorant 2-butanone is repeatedly presented in association with food, which results in formation of an LTAM toward 2-butanone (Kauffman et al., 2011). Previously, positive olfactory associative LTAM was shown to be dependent on CREB activity (Kauffman et al., 2011). To further support that the different LTAMs in C. elegans share common mechanisms, we tested the effect of crh-1 deletion on aversive LTAM. We found that deletion of crh-1 impaired aversive LTAM (Fig. 5a), which is in line with the role of CREB in different types of long-term memory in worms (Kauffman et al., 2011; Timbers and Rankin, 2011; Lakhina et al., 2015). Next, we analyzed the phosphorylation status of crh-1 after aversive LTAM conditioning. Similar to vertebrates, phosphorylation of crh-1 occurs after olfactory LTAM and reflects the activation of the protein (Kauffman et al., 2011). In accordance with our behavioral test result, we could detect an increase in crh-1 phosphorylation during aversive LTAM peaking directly after training. However, an elevated phosphorylation of crh-1 could be detected up to 16 h after conditioning (Fig. 5b). Finally, we analyzed the temporal requirement for CREB activity. For this, we first generated a crh-1(lf) mutant transgenic strain carrying the heat-shock promoter-driven wild-type crh-1::flag DNA and tested the expression kinetics of CRH-1 after heat shock (Fig. 5c). Using the FLAG-tag to detect the wild-type CRH-1 after 30 min of heat shock, we found that the protein is transiently expressed in a narrow time window (Fig. 5c), allowing us to test the temporal requirement for CREB during aversive LTAM. Thus, we performed LTAM tests using our transgenic strain with or without heat shock before, or at different time points after, conditioning. Whereas transient expression of the wild-type crh-1 before conditioning was not able to rescue the crh-1(lf) mutant LTAM defect (data not shown), transient expression of the wild-type protein in crh-1(lf) worms within the first 2 h after conditioning was sufficient to partially rescue the 24 h memory impairment of the mutant worms (Fig. 5d). On the other hand, later expression of CRH-1 was not sufficient to rescue LTAM (Fig. 5d). These results suggest that crh-1 activity is necessary within the first 2 h after conditioning. However, we cannot exclude the possibility that the initial CREB-dependent gene activation is a prerequisite for later CREB-mediated gene activation processes; thus, the expression of the wild-type protein in crh-1(lf) animals at later time points is not sufficient to rescue LTAM. Altogether, our results show that, like positive olfactory LTAM (Kauffman et al., 2011; Lakhina et al., 2015), aversive olfactory LTAM requires CREB activity and that early CREB activity after conditioning is necessary for aversive LTAM.
To test potential overlap between positive and aversive LTAM-induced genes, we compared our core set of 582 probes that correspond to 538 genes upregulated after aversive conditioning with the set of genes identified as upregulated after positive conditioning, in either a CREB-dependent or an independent manner (Lakhina et al., 2015; see also Materials and Methods). Comparison of the two datasets using different LTAM paradigms demonstrated a significant overlap between the genes identified (Fisher's exact test, p < 1e−50), with 269 (50.0% relative to 538) genes in common (Fig. 5e). In addition, only six upregulated core memory-regulated genes (1.1%) were previously defined as downregulated during positive LTAM, indicating high consistency between the two conditioning paradigms. We also used the CREB-dependency categorization reported by Lakhina et al. (2015) to get an insight on possible common mechanisms, and we observed in our aversive conditioning core set an almost equal representation of genes previously defined as CREB dependent and CREB independent (Lakhina et al., 2015; Fig. 5e). Interestingly, we found differences in the distribution of previously classified CREB-dependent/independent upregulated genes (Lakhina et al., 2015) across the different clusters. Genes classified as CREB independent were enriched in cluster 1 (Fisher's exact test, p = 1.3e−15), cluster 2 was enriched for both CREB dependent and independent to a similar extent (p = 7.9e−17 and p < 1e−50 for CREB independent and CREB dependent, respectively), and cluster 3 showed enrichment of previously defined CREB-dependent genes (Lakhina et al., 2015; p = 1.2e−24; Fig. 5f). Altogether, it is likely that both aversive and positive olfactory conditioning induce stimulus-independent gene expression changes related to memory, with a second group of differentially expressed genes being specific to the learning and memory assay used.
To confirm the implication of the identified genes in memory, we selected mutant worm strains for 10 genes from clusters 1 and 2 and tested their role in aversive LTAM. As a control, we first tested the impact of the deletion of the candidate genes on the chemotaxis toward diacetyl. Four of the mutants showed reduced chemotaxis, whereas the remaining six had no significant alteration in their attraction to DA (Fig. 6a). In the case of syd-2(lf), the chemotaxis to DA was severely impaired, which did not allow further analysis of the LTAM. Therefore, we tested the role of the remaining nine candidates on aversive olfactory learning and LTAM. None of the genes tested showed a defect in learning, whereas five demonstrated significant impairment of LTAM memory. Surprisingly, we found that loss of rpm-1 improves LTAM, which suggests that rpm-1 may regulate forgetting (Fig. 6b).
Discussion
In the current study, we investigated temporal gene regulation after aversive LTAM in C. elegans. We found that gene activation occurs in several waves after training. Based on the actinomycin D pulse treatment, two time points seem to be critical for establishment of long-term memory; an immediate and a second 4 h gene activation wave. Interestingly, most of the genes in cluster 2 that show a major activation 4 h after conditioning are also transiently activated immediately after conditioning, which first decreased before peaking again at 4 h after training. This biphasic activation pattern together with the effect of transient inhibition of transcription at 3–4 h after training with actinomycin D, support the idea that several transcriptional activation waves regulate long-term memory. However, our results cannot fully exclude that the delayed high expression of some transcripts might be attributed to increased mRNA stability induced after conditioning. In addition, we cannot clearly define the hierarchical connections between the observed transcriptional activation events; thus, it is possible that the initial gene activations are prerequisites for later transcriptional regulation processes and sequential, interdependent transcriptional activation waves act in concert during LTAM. Interestingly, based on clustering analysis of transcriptionally activated genes, we identified a third gene activation wave peaking at 8 h after treatment; however, this seems dispensable for LTAM maintenance. Previously, a biphasic CREB activation pattern has been suggested in the dentate gyrus of rats and during fear conditioning in mice (Schulz et al., 1999; Trifilieff et al., 2006), suggesting multiple transcriptional activation waves. Our data are in line with these results, suggesting an evolutionary conserved multiphasic gene activation mechanism during memory formation. In addition to the temporal profiling, we also introduced a second filtering step to eliminate genes activated during aversive conditioning unrelated to memory, and we built a core gene set that is more likely memory specific. Yet, we observed that the downregulated genes are less consistent across experiments and associate mostly with metabolic GO terms and are likely independent of olfactory learning but linked to stress and metabolic changes in the animals.
We also compared the core aversive olfactory memory-regulated gene set defined here with the results of the previous study by Lakhina et al. (2015) that uses a substantially different memory task in C. elegans. Interestingly, we found a large overlap between the genes regulated in the different assays, suggesting that there is a gene set that is activated during memory formation irrespectively of the training mode. Thus, these genes may represent general memory-regulated targets, whereas other groups of genes might be specific to the memory task applied. Alternatively, some genes are activated only at a defined time window and were not detected previously.
Finally, we analyzed the role of CREB and found that aversive LTAM is CREB dependent. This is in line with previous results demonstrating the role of CREB in different long-term memories in worms (Kauffman et al., 2011; Timbers and Rankin, 2011; Lakhina et al., 2015), mollusks, flies, and vertebrates (Yin and Tully, 1996).
Although CREB is essential for memory in all species investigated so far (Frank and Greenberg, 1994), the time points at which CREB activity is required are less understood. Phosphorylation of Ser133 has been identified as the main essential regulator of CREB (Yamamoto et al., 1990) and Western blot analysis demonstrated a biphasic increase in phosphorylation, for example, in the hippocampus (Trifilieff et al., 2006). However, a recent study using Ser133 mutant mice demonstrated that phosphorylation of this serine site is not needed for memory (Briand et al., 2015). Previously, Kauffman et al. (2011) demonstrated that the C. elegans CREB homolog crh-1 is phosphorylated at the conserved serine site during positive olfactory LTAM. In accordance with vertebrate studies and previous results from C. elegans, we found that crh-1 is phosphorylated during aversive LTAM. The temporal kinetics of crh-1 phosphorylation coincides with the immediate gene activation peak, suggesting that CREB plays a crucial role directly after learning. The possible immediate role of CREB activity is also supported by the temporal rescue of crh-1 mutant worms (Fig. 5d), which shows that crh-1 is necessary directly after conditioning. However, our results cannot rule out the possibility that the early CREB activation is required for a later, also CREB-dependent transcriptional activation that occurs at later time points. Future studies will also be necessary to investigate whether the transcriptional activation of the genes induced by the different LTAM assays shows the same dependence on CREB. Altogether, in the current study we identified a core memory-regulated gene set that contains general conditioning independent as well as aversive LTAM-regulated genes that are regulated, at least in part, by an evolutionary conserved early CREB-dependent gene activation mechanism in C. elegans aversive LTAM.
Notes
Supplemental material for this article is available at http://mcn.unibas.ch/files/Celegans_LTAM_Expression_SuppMaterial.zip. This material has not been peer reviewed.
Footnotes
This work was supported by Swiss National Science Foundation Grants 31003A_156579 (A.S.) and 320030_163434 (A.P.). Strains were provided by the Caenorhabditis Genetics Center, which is funded by the NIH Office of Research Infrastructure Programs (Grant P40 OD010440).
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Attila Stetak, University of Basel, Birmannsgasse 8, 4055 Basel, Switzerland. a.stetak{at}unibas.ch