Skip to main content

Umbrella menu

  • SfN.org
  • eNeuro
  • The Journal of Neuroscience
  • Neuronline
  • BrainFacts.org

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Preparing a Manuscript
    • Submission Guidelines
    • Fees
    • Journal Club
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
  • SfN.org
  • eNeuro
  • The Journal of Neuroscience
  • Neuronline
  • BrainFacts.org

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • Subscribe
  • My alerts
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • Subscribe
  • My alerts
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Preparing a Manuscript
    • Submission Guidelines
    • Fees
    • Journal Club
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Time of Day Differences in Neural Reward Functioning in Healthy Young Men

Jamie E.M. Byrne, Matthew E. Hughes, Susan L. Rossell, Sheri L. Johnson and Greg Murray
Journal of Neuroscience 13 September 2017, 37 (37) 8895-8900; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0918-17.2017
Jamie E.M. Byrne
1Centre for Mental Health, Faculty Health, Arts and Design, Swinburne University, Hawthorn, Victoria 3122, Australia,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Jamie E.M. Byrne
Matthew E. Hughes
1Centre for Mental Health, Faculty Health, Arts and Design, Swinburne University, Hawthorn, Victoria 3122, Australia,
4The Australian National Imaging Facility, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Susan L. Rossell
1Centre for Mental Health, Faculty Health, Arts and Design, Swinburne University, Hawthorn, Victoria 3122, Australia,
2Monash Alfred Psychiatry Research Centre, Alfred and Central Clinical School, Monash University, Melbourne, 3004 Victoria, Australia,
3Psychiatry, St Vincent's Hospital, Fitzroy, Melbourne, 3065 Victoria, Australia,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sheri L. Johnson
5University of California, Berkeley, California 94720
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Greg Murray
1Centre for Mental Health, Faculty Health, Arts and Design, Swinburne University, Hawthorn, Victoria 3122, Australia,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Reward function appears to be modulated by the circadian system, but little is known about the neural basis of this interaction. Previous research suggests that the neural reward response may be different in the afternoon; however, the direction of this effect is contentious. Reward response may follow the diurnal rhythm in self-reported positive affect, peaking in the early afternoon. An alternative is that daily reward response represents a type of prediction error, with neural reward activation relatively high at times of day when rewards are unexpected (i.e., early and late in the day). The present study measured neural reward activation in the context of a validated reward task at 10.00 h, 14.00 h, and 19.00 h in healthy human males. A region of interest BOLD fMRI protocol was used to investigate the diurnal waveform of activation in reward-related brain regions. Multilevel modeling found, as expected, a highly significant quadratic time-of-day effect focusing on the left putamen (p < 0.001). Consistent with the “prediction error” hypothesis, activation was significantly higher at 10.00 h and 19.00 h compared with 14.00 h. It is provisionally concluded that the putamen may be particularly important in endogenous priming of reward motivation at different times of day, with the pattern of activation consistent with circadian-modulated reward expectancies in neural pathways (i.e., greater activation to reward stimuli at unexpected times of day). This study encourages further research into circadian modulation of reward and underscores the methodological importance of accounting for time of day in fMRI protocols.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT This is one of the first studies to use a repeated-measures imaging procedure to explore the diurnal rhythm of reward activation. Although self-reported reward (most often operationalized as positive affect) peaks in the afternoon, the present findings indicate that neural activation is lowest at this time. We conclude that the diurnal neural activation pattern may reflect a prediction error of the brain, where rewards at unexpected times (10.00 h and 19.00 h) elicit higher activation in reward brain regions than at expected (14.00 h) times. These data also have methodological significance, suggesting that there may be a time of day influence, which should be accounted for in neural reward studies.

  • circadian
  • diurnal
  • putamen
  • reward
  • sleep
  • striatum

Introduction

Reward function in animals and humans appears to be adaptively modulated by the circadian system (Murray et al., 2002, 2009). Theoretically, the human reward system is primed to be more active during daytime hours when reward potential is high and risk relatively low, and less active overnight when this balance is reversed due to poor night vision (Watson, 2000). Existing research has found a circadian rhythm in self-reported positive affect (the subjective manifestation of reward activation), peaking in the early afternoon and paralleling the circadian rhythm of core body temperature under naturalistic conditions (e.g., Boivin et al., 1997; Murray et al., 2009). It has been hypothesized that this circadian reward rhythm should also be measurable in reward neurocircuitry (e.g., Murray et al., 2009), but research to date is limited.

A small number of studies have taken the preliminary step of testing for a diurnal rhythm (a waveform across the waking day that may or may not be of endogenous circadian origin) in various measures of neural activation in humans. Hasler et al. (2014) used a within-subjects fMRI procedure to observe greater striatal activity to monetary rewards in the afternoon compared with morning. A recent study (Masterson et al., 2016) found a significant time-of-day effect on neural activation in the right ventral striatum and left putamen in response to visual food stimuli. Activation was higher in these reward regions during the morning (06.30–08.30 h) compared with the evening scan (17.30–19.30 h) (Masterson et al., 2016). Together, these studies indicate the presence of a neural diurnal rhythm in response to rewards; however, the timing of this rhythm is ambiguous and has been limited to two measurement points thus far.

The reliable finding of a mid-afternoon peak in positive affect (above) does not necessarily suggest that a circadian rhythm in neural reward activation would have the same waveform. It has been argued (Schultz et al., 1992; Schultz, 2002, 2016) that dopaminergic neurons innervate the terminal striatal regions with greater intensity when an environmental reward deviates from previous reward expectancies. Additional work (McClure et al., 2003) has demonstrated higher levels of striatal activation (with strongest lateralized activation in the left putamen) in response to unexpected reward stimuli. Other human fMRI studies have found this same “prediction error” in response to reward with a positive correlation between unexpected reward and ventral striatum activity (O'Doherty et al., 2003; Hare et al., 2008; Valentin and O'Doherty, 2009) and more dorsal striatal regions (Valentin and O'Doherty, 2009). In sum, a circadian rhythm in neural reward activation may be measurable as an elevated response to rewards at unexpected times of day: earlier and later in the day in humans (Watson, 2000; Murray et al., 2009), and inverted relative to the waveform observed in positive affect under naturalistic conditions.

The present study used an fMRI procedure to examine the diurnal rhythm of activation in reward neurocircuitry in response to a reward stimulus. Activity in the mPFC, VTA, anterior cingulate cortex, caudate, NAc, and putamen was examined. To model the diurnal rhythm, we advanced existing research by scanning participants at three times of day (10.00, 14.00, and 19.00 h). Reward activation was hypothesized to vary with a quadratic waveform, consistent with an underlying circadian driver: that is, with 14.00 h having an altered neural reward level relative to 10.00 h and 19.00 h. Based on the “prediction error” assumption, higher levels of neural activation were predicted earlier in the day (10.00 h) and later in the day (19.00 h); however, the fitted sine curve was also modeled with a peak in the quadratic waveform at 14.00 h to align with Hasler et al. (2014) whose data indicate that neural reward activation may mirror the demonstrated rhythm in self-reported positive affect, peaking later in the day relative to earlier.

Materials and Methods

Participants.

Participants were 16 right-handed males (mean ± SD, 22.65 ± 2.87 years) screened to exclude previous and current mental illness, shift work, and transmeridian travel within 3 months of participation.

fMRI task.

The gambling reward procedure (Delgado et al., 2000) from the Human Connectome Project is a pseudo-reward task, which involves guessing the value of a card (1–9). The trial begins with a question mark displayed on the screen for 1500 ms with responses recorded on a response box. A white fixation cross is presented if a response is made before 1500 ms with feedback for 1000 ms. Cards are predetermined so that 40% of trials are rewarding ($1, green up arrow), 40% loss (−50 cents, red down arrow), and 20% neutral trials (−, double-headed gray arrow). Nonresponses are presented with a screen stating that response was too slow.

In the present study, eight trials were presented in four blocks, of which two were mostly reward (Reward Block = six reward trials, and two neutral or loss trials) and two mostly loss trials (Loss Block = six loss trials, and two neutral or reward trials). Following each block of eight trials, there was a 15 s fixation cross, which was used as the Baseline comparison. The data were acquired during four scanning runs (each consisting of four blocks taking 3 min: 12 s per run) at each time point with a short break between runs. Before the first run at each time point, a practice run acclimatized participants to the task. To increase task motivation across sessions, participants were informed that they would receive additional reimbursement for their best task performance across the three sessions. Because of the predetermined values, participants received $27 AUD.

MRI data acquisition.

Structural and functional images were acquired with a 3T Siemens TIM Trio MRI scanner at Swinburne University of Technology (Hawthorn, Victoria, Australia). During fMRI scanning, visual stimuli were presented on a rear projection screen viewed by participants through a mirror attached to the 32-channel head coil. All aspects of stimulus delivery and response logging were performed using E-Prime 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools 2002) for Windows.

Each scanning session began with the acquisition of a high-resolution T1-weighted scan using an MP-RAGE sequence (192 sagittal slices; 1 mm isotropic voxels; flip angle 9°; FOV = 256 × 192 mm; TR = 2200 ms; TE = 3.29 ms; matrix = 256 × 192). During each of the four fMRI task scanning runs in each session, 73 T2*-weighted images were acquired using a gradient echo EPI sequence (39 interleaved axial slices; 3 mm isotropic voxels; flip angle 90°; FOV = 205 mm, TR = 2000 ms TE = 25 ms; matrix = 64 × 64).

MRI data preprocessing.

All aspects of MRI image preprocessing and statistical analysis were conducted using SPM12 (Ashburner et al., 2014; Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London) and associated toolboxes. Initially, the high-resolution structural image and functional time-series were manually realigned to closely match the MNI template in SPM12. Subsequently, highly variant EPI slices were corrected using an interpolation algorithm in ArtRepair tools (version 5b) (Mazaika et al., 2009). These artifact-corrected images were slice time corrected using the middle slice acquired in time as a reference, then realigned to the first EPI acquired. The realigned images were then coregistered to the T1 image, which was then transformed (normalized) into MNI space. The parameters of this transformation were applied to coregistered EPIs, which were then smoothed with a 6 mm FWHM Gaussian filter and high-pass filtered (<128 s). Finally, Artifact Detection Tools (Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., 2011) were used to determine outlying images, defined as any image ±3 SDs from mean signal intensity of the time-series, or images exhibiting >1.5 mm of movement from the preceding image.

Statistical analysis of fMRI data.

Participant level modeling was performed using an epoch-based general linear model in SPM12. The BOLD signal for Reward and Loss blocks was modeled using a boxcar function defined by the onset and duration of each block convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function supplied with SPM12. The periods of fixation cross presentation constituted the Baseline; but as is common practice in fMRI analyses, these blocks were not explicitly modeled as this leads to the model being overdetermined. Additionally, regressors of no interest were modeled, including one regressor for each motion realignment parameter (3 translational, 3 rotational) and one regressor for each outlying image determined using Artifact Detection Tools (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/artifact_detect/); <10% of images for each participant. After model estimation, the contrasts of Reward > Baseline, Reward > Loss, and Loss > Baseline were computed and then entered into a second-level, random-effects, repeated-measures ANOVA model with factor Time of Day (10.00, 14.00, and 19.00 h). We then examined the data for a main effect of Time of Day by using an uncorrected voxel level threshold of p < 0.001. Given the a priori reward regions of interest (ROIs), we used a small volume correction at the cluster level (Worsley et al., 1996).

Procedure.

Participants maintained sleep and daily activity diaries and wore an actigraph in the week before the study to test for sleep-related variables on the testing day. To account for repeated-measures confounds, participants' start times were counterbalanced, with all testing completed within 24 h. Scan time was 1 h for the first session (with structural scans completed) and 30 min for the second and third sessions. The task was run in E-Prime 2.0, with a BOLD signal fMRI time-series used to acquire images for the voxels within each ROI.

Results

A main effect was observed in the ventral portion of the left putamen (MNI coordinates of peak voxel: −28, 4, −2, peak F value: 13.97, cluster size: 23 voxels; for the Reward > Baseline contrasts, see Fig. 1). This effect was observed in a similar location in the Reward > Loss contrast (MNI coordinates of peak voxel: −28, 10, −8, peak F value: 11.51, cluster size: 4 voxels). In the bilateral caudate, a small cluster was observed; however, it did not survive cluster level correction (left caudate 1 voxel, F = 6.11, p = 0.61, MNI coordinates: −12, 14, 14; right caudate 4 voxels, F = 6.34, p = 0.49, MNI coordinates: 14, 16, 10). Even at a more liberal voxelwise threshold (p < 0.05, uncorrected) bilaterally, the mPFC, VTA, anterior cingulate cortex, NAc, and the right putamen were all nonsignificant to different activation at time of day. No clusters in the a priori reward regions showed a time-of-day effect for the Loss > Baseline contrasts.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

BOLD contrast of Reward > Baseline with a repeated-measures Time of Day factor entered into the model. A, Activation of left putamen significant (p < 0.001) for a Time-of-day effect. B, Activation of left putamen significantly decreased at 14.00 h, compared with 10.00 h or 19.00 h. Error bars represent SEM.

A volume of interest (sphere, 2 mm radius) centered on the peak voxel of this cluster was constructed, and the first eigenvariate extracted from the modeled contrast images. These data were entered into multilevel modeling that tested the quadratic waveform fit of the repeated-measures Level 1 variable (Time of Day) with a nadir at 14.00 h. An intercept only model was conducted for the left putamen voxel cluster. The Level 1 Model (Left putamen activationij = β0j + β1j (time of day) + rij) tested the time-of-day effect, with group mean centering performed before model inclusion. β0j represents each participant's neural activation in the significant voxel, and rij represents the within-person variance. β1j represents the time of day slope of the fitted quadratic waveform with a nadir at 14.00 h for each participant, and no difference modeled between 10.00 and 19.00 h. The analyses dummy coded this as 1 (10.00 h), −2 (14.00 h), and 1 (19.00 h). The quadratic waveform provided a highly significant fit to the data (p < 0.001), with activation in the left putamen being significantly lower at 14.00 h than 10.00 h or 19.00 h.

For completeness, a whole-brain analysis was performed. Using an arbitrary clusterwise threshold of 10 contiguous voxels, a cluster in the left insula (in the posterior region; 18 voxels, MNI coordinates: −32, −30, 20) and the middle frontal gyrus (in the anterior region; 26 voxels, MNI coordinates: −32, 52, 6) was found for the reward > baseline analyses; these effects were all nonsignificant (p values >0.5). No other significant time-of-day threshold voxel clusters were observed, even when a liberal (p < 0.05, uncorrected) threshold was applied. There was no iteration effect of repeated-measures testing of the neural reward rhythm on putamen activation.

Discussion

This study investigated the rhythm of neural activation to rewards across the course of the waking day. The hypothesis that activation of reward circuitry would be lowest at 14.00 h compared with 10.00 h and 19.00 h gained preliminary support, with a significant waveform fit found in left putamen activation in the context of a validated reward task. Other reward ROIs did not show a significant time-of-day effect.

Left putamen exhibits diurnal changes

Left putamen activation exhibited a diurnal waveform with relatively decreased activation in the early afternoon. Existing literature suggests that the putamen is a core component of reward-related function in humans (O'Doherty et al., 2003), rodents (Gallardo et al., 2014), and monkeys (Muranishi et al., 2011). Muranishi et al. (2011) demonstrated that pharmacological inhibition of the left putamen in monkeys impaired reward-based decision making. Furthermore, Szczypka et al. (2001) found that sucrose preference in dopamine-deficient mice was restored with supplanted dopamine in the caudate-putamen or nucleus accumbens; however, dopamine replacement only in the caudate-putamen restored feeding behavior, suggesting that the putamen is core to the neural reward circuitry and has specified reward functions.

This study provides preliminary evidence of the importance of the putamen in understanding the putative interaction between circadian and reward function. In animal studies, the putamen has been innervated by the endogenous circadian system in reward functions, including the following: food anticipation (Gallardo et al., 2014), the circadian locomotor rhythm (Masubuchi et al., 2000), and in circadian gene expression following methamphetamine injection in rodents (Nikaido et al., 2001). Similarly, two earlier human diurnal imaging studies have found time-of-day variation in left putamen activation (Hasler et al., 2012b; Masterson et al., 2016). Given the interconnectedness of neural reward pathways and broader striatum region (Haber and Knutson, 2010), more work is now needed to investigate diurnal variation in other reward-related regions, including the mPFC, ventral striatum caudate, and anterior cingulate cortex. The present results suggest that these reward regions do not exhibit time-of-day effects, but larger samples and alternative imaging methods (discussed below) may detect additional signals of circadian modulation.

Putamen activation to reward is lowest in the early afternoon

Prior studies have found that positive affect, a subjective manifestation of reward activation, is highest in the mid-afternoon (Clark et al., 1989; Watson et al., 1999; Murray et al., 2009). This finding has been interpreted as indexing an adaptive preparedness to pursue rewards when environmental conditions are optimal (Wehr, 1990). In the present neuroimaging study, by contrast, neural reward circuitry was relatively low in the mid-afternoon: The diurnal waveform in the left putamen reward region had its nadir at 14.00 h.

As noted above, we propose that this pattern of findings can be understood as a type of prediction error. Specifically, we propose that rewards presented at 14.00 are expected (by circadian priming), and thus lack the novelty of rewards appearing at 10.00 h or 19.00 h. Consistent with this explanation, we note that a bigger hemodynamic response to unexpected reward has previously been observed in the left putamen compared with expected rewards (McClure et al., 2003; O'Doherty et al., 2003). When the brain expects rewards to be in abundance, then reward accrual elicits less neural excitation (Schultz et al., 1992; Schultz, 2016). Schultz et al. (1997) proposed that optimal reward functioning is contingent upon an organism's prior conditioning that predicts the timing and magnitude of rewarding events. Here, we extend this contention to the 24 h time frame by suggesting that the circadian system is the primary endogenous mechanism that conditions individuals to anticipate reward at different times of day.

An intriguing extension to these findings is that the neural reward response to reward stimuli may have an inverted diurnal waveform to self-reported ratings of positive affect. Multiple ambulatory (Clark et al., 1989; Watson et al., 1999; Stone et al., 2006; Miller et al., 2015) and circadian (Boivin et al., 1997; Murray et al., 2002, 2009) studies have found a peak in self-reported positive affect in the afternoon hours. Two explanations for the finding of lowered neural intensity in reward regions at times typically associated with higher self-reported positive affect warrant consideration. First, Masterson et al. (2016) found decreased activation in reward regions in response to food stimuli from 17.30 to 19.30 in the evening (as opposed to 0630–0830 in the morning), whereas self-reported interest in food and hunger was greater in the evening. The authors conclude that the dampened neural sensitivity to food stimuli in the evening may instigate a greater behavioral drive for food to obtain the same reward levels as observed in the morning. The type of reward stimuli is an important consideration, with Masterson et al. (2016) using food and our study and previous work in diurnal rhythms (e.g., Hasler et al., 2014) using monetary rewards. Neural reward activity may exhibit different daily activation patterns depending on stimuli used, and future work should attempt to investigate the role of diurnal rhythms across various reward stimuli.

Second, the observed decrease in neural activation of the putamen may be explained by a methodological limitation of BOLD fMRI imaging. The associated energy demands that the BOLD response is capturing are not sensitive to differences between excitation and inhibition of neuronal activity (Nair, 2005). Higher activation at unexpected times may indeed be capturing greater inhibition of reward regions to monetary incentives in the putamen. Future studies should monitor self-reported positive affect while collecting repeated-measures neural data to test this important proposition that self-report positive affect and neural activation in response to rewards may be inverted. Complex relationships between neural, subjective, and behavioral measures of reward function have been reliably documented (Berridge et al., 2009), and more research is required to understand this interplay in the diurnal/circadian context.

Although there are strong reasons to expect neural reward activation to be lowest in the afternoon hours, it is important to note that the present results conflict with the findings of Hasler et al. (2014). Several methodological differences should be noted between the two studies. First, whereas we found an effect in the dorsal striatum (left putamen), Hasler et al. (2014) found a time-of-day effect in the ventral portion of the striatum. Although both regions are associated with reward neurocircuitry, we do not have enough literature to assert whether reward regions exhibit similar diurnal neural rhythms. Second, the study by Hasler et al. (2014) had a single PM time point (15.06–18.38 h) sitting between our afternoon (14.00 h) and evening (19.00 h) scans. Third, unlike the present design, Hasler et al. (2014) experimentally controlled for individual sleep and wake times; a post hoc investigation revealed no significant effect of sleep variables on the diurnal neural waveform found here. Last, although the fMRI task was similar in both studies, the present design used more trials and runs and used a nonmotor Baseline, whereas Hasler et al. (2014) used a button-pressing control condition. More broadly, neuroimaging data analysis has been notoriously difficult to replicate particularly for less established or weaker findings (Poldrack and Poline, 2015; Poldrack et al., 2017). In sum, multiple methodological differences may partially explain the difference in findings here versus the sole related study, and we propose that this discrepancy should motivate more systematic and intensive research. Additionally, we agree with Hasler et al. (2014) that event-related designs, which allow for distinguishing between anticipation and consumption of rewards, may help to extend our understanding of the neural reward rhythm.

Limitations, clinical application, and future research

Although this study provides an important advance to our understanding of the diurnal rhythm of neural reward circuitry, several limitations should be noted. The primary limitation of this study was that, whereas the ultimate framework for this project is circadian, the data collected only speak to a diurnal rhythm, the endogeneity of which is unknown. To confirm the endogeneity of this rhythm, future research should examine whether the timing of this reward rhythm is generated from internal cues rather than external learned associations of the rewarding potential of a certain time of day. Although this would traditionally be done through constant routine or forced desynchrony circadian rhythm protocols, practical constraints around mobility of sleep laboratories and imaging equipment do not allow for this. Novel approaches are now required to consider how time-free environments could be created to test for a circadian relationship. Future work should consider using more testing sessions to examine more precise waveform characteristics over the waking day, similarly extending these findings to a larger sample, women, and a wider age range will help generalize the present results.

Understanding the role of the circadian system in modulating the neural reward response has potential clinical implications. Abnormalities of circadian reward functioning have been noted in individuals experiencing mood disturbance. For example, in studies of diurnal mood variation, individuals with depressive symptoms (Gordijn et al., 1994; Murray, 2007) exhibit lower variability in daily positive mood, and altered waveform patterns. Von Zerssen et al. (1987) found, for example, lower mood for clinically depressed individuals in the morning, whereas for healthy matched controls it was lowest in the subjective night when sleep was interrupted to take measurements. Beyond circadian phase, recent work suggests that individuals with depression and bipolar disorder may have a decreased circadian amplitude in positive mood variation (Murray, 2007; Grierson et al., 2016). Disruptions to both the phase and amplitude of circadian rhythms have long been hypothesized to contribute to the pathogenesis and maintenance of mood disorders (Czeisler et al., 1987).

These preliminary findings raise different avenues for future research. In the present study, task-based fMRI was used to examine neural reward activity in the context of rewarding stimuli. Alternative imaging methods, such as arterial spin labeling, may better capture hourly temporal changes as a more sensitive measure of the diurnal changes in regional brain function intensity (Hermes et al., 2007; Goel et al., 2013; Mikita et al., 2015). Future research should endeavor to investigate whether the known mid-afternoon circadian dip in alertness may be relevant to interpreting the neural signal in reward activation. Methodologically, the present findings speak to the necessity of controlling for time of day when performing neuroimaging studies. The diurnal rhythm in reward functioning observed here raises questions about findings from neuroimaging protocols that have neither controlled for nor reported time of day.

Within its limitations, this study is among the first to examine variation in human neural reward functioning in relation to time of day. This preliminary evidence suggests that there is a diurnal rhythm in left putamen activation, part of the neural circuitry involved in reward. It extends the small collection of studies that have looked at this relationship by using three time points to better capture the shape of the diurnal neural reward rhythm in the context of reward stimuli. This work is a first step in testing the chronobiological hypothesis of a circadian reward rhythm, adding to a burgeoning imaging literature interested in how the circadian system regulates reward circuitry (Forbes et al., 2012; Hasler et al., 2012a, 2014; Byrne and Murray, 2017). This novel finding also underscores the importance of future research between how vulnerability to, or experience of mental illnesses known to affect reward and circadian pathways may differ from healthy individuals, which can facilitate targeted clinical intervention. These insights provide a foundation for understanding that diurnal neural reward rhythms exist in healthy individuals.

Note added in Proof: The contributions of one of the authors, Sheri L. Johnson, was accidentally left out of the early release version, published August 21, 2017. The author contribution line has now been corrected.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the Barbara Dicker Brain Sciences Foundation

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Professor Greg Murray, Centre for Mental Health, Faculty Health, Arts and Design, Swinburne University of Technology, John Street, Hawthorn, VIC 3122, Australia. gwm{at}swin.edu.au

References

  1. ↵
    1. Ashburner J,
    2. Barnes G,
    3. Chen C,
    4. Daunizeau J,
    5. Flandin G,
    6. Friston K,
    7. Kiebel S,
    8. Kilner J,
    9. Litvak V,
    10. Moran R
    (2014) SPM12 manual. London: Wellcome Trust.
  2. ↵
    1. Berridge KC,
    2. Robinson TE,
    3. Aldridge JW
    (2009) Dissecting components of reward: ‘liking,’ ‘wanting,’ and learning. Curr Opin Pharmacol 9:65–73. doi:10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.014 pmid:19162544
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Boivin DB,
    2. Czeisler CA,
    3. Dijk DJ,
    4. Duffy JF,
    5. Folkard S,
    6. Minors DS,
    7. Totterdell P,
    8. Waterhouse JM
    (1997) Complex interaction of the sleep-wake cycle and circadian phase modulates mood in healthy subjects. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54:145–152. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830140055010 pmid:9040282
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Byrne JE,
    2. Murray G
    (2017) Diurnal rhythms in psychological reward functioning in healthy young men: ‘wanting,’ liking and learning. Chronobiol Int 34:287–295. doi:10.1080/07420528.2016.1272607 pmid:28060525
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Clark LA,
    2. Watson D,
    3. Leeka J
    (1989) Diurnal variation in the positive affects. Motivation Emotion 13:205–234. doi:10.1007/BF00995536
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  6. ↵
    1. Czeisler CA,
    2. Kronauer RE,
    3. Mooney JJ,
    4. Anderson JL,
    5. Allan JS
    (1987) Biologic rhythm disorders, depression, and phototherapy: a new hypothesis. Psychiatr Clin North Am 10:687–709. pmid:3332326
    OpenUrlPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Delgado MR,
    2. Nystrom LE,
    3. Fissell C,
    4. Noll DC,
    5. Fiez JA
    (2000) Tracking the hemodynamic responses to reward and punishment in the striatum. J Neurophysiol 84:3072–3077. pmid:11110834
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Forbes EE,
    2. Dahl RE,
    3. Almeida JR,
    4. Ferrell RE,
    5. Nimgaonkar VL,
    6. Mansour H,
    7. Sciarrillo SR,
    8. Holm SM,
    9. Rodriguez EE,
    10. Phillips ML
    (2012) PER2 rs2304672 polymorphism moderates circadian-relevant reward circuitry activity in adolescents. Biol Psychiatry 71:451–457. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.012 pmid:22137505
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Gallardo CM,
    2. Darvas M,
    3. Oviatt M,
    4. Chang CH,
    5. Michalik M,
    6. Huddy TF,
    7. Meyer EE,
    8. Shuster SA,
    9. Aguayo A,
    10. Hill EM,
    11. Kiani K,
    12. Ikpeazu J,
    13. Martinez JS,
    14. Purpura M,
    15. Smit AN,
    16. Patton DF,
    17. Mistlberger RE,
    18. Palmiter RD,
    19. Steele AD
    (2014) Dopamine receptor 1 neurons in the dorsal striatum regulate food anticipatory circadian activity rhythms in mice. eLife 3:e03781. doi:10.7554/eLife.03781 pmid:25217530
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Goel N,
    2. Basner M,
    3. Rao H,
    4. Dinges DF
    (2013) Circadian rhythms, sleep deprivation, and human performance. Prog Mol Biol Transl Sci 119:155–190. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-396971-2.00007-5 pmid:23899598
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Gordijn MC,
    2. Beersma DG,
    3. Bouhuys AL,
    4. Reinink E,
    5. Van den Hoofdakker RH
    (1994) A longitudinal study of diurnal mood variation in depression; characteristics and significance. J Affect Disord 31:261–273. doi:10.1016/0165-0327(94)90102-3 pmid:7989641
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Grierson AB,
    2. Hickie IB,
    3. Naismith SL,
    4. Hermens DF,
    5. Scott EM,
    6. Scott J
    (2016) Circadian rhythmicity in emerging mood disorders: state or trait marker? Int J Bipolar Disord 4:1–7. doi:10.1186/s40345-015-0043-z pmid:26763505
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Haber SN,
    2. Knutson B
    (2010) The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 35:4–26. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.129 pmid:19812543
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Hare TA,
    2. O'Doherty J,
    3. Camerer CF,
    4. Schultz W,
    5. Rangel A
    (2008) Dissociating the role of the orbitofrontal cortex and the striatum in the computation of goal values and prediction errors. J Neurosci 28:5623–5630. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1309-08.2008 pmid:18509023
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Hasler BP,
    2. Dahl RE,
    3. Holm SM,
    4. Jakubcak JL,
    5. Ryan ND,
    6. Silk JS,
    7. Phillips ML,
    8. Forbes EE
    (2012a) Weekend-weekday advances in sleep timing are associated with altered reward-related brain function in healthy adolescents. Biol Psychol 91:334–341. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.08.008 pmid:22960270
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Hasler BP,
    2. Germain A,
    3. Nofzinger EA,
    4. Kupfer DJ,
    5. Krafty RT,
    6. Rothenberger SD,
    7. James JA,
    8. Bi W,
    9. Buysse DJ
    (2012b) Chronotype and diurnal patterns of positive affect and affective neural circuitry in primary insomnia. J Sleep Res 21:515–526. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2012.01002.x pmid:22369504
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Hasler BP,
    2. Forbes EE,
    3. Franzen PL
    (2014) Time-of-day differences and short-term stability of the neural response to monetary reward: a pilot study. Psychiatry Res 224:22–27. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.07.005 pmid:25092525
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Hermes M,
    2. Hagemann D,
    3. Britz P,
    4. Lieser S,
    5. Rock J,
    6. Naumann E,
    7. Walter C
    (2007) Reproducibility of continuous arterial spin labeling perfusion MRI after 7 weeks. MAGMA 20:103–115. doi:10.1007/s10334-007-0073-3 pmid:17429703
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Masterson TD,
    2. Kirwan CB,
    3. Davidson LE,
    4. LeCheminant JD
    (2016) Neural reactivity to visual food stimuli is reduced in some areas of the brain during evening hours compared to morning hours: an fMRI study in women. Brain Imaging Behav 10:68–78. doi:10.1007/s11682-015-9366-8 pmid:25753781
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Masubuchi S,
    2. Honma S,
    3. Abe H,
    4. Ishizaki K,
    5. Namihira M,
    6. Ikeda M,
    7. Honma K
    (2000) Clock genes outside the suprachiasmatic nucleus involved in manifestation of locomotor activity rhythm in rats. Eur J Neurosci 12:4206–4214. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2000.01313.x pmid:11122332
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Mazaika PK,
    2. Hoeft F,
    3. Glover GH,
    4. Reiss AL
    (2009) Methods and software for fMRI analysis of clinical subjects. Neuroimage 47:S58.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  22. ↵
    1. McClure SM,
    2. Berns GS,
    3. Montague PR
    (2003) Temporal prediction errors in a passive learning task activate human striatum. Neuron 38:339–346. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00154-5 pmid:12718866
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Mikita N,
    2. Mehta MA,
    3. Zelaya FO,
    4. Stringaris A
    (2015) Using arterial spin labeling to examine mood states in youth. Brain Behav 5:e00339. doi:10.1002/brb3.339 pmid:26085964
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Miller MA,
    2. Rothenberger SD,
    3. Hasler BP,
    4. Donofry SD,
    5. Wong PM,
    6. Manuck SB,
    7. Kamarck TW,
    8. Roecklein KA
    (2015) Chronotype predicts positive affect rhythms measured by ecological momentary assessment. Chronobiol Int 32:376–384. doi:10.3109/07420528.2014.983602 pmid:25410882
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Muranishi M,
    2. Inokawa H,
    3. Yamada H,
    4. Ueda Y,
    5. Matsumoto N,
    6. Nakagawa M,
    7. Kimura M
    (2011) Inactivation of the putamen selectively impairs reward history-based action selection. Exp Brain Res 209:235–246. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2545-y pmid:21298425
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Murray G
    (2007) Diurnal mood variation in depression: a signal of disturbed circadian function? J Affect Disord 102:47–53. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2006.12.001 pmid:17239958
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Murray G,
    2. Allen NB,
    3. Trinder J
    (2002) Mood and the circadian system: investigation of a circadian component in positive affect. Chronobiol Int 19:1151–1169. doi:10.1081/CBI-120015956 pmid:12511032
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Murray G,
    2. Nicholas CL,
    3. Kleiman J,
    4. Dwyer R,
    5. Carrington MJ,
    6. Allen NB,
    7. Trinder J
    (2009) Nature's clocks and human mood: the circadian system modulates reward motivation. Emotion 9:705–716. doi:10.1037/a0017080 pmid:19803592
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Nair DG
    (2005) About being BOLD. Brain Res Rev 50:229–243. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.07.001 pmid:16213027
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Nikaido T,
    2. Akiyama M,
    3. Moriya T,
    4. Shibata S
    (2001) Sensitized increase of period gene expression in the mouse caudate/putamen caused by repeated injection of methamphetamine. Mol Pharmacol 59:894–900. doi:10.1124/mol.59.4.894 pmid:11259635
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    1. O'Doherty JP,
    2. Dayan P,
    3. Friston K,
    4. Critchley H,
    5. Dolan RJ
    (2003) Temporal difference models and reward-related learning in the human brain. Neuron 38:329–337. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00169-7 pmid:12718865
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Poldrack RA,
    2. Poline JB
    (2015) The publication and reproducibility challenges of shared data. Trends Cogn Sci 19:59–61. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.11.008 pmid:25532702
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Poldrack RA,
    2. Baker CI,
    3. Durnez J,
    4. Gorgolewski KJ,
    5. Matthews PM,
    6. Munafò MR,
    7. Nichols TE,
    8. Poline JB,
    9. Vul E,
    10. Yarkoni T
    (2017) Scanning the horizon: towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research. Nat Rev Neurosci 18:115–126. doi:10.1038/nrn.2016.167 pmid:28053326
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    Psychology Software Tools (2002) E-Prime (version 2.0) [computer software]. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools.
  35. ↵
    1. Schultz W
    (2002) Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron 36:241–263. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00967-4 pmid:12383780
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Schultz W
    (2016) Dopamine reward prediction error coding. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 18:23–32. pmid:27069377
    OpenUrlPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Schultz W,
    2. Apicella P,
    3. Scarnati E,
    4. Ljungberg T
    (1992) Neuronal activity in monkey ventral striatum related to the expectation of reward. J Neurosci 12:4595–4610. pmid:1464759
    OpenUrlAbstract
  38. ↵
    1. Schultz W,
    2. Dayan P,
    3. Montague PR
    (1997) A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275:1593–1599. doi:10.1126/science.275.5306.1593 pmid:9054347
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. ↵
    1. Stone AA,
    2. Schwartz JE,
    3. Schkade D,
    4. Schwarz N,
    5. Krueger A,
    6. Kahneman D
    (2006) A population approach to the study of emotion: diurnal rhythms of a working day examined with the Day Reconstruction Method. Emotion 6:139–149. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.139 pmid:16637757
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Szczypka MS,
    2. Kwok K,
    3. Brot MD,
    4. Marck BT,
    5. Matsumoto AM,
    6. Donahue BA,
    7. Palmiter RD
    (2001) Dopamine production in the caudate putamen restores feeding in dopamine-deficient mice. Neuron 30:819–828. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00319-1 pmid:11430814
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Valentin VV,
    2. O'Doherty JP
    (2009) Overlapping prediction errors in dorsal striatum during instrumental learning with juice and money reward in the human brain. J Neurophysiol 102:3384–3391. doi:10.1152/jn.91195.2008 pmid:19793875
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  42. ↵
    1. von Zerssen D,
    2. Doerr P,
    3. Emrich HM,
    4. Lund R,
    5. Pirke KM
    (1987) Diurnal variation of mood and the cortisol rhythm in depression and normal states of mind. Eur Arch Psychiatr Neurol Sci 237:36–45. doi:10.1007/BF00385665 pmid:3428315
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Watson D
    (2000) Mood and temperament. New York: Guilford.
  44. ↵
    1. Watson D,
    2. Wiese D,
    3. Vaidya J,
    4. Tellegen A
    (1999) The two general activation systems of affect: structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. J Pers Soc Psychol 76:820–838. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.5.820
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  45. ↵
    1. Wehr TA
    (1990) Reply to-Healy D. and Waterhouse J.M.: the circadian system and affective disorders: clocks or rhythms? Chronobiol Int 7:11–14. doi:10.3109/07420529009056948
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  46. ↵
    1. Whitfield-Gabrieli S,
    2. Nieto-Castanon A,
    3. Ghosh S
    (2011) Artifact detection tools (ART), Release version 7:11. Cambridge, MA: Artifact Detection Tools.
  47. ↵
    1. Worsley KJ,
    2. Marrett S,
    3. Neelin P,
    4. Vandal AC,
    5. Friston KJ,
    6. Evans AC
    (1996) A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation. Hum Brain Mapp 4:58–73. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1996)4:1%3C58::AID-HBM4%3E3.0.CO;2-O pmid:20408186
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 37 (37)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 37, Issue 37
13 Sep 2017
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Time of Day Differences in Neural Reward Functioning in Healthy Young Men
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Article Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Citation Tools
Time of Day Differences in Neural Reward Functioning in Healthy Young Men
Jamie E.M. Byrne, Matthew E. Hughes, Susan L. Rossell, Sheri L. Johnson, Greg Murray
Journal of Neuroscience 13 September 2017, 37 (37) 8895-8900; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0918-17.2017

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Time of Day Differences in Neural Reward Functioning in Healthy Young Men
Jamie E.M. Byrne, Matthew E. Hughes, Susan L. Rossell, Sheri L. Johnson, Greg Murray
Journal of Neuroscience 13 September 2017, 37 (37) 8895-8900; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0918-17.2017
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • circadian
  • diurnal
  • putamen
  • reward
  • sleep
  • striatum

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

  • RE: Time of Day Differences in Neural Reward Functioning in Healthy Young Men
    GREG MURRAY, CATHERINE ORR, JAMIE E M BYRNE, MATTHEW E HUGHES, SUSAN L ROSSELL and SHERI L JOHNSON
    Published on: 29 November 2018
  • RE: Effect of time of day on reward circuitry.
    Adam Steel, Cibu Thomas and Chris Baker
    Published on: 05 February 2018
  • Published on: (29 November 2018)
    RE: Time of Day Differences in Neural Reward Functioning in Healthy Young Men
    • GREG MURRAY, Academic, Swinburne University
    • Other Contributors:
      • CATHERINE ORR
      • JAMIE E M BYRNE
      • MATTHEW E HUGHES
      • SUSAN L ROSSELL
      • SHERI L JOHNSON

    The interplay between circadian and reward function is well understood in animal models, and it is of growing interest as an aetiological explanation in psychopathologies. Circadian modulation of reward function has been demonstrated in human behavioural data, but understanding at the neural level is limited. In 2017, our group published results of a first step in addressing this deficit, demonstrating a diurnal rhythm in fMRI-measured reward activation. In 2018, Steel et al. wrote a constructive critique of our findings, and the aim of this paper is to outline how future research could improve on our first proof-of-concept study. Key challenges include addressing divergent and convergent validity (by addressing non-reward neural variation, and testing for absence of variation in threat-related pathways), preregistration and power analysis to protect against false positives, wider range of fMRI methods (to directly test our post-hoc hypothesis of some form of reward prediction error, and multiple phases of reward), the parallel collection of behavioural data (particularly self-reported positive affect, and actigraphically measured activity) to illuminate the nature of the reward activation across the day, and some attempt to parse out circadian versus homeostatic/masking influences on any observed diurnal rhythm in neural reward activation. We have outlined these issues in a commentary available on the arXiv using the following link:...

    Show More

    The interplay between circadian and reward function is well understood in animal models, and it is of growing interest as an aetiological explanation in psychopathologies. Circadian modulation of reward function has been demonstrated in human behavioural data, but understanding at the neural level is limited. In 2017, our group published results of a first step in addressing this deficit, demonstrating a diurnal rhythm in fMRI-measured reward activation. In 2018, Steel et al. wrote a constructive critique of our findings, and the aim of this paper is to outline how future research could improve on our first proof-of-concept study. Key challenges include addressing divergent and convergent validity (by addressing non-reward neural variation, and testing for absence of variation in threat-related pathways), preregistration and power analysis to protect against false positives, wider range of fMRI methods (to directly test our post-hoc hypothesis of some form of reward prediction error, and multiple phases of reward), the parallel collection of behavioural data (particularly self-reported positive affect, and actigraphically measured activity) to illuminate the nature of the reward activation across the day, and some attempt to parse out circadian versus homeostatic/masking influences on any observed diurnal rhythm in neural reward activation. We have outlined these issues in a commentary available on the arXiv using the following link: http://arxiv.org/abs/1811.12245

    Show Less
    Competing Interests: None declared.
  • Published on: (5 February 2018)
    RE: Effect of time of day on reward circuitry.
    • Adam Steel, DPhil Candidate, NIMH
    • Other Contributors:
      • Cibu Thomas
      • Chris Baker

    Byrne and colleagues present a paper on a timely topic with potentially important results. However, we think that issues in the design and analysis complicate the interpretation and limit the generalizability of the findings. Specifically, the details of the small volume correction used in the primary analysis are not adequately described and, moreover, the results do not appear to be corroborated by the whole-brain analysis. In addition, the follow-up multilevel modeling, which is fundamental to the conclusions of the paper, is inherently circular thereby guaranteeing discovery of the reported effect. Finally, the study does not control for other factors that vary over the course of the day and are known to impact MRI measurements, and fails to link the neural results directly to any relevant behavior.

    We have outlined these issues in detail in a commentary available on the arXiv using the following link: http://arxiv.org/abs/1802.00753.

    Competing Interests: None declared.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • The phosphoprotein Synapsin Ia regulates the kinetics of dense-core vesicle release
  • Tuba Activates Cdc42 during Neuronal Polarization Downstream of the Small GTPase Rab8a
  • Frontotemporal Regulation of Subjective Value to Suppress Impulsivity in Intertemporal Choices
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • The phosphoprotein Synapsin Ia regulates the kinetics of dense-core vesicle release
  • Tuba Activates Cdc42 during Neuronal Polarization Downstream of the Small GTPase Rab8a
  • Frontotemporal Regulation of Subjective Value to Suppress Impulsivity in Intertemporal Choices
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2021 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.