Abstract
In humans, recognition of others' actions involves a cortical network that comprises, among other cortical regions, the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS), where biological motion is coded and the anterior intraparietal sulcus (aIPS), where movement information is elaborated in terms of meaningful goal-directed actions. This action observation system (AOS) is thought to encode neutral voluntary actions, and possibly some aspects of affective motor repertoire, but the role of the AOS' areas in processing affective kinematic information has never been examined. Here we investigated whether the AOS plays a role in representing dynamic emotional bodily expressions. In the first experiment, we assessed behavioral adaptation effects of observed affective movements. Participants watched series of happy or fearful whole-body point-light displays (PLDs) as adapters and were then asked to perform an explicit categorization of the emotion expressed in test PLDs. Participants were slower when categorizing any of the two emotions as long as it was congruent with the emotion in the adapter sequence. We interpreted this effect as adaptation to the emotional content of PLDs. In the second experiment, we combined this paradigm with TMS applied over either the right aIPS, pSTS, and the right half of the occipital pole (corresponding to Brodmann's area 17 and serving as control) to examine the neural locus of the adaptation effect. TMS over the aIPS (but not over the other sites) reversed the behavioral cost of adaptation, specifically for fearful contents. This demonstrates that aIPS contains an explicit representation of affective body movements.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT In humans, a network of areas, the action observation system, encodes voluntary actions. However, the role of these brain regions in processing affective kinematic information has not been investigated. Here we demonstrate that the aIPS contains a representation of affective body movements. First, in a behavioral experiment, we found an adaptation after-effect for emotional PLDs, indicating the existence of a neural representation selective for affective information in biological motion. To examine the neural locus of this effect, we then combined the adaptation paradigm with TMS. Stimulation of the aIPS (but not over pSTS and control site) reversed the behavioral cost of adaptation, specifically for fearful contents, demonstrating that aIPS contains a representation of affective body movements.
Introduction
Perception of movements of other living beings is crucial for survival in most species, to the extent that many vertebrate species have specialized neural systems for action observation. In humans, a widespread network of interconnected brain areas [known as the action observation system (AOS)] underlies the comprehension of conspecifics' body movements and actions. This network includes the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS; Puce and Perrett, 2003), and two mirror system areas, the putative human anterior intraparietal area (aIPS) and the ventral premotor/caudal inferior frontal gyrus complex (PMv/cIFG; Cattaneo and Rizzolatti, 2009). Several TMS studies have demonstrated that stimulating the pSTS, aIPS, and PMv/cIFG regions produces selective impairment in visual recognition of neutral actions (Grossman et al., 2005; Pobric and Hamilton, 2006; Candidi et al., 2008; Cattaneo et al., 2010; van Kemenade et al., 2012). But is the AOS also important for the encoding of the emotional aspects of biological motion?
The perception of affective stimuli, regardless of stimulus type, generally enhances the neural response of core affective systems, situated within the limbic system (Adolphs, 2002; Phillips et al., 2003) but emotional body movements are complex and their perception activates also a more widespread network of subcortical and cortical regions, related to analysis of visual body features and more generally to action observation and preparation (de Gelder, 2006, de Gelder et al., 2010, 2015; Tamietto and de Gelder, 2011). It is thus crucial to understand whether the activation within the AOS is a mere side-effect of the type of stimuli (body actions), independent from their content, or whether AOS activity is causally linked to emotional recognition. This issue has been explored in the literature in only two TMS studies; these found that perturbation of pSTS (Candidi et al., 2011) and inferior parietal lobule (Engelen et al., 2015) selectively improved the recognition of fearful body images. However, a limitation of both of these studies was that participants observed static images; human bodies are dynamic in nature and the brain substrates used in processing static postures are likely to differ from those engaged in perception of body movements. Furthermore, although conventional TMS paradigms can reveal the causal role of cortical regions in cognitive functions, they do not inform us about the neural representations in those regions.
Here we examined whether specific regions of the action observation network contain representations of affective body movements. This was accomplished by the use of state-dependent TMS, which enables the selectivity of neural representations in a cortical region to be assessed (Silvanto et al., 2008; Romei et al., 2016). This approach has been previously used to examine the selectivity of neural representations in various cognitive functions such as color and motion perception (Silvanto et al., 2007a; Cattaneo and Silvanto, 2008), numerical cognition (Kadosh et al., 2010), and action observation (Cattaneo et al., 2011, 2010; Sato et al., 2011; Jacquet and Avenanti, 2015). To examine the role of AOS in encoding the emotional aspects of dynamic biological motion, we used point-light displays (PLDs), also referred to as biological motion (BM) stimuli (Johansson, 1973), which allow isolation of motion signals from others visual cues. Kinematic information contained in PLDs is sufficient for detection of emotional content of human movements (Dittrich et al., 1996; Atkinson et al., 2004, 2007, 2012; Clarke et al., 2005; Chouchourelou et al., 2006; Alaerts et al., 2011). In Experiment 1, we examined behavioral adaptation effects of observed affective PLDs. We found an adaptation-like bias with incongruent stimuli recognized faster than congruent ones. In Experiment 2, we used the TMS-adaptation paradigm to examine the cortical locus of adaptation effects observed in Experiment 1. TMS over the aIPS, but not over pSTS nor over a visual control area, reversed the behavioral adaptation for fearful stimuli, indicating that this region contains neural representations selective for the fearful characteristics of human movements.
Materials and Methods
Visual stimuli and validation of emotional valence.
A total of 20 PLDs were presented, depicting 10 different expressions of happiness and fear, respectively. These stimuli are part of a wider dataset created by Atkinson et al. (2004, 2012). The PLDs consisted of 2-s-long digitalized video clips (for details, see Atkinson et al., 2012), displaying a single actor represented as 13 white dot-lights moving on a black background. The dots were positioned over the head and the main joints (1 dot over each ankle, knee, hip, elbow, shoulder, and hand) of the actor. Examples of the stimuli can be viewed at http://community.dur.ac.uk/a.p.atkinson/Stimuli.html. We selected happy and fearful stimuli because they are approximately equally arousing emotions, with opposite emotional valences (positive or negative). Before the main experiments, we ran a pilot study to validate the PLDs in terms of quantity of movement contained in the PLDs and of type and intensity of portrayed emotion. Sixteen healthy adults took part in this pilot experiment (13 females, mean age = 29.63, SD = 7.65). All the participants provided informed consent before participating in the experiment. They were seated in front of a 24 in monitor at a distance of ∼60 cm. The stimuli were presented foveally. Each PLD was presented once, and for each video participants were asked to recognize the conveyed emotion among four options (fear, happiness, neutral, and other) by pressing the corresponding button on the keyboard. The response options (appearing on the screen after each stimulus) were indicated with a label placed over the keys “F–H, J” and were randomized across participants. After the emotion recognition task, participants were asked to rate the “intensity of the emotion” and the “quantity of movement” on a scale from 1 to 5, using the numeric keys on the top of the keyboard. Stimuli were presented and responses recorded with E-Prime 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools). For each individual PLD, we calculated the accuracy of emotion categorization, the rated intensity of the emotion, and the rated quantity of movement. Data distribution was tested for normality with Shapiro–Wilk's test. Accuracy, intensity, and movement were not normally distributed, so they were analyzed using a nonparametric test for paired data, the Wilcoxon signed rank test with continuity correction. Significance thresholds were Bonferroni-corrected for three multiple comparisons (for each variable, we compared results between the 3 emotional valences; hence, the critical α was set as p < 0.017). There were no significant differences between the happy and fearful movements for accuracy, movement, and intensity, whereas, predictably, the neutral movements were rated as less intense compared with the two emotions (Table 1). This implies that the stimuli used in Experiments 1 and 2 (i.e., fearful and happy PLDs) do not differ in terms of 1) recognizability between the emotional categories, 2) intensity of the expressed emotion, or 3) quantity of movement contained in the stimuli.
Results of comparisons between the three emotional valences of PLDs for accuracy, intensity, and movement assessed in the pilot study
Experiment 1: behavioral assessment of adaptation to observed emotional body movements
Participants.
Twenty-six healthy adults (14 females, 12 males; mean age = 23.58 years, SD = 2.95 years) took part in the behavioral study (Experiment 1). All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Before the experiment, all participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Design and procedure.
Participants were seated in a comfortable chair in front of a 24 in computer screen at a distance of ∼60 cm. E-Prime version 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools) software was used for stimulus presentation and response recordings. The study consisted of 12 adaptation blocks (6 with happy and 6 with fearful adapters), consisting of a 1 min adapting period followed by eight test trials. Each trial began with a white central fixation cross over a black background, lasting for 10 s. This was followed by an adaptation period in which the same PLD was repeated 30 times (for a total duration of 60 s). Participants were asked to simply watch the stimuli and focus on the emotion expressed by the actor. The order of adaptation blocks was randomized. At the end of adaptation, a screen appeared asking participant to “Get ready for the task”, after which eight test stimuli (4 fearful and 4 happy PLDs) were presented. Half of the test stimuli were emotionally congruent and half were emotionally incongruent with the adapter, and their order was randomized. The test stimuli and the adapter stimuli belong to the same dataset, i.e., the same stimulus could be used as an adapter in one block or as a test stimulus in another block. However, in single blocks, the adapter stimulus was always different from the test stimuli presented thereafter. In other words, every stimulus could appear randomly as adapter or as a test in different blocks, but not in the same block. The movie clip was presented centrally. Simultaneously with the stimulus presentation, the question “Which emotion?” appeared on the upper part of the screen, and the two response options (“Fear” and “Happiness”) were presented on the lower part of the monitor. For each test stimulus, participants were asked to categorize the expressed emotion as fast as possible by key-press. The response options were indicated with a label placed over the keys “G” and “H”, and the key-emotion correspondence was randomized across participants. Participants were asked to respond using the index and the middle finger of their right hand. The PLD was presented for a maximum of 2 s, whereas the question and the response period lasted until participants responded. Accuracy and response times (RTs) were recorded.
Data analyses.
The dependent variable was mean RTs. Only correct responses were included in the analyses (the overall error rate was 4.43%). Data distributions failed the normality (Shapiro–Wilk's test) and homoscedasticity of variance (Bartlett's test) tests. To normalize the distribution, the averaged RTs were log-transformed before analyses (logRT). A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted with emotional content of the test stimuli (“emoTest”: fear or happiness) and emotional congruence between test and adapter stimuli (congruent or incongruent) as within-subject factors. Post hoc comparisons were performed with two-tailed paired-samples t tests with correction of the significance threshold for multiple comparisons whenever appropriate. All analyses were performed using R v3.3.1 (R Development Core Team, 2016).
Experiment 2: effects of TMS on perceptual adaptation
Participants.
Seventeen healthy adults (11 females, 6 males; mean age = 25.63, SD = 5.17) participated in the TMS experiment (Experiment 2). Three participants were excluded from the analysis because of difficulties in determining their resting motor threshold. In these participants, the TMS stimulation over M1 did not produce any visible hand twitch, and no motor sensation was perceived. Hence, the final analyses were performed on a total of 14 participants. Participants in the TMS experiment were screened for MRI and TMS contraindication before the experiment and received a £15 voucher refund for their participation. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Before the experiment, all participants provided written informed consent. The protocol was approved by the University of Westminster's ethical committee in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Neuronavigation and identification of stimulation sites on individual anatomy.
We used MRI-guided neuronavigation (BrainInnovation BV) for accurate positioning of the TMS coil. For each participant, a high resolution T1-weighted MPRAGE scan (176 partitions, 1 × 1 × 1 mm, flip angle = 7°, TI = 1000 ms, TE = 3.57 ms, TR = 8.4 ms) was acquired before the TMS experiment. Structural MRI images were obtained with a 1.5 T whole-body TIM Avanto System (Siemens Healthcare), at the Birkbeck/University College London Centre for NeuroImaging, with a 32-channel head coil. A 3D reconstruction of the gray matter surfaces and the scalp was created for each participant, which were coregistered to the participant's head to position the coil over the site of stimulation and to control coil position throughout the experiment. In each participant, three different sites in the right hemisphere were stimulated: the right pSTS, the right aIPS, and a posterior occipital control area located next to the midline. The three loci were identified on the basis of macro-anatomical landmarks. Specifically, the pSTS was targeted over the transition between its posterior segment and its horizontal segment (for an overview of STS anatomy, see Ochiai et al., 2004). We defined the aIPS as the most rostral part of the IPS at the intersection between the postcentral gyrus and the IPS (Caspers et al., 2006). Control TMS was applied to a site corresponding to a secondary visual area not primarily implied in coding for emotional aspect of visual stimuli, located between BA 17 and BA 18 (Fig. 1).
Representation of stimulation sites and respective anatomical landmarks. Right, Individual renderings of the gray-white matter border in each of the 14 participants. Left, The same brains as in the right are shown with the main anatomical landmarks used for localization of TMS targets. Blue, Central sulcus; green, postcentral sulcus; yellow, intraparietal sulcus; purple, Silvian fissure; red, superior temporal sulcus. The three stimulation sites (aIPS, pSTS, and control) are represented with white spots.
TMS.
Biphasic TMS pulses were applied with a figure-of-eight coil (D70 mm coil) connected to a Magstim Rapid2 stimulator. At first we searched in each participant the visually assessed resting motor threshold (rMT), defined as the stimulator's output intensity necessary to obtain a visible twitch in the contralateral intrinsic hand muscles in exactly 50% of trials in a series of at least eight consecutive pulses (Rossini et al., 1994). The intensity of stimulation in the actual experiment was set to 120% of the individual's rMT with a maximum of 65% maximal stimulator output due to coil overheating and limiting discomfort to participants. The coil was attached to a Magstim coil stand and placed tangentially to the scalp. Coil orientation was medial-lateral with the handle pointing laterally and slightly posteriorly (70° from the midline) for the aIPS position, to induce a current in the underlying cortical tissue approximately perpendicular to the IPS. A similar orientation was used for the stimulation of pSTS, but with the coil handles pointing upward. Due to pSTS proximity to the ears, in some participants the coil orientation was changed to minimize discomfort. For the occipital (control) stimulation, the coil was positioned perpendicular to the midline with the handle pointing outward. TMS was delivered in triplets. In every trial participants received three 10 Hz pulses time-locked to the onset of the PLD, starting synchronously with the visual stimulus.
Procedure.
The TMS paradigm was identical to that used in Experiment 1 described above. Every block consisted of 1 min of adapting period followed by eight test trials. A total of 12 adapter stimuli (6 happy and 6 fearful PLDs) and 96 test stimuli were presented for each of the three sites of stimulation. The order of adaptation blocks was randomized. During the adaptation period the same PLD was repeated 30 times (for 60 s). Participants were asked to simply watch the adapter stimuli and focus on the emotion expressed by the actor. At the end of adaptation, eight test stimuli (4 fearful and 4 happy PLDs) were presented. Half of the test stimuli were emotionally congruent (i.e., same emotion) and half were emotionally incongruent (i.e., different emotion) with the adapter, and their order was randomized. Participants were asked to categorize the expressed emotion (fear or happiness) as fast as possible by key-press, using the index and the middle finger of their right hand (Fig. 2). Accuracy and RTs were recorded. The three stimulation sites (right pSTS, right aIPS, and the control site) were stimulated on the same day with 30 min of delay between sessions. The order of stimulation sites was counterbalanced between participants. Participants wore earplugs and were seated in a comfortable chair in a quiet room, in front of a 24 in computer screen at a distance of 60 cm, with their head on a chinrest.
Timeline of TMS experiments. Every block consisted of 1 min of adapting period followed by eight test trials. During the adaptation period the same PLD was presented 30 times and participants were asked to simply watch the adapter stimuli. At the end of adaptation, 4 fearful and 4 happy test stimuli were presented. Participants were asked to categorize the expressed emotion by key press. Three 10 Hz TMS pulses were applied at the onset of every test stimulus.
Data analyses.
All analyses were performed using R v3.3.1 (R Development Core Team, 2016). The dependent variable was the mean of RTs. Only correct responses were included in the analyses. Data were tested for normality (Shapiro test) and homoscedasticity of variance (Bartlett test). To normalize the distribution, the averaged RTs were log-transformed before analyses (logRT). A three-way repeated-measures ANOVA (3 × 2 × 2) was performed. The site of TMS stimulation (“stimSite”), the emotional valence of the test stimuli (emoTest) and the emotional congruence between test and adapter stimuli (“congruence”) were entered as within-subject factors. Post hoc comparisons were performed with two-tailed paired-samples t tests. The significance threshold for the p values was corrected for multiple comparisons when appropriate. As a measure of the effects size, the generalized eta-squared (η2) is reported when appropriate. In addition, we calculated the Cohen's d for the significant comparisons using bootstrap resamples method (Gerlanc and Kirby, 2015). The number of bootstrap resamples (R) was set at 2000. Bootstrap Cohen's d effect size measures and their corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs) are also reported when appropriate.
Results
Experiment 1: behavioral evidence of perceptual adaptation to the emotional content of PLDs
In Experiment 1, the overall error rate was 4.43%. A summary of the results of Experiment 1 is presented in Table 2 and Figure 3. The two-way ANOVA showed a significant main effect of congruence (F(1,25) = 7.31, p = 0.012) with incongruent stimuli being recognized faster than congruent ones, while the interaction between emoTest and congruence was not significant (F(1,25) = 0.856, p = 0.364; η2 = 0.014; Cohen's d = −0.236; CI = −0.660, 0.166).
Mean and SE of RTs in all the conditions in Experiment 1
Visualization of results from Experiment 1. The performance of each participant is represented with a black bar. The gray columns represent the mean of RTs in congruent and incongruent conditions. Main analysis revealed an adaptation after-effect for affective PLDs with congruent stimuli being recognized significantly slower than incongruent ones. * indicates significant difference between congruent and incongruent condition.
Experiment 2: state-dependent effects of TMS over aIPS on explicit categorization of fearful PLDs
In Experiment 2, the overall error rate was 3.87%. The three-way ANOVA showed a significant main effect of congruence (F(1,13) = 14.994, p = 0.002), with congruent stimuli being recognized slower than incongruent ones (mean RTs: congruent = 1194 ms; incongruent = 1148 ms), suggesting the presence of an adaptation after-effect for affective PLDs and confirming the results of the behavioral experiment (Experiment 1). More importantly, we found a significant three-way interaction between stimSite, emoTest, and congruence (F(2,26) = 3.546, p = 0.043). To better understand this interaction, we performed three 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVAs in the three stimulation sites separately, with emoTest and congruence as within factors. We found a significant main effect of congruence in the control site (F(1,13) = 9.329; p = 0.009; η2 = 0.017) and in pSTS (F(1,13) = 9.393; p = 0.009; η2 = 0.029), showing that the adaptation after-effect persisted and hence suggesting that TMS stimulation did not have any effect on those two brain areas. On the contrary, ANOVA in aIPS showed a significant interaction between emoTest and congruence (F(1,13) = 8.474; p = 0.012; η2 = 0.022), but no significant main effects. In particular, the adaptation after-effect was still present for happy test stimuli (p = 0.009; Cohen's d = −0.311; CI = −1.114, 0.459) with incongruent stimuli recognized faster than congruent ones. Conversely, the adaptation after-effect was completely abolished for fearful test stimuli, to the point that we observed a trend toward an inversion of the adaptation effects, i.e., congruent test stimuli were recognized faster than congruent ones (p = 0.066; Cohen's d = 0.267; CI = −0.459, 1.075).
Discussion
Perceptual adaptation to emotional content of PLDs
In the first experiment, we investigated the perceptual aftereffects produced by repeated observation of emotional PLDs. When categorizing an affective PLD, participants' performance was markedly biased (slower RTs) by their previous exposure to congruent emotions. Adaptation aftereffects for PLDs have been reported previously for different features of biological motion, including gender characteristics (Troje et al., 2006), action category (van Boxtel and Lu, 2013; de la Rosa et al., 2014), and spatial orientation of observed bodily trajectories (Jackson and Blake, 2010; Theusner et al., 2011). Also judgments on-hand-object interactions in PLDs are susceptible to visual adaptation: viewing the grasping of a light object biases the judgment on subsequent grasped objects that appear heavier (Barraclough et al., 2009). In addition to, a number of studies reported adaptation aftereffects to affective facial (Russell and Fehr, 1987; Webster et al., 2004; Fox and Barton, 2007; Webster and MacLeod, 2011) and vocal expressions (Skuk and Schweinberger, 2013; Bestelmeyer et al., 2014). It remained unexplored whether emotional bodily movements can produce adaptation aftereffects. Our study fills this gap, providing the first evidence that perception of emotional whole-body movements can undergo selective perceptual adaptation.
Absence of state-dependent effects of TMS on the early visual cortex (control condition)
The aim of Experiment 2 was to examine the neural locus of this adaptation effect for affective dynamic bodily expressions. Following control stimulation we found adaptation after-effects similar to those observed in Experiment 1, i.e., a disadvantage in recognizing PLDs emotionally congruent with the adapter sequences (Fig. 4). Given the assumptions of TMS-adaptation paradigms, we did not expect any effect of TMS on this region, because the adapted features (bodily movements) are not supposed to be coded in the early visual cortex. Indeed, the earliest visual representation of bodies along the visual pathways is in the lateral occipital complex, way more rostral than the area that we chose as control (Downing et al., 2001). Studies in blindsight patients suggest that the processing of emotional information can efficaciously occur despite lesions of the early visual areas, either when conveyed by faces (de Gelder et al., 1999; Morris et al., 2001) or by body postures (de Gelder and Hadjikhani, 2006). Accordingly, in another study, TMS perturbation of V1 impaired the discrimination of neutral, but not emotional, body postures, supporting the hypothesis that the encoding of the emotional content does not depend on V1 (Filmer and Monsell, 2013).
Visualization of results from Experiment 2. Mean RTs are shown, classified according to emotion in the test PLD (happiness or fear); congruence with the adapter sequence (congruent or incongruent, indicated in the figure as “congr.” or “inc.” respectively); and to the site of TMS (aIPS, pSTS, or occipital control). The vertical bars represent the SE.
Absence of state-dependent effects of TMS on the pSTS
In contrast to the early visual cortex, the pSTS is tuned to biological motion. However, to our surprise, no state-dependent effects of TMS were found. We interpreted this finding in the light of the functional specialization of the pSTS. The integrity of STS is fundamental to biological motion identification (Vaina et al., 1990; Grossman et al., 2005; Saygin, 2007), it encodes low-level pictorial aspects of BM (Cattaneo et al., 2010), and it represents bodily movements separately for different body parts (upper limb, face, whole-body, gaze; Hein and Knight, 2008), probably in a viewpoint-invariant manner (Grossman et al., 2010). In one TMS study, stimulation of pSTS improved the visual match of body forms specifically for fearful body postures (Candidi et al., 2011). However this type of task relies on pictorial analysis likely encoded in pSTS, whereas we asked to recognize the emotional meaning of dynamic PLDs, potentially related to higher level of action representation implemented in aIPS (Fogassi et al., 2005; Hamilton and Grafton, 2006; Shmuelof and Zohary, 2006; Cattaneo et al., 2010). Similarly, another study (Tseng et al., 2014) showed that the specific effects of static fearful facial displays as distracters in a visual search task could be disrupted by anodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the right pSTS.
State-dependent effects of TMS on the aIPS
TMS stimulation over aIPS reduced significantly the cost of adaptation, and even produced a reversal of the cost of this effect, turning it into behavioral advantage. This finding is diagnostic for the presence of neurons that were affected by adaptation changes in the stimulated area (Silvanto et al., 2008; Romei et al., 2016; Silvanto and Pascual-Leone, 2008). TMS-adaptation is based on the phenomenon that the impact of TMS depends on the ongoing activity in the targeted region. TMS, which in the absence of adaptation impairs behavior, can induce a facilitatory effect if neurons in the targeted area have undergone adaptation (Silvanto et al., 2007a, b). The outcome of this differential effect of TMS on adapted versus nonadapted neuronal representations is the removal/reversal of the behavioral adaptation effect (Silvanto et al., 2007b; Silvanto and Muggleton, 2008; Romei et al., 2016). In the present study, the removal of behavioral adaptation to fearful stimuli by aIPS TMS indicates that this region contains neuronal representations tuned to affective movements.
Interestingly, the effects of TMS over aIPS were limited to fearful PLDs, and were absent for happy PLDs. What do we know about action representation in the aIPS? Several lines of evidence in both human (Arfeller et al., 2013) and nonhuman primates (Borra et al., 2008; Nelissen et al., 2011; Rizzolatti et al., 2014) indicate that action representations are hierarchically organized between a low-level pictorial representation in pSTS, and a more abstract representation of action goals in the parietofrontal system including the aIPS (Tunik et al., 2007; Cattaneo et al., 2010). The aIPS generalizes actions across effectors (Cattaneo et al., 2010) and is capable of encoding action invariants such as action endpoints, outcomes, and environmental changes produced by actions (Hamilton and Grafton, 2006, 2008). In Experiment 2, we found evidence that the explicit recognition of the emotional component of body movements relies in part on the parietal node of the AOS. Visual observation of emotional body movements produces activity in several brain networks, such as visual regions, the limbic network, and the AOS (de Gelder et al., 2004, 2010; Tamietto et al., 2007; Pichon et al., 2008; van de Riet et al., 2009; Meeren et al., 2013). There are several different neural mechanisms by which the human brain can identify and categorize observed affective displays. The capacity to recognize nonverbal affective communications generally relies on a core system that is likely to be located within the limbic system (LeDoux, 1996; Öhman and Mineka, 2001; Adolphs et al., 2003). However, our findings indicate that (limitedly to explicit processes) some subtypes of emotional body movements may be encoded as purposeful, goal-directed actions in the aIPS. Conversely, the pSTS, being the site of simple movement representation, does not seem to contain a specific representation of affective movements.
Dissociation between fear and happiness in the aIPS
State-dependent effects of TMS in aIPS were specific to fearful PLDs (Fig. 4). Why do fearful stimuli seem to be predominantly represented in the aIPS compared with happy stimuli? A possible explanation is that the affective state of fear itself is represented in the aIPS. Alternatively, it is possible the motor patterns expressing fear have characteristics that are best encoded by the aIPS, which preferentially processes goal-directed, purposeful movements (Cattaneo et al., 2010). The fearful bodily movements represented in our stimuli were in most cases directed toward a position in space as they depicted self-protective or avoidance body movements directed away from specific threatening agents (see example videos at http://community.dur.ac.uk/a.p.atkinson/Stimuli.html). On the contrary, happy stimuli (e.g., exulting, clapping hands, joyful hopping) were not directed toward or away from specific sectors in space. Therefore, the fear–happiness dissociation could be explained by a higher goal-directedness or space orientation in fearful movements compared with happy ones. From an evolutionary point of view, the emotional movements are communicative in nature, and our brain's prompt reactions to them is essential for the survival (Darwin, 1872; Ekman, 1957; Grèzes et al., 2007). In this sense, each emotional subtype has an own identity, and its affective state is not dissociable from its stereotyped communicative motor behavior. The effective communication of fearful content is more likely relied on goal-directed and spatially oriented actions than happiness. We therefore favor the hypothesis that fearful movements have a more “praxic” and “goal-directed” quality compared with happiness. In line with that, several studies have reported that the motor system is specifically tuned to fearful body movements as shown by changes in corticospinal excitability in response to fearful body postures (Borgomaneri et al., 2012, 2015), fearful facial expressions (Schutter et al., 2008), and negative natural complex scenes (Borgomaneri et al., 2014). However, the role of activity in the corticospinal system in action comprehension remains unclear.
Conclusions
We conclude that, while performing explicit categorizations (i.e., high-level cognitive task), the human brain considers fearful emotional body movements as goal-directed actions. This conclusion is supported by the specific recruitment of the cortical network that is specialized in processing actions. The AOS therefore contains representations of affective movements, as long as these are interpreted as finalistic, goal-directed, meaningful actions. On the contrary, the pSTS is known to encode BM according to its characteristic kinematic, distinguishing it from nonhuman motion, and is apparently not encoding specifically neither fearful nor happy bodily actions.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the ERC (336152) to J.S. and the F.R.S.-F.N.R.S. (“Charge de recherches”) to C.J. We thank Antony P. Atkinson (Durham University), Paola Ricciardelli, and Rossana Actis-Grosso (University of Milano-Bicocca) for sharing with us the stimuli; Birkbeck-University College of London Centre for NeuroImaging (BUCNI); and Christina Moutsiana, Benjamin de Haas, and Iroise Dumontheil for technical assistance during MRI scan acquisition.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Noemi Mazzoni, ODFLab, Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, Via Matteo del Ben 5b, 38068, Rovereto (TN) Italy. noemi.mazzoni{at}unitn.it