Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Drosophila Neprilysin 1 Rescues Memory Deficits Caused by Amyloid-β Peptide

Oriane Turrel, Valérie Goguel and Thomas Preat
Journal of Neuroscience 25 October 2017, 37 (43) 10334-10345; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1634-17.2017
Oriane Turrel
Genes and Dynamics of Memory Systems, Brain Plasticity Unit, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, ESPCI Paris, PSL Research University, 75005 Paris, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Valérie Goguel
Genes and Dynamics of Memory Systems, Brain Plasticity Unit, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, ESPCI Paris, PSL Research University, 75005 Paris, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Valérie Goguel
Thomas Preat
Genes and Dynamics of Memory Systems, Brain Plasticity Unit, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, ESPCI Paris, PSL Research University, 75005 Paris, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Thomas Preat
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Neprilysins are Type II metalloproteinases known to degrade and inactivate a number of small peptides, in particular the mammalian amyloid-β peptide (Aβ). In Drosophila, several neprilysins expressed in the brain are required for middle-term (MTM) and long-term memory (LTM) in the dorsal paired medial (DPM) neurons, a pair of large neurons that broadly innervate the mushroom bodies (MB), the center of olfactory memory. These data indicate that one or several peptides need to be degraded for MTM and LTM. We have previously shown that the fly amyloid precursor protein (APPL) is required for memory in the MB. We show here that APPL is also required in adult DPM neurons for MTM and LTM formation. This finding prompted us to search for an interaction between neprilysins and Drosophila Aβ (dAβ), a cleavage product of APPL. To find out whether dAβ was a neprilysin's target, we used inducible drivers to modulate neprilysin 1 (Nep1) and dAβ expression in adult DPM neurons. Experiments were conducted either in both sexes or in females. We show that Nep1 inhibition makes dAβ expression detrimental to both MTM and LTM. Conversely, memory deficits displayed by dAβ-expressing flies are rescued by Nep1 overexpression. Consistent with behavioral data, biochemical analyses confirmed that Nep1 degrades dAβ. Together, our findings establish that Nep1 and dAβ expressed in DPM neurons are functionally linked for memory processes, suggesting that dAβ is a physiological target for Nep1.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Neprilysins are endopeptidases known to degrade a number of small peptides and in particular the amyloid peptide. We previously showed that all four neprilysins expressed in the Drosophila brain are involved in specific phases of olfactory memory. Here we show that an increase in the level of the neprilysin 1 peptidase overcomes memory deficits induced by amyloid peptide in young flies. Together, the data reveal a functional interaction between neprilysin 1 and amyloid peptide, suggesting that neprilysin 1 degrades amyloid peptide. These findings raise the possibility that, under nonpathological conditions, mammalian neprilysins degrade amyloid peptide to ensure memory formation.

  • amyloid peptide
  • dorsal paired medial neurons
  • Drosophila
  • learning and memory
  • neprilysin
  • olfactory conditioning

Introduction

Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a progressive neurodegenerative disorder that first manifests as a memory decline. Its dominant histological hallmark is the accumulation of amyloid-β peptides (Aβ) produced by the processing of the amyloid precursor protein (APP) following the amyloidogenic pathway (Turner et al., 2003). This proteolytic pathway is initiated by the rate-limiting enzyme β-secretase (BACE) (Cole and Vassar, 2007). As for any protein, the Aβ steady-state level results from a balance between its synthesis and its degradation. Neprilysins (Nep), known as membrane metallo-endopeptidases, have been highlighted as the main enzyme for Aβ degradation in humans (Turner, 2003; Nalivaeva et al., 2012). It has been shown that Nep level and activity decrease during AD progression, suggesting that a reduction in Aβ degradation may contribute to the development of the disease (Yasojima et al., 2001; Caccamo et al., 2005; Wang et al., 2010; Zhou et al., 2013). In mouse models of AD, an increase in Nep activity ameliorates the memory deficits observed (Poirier et al., 2006; Spencer et al., 2008; Park et al., 2013), whereas Nep deficiency worsens them (Hüttenrauch et al., 2015).

Drosophila melanogaster is a model system well suited to analyzing the influence of the APP pathway on memory. Importantly, the fly genome contains homologs of 75% of human disease-related genes (Reiter et al., 2001), and molecules and mechanisms underlying memory processes are conserved from flies to mammals. Although relatively simple, the fly brain is highly organized and sustains complex forms of learning and memory. The mushroom bodies (MB) constitute the central integrative brain structure for olfactory associative memory. They are composed of ∼4000 neurons, the Kenyon cells (KC), classified into three subtypes whose axons form five lobes: two vertical (α and α′) and three medial (β, β′, and γ) (Crittenden et al., 1998).

Four Neps (Nep1-4) are expressed in the Drosophila CNS (Meyer et al., 2011; Sitnik et al., 2014). We previously showed that they were involved in both middle-term (MTM) and long-term memory (LTM) in the dorsal paired medial (DPM) neurons, a pair of large neurons that broadly innervate all the MB lobes (Turrel et al., 2016). DPM neurons are thought to regulate the MB outcome with a feedback system, a process involved in MTM formation (Keene et al., 2004). Nep1 expression is also required in the MB for MTM and LTM, and functional redundancy is observed between Nep2, Nep3, and Nep4 for LTM formation in the MB (Turrel et al., 2016). The Nep targets involved in memory processes are still unknown.

The fly expresses a single nonessential APP ortholog called APP-Like (APPL) (Rosen et al., 1989; Luo et al., 1992). Importantly, APPL follows proteolytic pathways similar to APP (Poeck et al., 2012), and the homologs of all mammalian secretases have been characterized in the fly (Rooke et al., 1996; Boulianne et al., 1997; Hong and Koo, 1997; Carmine-Simmen et al., 2009). APPL silencing in the MB of adult flies was shown to specifically disrupt MTM and LTM (Goguel et al., 2011; Bourdet et al., 2015a).

It has been shown previously that Nep2 can degrade human Aβ42 (Finelli et al., 2004; Cao et al., 2008). However, whether or not a Drosophila Nep degrades endogenous Aβ is still an open question. Although there is no sequence similarity at the level of Aβ sequences between APP and APPL, a Drosophila Aβ-like peptide (dAβ) was identified in old flies overexpressing APPL (Carmine-Simmen et al., 2009).

In the work reported here, using conditional expression, we modulated Nep1 and dAβ levels in young adult flies. Together, our findings reveal a functional interaction between Nep1 and amyloid peptide for memory processes.

Materials and Methods

Drosophila stocks.

D. melanogaster wild-type strain Canton Special (CS) and mutant flies were raised on standard medium at 18°C and 60% humidity with a 12 h light/dark cycle. All the strains used for memory experiments were outcrossed to the CS background. Nep1 and Appl-RNAi lines were obtained from the Vienna Drosophila Resource Center (Nep1A: 27537; Nep1C: 108860; Appl: 108312 and 42673). To overexpress Nep1, we used the P{EPgy2}Nep1EY21255 line (Nep1EY21255) obtained from the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center (Indiana University, #22465). UAS-APPL (APPL) was previously described by Torroja et al. (1999). UAS-dAβ (dAβ) and UAS-dBACE (dBACE) were kindly provided by Doris Kretzschmar (Carmine-Simmen et al., 2009). To generate a Nep1 mutant, a 2.59 kb PCR fragment was amplified using the Nep1 cDNA as a template (Drosophila Genomics Resource Center, Bloomington, IN, #GH03315), and cloned into the BglII site of pUAST (Brand and Perrimon, 1993). The resulting plasmid was verified by sequencing, and the 1.24 kb BstEII-KpnI restriction fragment was replaced by the mutant fragment obtained from GenScript. The mutated Nep1 (Nep1mut) displays a mutation in position 685 (E → V) that disrupts the catalytic activity of the enzyme (Devault et al., 1988). GAL4 drivers used were VT64246 (Vienna Drosophila Resource Center, #264246) for expression in DPM neurons and c739 for expression in MB α/β neurons. We used the TARGET system to specifically induce RNAi expression in adults (McGuire et al., 2003). To achieve GAL4 induction, flies were kept at 30°C for 3 d before conditioning, and until memory test for LTM analyses.

Behavioral experiments.

Flies were trained with classical olfactory aversive conditioning protocols as described by Pascual and Preat (2001). Training and testing were performed at 25°C and 80% humidity. Conditioning was performed on samples of 25–35 flies between 4 and 5 d of age with 3-octanol (∼95% purity, Sigma-Aldrich) and 4-methylcyclohexanol (99% purity, Sigma-Aldrich) at 0.360 mm and 0.325 mm, respectively. Odors were diluted in paraffin oil (VWR International). Memory tests were performed with a T-maze apparatus (Tully and Quinn, 1985). Flies were given 1 min to choose between two arms, each delivering a distinct odor. An index was calculated as the difference between the numbers of flies in each arm divided by the number in both. The average of two reciprocal experiments gave a performance index. For learning analyses, flies were tested immediately after a single training cycle. To assess MTM, flies underwent one-cycle training, and memory was tested 2 h later. For LTM analyses, flies were trained with 5 cycles spaced at 15 min rest intervals, and tested 24 h later. For odor avoidance tests after electric shock and response to electric shock, flies were treated as described by Pascual and Preat (2001). For experiments including genotypes with a construct on the X chromosome (Nep1EY21255 and dBACE), only females were analyzed.

qPCR analyses.

For elav genotypes, flies were raised at 25°C. Gal80ts;VT64246 flies were raised at 18°C, and GAL4-transcription was induced for 3 d at 30°C before RNA extraction. Total RNA was extracted from 60 female heads using the RNeasy Plant Mini Kit (QIAGEN). Preparations underwent a DNaseI treatment (Biolabs) for 15 min at 37°C. DNase was heat-inactivated with EDTA (10 mm). Samples were cleaned with the RNA Minielute Cleanup kit (QIAGEN), and reverse-transcribed with oligo(dT)20 primers using the SuperScript III First-Strand kit (Invitrogen) according to the manufacturer's instructions. Level of the target cDNA was compared against level of α-Tub84B (Tub, CG1913) cDNA used as a reference. Amplification was performed using a LightCycler 480 (Roche) and the SYBR Green I Master mix (Roche). Reactions were performed in triplicate. Specificity and size of amplification products were assessed by melting curve analyses and agarose gel electrophoresis, respectively. Expression relative to reference is expressed as a ratio (2−ΔCp, where Cp is the crossing point).

Western blot analyses.

To analyze dAβ level, we used a tub-Gal80ts;elav pan-neuronal driver. Proteins were extracted from 50 female fly heads after 48 h of induction at 30°C. Samples were prepared as in Burnouf et al. (2015). Soluble proteins were separated using SDS-PAGE gels containing a gradient of 10%–20% acrylamide (Novex 10%–20% Tricine, Invitrogen) and transferred to nitrocellulose membrane (GE Healthcare). Membranes were cut into two parts: the lower one was probed with anti-HA (ab130275, Abcam, 1:10,000 dilution), and the upper one was probed with anti-α-Tub (T6199; Sigma-Aldrich, 1:40,000 dilution). Western blots were routinely developed using the ECL system (Invitrogen). Protein levels were quantified using ImageQuant TL software (GE Healthcare) and expressed as a ratio relative to α-Tub.

Experimental design and statistical analyses.

For behavioral experiments, memory scores were displayed as mean ± SEM. Scores resulting from all genotypes were analyzed using one-way ANOVA followed, if significant at p ≤ 0.05, by Newman–Keuls multiple-comparison tests. Overall, ANOVA p value is given in the legends along with the value of the corresponding Fisher distribution F(x,y), where x is number of degrees of freedom for groups and y is the total number of degrees of freedom for the distribution. Asterisks in the figure denote the least significant of the pairwise post hoc comparisons between the genotype of interest and its controls, following the usual nomenclature. Quantitative mRNA measurements were analyzed from 2−ΔCp using Student's t tests, with significance threshold set at p ≤ 0.05. Western blot quantification was analyzed using Student's one-sample t test.

Results

Modulation of Nep1 expression in the adult MB does not impact memory of flies expressing dAβ

We previously showed that Nep1 expression is required in the α/β neurons of the MB for MTM (Turrel et al., 2016). Using a classical conditioning paradigm in which an odorant is paired with the delivery of electric shocks, the fly is able to form six discrete aversive memory phases reflected at the neural network level (Bouzaiane et al., 2015). Learning is measured immediately after a single conditioning, whereas MTM is assessed 1–3 h later. The fly can also produce two antagonistic forms of consolidated memory (Isabel et al., 2004). Among them, the robust LTM is only formed after multiple conditioning cycles spaced by rest intervals. Crucially, LTM is the only memory phase-dependent on de novo protein synthesis (Tully et al., 1994).

To find out whether amyloid peptide was a target for Nep1 for memory processes, we proceeded to modulate Nep1 expression in transgenic flies expressing Drosophila amyloid peptides. To this end, we used a construct allowing expression of a dAβ derived from APPL (Carmine-Simmen et al., 2009). dAβ ectopic expression in the MB was achieved with the c739 driver that labels the α/β KC (Yang et al., 1995). To restrict GAL4 expression to adulthood, we used a tub-Gal80ts;c739 line (Gal80ts;c739) that ubiquitously expresses a thermosensitive GAL4 inhibitor (Gal80ts) inactive at 30°C (McGuire et al., 2003). As previously shown (Bourdet et al., 2015b), dAβ expression in the adult α/β KC did not impair MTM (Fig. 1A). To determine whether Nep1 could functionally interact with dAβ, we examined whether the MTM deficit displayed by Nep1-knockdown flies (Turrel et al., 2016) was exacerbated by dAβ expression: if Nep1 degrades dAβ, then Nep1 knockdown should lead to an increase in dAβ level that might impair MTM formation.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Modulation of Nep1 expression in the adult MB does not impact memory of flies expressing dAβ. A, dAβ expression in α/β neurons does not alter MTM (F(2,35) = 0.7471, p = 0.4816, n = 12). B, dAβ expression in α/β neurons does not aggravate the MTM defect caused by RNAi-Nep1A expression. Gal80ts;c739/dAβ;RNAi-Nep1A flies exhibit an MTM deficit that is not significantly different from that of Gal80ts;c739/RNAi-Nep1A flies (F(4,69) = 5.086, p = 0.0013, n ≥ 14, post hoc Newman–Keuls test, **p < 0.01, Gal80ts;c739/RNAi-Nep1A versus Gal80ts;c739/+. *p < 0.05, Gal80ts;c739/RNAi-Nep1A versus +/RNAi-Nep1A. p > 0.05, Gal80ts;c739/RNAi-Nep1A versus Gal80ts;c739/dAβ;RNAi-Nep1A). C, qPCR analysis of Nep1 mRNA targeting by the RNAi-Nep1C construct. Total RNA was extracted from elav/+ and elav/RNAi-Nep1C fly heads, and further reverse-transcribed with oligo(dT) primers. Resulting cDNA was quantified using Tub expression as a reference. Results are shown as ratios to the reference (unpaired t test, t(6) = 11.07, ***p < 0.0001, n = 4) (4 values from two independent RNA extractions). D, dAβ expression in α/β neurons does not alter MTM of RNAi-Nep1C-expressing flies. Gal80ts;c739/RNAi-Nep1C and Gal80ts;c739/dAβ;RNAi-Nep1C show normal MTM (F(4,71) = 2.354, p = 0.0626, n ≥ 14). E, Flies expressing dAβ in α/β neurons present an LTM deficit that is not alleviated by Nep1 overexpression. Gal80ts;c739/dAβ flies exhibit an LTM deficit (F(6,83) = 17.86, p < 0.0001, n = 12, post hoc Newman–Keuls test, ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;c739/dAβ versus Gal80ts;c739/+. ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;c739/dAβ versus +/dAβ). Flies overexpressing Nep1 in the α/β neurons show normal LTM (p > 0.05, Gal80ts;c739/Nep1EY21255 vs Gal80ts;c739/+. p > 0.05, Gal80ts;c739/Nep1EY21255 versus +/Nep1EY21255). Gal80ts;c739/dAβ;Nep1EY21255 flies show an LTM deficit similar to that of Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ (***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;c739/dAβ;Nep1EY21255 vs Gal80ts;c739/+. ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;c739/dAβ;Nep1EY21255 versus +/dAβ;Nep1EY21255. p > 0.05, Gal80ts;c739/dAβ versus Gal80ts;c739/dAβ;Nep1EY21255). In the absence of induction, all genotypes exhibit normal LTM scores (F(4,59) = 0.7078, p = 0.5901, n = 12). F, qPCR analysis of Nep1 overexpression using the Nep1EY21255 line. Total RNA was extracted from elav/+ and elav/ Nep1EY21255 fly heads, and further reverse-transcribed with oligo(dT) primers. Resulting cDNA was quantified using Tub expression as a reference. Results are shown as ratios to the reference (unpaired t test, t(14) = 2.761, *p = 0.0153, n = 8) (8 values from four independent RNA extractions).

To reduce Nep1 expression, we used specific RNAi constructs. The RNAi-Nep1A construct leads to a 75% decrease in Nep1 expression in elav/RNAi-Nep1A flies and caused an MTM deficit when expressed in adult α/β KC (Turrel et al., 2016). Concomitant dAβ overexpression did not further affect MTM of RNAi-Nep1A-expressing flies (Fig. 1B). We next used a second nonoverlapping construct, RNAi-Nep1C. qPCR experiments showed that elav/RNAi-Nep1C flies exhibited a 63% decrease in Nep1 expression (Fig. 1C). We further observed that reducing Nep1 expression in adult α/β KC to a smaller extent than that previously achieved with the RNAi-Nep1A construct appeared to have no impact on MTM (Fig. 1D). Concomitant dAβ overexpression did not alter MTM (Fig. 1D). Thus, reducing Nep1 level in adult α/β KC of flies expressing amyloid peptide did not reveal any functional interaction between Nep1 and dAβ.

We next analyzed LTM and observed that Gal80ts;c739/dAβ induced flies displayed a severe LTM defect, whereas noninduced flies showed normal LTM scores (Fig. 1E). We analyzed the sensory-motor capacities of the Gal80ts;c739/dAβ flies, and found that they displayed normal shock reactivity and olfactory acuity (Table 1). We conclude that dAβ expression in adult α/β KC induces an LTM deficit. We thus asked whether Nep1 overexpression would alleviate the LTM defect caused by dAβ expression. To this end, we took advantage of the Nep1EY21255 line that contains a P-element inserted upstream of the first noncoding Nep1-RA exon. This P-element construct carries UAS binding sites for the GAL4 transcriptional regulator, allowing GAL4-mediated expression of genes proximate to the site of the insertion (Bellen et al., 2004). It was previously shown that this line allows Nep1 overexpression (Panz et al., 2012). We performed qPCR experiments to assess Nep1 expression level in elav/Nep1EY21255 flies. The data show that fly heads express a 2.4-fold increase in Nep1 mRNA level (Fig. 1F).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Shock reactivity and olfactory acuity of flies expressing dAβ in adult α/β neuronsa

Flies overexpressing Nep1 (Gal80ts;c739/Nep1EY21255) showed normal LTM (Fig. 1E). Simultaneous overexpression of Nep1 and dAβ in adult α/β KC did not rescue the LTM deficit caused by dAβ expression (Fig. 1E). In conclusion, we did not observe any functional interaction when Nep1 expression was modulated in the MB of dAβ-expressing flies.

dAβ expression impairs MTM formation when combined with Nep1 knockdown in adult DPM neurons

As Nep1 expression was also shown to be required in DPM neurons for memory formation (Turrel et al., 2016), we repeated the experiments using a tub-Gal80ts;VT64246 GAL4-line (Gal80ts;VT64246) to drive specific expression in adult DPM neurons. We first expressed dAβ in adult DPM neurons. Like dAβ expression driven in KC, expression driven in adult DPM neurons did not impact MTM (Fig. 2A). As previously reported (Turrel et al., 2016), RNAi-Nep1A expression in adult DPM neurons led to an MTM defect (Fig. 2B). Strikingly, concomitant dAβ expression aggravated the MTM defect caused by Nep1 inhibition in DPM neurons (Fig. 2B). In the absence of GAL4 induction, all genotypes showed normal MTM scores (Fig. 2B). We verified that the ability of induced Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;RNAi-Nep1A flies to avoid electric shocks and their olfactory acuity to each odor after electric shock exposure, were unaffected (Table 2). We next analyzed learning capacity and observed that, like Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-Nep1A flies (Turrel et al., 2016), Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;RNAi-Nep1A flies exhibited normal learning (Fig. 2C). Thus, the association of Nep1 knockdown with dAβ expression in adult DPM neurons does not perturb learning.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

dAβ expression is detrimental to MTM formation when combined with Nep1 knockdown in adult DPM neurons. A, Flies expressing dAβ in adult DPM neurons show normal MTM (F(2,32) = 0.0550, p = 0.9466, n = 11). B, dAβ expression in Nep1-knockdown flies exacerbates their MTM deficit. Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-Nep1A flies exhibit an MTM deficit (F(4,69) = 17.62, p < 0.0001, n ≥ 13, post hoc Newman–Keuls test, *p < 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-Nep1A versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. **p < 0.01, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-Nep1A versus +/RNAi-Nep1A), which is aggravated by concomitant expression of dAβ (***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;RNAi-Nep1A versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;RNAi-Nep1A versus +/dAβ;RNAi-Nep1A. ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;RNAi-Nep1A versus Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-Nep1A). In the absence of induction, all genotypes exhibit normal MTM scores (F(4,54) = 0.6991, p = 0.5962, n = 11). C, Learning is not affected by dAβ expression and concomitant Nep1 knockdown in DPM neurons (F(2,23) = 1.251, p = 0.3066, n = 8). D, Flies expressing RNAi-Nep1C do not exhibit an MTM deficit (F(4,80) = 8.951, p < 0.0001, n ≥ 16). In contrast, RNAi-Nep1C coexpression with dAβ in adult DPM neurons induces an MTM deficit (**p < 0.01, Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;RNAi-Nep1C versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;RNAi-Nep1C versus +/dAβ;RNAi-Nep1C. **p < 0.01, Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;RNAi-Nep1C versus Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-Nep1C). In the absence of induction, all genotypes exhibit normal MTM scores (F(3,66) = 3.655, p = 0.0170, n = 17). E, dAβ level is increased when Nep1 is knocked down. Western blot analyses. Left, Membranes were cut in two halves: the upper part was stained using an α-Tub antibody, and the lower part was stained using an HA-specific antibody. Right, Quantification. dAβ is expressed as a ratio to α-Tub, relative to the Gal80ts;elav/dAβ control (one-sample t test, t(7) = 2.712, *p = 0.0301, n = 8) (8 values from eight independent extractions). Error bar indicates SEM.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Shock reactivity and olfactory acuity of flies expressing dAβ and Nep1-RNAi in adult DPM neuronsa

To confirm these results, we used RNAi-Nep1C. Similarly to its expression in KC, its expression driven in adult DPM neurons did not impact MTM (Fig. 2D), probably because this RNAi induces a smaller decrease in Nep1 than RNAi-Nep1A. Interestingly, whereas flies expressing only RNAi-Nep1C showed normal MTM, flies expressing both dAβ and RNAi-Nep1C showed an MTM deficit (Fig. 2D). As expected, this deficit was not observed in the absence of GAL4 induction (Fig. 2D). We verified that the ability of induced Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;RNAi-Nep1C flies to perceive conditioning stimuli was normal (Table 2). We conclude that, in DPM neurons, Nep1 reduction makes dAβ overexpression detrimental for MTM, revealing a functional link between Nep1 and dAβ. We hypothesize that Nep1 reduction leads to a failure in dAβ degradation, thus generating increased levels of dAβ that impair memory.

To directly assess the effect of Nep1 inhibition on dAβ levels, we used the HA tag-sequence inserted downstream of the dAβ sequence (Carmine-Simmen et al., 2009). We used a tub-Gal80ts;elav line (Gal80ts;elav) to drive dAβ pan-neuronal expression in adult flies. Head protein extracts were analyzed by Western blotting. Quantification revealed that dAβ levels were increased when Nep1 was knocked down (Fig. 2E). The data indicate that Nep1 degrades dAβ, consistent with functional analyses described above.

Nep1 overexpression in adult DPM neurons rescues the LTM defect caused by dAβ expression

We next examined the Nep1-dAβ interaction in LTM formation. Like expression in KC, dAβ expression in DPM neurons caused a strong LTM defect (Fig. 3A). In the absence of induction, no LTM defect was observed (Fig. 3A). We tested the sensory-motor capacities of Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ flies, and found that they displayed wild-type shock reactivity and olfactory acuity (Table 3). We conclude that, like expression in the MB, dAβ expression in adult DPM neurons induces an LTM deficit. We went on to determine whether Nep1 overexpression could overcome the negative effect of dAβ expression on LTM. We observed that Gal80ts;VT64246/Nep1EY21255 flies overexpressing Nep1 in adult DPM neurons showed normal LTM (Fig. 3A). Strikingly, flies that overexpressed Nep1 in addition to dAβ in adult DPM neurons showed wild-type LTM (Fig. 3A), suggesting that Nep1 enzyme could degrade dAβ. We reasoned that the presence of two UAS-GAL4 could result in the decreased expression of each of them caused by GAL4 dilution. Thus, to check that the rescued phenotype observed was not caused by a decrease in dAβ expression, we performed qPCR experiments to measure dAβ mRNA level in Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ and Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;Nep1EY21255 fly heads. We observed that dAβ expression was not decreased by the presence of Nep1EY21255 (Fig. 3B). We thus conclude that, in DPM neurons, Nep1 overexpression rescues the LTM deficit caused by dAβ expression.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Nep1 overexpression in adult DPM neurons rescues the LTM defect caused by dAβ expression. A, Nep1 overexpression in adult DPM neurons rescues the LTM deficit of dAβ-expressing flies. Flies expressing dAβ in DPM neurons exhibit a strong LTM defect (F(6,74) = 7.606, p < 0.0001, n ≥ 10, post hoc Newman–Keuls test, ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ versus +/dAβ). Flies overexpressing Nep1 have normal LTM (p > 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/Nep1EY21255 versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. p > 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/Nep1EY21255 versus +/Nep1EY21255). Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;Nep1EY21255 exhibit wild-type LTM (p > 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;Nep1EY21255 versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. p > 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;Nep1EY21255 versus +/dAβ;Nep1EY21255. p > 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;Nep1EY21255 versus Gal80ts;VT64246/Nep1EY21255). In the absence of induction, all genotypes exhibit normal LTM scores (F(6,73) = 0.6718, p = 0.6727, n ≥ 10). B, dAβ expression is not decreased by Nep1EY21255 presence. qPCR analyses of dAβ mRNA levels. Total RNA was extracted from Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ and Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;Nep1EY21255 fly heads after induction, and further reverse-transcribed with oligo(dT) primers. Resulting cDNA was quantified using Tub expression as a reference. Results are shown as ratios to the reference (unpaired t test, t(5) = 2.774, p = 0.4162, n = 6) (6 values from three independent RNA extractions). C, qPCR analysis of Nep1mut expression. Total RNA was extracted from elav/+ and elav/Nep1mut fly heads, and further reverse-transcribed with oligo(dT) primers. Resulting cDNA was quantified using Tub expression as a reference. Results are shown as ratios to the reference (unpaired t test, t(10) = 18.55, ***p < 0.0001, n = 6) (6 values from three independent RNA extractions). D, Flies overexpressing Nep1mut, a mutant inactive Nep1 form, in adult DPM neurons, show normal LTM (F(2,35) = 0.1371, p = 0.8724, n = 12). E, Nep1mut expression does not rescue the LTM deficit caused by dAβ (F(4,109) = 6.035, p = 0.0002, n = 22, post hoc Newman–Keuls test, *p < 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ vs Gal80ts;VT64246/+. *p < 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ vs +/dAβ. **p < 0.01, Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;Nep1mut versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. **p < 0.01, Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;Nep1mut versus +/dAβ;Nep1mut. p > 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ;Nep1mut versus Gal80ts;VT64246/dAβ). In the absence of induction, all genotypes exhibit normal LTM scores (F(4,59) = 1.575, p = 0.1939, n = 12).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Shock reactivity and olfactory acuity of flies expressing dAβ in adult DPM neuronsa

Finally, we examined whether the capacity of Nep1 overexpression to overcome dAβ-induced LTM defect was mediated by its catalytic activity. To this end, we generated a Nep1 form with a mutation in its catalytic site (Devault et al., 1988). To verify the expression level of this mutated Nep1 protein (Nep1mut), we performed qPCR experiments with oligonucleotides that amplify Nep1 both wild-type and mutant sequences. We observed that Nep1 mRNA levels were increased 5.7-fold in elav/Nep1mut fly heads compared with the elav/+ control, showing efficient Nep1mut expression (Fig. 3C). Nep1mut expression in adult DPM neurons did not affect LTM (Fig. 3D). Flies expressing both dAβ and Nep1mut showed LTM scores similar to that of flies expressing only dAβ (Fig. 3E). Thus, unlike wild-type Nep1 overexpression, that of Nep1mut does not rescue the LTM defect caused by dAβ expression in adult DPM neurons, indicating that the LTM rescue previously observed depends on Nep1 catalytic activity.

APPL is required in adult DPM neurons for both MTM and LTM

As we evidenced a functional interaction between ectopic dAβ and Nep1 activity in adult DPM neurons, we next investigated whether DPM neurons could physiologically express the amyloid peptide. Because APPL is highly expressed in the MB, it could reveal difficult to determine with immunohistochemistry experiments staining corresponding to DPM neuritis in addition to MB lobes. Instead, we asked whether APPL expression, if any, was required for memory in DPM neurons. We used two nonoverlapping RNAi constructs (RNAi-108312, Bourdet et al., 2015a; RNAi-42673, Goguel et al., 2011) to specifically inhibit APPL expression in adult DPM neurons. Interestingly, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-42673 and Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-108312 flies displayed both mutant MTM (Fig. 4A) and mutant LTM (Fig. 4B). Noninduced flies displayed normal MTM (Fig. 4C) and LTM scores (Fig. 4D), showing that the deficits were specifically caused by induction of APPL RNAi in adult DPM neurons. Next, we analyzed learning and observed that RNAi-APPL induced flies showed normal learning performance (Fig. 4E). We checked that induced flies showed normal shock reactivity and normal olfactory acuity to each odor after electric shock exposure (Table 4). We conclude that, in addition to expression in the MB, APPL expression in DPM neurons is required for both MTM and LTM processes. APPL being expressed in DPM neurons, dAβ may possibly be produced physiologically by adult DPM neurons.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

APPL expression is required in adult DPM neurons for MTM and LTM. A, Flies expressing APPL-RNAi in adult DPM neurons present MTM defects (RNAi-42673: F(2,41) = 7.990, p = 0.0012, n = 14, post hoc Newman–Keuls test, **p < 0.01, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-42673 versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. **p < 0.01, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-42673 versus +/RNAi-42673; RNAi-108312: F(2,37) = 4.657, p = 0.0161, n ≥ 12, post hoc Newman–Keuls test, *p < 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-108312 versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. *p < 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-108312 versus +/RNAi-108312). B, Flies expressing APPL-RNAi in adult DPM neurons present LTM defects (RNAi-42673: F(2,46) = 13.85, p < 0.0001, n ≥ 15, post hoc Newman–Keuls test, ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-42673 versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. **p < 0.01, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-42673 versus +/RNAi-42673; RNAi-108312: F(2,63) = 4.530, p = 0.0146, n ≥ 21, post hoc Newman–Keuls test, *p < 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-108312 versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. *p < 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-108312 versus +/RNAi-108312). C, In the absence of induction, all genotypes exhibit normal MTM scores (RNAi-42673: F(2,29) = 1.250, p = 0.3025, n = 10; RNAi-108312: F(2,23) = 0.6399, p = 0.5373, n = 8). D, In the absence of induction, all genotypes exhibit normal LTM scores (RNAi-42673: F(2,57) = 1.213, p = 0.3051, n ≥ 19; RNAi-108312: F(2,59) = 2.506, p = 0.0905, n ≥ 19). E, Learning is not affected by APPL-RNAi expression in adult DPM neurons (RNAi-42673: F(2,23) = 6.108, p = 0.0081, n = 8, post hoc Newman–Keuls test, p > 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-42673 versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. **p < 0.01, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-42673 versus +/RNAi-42673; RNAi-108312: F(2,23) = 0.6399, p = 0.5373, n = 8).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 4.

Shock reactivity and olfactory acuity of flies expressing APPL-RNAi in adult DPM neuronsa

dBACE overexpression aggravates MTM and LTM defects caused by Nep1 knockdown in adult DPM neurons

We next asked whether overexpression of endogenous dAβ impaired memory. The first enzyme responsible for dAβ synthesis is the dBACE rate-limiting enzyme (Carmine-Simmen et al., 2009). Using an UAS-dBACE construct that encodes the fly β-secretase (Carmine-Simmen et al., 2009), we analyzed the effect on memory of dBACE overexpression in adult DPM neurons. After induction, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE flies showed both normal MTM and normal LTM (Fig. 5A). If dBACE overexpression results in dAβ overproduction, dAβ level remains insufficient to disrupt memory. We reasoned that, if Nep1 can degrade endogenous Aβ peptide, Nep1 knockdown should cause a further increase in dAβ level that could thus reach a level where it could impair memory. Interestingly, flies expressing both RNAi-Nep1A and dBACE in DPM neurons showed an MTM deficit significantly higher than that of flies expressing only RNAi-Nep1A (Fig. 5B). Noninduced Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;RNAi-Nep1A flies displayed normal MTM scores (Fig. 5B). Similar results were observed for LTM: flies expressing both RNAi-Nep1A and dBACE in DPM neurons showed a significantly higher LTM deficit than flies expressing only RNAi-Nep1A, whereas in the absence of GAL4 induction, these flies showed normal scores (Fig. 5C). We verified that induced flies showed normal response to electric shock exposure and normal olfactory acuity (Table 5). Together, the data reveal a functional interaction between dBACE and Nep1, suggesting that dBACE overexpression produces a substrate for Nep1. On Nep1 knockdown, an accumulation of this substrate, presumably dAβ, impairs memory.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

dBACE overexpression aggravates MTM and LTM defects caused by Nep1 knockdown in adult DPM neurons. A, dBACE overexpression in adult DPM neurons does not alter MTM (F(2,35) = 0.0788, p = 0.9244, n = 12) or LTM (F(2,36) = 0.0072, p = 0.9928, n ≥ 11). B, dBACE overexpression in DPM neurons exacerbates the MTM deficit cause by Nep1 knockdown (F(4,42) = 14.39, p < 0.0001, n ≥ 8, post hoc Newman–Keuls test, *p < 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-Nep1A versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. *p < 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-Nep1A versus +/RNAi-Nep1A. ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;RNAi-Nep1A versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;RNAi-Nep1A versus +/dBACE;RNAi-Nep1A. **p < 0.01, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;RNAi-Nep1A versus Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-Nep1A). In the absence of induction, all genotypes exhibit normal MTM scores (F(4,49) = 0.3999, p = 0.8077, n = 10). C, dBACE overexpression in DPM neurons exacerbates the LTM deficit caused by Nep1 knockdown (F(4,66) = 15.06, p < 0.0001, n ≥ 13, post hoc Newman–Keuls test, ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-Nep1A versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-Nep1A versus +/RNAi-Nep1A. ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;RNAi-Nep1A versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;RNAi-Nep1A versus +/dBACE;RNAi-Nep1A. *p < 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;RNAi-Nep1A versus Gal80ts;VT64246/RNAi-Nep1A). In the absence of induction, all genotypes exhibit normal LTM scores (F(4,51) = 0.1327, p = 0.9696, n ≥ 10).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 5.

Shock reactivity and olfactory acuity of flies expressing dBACE and RNAi-Nep1A in adult DPM neuronsa

Nep1 overexpression rescues the LTM deficit caused by APPL and dBACE coexpression in adult DPM neurons

It has been shown that dBACE constitutive neuronal coexpression with APPL generates numerous amyloid deposits in old flies (Carmine-Simmen et al., 2009). As we observed that dBACE overexpression alone did not impair memory, we analyzed dBACE coexpression with APPL. First, we assessed APPL overexpression alone. Like dBACE, APPL overexpression driven in adult DPM neurons did not impact MTM (Fig. 6A). By contrast, flies overexpressing both APPL and dBACE showed decreased MTM scores compared with control flies (Fig. 6A). In the absence of GAL4 induction, these flies showed normal MTM (Fig. 6A). We verified that induced Gal80ts;VT64246/APPL;dBACE flies displayed normal responses to the conditioning stimuli (Table 6). In conclusion, while neither APPL nor dBACE overexpression alone impacts MTM, concomitant overexpression does, and this effect is very likely caused by an overproduction of dAβ. We further observed that flies overexpressing Nep1 in addition to APPL and dBACE showed normal MTM (Fig. 6A), indicating that Nep1 overexpression rescues the MTM deficit generated by APPL and dBACE coexpression. We conclude that Nep1 acts in the same pathway as APPL and dBACE.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Nep1 overexpression rescues the memory deficit caused by APPL and dBACE coexpression in adult DPM neurons. A, Nep1 overexpression rescues the MTM deficit caused by APPL and dBACE coexpression in adult DPM neurons. APPL overexpression in DPM neurons does not alter MTM (F(2,35) = 1.575, p = 0.2222, n = 12). Flies coexpressing APPL and dBACE in DPM neurons present an MTM defect (F(6,70) = 4.559, p = 0.0007, n ≥ 10, post hoc Newman–Keuls test, ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;APPL versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;APPL versus +/dBACE;APPL). Flies overexpressing Nep1 exhibit normal MTM (p > 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/Nep1EY21255 versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. p > 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/Nep1EY21255 versus +/Nep1EY21255). Flies coexpressing dBACE, APPL, and Nep1 present wild-type MTM (p > 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;APPL;Nep1EY21255 versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. p > 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;APPL;Nep1EY21255 versus +/dBACE;APPL;Nep1EY21255. **p < 0.01, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;APPL;Nep1EY21255 versus Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;APPL). In the absence of induction, all genotypes exhibit normal MTM scores (F(2,29) = 0.7477, p = 0.4830, n = 10). B, Nep1 overexpression rescues the LTM deficit caused by APPL and dBACE coexpression in adult DPM neurons. APPL overexpression in DPM neurons does not alter LTM (F(2,23) = 0.7440, p = 0.4873, n = 8). Flies coexpressing APPL and dBACE in DPM neurons present an LTM defect (F(6,69) = 7.299, p < 0.0001, n = 10, post hoc Newman–Keuls test, ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;APPL versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;APPL versus +/dBACE;APPL). Flies coexpressing dBACE, APPL, and Nep1 present wild-type LTM (p > 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;APPL;Nep1EY21255 versus Gal80ts;VT64246/+. p > 0.05, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;APPL;Nep1EY21255 versus +/dBACE;APPL;Nep1EY21255. ***p < 0.001, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;APPL;Nep1EY21255 versus Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;APPL). In the absence of induction, all genotypes exhibit normal LTM scores (F(2,29) = 0.6927, p = 0.5089, n = 10). C, D, qPCR experiments. Total RNA was extracted from Gal80ts;VT64246/+, Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;APPL, and Gal80ts;VT64246/dBACE;APPL;Nep1EY21255 fly heads after induction, and further reverse-transcribed with oligo(dT) primers. Resulting cDNA was quantified using Tub expression as a reference. Results are shown as ratios to the reference. C, Analysis of APPL and dBACE overexpression driven by Gal80ts;VT64246 (Appl: unpaired t test, t(18) = 3.328, **p = 0.0037, n = 10 (10 values from five independent RNA extractions); dBACE: unpaired t test, t10 = 13.83, ***p < 0.0001, n = 6 (6 values from three independent RNA extractions)). D, Appl and dBACE mRNA expression is not modified by the presence of Nep1EY21255. (Appl: unpaired t test, t(10) = 0.08343, p = 0.4236, n = 6 (6 values from three independent RNA extractions); dBACE: unpaired t test, t(6) = 2.319, p = 0.0595, n = 4 (4 values from two independent RNA extractions)). E, Schematic interpretation. E1, In a wild-type context, dAβ is degraded by Nep1 to ensure wild-type memory. E2, APPL and dBACE co-overexpression leads to an increased level of dAβ that is detrimental to memory formation. E3, The memory defect caused by dAβ overproduction is overcome by an increase in its degradation by Nep1. Blue circles represent dAβ.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 6.

Shock reactivity and olfactory acuity of flies expressing dBACE and APPL in adult DPM neuronsa

We next analyzed LTM. Like dBACE, APPL overexpression in adult DPM neurons had no impact on LTM (Fig. 6B). In sharp contrast, APPL and dBACE coexpression caused an LTM deficit that was not observed in the absence of induction (Fig. 6B). Strikingly, concomitant overexpression of Nep1 resulted in normal LTM (Fig. 6B).

We then checked that flies coexpressing APPL, dBACE, and Nep1 expressed APPL and dBACE at similar levels to flies coexpressing only APPL and dBACE. We first examined whether APPL and dBACE overexpression could be assessed by qPCR experiments driven on cDNA synthesized from Gal80ts;VT64246/APPL;dBACE fly heads RNA. We observed a significant increase in APPL level in Gal80ts;VT64246/APPL;dBACE fly heads compared with that of the control (Fig. 6C). In addition, we observed a substantial increase in dBACE expression in Gal80ts;VT64246/APPL;dBACE flies compared with the control (Fig. 6C). We next observed that flies coexpressing APPL, dBACE, and Nep1 did not express less APPL, or less dBACE, than flies coexpressing only APPL and dBACE (Fig. 6D).

In conclusion, the data show that the negative effect on MTM and LTM of APPL and dBACE coexpression is rescued by Nep1 overexpression (Fig. 6E).

Discussion

We showed previously that Nep1 is required in KC and DPM neurons for MTM and LTM, revealing that one or several neuropeptides need to be degraded for memory formation. Here we report that, in adult DPM neurons, Nep1 inhibition makes dAβ expression detrimental to both MTM and LTM. Conversely, Nep1 overexpression rescues dAβ-induced memory defects. Together, the data strongly suggest that Nep1 expressed in DPM neurons degrades dAβ, raising the possibility that dAβ is a Nep1 physiological substrate for memory processes.

Drosophila is currently used to model AD, in particular Aβ-induced toxicity (Iijima et al., 2004; Fang et al., 2012; Lin et al., 2014; Niccoli et al., 2016; Sofola-Adesakin et al., 2016). Transgenic flies overexpressing human Aβ42 in the eyes display dose- and age-dependent phenotypes that are alleviated by Nep2 upregulation (Finelli et al., 2004; Cao et al., 2008). One of the limits of these models is that ectopic human Aβ is expressed constitutively, and is therefore highly toxic, probably by disrupting normal development and/or cell physiology. Here we performed an inducible analysis of dAβ expression in young flies, thereby providing a more physiological model. We observe identical memory phenotypes when dAβ is expressed either in KC or in DPM neurons of young flies: MTM is normal, whereas LTM is disrupted. One hypothesis is that dAβ expression alters general functional features of the neuron biology and that its toxicity is therefore unspecific to memory. However, dAβ-expressing flies show normal reactivity to the conditioning stimuli used and display normal MTM, suggesting that such dAβ production might instead interfere with specific mechanisms in play for memory formation. One hypothesis is that dAβ impacts mechanisms specific to LTM. Alternatively, dAβ expression level may be an important feature for both MTM and LTM: dAβ levels reached in our experiments may alter LTM but may not be sufficient to alter MTM. We favor this latter hypothesis as APPL and dBACE coexpression disrupts both MTM and LTM. Indeed, as overexpression of APPL or dBACE alone does not affect memory, we conclude that memory defects observed on coexpression result from APPL processing by dBACE, and thus very likely from dAβ production.

In addition to Aβ, APP complex processing leads to numerous biologically active fragments that are secreted, membranous or intracellular (O'Brien and Wong, 2011; Zheng and Koo, 2011). We previously showed that a secreted APPL and a noncleaved APPL form were involved in MTM in adult α/β KC (Bourdet et al., 2015a). We now show that APPL expression is also required in DPM neurons. Interestingly, APPL conditional silencing specifically in KC or in DPM neurons leads to similar phenotypes: MTM and LTM are impaired, whereas learning remains unaffected. These data highlight the close relationship between DPM neurons and KC, a circuit thought to stabilize memories (Keene et al., 2004; Pitman et al., 2011), and suggest that APPL plays similar roles in both neuronal structures. In mammals, APP is expressed at presynaptic and postsynaptic sites (Yamazaki et al., 1995; Back et al., 2007; Hoe et al., 2009), and APP/APP transdimers have been shown to play a role in synaptic connectivity (Wang et al., 2009). It is therefore possible that APPL/APPL transdimers play a role at KC/DPM synapses. Another hypothesis is that an APPL fragment secreted from one cell type is a ligand for APPL located at the membrane of the other cell type, consistent with reports suggesting that APPL may function as a receptor (Cassar and Kretzschmar, 2016).

Nep peptidases constitute the main Aβ-degrading enzymes in mammals. They have also been implicated in diverse pathologies, but their substrates have remained poorly described. In Drosophila, a nonidentified neprilysin-like neuropeptidase was shown to play a role in the regulation of the pigment-dispersing factor signaling within circadian neural circuits (Isaac et al., 2007). More recently, Nep4 was shown to be involved in the expression of a number of insulin-like peptides (Hallier et al., 2016). Our data show that an increased expression of dAβ is detrimental to memory. In a context where dAβ level is below a deleterious threshold, knocking down Nep1 is sufficient to raise this level above the threshold, thus leading to memory deficits. Conversely, if dAβ level is above a deleterious threshold, concomitant overexpression of Nep1 restores wild-type memory. Although a noncatalytic role has been described for Nep4 intracellular domain (Panz et al., 2012), Nep1 catalytic activity is required to observe the rescue of the dAβ-induced memory deficit, indicating that Nep1 degrades dAβ. This conclusion is further supported by Western blot experiments showing that Nep1 inhibition results in an increase in dAβ levels. Together, our data strongly suggest that Nep1, a membrane proteinase whose active site faces the extracellular compartment, cleaves and inactivates dAβ in the extracellular space, consistent with the consensus that Nep function is to turn off neuropeptide signals at the synapse (Turner, 2003).

A functional interaction between Nep1 and dAβ is evidenced when modulation of their expression is achieved in DPM neurons, and not in KC. One hypothesis is that Nep1 is not expressed at the synapses where dAβ is secreted in α/β KC. Another possibility is that Nep1 is differently regulated in α/β KC and DPM neurons, leading to distinct specificity: Nep peptidases exhibit high substrate variability and are thus known to target numerous peptides (Turner et al., 2001; Nalivaeva et al., 2012). A recent study suggested that Nep4 activity in the CNS affected different neuropeptide regulatory systems from the corresponding muscle-bound activity (Hallier et al., 2016). Thus, Nep1 expressed at DPM membranes could target dAβ, and a distinct peptide when expressed in KC.

Learning and memory are associated with activity-dependent functional plasticity and changes in the structure and number of synapses (Korte and Schmitz, 2016). In AD patients, synaptic dysfunction has been closely correlated with cognitive decline (Querfurth and LaFerla, 2010). A large body of evidence points to a pathological role of Aβ, particularly as a primary cause of synaptic failure (Hardy and Selkoe, 2002; O'Brien and Wong, 2011). Importantly, Aβ is present in the CSF of healthy individuals, although at much lower concentrations than its neurotoxic concentrations, suggesting the possibility of a physiological role for Aβ (Fedele et al., 2015). In primary cultures of neurons, immunodepletion of endogenous Aβ causes neuronal cell death, a phenomenon rescued by addition of physiological picomolar levels of Aβ peptides (Plant et al., 2003). Furthermore, several reports have shown that, at very low physiological concentrations, Aβ modulates synaptic strength (Kamenetz et al., 2003; Abramov et al., 2009). Puzzo et al. (2008) reported that picomolar amounts of Aβ were able to enhance LTP and hippocampal-dependent memory in mice, whereas nanomolar concentrations of the peptide gave opposite results. Several additional studies confirmed the role of Aβ at low concentrations in synaptic plasticity and memory (Garcia-Osta and Alberini, 2009; Morley et al., 2010; Puzzo et al., 2011), suggesting that Aβ physiologically produced in the healthy brain during neuronal activity is needed for memory processes. Thus, Aβ appears to be a modulator of synaptic activity whose concentration needs to be strictly regulated: at the level of its production, degradation, or trafficking. We report here in Drosophila that memory disruption caused by an increased synthesis of dAβ is rescued by Nep1 overexpression, raising the possibility that Nep-dependent degradation of dAβ plays a physiological role in memory. A deficit in Aβ degradation would thus trigger memory deficits, the first cognitive signs of AD. Such mechanisms are consistent with evidence showing that late-onset AD is associated with impaired clearance of Aβ (Mawuenyega et al., 2010), and that AD brains show decreased levels of Nep expression and activity (Yasojima et al., 2001; Caccamo et al., 2005; Wang et al., 2010; Zhou et al., 2013).

Footnotes

  • T.P. was supported by Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale DEQ20140329540. We thank Aurélie Lampin-Saint-Amaux, Honorine Lucchi, and Noé Testa for technical help; and all the members of our laboratory for valuable discussions and for critically reading this manuscript.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to either Dr. Valérie Goguel or Dr. Thomas Preat, Genes and Dynamics of Memory Systems, Brain Plasticity Unit, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, ESPCI Paris, PSL Research University, 10 rue Vauquelin, 75005 Paris, France. valerie.goguel{at}espci.fr or thomas.preat{at}espci.fr

References

  1. ↵
    1. Abramov E,
    2. Dolev I,
    3. Fogel H,
    4. Ciccotosto GD,
    5. Ruff E,
    6. Slutsky I
    (2009) Amyloid-β as a positive endogenous regulator of release probability at hippocampal synapses. Nat Neurosci 12:1567–1576. doi:10.1038/nn.2433 pmid:19935655
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Back S,
    2. Haas P,
    3. Tschäpe JA,
    4. Gruebl T,
    5. Kirsch J,
    6. Müller U,
    7. Beyreuther K,
    8. Kins S
    (2007) β-Amyloid precursor protein can be transported independent of any sorting signal to the axonal and dendritic compartment. J Neurosci Res 85:2580–2590. doi:10.1002/jnr.21239 pmid:17335075
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Bellen HJ,
    2. Levis RW,
    3. Liao G,
    4. He Y,
    5. Carlson JW,
    6. Tsang G,
    7. Evans-Holm M,
    8. Hiesinger PR,
    9. Schulze KL,
    10. Rubin GM,
    11. Hoskins RA,
    12. Spradling AC
    (2004) The BDGP gene disruption project: single transposon insertions associated with 40% of Drosophila genes. Genetics 167:761–781. doi:10.1534/genetics.104.026427 pmid:15238527
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Boulianne GL,
    2. Livne-Bar I,
    3. Humphreys JM,
    4. Liang Y,
    5. Lin C,
    6. Rogaev E,
    7. St George-Hyslop P
    (1997) Cloning and characterization of the Drosophila presenilin homologue. Neuroreport 8:1025–1029. doi:10.1097/00001756-199703030-00041 pmid:9141085
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Bourdet I,
    2. Preat T,
    3. Goguel V
    (2015a) The full-length form of the Drosophila amyloid precursor protein is involved in memory formation. J Neurosci 35:1043–1051. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2093-14.2015 pmid:25609621
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Bourdet I,
    2. Lampin-Saint-Amaux A,
    3. Preat T,
    4. Goguel V
    (2015b) Amyloid-β peptide exacerbates the memory deficit caused by amyloid precursor protein loss-of-function in Drosophila. PLoS One 10:e0135741–14. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0135741 pmid:26274614
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Bouzaiane E,
    2. Trannoy S,
    3. Scheunemann L,
    4. Plaçais PY,
    5. Preat T
    (2015) Two independent mushroom body output circuits retrieve the six discrete components of Drosophila aversive memory. Cell Rep 11:1280–1292. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2015.04.044 pmid:25981036
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Brand AH,
    2. Perrimon N
    (1993) Targeted gene expression as a means of altering cell fates and generating dominant phenotypes. Development 118:401–415. pmid:8223268
    OpenUrlAbstract
  9. ↵
    1. Burnouf S,
    2. Gorsky MK,
    3. Dols J,
    4. Grönke S,
    5. Partridge L
    (2015) Aβ43 is neurotoxic and primes aggregation of Aβ40 in vivo. Acta Neuropathol 130:35–47. doi:10.1007/s00401-015-1419-y pmid:25862636
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Caccamo A,
    2. Oddo S,
    3. Sugarman MC,
    4. Akbari Y,
    5. LaFerla FM
    (2005) Age- and region-dependent alterations in Aβ-degrading enzymes: implications for Aβ-induced disorders. Neurobiol Aging 26:645–654. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.06.013 pmid:15708439
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Cao W,
    2. Song HJ,
    3. Gangi T,
    4. Kelkar A,
    5. Antani I,
    6. Garza D,
    7. Konsolaki M
    (2008) Identification of novel genes that modify phenotypes induced by Alzheimer's β-amyloid overexpression in Drosophila. Genetics 178:1457–1471. doi:10.1534/genetics.107.078394 pmid:18245849
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. Carmine-Simmen K,
    2. Proctor T,
    3. Tschäpe J,
    4. Poeck B,
    5. Triphan T,
    6. Strauss R,
    7. Kretzschmar D
    (2009) Neurotoxic effects induced by the Drosophila amyloid-β peptide suggest a conserved toxic function. Neurobiol Dis 33:274–281. doi:10.1016/j.nbd.2008.10.014 pmid:19049874
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Cassar M,
    2. Kretzschmar D
    (2016) Analysis of amyloid precursor protein function in Drosophila melanogaster. Front Mol Neurosci 9:17–19. doi:10.3389/fnmol.2016.00061 pmid:27507933
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Cole SL,
    2. Vassar R
    (2007) The Alzheimer's disease β-secretase enzyme, BACE1. Mol Neurodegener 2:22–25. doi:10.1186/1750-1326-2-22 pmid:18005427
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Crittenden JR,
    2. Skoulakis EM,
    3. Han KA,
    4. Kalderon D,
    5. Davis RL
    (1998) Tripartite mushroom body architecture revealed by antigenic markers. Learn Mem 5:38–51. pmid:10454371
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Devault A,
    2. Nault C,
    3. Zollinger M,
    4. Fournie-Zaluski MC,
    5. Roques BP,
    6. Crine P,
    7. Boileau G
    (1988) Expression of neutral endopeptidase (enkephalinase) in heterologous COS-1 cells: characterization of the recombinant enzyme and evidence for a glutamic acid residue at the active site. J Biol Chem 263:4033–4040. pmid:2894375
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    1. Fang L,
    2. Duan J,
    3. Ran D,
    4. Fan Z,
    5. Yan Y,
    6. Huang N,
    7. Gu H,
    8. Zhu Y
    (2012) Amyloid-β depresses excitatory cholinergic synaptic transmission in Drosophila. Neurosci Bull 28:585–594. doi:10.1007/s12264-012-1267-x pmid:23054636
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Fedele E,
    2. Rivera D,
    3. Marengo B,
    4. Pronzato MA,
    5. Ricciarelli R
    (2015) Amyloid β: walking on the dark side of the moon. Mech Ageing Dev 152:1–4. doi:10.1016/j.mad.2015.09.001 pmid:26362724
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Finelli A,
    2. Kelkar A,
    3. Song HJ,
    4. Yang H,
    5. Konsolaki M
    (2004) A model for studying Alzheimer's Aβ42-induced toxicity in Drosophila melanogaster. Mol Cell Neurosci 26:365–375. doi:10.1016/j.mcn.2004.03.001 pmid:15234342
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Garcia-Osta A,
    2. Alberini CM
    (2009) Amyloid beta mediates memory formation. Learn Mem 16:267–272. doi:10.1101/lm.1310209 pmid:19318468
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Goguel V,
    2. Belair AL,
    3. Ayaz D,
    4. Lampin-Saint-Amaux A,
    5. Scaplehorn N,
    6. Hassan BA,
    7. Preat T
    (2011) Drosophila amyloid precursor protein-like is required for long-term memory. J Neurosci 31:1032–1037. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2896-10.2011 pmid:21248128
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Hallier B,
    2. Schiemann R,
    3. Cordes E,
    4. Vitos-Faleato J,
    5. Walter S,
    6. Heinisch JJ,
    7. Malmendal A,
    8. Paululat A,
    9. Meyer H
    (2016) Drosophila neprilysins control insulin signaling and food intake via cleavage of regulatory peptides. eLife 5:e19430. doi:10.7554/eLife.19430 pmid:27919317
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Hardy J,
    2. Selkoe DJ
    (2002) The amyloid hypothesis of Alzheimer's disease: progress and problems on the road to therapeutics. Science 297:353–356. doi:10.1126/science.1072994 pmid:12130773
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  24. ↵
    1. Hoe HS,
    2. Fu Z,
    3. Makarova A,
    4. Lee JY,
    5. Lu C,
    6. Feng L,
    7. Pajoohesh-Ganji A,
    8. Matsuoka Y,
    9. Hyman BT,
    10. Ehlers MD,
    11. Vicini S,
    12. Pak DT,
    13. Rebeck GW
    (2009) The effects of amyloid precursor protein on postsynaptic composition and activity. J Biol Chem 284:8495–8506. doi:10.1074/jbc.M900141200 pmid:19164281
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    1. Hong CS,
    2. Koo EH
    (1997) Isolation and characterization of Drosophila presenilin homolog. Neuroreport 8:665–668. doi:10.1097/00001756-199702100-00017 pmid:9106743
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Hüttenrauch M,
    2. Baches S,
    3. Gerth J,
    4. Bayer TA,
    5. Weggen S,
    6. Wirths O
    (2015) Neprilysin deficiency alters the neuropathological and behavioral phenotype in the 5XFAD mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimers Dis 44:1291–1302. doi:10.3233/JAD-142463 pmid:25408216
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Iijima K,
    2. Liu HP,
    3. Chiang AS,
    4. Hearn SA,
    5. Konsolaki M,
    6. Zhong Y
    (2004) Dissecting the pathological effects of human Aβ40 and Aβ42 in Drosophila: a potential model for Alzheimer's disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:6623–6628. doi:10.1073/pnas.0400895101 pmid:15069204
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    1. Isaac RE,
    2. Johnson EC,
    3. Audsley N,
    4. Shirras AD
    (2007) Metabolic inactivation of the circadian transmitter, pigment dispersing factor (PDF), by neprilysin-like peptidases in Drosophila. J Exp Biol 210:4465–4470. doi:10.1242/jeb.012088 pmid:18055635
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Isabel G,
    2. Pascual A,
    3. Preat T
    (2004) Exclusive consolidated memory phases in Drosophila. Science 304:1024–1027. doi:10.1126/science.1094932 pmid:15143285
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. ↵
    1. Kamenetz F,
    2. Tomita T,
    3. Hsieh H,
    4. Seabrook G,
    5. Borchelt D,
    6. Iwatsubo T,
    7. Sisodia S,
    8. Malinow R
    (2003) APP processing and synaptic function. Neuron 37:925–937. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00124-7 pmid:12670422
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Keene AC,
    2. Stratmann M,
    3. Keller A,
    4. Perrat PN,
    5. Vosshall LB,
    6. Waddell S
    (2004) Diverse odor-conditioned memories require uniquely timed dorsal paired medial neuron output. Neuron 44:521–533. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.006 pmid:15504331
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Korte M,
    2. Schmitz D
    (2016) Cellular and system biology of memory: timing, molecules, and beyond. Physiol Rev 96:647–693. doi:10.1152/physrev.00010.2015 pmid:26960344
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    1. Lin JY,
    2. Wang WA,
    3. Zhang X,
    4. Liu HY,
    5. Zhao XL,
    6. Huang FD
    (2014) Intraneuronal accumulation of Aβ42 induces age-dependent slowing of neuronal transmission in Drosophila. Neurosci Bull 30:185–190. doi:10.1007/s12264-013-1409-9 pmid:24733651
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Luo L,
    2. Tully T,
    3. White K
    (1992) Human amyloid precursor protein ameliorates behavioral deficit of flies deleted for Appl gene. Neuron 9:595–605. doi:10.1016/0896-6273(92)90024-8 pmid:1389179
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Mawuenyega KG,
    2. Sigurdson W,
    3. Ovod V,
    4. Munsell L,
    5. Kasten T,
    6. Morris JC,
    7. Yarasheski KE,
    8. Bateman RJ
    (2010) Decreased clearance of CNS beta-amyloid in Alzheimer's disease. Science 330:1774. doi:10.1126/science.1197623 pmid:21148344
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    1. McGuire SE,
    2. Le PT,
    3. Osborn AJ,
    4. Matsumoto K,
    5. Davis RL
    (2003) Spatiotemporal rescue of memory dysfunction in Drosophila. Science 302:1765–1768. doi:10.1126/science.1089035 pmid:14657498
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    1. Meyer H,
    2. Panz M,
    3. Albrecht S,
    4. Drechsler M,
    5. Wang S,
    6. Hüsken M,
    7. Lehmacher C,
    8. Paululat A
    (2011) Drosophila metalloproteases in development and differentiation: the role of ADAM proteins and their relatives. Eur J Cell Biol 90:770–778. doi:10.1016/j.ejcb.2011.04.015 pmid:21684629
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Morley JE,
    2. Farr SA,
    3. Banks WA,
    4. Johnson SN,
    5. Yamada KA,
    6. Xu L
    (2010) A physiological role for amyloid-β protein: enhancement of learning and memory. J Alzheimers Dis 19:441–449. doi:10.3233/JAD-2010-1230 pmid:19749407
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Nalivaeva NN,
    2. Belyaev ND,
    3. Zhuravin IA,
    4. Turner AJ
    (2012) The Alzheimer's amyloid-degrading peptidase, neprilysin: can we control it? Int J Alzheimers Dis 2012:383796. doi:10.1155/2012/383796 pmid:22900228
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Niccoli T,
    2. Cabecinha M,
    3. Tillmann A,
    4. Kerr F,
    5. Wong CT,
    6. Cardenes D,
    7. Vincent AJ,
    8. Bettedi L,
    9. Li L,
    10. Grönke S,
    11. Dols J,
    12. Partridge L
    (2016) Increased glucose transport into neurons rescues Aβ toxicity in Drosophila. Curr Biol 26:2291–2300. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2016.07.017 pmid:27524482
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. O'Brien RJ,
    2. Wong PC
    (2011) Amyloid precursor protein processing and Alzheimer's disease. Annu Rev Neurosci 34:185–204. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113613 pmid:21456963
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Panz M,
    2. Vitos-Faleato J,
    3. Jendretzki A,
    4. Heinisch JJ,
    5. Paululat A,
    6. Meyer H
    (2012) A novel role for the noncatalytic intracellular domain of Neprilysins in muscle physiology. Biol Cell 104:553–568. doi:10.1111/boc.201100069 pmid:22583317
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Park MH,
    2. Lee JK,
    3. Choi S,
    4. Ahn J,
    5. Jin HK,
    6. Park JS,
    7. Bae JS
    (2013) Recombinant soluble neprilysin reduces amyloid-β accumulation and improves memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease mice. Brain Res 1529:113–124. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2013.05.045 pmid:23831521
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Pascual A,
    2. Preat T
    (2001) Localization of long-term memory within the Drosophila mushroom body. Science 294:1115–1117. doi:10.1126/science.1064200 pmid:11691997
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    1. Pitman JL,
    2. Huetteroth W,
    3. Burke CJ,
    4. Krashes MJ,
    5. Lai SL,
    6. Lee T,
    7. Waddell S
    (2011) A pair of inhibitory neurons are required to sustain labile memory in the Drosophila mushroom body. Curr Biol 21:855–861. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.069 pmid:21530258
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Plant LD,
    2. Boyle JP,
    3. Smith IF,
    4. Peers C,
    5. Pearson HA
    (2003) The production of amyloid β peptide is a critical requirement for the viability of central neurons. J Neurosci 23:5531–5535. pmid:12843253
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  47. ↵
    1. Poeck B,
    2. Strauss R,
    3. Kretzschmar D
    (2012) Analysis of amyloid precursor protein function in Drosophila melanogaster. Exp Brain Res 217:413–421. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2860-3 pmid:21912928
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Poirier R,
    2. Wolfer DP,
    3. Welzl H,
    4. Tracy J,
    5. Galsworthy MJ,
    6. Nitsch RM,
    7. Mohajeri MH
    (2006) Neuronal neprilysin overexpression is associated with attenuation of Aβ-related spatial memory deficit. Neurobiol Dis 24:475–483. doi:10.1016/j.nbd.2006.08.003 pmid:17008108
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Puzzo D,
    2. Privitera L,
    3. Leznik E,
    4. Fà M,
    5. Staniszewski A,
    6. Palmeri A,
    7. Arancio O
    (2008) Picomolar amyloid-β positively modulates synaptic plasticity and memory in hippocampus. J Neurosci 28:14537–14545. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2692-08.2008 pmid:19118188
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  50. ↵
    1. Puzzo D,
    2. Privitera L,
    3. Fà M,
    4. Staniszewski A,
    5. Hashimoto G,
    6. Aziz F,
    7. Sakurai M,
    8. Ribe EM,
    9. Troy CM,
    10. Mercken M,
    11. Jung SS,
    12. Palmeri A,
    13. Arancio O
    (2011) Endogenous amyloid-β is necessary for hippocampal synaptic plasticity and memory. Ann Neurol 69:819–830. doi:10.1002/ana.22313 pmid:21472769
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Querfurth HW,
    2. LaFerla FM
    (2010) Alzheimer's disease. N Engl J Med 362:329–344. doi:10.1056/NEJMra0909142 pmid:20107219
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Reiter LT,
    2. Potocki L,
    3. Chien S,
    4. Gribskov M,
    5. Bier E
    (2001) A systematic analysis of human disease-associated gene sequences in Drosophila melanogaster. Genome Res 11:1114–1125. doi:10.1101/gr.169101 pmid:11381037
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  53. ↵
    1. Rooke J,
    2. Pan D,
    3. Xu T,
    4. Rubin GM
    (1996) KUZ, a conserved metalloprotease-disintegrin protein with two roles in Drosophila neurogenesis. Science 273:1227–1231. doi:10.1126/science.273.5279.1227 pmid:8703057
    OpenUrlAbstract
  54. ↵
    1. Rosen DR,
    2. Martin-Morris L,
    3. Luo LQ,
    4. White K
    (1989) A Drosophila gene encoding a protein resembling the human β-amyloid protein precursor. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 86:2478–2482. doi:10.1073/pnas.86.7.2478 pmid:2494667
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  55. ↵
    1. Sitnik JL,
    2. Francis C,
    3. Hens K,
    4. Huybrechts R,
    5. Wolfner MF,
    6. Callaerts P
    (2014) Neprilysins: an evolutionarily conserved family of metalloproteases that play important roles in reproduction in Drosophila. Genetics 196:781–797. doi:10.1534/genetics.113.160945 pmid:24395329
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. ↵
    1. Sofola-Adesakin O,
    2. Khericha M,
    3. Snoeren I,
    4. Tsuda L,
    5. Partridge L
    (2016) pGluAβ increases accumulation of Aβ in vivo and exacerbates its toxicity. Acta Neuropathol Commun 4:109. doi:10.1186/s40478-016-0380-x pmid:27717375
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Spencer B,
    2. Marr RA,
    3. Rockenstein E,
    4. Crews L,
    5. Adame A,
    6. Potkar R,
    7. Patrick C,
    8. Gage FH,
    9. Verma IM,
    10. Masliah E
    (2008) Long-term neprilysin gene transfer is associated with reduced levels of intracellular Abeta and behavioral improvement in APP transgenic mice. BMC Neurosci 9:109–112. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-9-109 pmid:19014502
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Torroja L,
    2. Chu H,
    3. Kotovsky I,
    4. White K
    (1999) Neuronal overexpression of APPL, the Drosophila homologue of the amyloid precursor protein (APP), disrupts axonal transport. Curr Biol 9:489–492. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(99)80215-2 pmid:10322116
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Tully T,
    2. Quinn WG
    (1985) Classical conditioning and retention in normal and mutant Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol A 157:263–277. doi:10.1007/BF01350033 pmid:3939242
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Tully T,
    2. Preat T,
    3. Boynton SC,
    4. Del Vecchio M
    (1994) Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in Drosophila. Cell 79:35–47. doi:10.1016/0092-8674(94)90398-0 pmid:7923375
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Turner AJ
    (2003) Exploring the structure and function of zinc metallopeptidases: old enzymes and new discoveries. Biochem Soc Trans 31:723–727. doi:10.1042/bst0310723 pmid:12773192
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  62. ↵
    1. Turner AJ,
    2. Isaac RE,
    3. Coates D
    (2001) The neprilysin (NEP) family of zinc metalloendopeptidases: genomics and function. Bioessays 23:261–269. doi:10.1002/1521-1878(200103)23:3%3C261::AID-BIES1036%3E3.0.CO%3B2-K pmid:11223883
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Turner PR,
    2. O'Connor K,
    3. Tate WP,
    4. Abraham WC
    (2003) Roles of amyloid precursor protein and its fragments in regulating neural activity, plasticity and memory. Prog Neurobiol 70:1–32. doi:10.1016/S0301-0082(03)00089-3 pmid:12927332
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Turrel O,
    2. Lampin-Saint-Amaux A,
    3. Preat T,
    4. Goguel V
    (2016) Drosophila neprilysins are involved in middle-term and long-term memory. J Neurosci 36:9535–9546. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3730-15.2016 pmid:27629706
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  65. ↵
    1. Wang S,
    2. Wang R,
    3. Chen L,
    4. Bennett DA,
    5. Dickson DW,
    6. Wang DS
    (2010) Expression and functional profiling of neprilysin, insulin-degrading enzyme, and endothelin-converting enzyme in prospectively studied elderly and Alzheimer's brain. J Neurochem 115:47–57. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.06899.x pmid:20663017
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Wang Z,
    2. Wang B,
    3. Yang L,
    4. Guo Q,
    5. Aithmitti N,
    6. Songyang Z,
    7. Zheng H
    (2009) Presynaptic and postsynaptic interaction of the amyloid precursor protein promotes peripheral and central synaptogenesis. J Neurosci 29:10788–10801. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2132-09.2009 pmid:19726636
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  67. ↵
    1. Yamazaki T,
    2. Selkoe DJ,
    3. Koo EH
    (1995) Trafficking of cell surface β-amyloid precursor protein: retrograde and transcytotic transport in cultured neurons. J Cell Biol 129:431–442. doi:10.1083/jcb.129.2.431 pmid:7721945
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  68. ↵
    1. Yang MY,
    2. Armstrong JD,
    3. Vilinsky I,
    4. Strausfeld NJ,
    5. Kaiser K
    (1995) Subdivision of the Drosophila mushroom bodies by enhancer-trap expression patterns. Neuron 15:45–54. doi:10.1016/0896-6273(95)90063-2 pmid:7619529
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Yasojima K,
    2. McGeer EG,
    3. McGeer PL
    (2001) Relationship between beta amyloid peptide generating molecules and neprilysin in Alzheimer disease and normal brain. Brain Res 919:115–121. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(01)03008-6 pmid:11689168
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Zheng H,
    2. Koo EH
    (2011) Biology and pathophysiology of the amyloid precursor protein. Mol Neurodegener 6:27. doi:10.1186/1750-1326-6-27 pmid:21527012
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Zhou L,
    2. Wei C,
    3. Huang W,
    4. Bennett DA,
    5. Dickson DW,
    6. Wang R
    (2013) Distinct subcellular patterns of neprilysin protein and activity in the brains of Alzheimer's disease patients, transgenic mice and cultured human neuronal cells. Am J Transl Res 5:608–621. pmid:24093058
    OpenUrlPubMed
View Abstract
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 37 (43)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 37, Issue 43
25 Oct 2017
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Drosophila Neprilysin 1 Rescues Memory Deficits Caused by Amyloid-β Peptide
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Drosophila Neprilysin 1 Rescues Memory Deficits Caused by Amyloid-β Peptide
Oriane Turrel, Valérie Goguel, Thomas Preat
Journal of Neuroscience 25 October 2017, 37 (43) 10334-10345; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1634-17.2017

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Drosophila Neprilysin 1 Rescues Memory Deficits Caused by Amyloid-β Peptide
Oriane Turrel, Valérie Goguel, Thomas Preat
Journal of Neuroscience 25 October 2017, 37 (43) 10334-10345; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1634-17.2017
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • amyloid peptide
  • dorsal paired medial neurons
  • Drosophila
  • learning and memory
  • neprilysin
  • olfactory conditioning

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Functional Roles of Gastrin-Releasing Peptide-Producing Neurons in the Suprachiasmatic Nucleus: Insights into Photic Entrainment and Circadian Regulation
  • Brain Topological Changes in Subjective Cognitive Decline and Associations with Amyloid Stages
  • The Functional Anatomy of Nociception: Effective Connectivity in Chronic Pain and Placebo Response
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Attention Alters Population Spatial Frequency Tuning
  • Complex Impact of Stimulus Envelope on Motor Synchronization to Sound
  • The Molecular Substrates of Second-Order Conditioned Fear in the Basolateral Amygdala Complex
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.