Abstract
Norepinephrine (NE) has been shown to influence sensory, and specifically olfactory processing at the behavioral and physiological levels, potentially by regulating signal-to-noise ratio (S/N). The present study is the first to look at NE modulation of olfactory bulb (OB) in regards to S/N in vivo. We show, in male rats, that locus ceruleus stimulation and pharmacological infusions of NE into the OB modulate both spontaneous and odor-evoked neural responses. NE in the OB generated a non-monotonic dose–response relationship, suppressing mitral cell activity at high and low, but not intermediate, NE levels. We propose that NE enhances odor responses not through direct potentiation of the afferent signal per se, but rather by reducing the intrinsic noise of the system. This has important implications for the ways in which an animal interacts with its olfactory environment, particularly as the animal shifts from a relaxed to an alert behavioral state.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Sensory perception can be modulated by behavioral states such as hunger, fear, stress, or a change in environmental context. Behavioral state often affects neural processing via the release of circulating neurochemicals such as hormones or neuromodulators. We here show that the neuromodulator norepinephrine modulates olfactory bulb spontaneous activity and odor responses so as to generate an increased signal-to-noise ratio at the output of the olfactory bulb. Our results help interpret and improve existing ideas for neural network mechanisms underlying behaviorally observed improvements in near-threshold odor detection and discrimination.
Introduction
Norepinephrine (NE) has been implicated in modulating signal-to-noise ratio (S/N) in a variety of sensory systems (Waterhouse et al., 1990, 1998; McLean and Waterhouse, 1994; Aston-Jones et al., 2000; Devilbiss and Waterhouse, 2004; Devilbiss et al., 2006; Hirata et al., 2006; Moxon et al., 2007). The olfactory bulb (OB) is the first central network in the olfactory sensory pathway and is heavily innervated by noradrenergic fibers from the locus ceruleus (LC; for review, see Aston-Jones et al., 2000; Sara, 2009).
Several lines of evidence suggest that NE may influence S/N in the OB. Behavioral experiments show that NE in the OB modulates the ability to detect and discriminate odors at concentrations below normal thresholds (Escanilla et al., 2010, 2012; Linster et al., 2011). Data from brain slice experiments show that NE leads to a simultaneous reduction in mitral cell spontaneous activity and an increase in mitral cell responsiveness to olfactory nerve stimulation (Hayar et al., 2001; Nai et al., 2010, 2012). When implemented in a computational model of the OB, these cellular modulations produce a net effect of increasing S/N (Linster et al., 2011; de Almeida et al., 2015), thereby implying the presence of a neural mechanism underlying this behavioral reduction of odor thresholds. Activation of NE receptors also influences various forms of short- and long-term plasticity in OB synapses, as evidenced by olfactory learning in neonatal (for review, see Moriceau et al., 2009) and adult rodents (Doucette et al., 2007; Mandairon et al., 2008; Shea et al., 2008; Linster et al., 2011; Devore and Linster, 2012; Manella et al., 2013). Although the brain slice data provided mechanisms to propose a computational model of S/N regulation, we here present the first in vivo evidence for enhancement of S/N at the OB output.
Within the glomerular layer of the OB, olfactory sensory neurons form synapses onto OB output neurons [mitral/tufted (MT) cells] as well as local interneurons (periglomerular and external tufted cells). Glomerular layer interneurons modulate MT cell activity, which is then further processed in a deeper layer of the OB, primarily by lateral inhibitory interactions with granule cell interneurons. Computations performed by bulbar neural networks have been described in detail (Cleland and Linster, 2005; Tan et al., 2010; Cleland, 2010). LC projections have been shown to target both the glomerular and deeper layers of the OB, with layer-specific distributions of noradrenergic receptor subtypes. Most previous analyses of NE effects in the OB have focused on NE modulation of mitral and granule cells (Linster et al., 2011; de Almeida et al., 2015; Li et al., 2015). However, a recent experiment by Eckmeier and Shea (2014) used imaging of the surface of the dorsal OB to show a direct and persistent reduction of sensory neuron input in response to LC stimulation. This effect was blocked by topical application of α- and β-adrenergic receptor antagonists, suggesting a functional effect of NE in the glomerular layer as well.
To relate these behavioral and slice physiological experiments to an in vivo model, we recorded single units from the OB MT cell layer, pairing odor stimulation with either local infusions of noradrenergic agents or electrical stimulation of the LC. We found that NE modulates mitral cell spontaneous activity, and increased both the number and amplitude of significant odor responses. An overall increase in S/N was observed in response to LC stimulation; however, rather than arising from a combination of inhibition and excitation of mitral cells as previously suggested (Escanilla et al., 2010), the effect depended primarily on the suppression of spontaneous activity. Pharmacological manipulations further demonstrated that the effects of LC stimulation are at least partially mediated within the OB. These results, in conjunction with data from other laboratories (Eckmeier and Shea, 2014) make a clear case for how perceptual S/N modulation can occur at the first level of olfactory processing.
Materials and Methods
Animals
Adult male Long–Evans hooded rats (RRID:RGD_1302656; 275–350 g) were obtained from Charles River Laboratories. Ninety-eight rats total were used, 35 in Experiment 1, 37 in Experiment 2, and 26 in Experiment 3. Rats were double-housed and maintained with Purina rat chow and water ad libitum on a 12 h light/dark cycle. Experiments were performed during the dark cycle. All procedures followed NIH guidelines under the supervision of the Cornell University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and an approved animal care protocol.
Surgery
Anesthesia was induced with 5% isoflurane in a stream of oxygen (1 L/min) through a Narishige anesthesia mask. Rats were then placed into a stereotaxic apparatus (Narishige Scientific Instruments) and maintained with 1–2% isoflurane in oxygen throughout the experiment. Viscous lidocaine (2%) was used as a local anesthetic before exposure of the skull via scalpel incision. A large craniotomy was made over the OB. For stimulation experiments, a craniotomy was also drilled over the locus ceruleus (AP 2.0 mm posterior from lambda and ML ±1.2 mm from the midline). A piezoelectric monitor was strapped around the animal's chest to monitor breathing cycles. This respiration signal was used to trigger odor delivery upon inspiration for experiments requiring odor delivery.
Electrophysiological recordings
One or two 2 MΩ tungsten, parylene-C-insulated, monopolar recording probes (A-M Systems) were used to record from putative MT cells of the OB. Electrodes were lowered from the surface of the OB using a stereotaxic micromanipulator (David Kopf Instruments). The MT cell layer was located by finding a layer exhibiting respiration-locked spiking activity. Units (5000× amplification, filtered from 600 Hz to 6 kHz, 20 kHz sampling rate) were recorded; data were digitized and transferred to the computer using a CED Power1401 and Spike2 software (Cambridge Electronic Design).
Electrical stimulation of the locus ceruleus
A 1 MΩ tungsten, parylene-C-insulated, monopolar stimulation probe (A-M Systems) was placed in the LC (AP 2.0 mm posterior from lambda and ML ±1.2 mm from the midline) under stereotaxic guidance. The LC was located by adjusting the electrode angle from vertical to ∼12° posterior, and lowering the electrode while recording under unit-recording amplifier settings. The LC was identified by finding characteristic broad and slow (0–2 Hz) tonic activity and a phasic response to tail pinch (Bouret and Sara, 2004). DV coordinates ranged from ∼4.1 to 6 mm ventral at the above angle from the surface of the brain (variance due to electrode/head angle). Once LC recording was verified, the electrode was connected to a constant current stimulus isolation unit (Grass model PSIU6) controlled by a Grass S88 stimulator (Grass Technologies). During the experiment, 1, 2, 3, or 5 Hz (100 μs, 100 μA) pulses were delivered to the LC while unit activity was recorded from the OB.
Drugs
Noradrenergic drugs were dissolved into 0.9% saline solution and then infused directly into the main OB (2.5 mm depth from the surface of the brain; 6 μl, delivered at 1 μl/min) using a 33 gauge cannula attached to an infusion pump. l-(-)-norepinephrine (+) bitartrate salt monohydrate was diluted to 1, 10, 100, or 1000 μm concentrations, aliquoted, and frozen. Drug mixtures also were made using 1000 μm NE mixed with specific NE receptor antagonists: the α1 adrenergic receptor antagonist prazosin hydrochloride (1 mm; Sigma-Aldrich), the α2 adrenergic receptor antagonist yohimbine hydrochloride (2 mm; MP Biomedical), and the β adrenergic receptor antagonist alprenolol hydrochloride (120 mm; Sigma-Aldrich). Dosages of NE and antagonists were based on previous studies from our laboratory, which showed significant perceptual effects of these drugs (Mandairon et al., 2008; Escanilla et al., 2010, 2012).
Odor presentations
An odor mixture was used to increase the probability of finding odor-responsive MT cells. Within this odor mixture, each odorant was first diluted to approximate a vapor-phase partial pressure of 1 Pa in mineral oil (Table 1), then added to the mixture, and the mixture was serially diluted to emit 0.1 and 0.01 Pa. To deliver the odor, a custom built olfactometer was used. Clean, charcoal-filtered, humidified air was pumped through the mask (rat anesthesia mask, Narishige Scientific Instruments) constantly at 1 L/min. A second constant airstream was routed through a selected bottle of odorized mineral oil. For odor delivery, the airstream in the nose mask was switched from the clean airstream to the odorized airstream; this strategy prevented a mechanical odor-delivery stimulus artifact and ensured that the odor delivered was at a stable concentration. Odor presentations lasted 2 s, and were separated by at least 30 s.
Experiment 1: effect of LC stimulation on MT spontaneous activity
In this experiment, MT cell spontaneous activity was recorded during 180 s trials. From 60 to 120 s, LC was electrically stimulated at a particular rate (see above; Fig. 1A). In some animals, more than one stimulation frequency was tested, in which case the above trials were separated by at least 30 min. In control experiments, either saline or a mixture of three NE antagonists (α1, α2, and β receptor antagonists) was infused into the OB 20 min before the beginning of the recording session.
Experiment 2: effect of local NE infusions on MT spontaneous activity.
Baseline MT cell recordings (180 s) were obtained, followed by an infusion of either NE or a combination of NE and two (of the three) receptor antagonists (concentrations above). Twenty minutes after the start of infusion, six 3 min recording trials were obtained at 10 min intertrial intervals (Fig. 1B) to see the progression of the drug effects. The 20 min wait after infusions mimics our established behavioral protocols (Mandairon et al., 2008; Escanilla et al., 2010, 2012). Due to drug being present for an indeterminate amount of time, this was the last/only experiment run on a given animal.
Experiment 3: effect of LC stimulation of MT odor responses
In this experiment, each trial block (Fig. 1C) consisted of three sets of six odor exposures at each of the following concentrations consecutively: 0.01, 0.1, and 1 Pa. For each odor trial within this block, the odor exposure lasted 2 s and inter-odor intervals were 30 s. Before LC stimulation, one trial block was used to determine baseline odor responsiveness. Next, there was one trial block where each odor trial was paired with a 5 s of 5 Hz LC stimulation commencing at 2.5 s before the onset of odor exposure and lasting until 0.5 s after the end of odor exposure, similar to previous studies (Shea et al., 2008; Eckmeier and Shea, 2014). Finally, two poststimulation trial blocks followed at 30 and 60 min after the start of the LC stimulation trial block (Fig. 1C).
Histological verification of electrode placement
At the end of successful experiments, a large positive DC electrical current (10 s duration, 15 mA) was run through each electrode (recording in the OB and stimulation in the LC) to generate an electrolytic lesion (Fig. 1D). Rats then were perfused with 10% neutral buffered formalin. Brains were extracted and later sectioned at 40 μm and stained with cresyl violet to stain for cell bodies. Locations of the electrodes were verified, and if recording electrodes in the main OB were not in the mitral cell layer or if stimulating electrodes were not in the LC, data were excluded from future analysis.
Experimental design and analysis
Spike sorting
Single units were extracted post hoc using Spike2 spike sorting software. Spike templates were derived from raw data, after which appropriate templates were selected and refined. To refine these waveforms, principal components analysis was used to group features of the waveforms onto a three-dimensional plot. Interspike interval distributions were used to assess the quality of each single unit. Multiunit and questionable single unit data were excluded from analysis.
Data analysis
Spontaneous activity.
After spike extraction, we calculated spontaneous activity by averaging the firing rates of individual neurons during 60 s epochs before, during, and after LC stimulation or NE infusion. To test for the overall effect of LC stimulation or NE infusion on spontaneous activity, we used a mixed model ANOVA with average firing rate of all recorded MT cells as the dependent variable, recording epoch (pre-, during, and post-manipulation) as a within-subjects factor and stimulation frequency or NE dosage as between-subject factors, α was set to 0.05. This was followed by ANOVA testing for effects of recording epoch at each stimulation frequency individually with α set to 0.0125 to account for multiple comparisons, or for individual NE dosages with α set to 0.01 to account for multiple comparisons. When testing the effect of local NE receptor blockade on LC stimulation we used an ANOVA to test for effect of recording epoch after saline or antagonist infusions (α was set to 0.025 to account for multiple comparisons). To test for the role of individual NE receptors we used an ANOVA with drug (saline, α1 + α2 blocker, α1 + β blocker, α2 + β blocker) as between-subjects factors and recording epoch (pre and post) as within-subjects factor with α set to 0.05. To assess whether activity during pre- and post-recording was different for individual drug groups this was followed by individual ANOVAs for each drug group with α set to 0.0125 to account for multiple comparisons. To assess how many individual neurons exhibited significant modulation (change in firing rate) in response to LC stimulation or NE infusion we compared pre- and post-manipulation firing rates using average rate over 1 s bins as a dependent variable for individual neurons using a t test (α = 0.05). The degree of modulation [modulation index (MI)] of individual neurons was calculated as the difference between during and before (or after and before) divided by the sum. To test whether the degree of modulation was correlated with initial firing rates, we calculated Pearson's r between modulation indices and prestimulation neuronal firing rates, with α set to 0.05.
Odor responses.
After spike extraction, we first tested for significant odor responses in individual cells by comparing the firing rate pre-odor application to that post-odor application (post-epoch started at odor delivery) over six trials (Student's t test, α = 0.05) for a given neuron. The degree of responsiveness to odorants was calculated as the difference between pre-odor and post-odor rates divided by the sum [odor response index (RI)]. To test for the effect of LC stimulation on odor responses we used a repeated-measures ANOVA with the odor RI under control and LC stimulation conditions as a within-subjects factor and odor concentration as a between-subjects factor, with α set to 0.05. S/N was calculated as the ratio between firing rates during odor application and the sum of firing rates during odor application and during spontaneous activity (Hasselmo et al., 1997). Modulation of S/N by LC stimulation was assessed using a mixed model ANOVA with S/N under control or LC stimulation conditions as within-subjects factor and odor concentration as between-subjects factor (α = 0.05). Correlations between spontaneous rates and modulation index, spontaneous activity and odor response modulation indices, and odor responses under both conditions were calculated using Pearson's r with α set to 0.05.
Results
Experiment 1
LC stimulation modulates spontaneous activity of MT cells in vivo
We recorded data from 123 cells in 35 rats; of these recordings, 63 stable MT units were used to test the effect of LC stimulation on spontaneous firing and 27 U were used to test whether the observed effect depended on bulbar NE receptors. Baseline spontaneous activity varied between 0.7 and 80 Hz, with a mean of 19 ± 1.4 Hz.
LC stimulation affected spontaneous activity of individual MT cells: 55 MT cells (87% of cells) significantly changed firing rate in response to LC stimulation at any frequency, with the highest numbers of significant changes elicited by 5 Hz stimulation (74% of cells recorded during 5 Hz stimulation; Fig. 2A,B). A repeated-measures analysis yielded a significant effect of the interaction between recording period (pre-, during, and post-LC stimulation) and LC stimulation frequency on overall rate (F(2,186) = 3.187; p = 0.001); both 1 Hz (F(2,19) = 6.320; p = 0.008) and 5 Hz (F(2,50) = 6.008; p = 0.005). Although we observed both increased and decreased rates in response to LC stimulation, the average effect of LC stimulation on spontaneous firing was a decrease: there was a mean rate of 17.6 ± 1.4 Hz during prestimulation, 16.53 ± 1.3 Hz during and 15.3 ± 1.27 Hz post-LC stimulation. Figure 2A shows examples of spontaneous firing rate modulations with the proportion of modulated epochs after each stimulation frequency shown in Figure 2B. Figure 2,C and D, shows a breakdown of modulation indices during poststimulation epochs (MI) by stimulation frequency. Overall the largest changes in firing rate were seen at 5 Hz stimulation, with both the most cells modulated and the strongest overall modulation (average of −0.355 and 0.244 MI for decrease and increase, respectively). Although significantly modulated cells were found at each stimulation frequency both during and post-LC stimulation, when we directly compared overall firing rates during and after LC stimulation to baseline using all recorded cells, significant changes were observed at 1, 3, and 5 Hz LC stimulation during poststimulation epochs only (p = 0.008, p = 0.003, and p = 0.007, respectively, using pairwise comparisons). Therefore, subsequent analyses will be performed on the period after LC stimulation (post-epoch) only unless otherwise noted.
Spontaneous rate modulation is proportional to initial firing rate
We then asked whether the magnitude of modulation by LC stimulation depended on the initial spontaneous activity of the neurons. Analyzing poststimulation epochs at all stimulation frequencies, we found that modulation by LC stimulation was significantly anticorrelated with initial firing rates, with low rates modulated up and high rates modulated down (Pearsons' r = −0.293; p = 0.004; Fig. 3). Overall these results show that initial firing rates of neurons help determine how LC stimulation influences spontaneous firing rate.
Effect of LC stimulation depends on activation of bulbar NE receptors
To verify that LC stimulation was affecting MT cells via direct action in the main OB, we infused a mixture of α1, α2, and β NE antagonists or saline as control into the OB before LC stimulation (see Materials and Methods). We stimulated the LC at 5 Hz, because we found the most dramatic changes in MT cells when stimulating at this frequency (Fig. 2B). We recorded from 34 MT cells total and confirmed that 5 Hz LC stimulation significantly modulated overall MT cell spontaneous activity [effect of epoch (pre and post): F(1,21) = 6.083; p = 0.022], whereas this effect was not observed in the presence of the NE antagonist mixture (F(1,11) = 0.222; p = 0.646). Figure 4B shows the distribution of modulation indices in the presence of the antagonist mixture (compare to Fig. 2Div). These results indicate that observed effects of LC stimulation are at least partially mediated by activation of OB receptors.
Experiment 2
Local NE infusions modulate spontaneous activity in a dose-dependent manner
A parallel set of recordings tested how local infusions of NE in the OB compared with LC stimulation. Baseline spontaneous activity was first recorded for a 180 s interval, followed by local infusion of 1, 10, 100, or 1000 μm NE followed by a series of 180 s recordings 10, 20, 30, 40, 50, and 60 min after the end of the drug infusion (Fig. 1B). We recorded from 50 MT cells in 37 rats (Fig. 5A shows an example recording). Cells displayed spontaneous activities ranging between 1.2 and 45 Hz with an average of 10.6 ± 1.3 Hz. In the following analyses, only data from the 10 and 20 min post-drug recording epochs were included because at these time points stable recordings were obtained for all 50 MT cells we recorded. A repeated-measures ANOVA with average firing rate as dependent variable, drug (0, 1, 10, 100, and 1000 μm NE) as between-subjects factor and recording epoch (pre-, 10 min after, and 20 min post-) as within-subjects factor yielded a significant effect of recording epoch (F(2,44) = 11.380; p = 0.001) as well as a significant interaction between recording epoch and drug (F(8,90) = 2.052; p = 0.049). Interestingly, significant modulation of spontaneous activity at the 10 min and 20 min post-drug recording epochs was observed at the two lowest (1 and 10 μm) and the highest (1000 μm) concentrations of NE. In contrast to LC stimulation, local infusions of NE into the OB produced almost exclusively reductions in spontaneous activity (Fig. 5B,D).
Blockade of α1 adrenergic receptors suppresses modulation of spontaneous activity in response to NE
To test for the role of individual NE receptor types in modulation of spontaneous activity, we recorded data from an additional 33 putative MT cells. In these recordings, 1000 μm NE was infused together with antagonists to two of three NE receptor types (see Materials and Methods). Data obtained with infusions of 1000 μm NE reported above were reused in this analysis. ANOVA with drug (1000 μm NE, 1000 μm NE + α1, α2 antagonist, 1000 μm NE + α1, β antagonist, 1000 μm NE + α2, β antagonist) as between-subjects factor and recording epoch (pre-drug and 10 min post-drug) as within-subjects factor yielded a significant effect of recording epoch (F(1,35) = 5.6; p = 0.024) as well as a significant interaction between drug and recording epoch (F(3,35) = 4.909; p = 0.006). Interestingly, the significant effect of recording epoch was present for rats infused with 1000 μm NE (F(1,7) = 9.078; p = 0.012) and 1000 μm NE + α2 + β antagonists (F(1,9) = 12.469); p = 0.011, whereas no significant effect of epoch was found when α1+β or α1 + α2 receptors were blocked (F(1,11) = 1.017; p = 0.335 and F(1,8) = 1.201; p = 0.305). These data suggest that α1 receptor activation is necessary to mediate the modulation of MT cell spontaneous activity under these conditions.
Experiment 3
LC stimulation enhances odor responsiveness in MT cells
Experiment 3 tested NE modulation of odor-evoked responses of MT cells in the OB by LC stimulation (Fig. 1C). Based on our results from Experiment 1, we used 5 s of 5 Hz LC stimulation. To enhance chances to record from odor responsive mitral cells as well as to test for possible odor concentration-dependent effects, we used three concentrations (corresponding to vapor-phase partial pressures of 1, 0.1, and 0.01 Pa) of a six-component odor mixture (Table 1). Note that we used low odor concentrations compared with experiments from other groups based on the idea that LC stimulation would enhance S/N ratio in mitral cells when responding to close to threshold concentrations (Escanilla et al., 2010, 2012; Linster et al., 2011); odor concentrations used here are behaviorally relevant (Cleland et al., 2002).
We recorded from 54 MT cells in 26 rats. Of these 54 cells, 31 presented with stable recordings long enough to complete recordings with at least two of three odor concentrations under control and experimental conditions. Eighteen of these 31 cells (58%) displayed a significant response to odor stimulation with at least one of the three odor concentrations (comparison of pre-odor versus during odor activity; see Materials and Methods). When odors were applied during the block with 5 Hz LC stimulation, 24 of 31 cells (77%) significantly changed firing rate to at least one of the three odor concentrations (Fig. 6B). A repeated-measures ANOVA with odor RI (measure for amplitude of odor responses; see Materials and Methods) as the dependent variable, control or LC stimulation as the within-subjects factor and odor concentration as the between-subjects factor showed a significant effect of LC stimulation on odor response index (F(1,83) = 7.264; p = 0.009) but no interaction with odor concentration (F(2,83) = 0.820; p = 0.44, Wilk's lambda). Overall odor response amplitudes were significantly greater under LC stimulation conditions when all neural responses (odor response – pre-odor activity; including nonsignificant odor responders) were considered (F(1,83) = 7.264; p = 0.009=; Fig. 6C).
As reported in previous experiments, MT cells responded with an increased or decreased firing rate to odor stimulation (Fig. 7A). Overall, the proportion of significant excitatory responses went up under LC stimulation conditions whereas the proportion of significant inhibitory responses went down. Figure 7B shows the distribution of odor RIs under control (Fig. 7Bi) and LC stimulation (Fig. 7Bii) conditions.
LC stimulation increases S/N during in response to odor stimulation
Previous behavioral results strongly suggest a role of bulbar NE for modulation of S/N in odor processing (Escanilla et al., 2010). We therefore used a classical definition for S/N in sensory networks with S/N = odor-response/spont + odor-response (Hasselmo et al., 1997). Analyzing data from all recorded odor stimulations (including nonsignificant responses), we found strong effects of LC stimulation on S/N (F(1,82) = 5.063; p = 0.028 with Wilk's lambda), with significant effects of LC stimulation for 1 Pa (F(1,23) = 4.499; p = 0.045) and 0.1 Pa (F(1,30) = 6.552; p = 0.16) odor concentrations (Fig. 8).
LC stimulation enhances odor responses through modulation of spontaneous activity
LC stimulation affected the modulation of the spontaneous activity recorded 2 s before each odor stimulus in the same manner than that recorded with longer time intervals reported above (Fig. 9A; Pearson's r = −302; p = 0.005). Interestingly, the degree of LC modulation (MI) during odor stimulation was predictable from that during spontaneous activity (Pearson's r = 0.872; p = 0.001); this strongly suggests that there was no additional modulation of the odor response (Fig. 9B). As shown in Figure 9C, the odor response amplitude under LC stimulation conditions was consistently higher than that under control conditions. Under LC stimulation conditions, the magnitude of odor responses was significantly anticorrelated with spontaneous activity (r = −0.260; p = 0.016 with Pearson's r), suggesting the low firing cells were more likely to respond with increased rates to odor stimulation and high firing cells were more likely to be inhibited. This relationship was not significant under control conditions (r = −0.44; p = 0.688; Fig. 9D). Together these data suggest that it was likely that because of overall decreased spontaneous activity by LC stimulation, odor responses were more likely to be detected without changing in absolute firing rate.
Discussion
The present experiments showed that noradrenaline modulates OB physiology in anesthetized rats. Electrical stimulation of the LC as well as NE pharmacological manipulations produced changes in spontaneous activity of MT cells in the OB. In experiments with electrical stimulation of the LC, the degree of spontaneous activity modulation was dependent on both LC stimulation frequency and MT cell baseline firing rate. Pharmacological controls using antagonists for NE receptors within the OB showed that activation of local NE receptors was necessary to see the effects of LC stimulation. Interestingly, and likely due to very little LC activity in anesthetized animals, antagonists by themselves did not have an effect on spontaneous activity.
We found a non-monotonic dose–response curve qualitatively similar between LC stimulation and direct infusion of NE on MT cell activity (Fig. 10). Medium dosages showed a much weaker effect than lower or higher dosages. We used commonly used stimulation frequencies and NE dosages, and as far we know ours is a first direct comparison between these two means of activating NE receptors. We showed that high dosages of NE (1000 μl) correlate with the effect to 5 Hz tonic LC stimulation. This type of non-monotonic dose–response curve has been observed in many physiological and behavioral studies concerning manipulations of LC, NE and stress on behavior and physiological measures alike (Joëls et al., 2006; Sandi and Pinelo-Nava, 2007).
Importantly, this complex dose-dependent relationship of NE has also been shown in olfactory behavioral and brain slice physiology experiments (Nai et al., 2009; Escanilla et al., 2010). The non-monotonic response can possibly be attributed to the difference in receptor affinities and efficacies of adrenergic receptor subtypes locally in the OB (Escanilla et al., 2010, 2012; Linster et al., 2011). In these studies, differential receptor activation led to different ratios of excitation and inhibition in the circuit. We showed that spontaneous activity of MT cells was suppressed at low and high, but not intermediate LC stimulation frequencies or NE dosages, and thus there is likely a dynamic relationship between type(s) and degree of receptor activation. At high dosages of NE (1000 μm, for instance), we showed that suppression in spontaneous activity was dependent upon activation of α1 receptors in the OB (Fig. 5). We also observed significant increases of some MT cell firing rates with intermediate LC stimulation frequencies but not with intermediate NE dosages. This disparity could be attributed to differences in local concentrations of NE in response to LC stimulation (McLean, 1989) compared with whole-bulb NE concentration distributions in response to bulbar infusions. Alternatively or additionally, the change could have been mediated by extrabulbar effects of NE that feed into the OB in the case of LC stimulation.
Slice data suggest dual effects of α1 receptor modulation in the OB: activation of these receptors depolarizes granule cells and therefore enhances inhibitory tone onto mitral cells, as well as increases excitability in mitral cells directly. Computational modeling suggests that the above effects could act to enhance S/N by suppressing mitral cell spontaneous activity via enhancement of inhibitory tone and increasing odor responses via enhanced mitral cell excitability (Escanilla et al., 2010; Linster et al., 2011). This enhanced S/N could underlie the improved detection thresholds we observed behaviorally (Escanilla et al., 2010). Other groups suggest an enhanced odor response independent of spontaneous activity, due to an increase in excitability in mitral cells (Jiang et al., 1996; Ciombor et al., 1999; Hayar et al., 2001). Here, we observed an increase in significant odor responses during activation of LC, but our analyses suggest that this increase was due to the modulation of the underlying spontaneous activity rather than an increase of odor responses per se as suggested by previous modeling studies (Escanilla et al., 2010; Linster et al., 2011; Fig. 9A). It is important to note that we used relatively low, behaviorally relevant odor concentrations, in contrast to other explorations of OB physiology [for example, our odor concentrations are 10–100× less than those used by Eckmeier and Shea (2014)]. We used tonic rather than phasic LC stimulation, which has been shown in other olfactory (Bouret and Sara, 2002) and sensory areas (Aston-Jones and Bloom, 1981; Bouret and Sara, 2004) to selectively enhance sensory responses. Again, differences in dosages as well as timing are likely to influence the effect that NE has on a brain area.
A previous behavioral study in the laboratory suggested that NE increases S/N ratio (Linster et al., 2011). We here show that S/N during odor stimulation was increased by LC stimulation, which could underlie increased odor detection capabilities, albeit with a different underlying mechanism than we originally proposed computationally (Linster et al., 2011). In our behavioral experiments the strongest effect of NE infusions were near the threshold for odor detection: rats infused with a high dosage of NE were capable of detecting odorants as low as 10−6 Pa vapor partial pressure compared with 10−2 vapor partial pressure odor detection in control animals (Escanilla et al., 2010, 2012). In the present experiments we did not observe a similar concentration-dependent effect on S/N. When comparing these results one must consider the fact that in the behavioral experiments we added NE into a system that was presumably already activated and contained some NE already. When blocking NE receptors, odor detection thresholds were considerably higher than in control animals, at concentrations more similar to the effects seen here. It is possible that our tonic stimulation replicated background noradrenergic tone, which enhances S/N through suppressing background noise and subsequent unmasking of already present odor responses. However, this is likely not the whole story: phasic NE release through the LC could additionally mediate a direct enhancement of signal/odor responsiveness.
In an experiment similar to the one reported here but monitoring OB input rather than output, Eckmeier and Shea (2014) showed that LC stimulation produced a NE-dependent depression of spontaneous and odor-evoked input activity at the input layer to the OB. Because this experiment used calcium imaging to monitor presynaptic activity arriving from the olfactory nerve, it is likely that NE modulation of glomerular circuits is involved in this process. Specifically, NE excitation of external tufted cells, as reported in brain slice experiments (Zhou et al., 2016) could activate local inhibitory neurons, which act presynaptically to reduce olfactory nerve inputs (Aroniadou-Anderjaska et al., 2000; Ennis et al., 2001). Similar to the results reported here, Eckmeier and Shea (2014) showed a stronger effect on spontaneous than odor-evoked activity, resulting in reduced background levels of activity at the OB input layer.
The LC and NE are involved in behavioral state, vigilance, attention, and memory processes in various sensory and memory systems (Rajkowski et al., 1994; Aston-Jones et al., 2000; Bouret and Sara, 2002, 2004; Berridge and Waterhouse, 2003; Valentino and Van Bockstaele, 2008). In our study, we specifically chose tonic LC stimulation to mimic NE levels released through changes in behavioral state by using protocols like those used by Shea et al. (2008), but unlike other studies that have used phasic LC stimulation (Bouret and Sara, 2002). It is possible that our high dosages of NE were correlated with the levels of NE released in the OB during these odor tasks, whereas our lower doses were analogous to a less active behavioral state. We observed lasting effects after the LC stimulation in Experiment 1, as reported previously in vivo (Shea et al., 2008; Eckmeier and Shea, 2014) and in vitro (Gire and Schoppa, 2008). In Experiment 3, we do see an immediate effect of LC stimulation on spontaneous activity (Fig. 9A) during the pre-odor epoch; however, in this experiment LC stimulation lasts over the course of several odor trials. Together with experiments from other groups this suggests that modulatory effects of LC activation may be delayed and long lasting, rather than being time-locked to specific bouts of LC activation. As such, the present study provides an insight into how state-dependent modifications in LC firing rates during different behavioral states affect odor processing in primary sensory areas in general. Given the altered state of neuromodulators due to anesthesia, we cannot directly compare the present results to data obtained in awake animals. Our results show a lack of effect of antagonists on MT cell activity, suggesting that the anesthetized state may correspond more closely to a “no NE” state in a behaving animal and our NE infusions mimic what happens in a behaving and motivated animal. Therefore, our study perhaps suggests that moderate arousal states may lead to enhanced S/N ratio in the OB, leading to cleaner, more discriminable signals exciting this olfactory center for use in higher-level processing.
Footnotes
This work was supported by NIH/NIDCD Grant DC008702. We thank Dr. Thomas Cleland and Matthew Einhorn for designing the recording and odor stimulation system. This article was prepared while L.C.M. was employed at Cornell University. The opinions expressed in this article are the author's own and do not reflect the view of the National Institutes of Health, the Department of Health and Human Services, or the United States government.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Christiane Linster, Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Cornell University, Mudd Hall, 215 Tower Road, Ithaca NY 14850. CL243{at}cornell.edu