Abstract
Stochastic accumulator models provide a comprehensive framework for how neural activity could produce behavior. Neural activity within the frontal eye fields (FEFs) and intermediate layers of the superior colliculus (iSC) support such models for saccade initiation by relating variations in saccade reaction time (SRT) to variations in such parameters as baseline, rate of accumulation of activity, and threshold. Here, by recording iSC activity during reversible cryogenic inactivation of the FEF in four male nonhuman primates, we causally tested which parameter(s) best explains concomitant increases in SRT. While FEF inactivation decreased all aspects of ipsilesional iSC activity, decreases in accumulation rate and threshold poorly predicted accompanying increases in SRT. Instead, SRT increases best correlated with delays in the onset of saccade-related accumulation. We conclude that FEF signals govern the onset of saccade-related accumulation within the iSC, and that the onset of accumulation is a relevant parameter for stochastic accumulation models of saccade initiation.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The superior colliculus (SC) and frontal eye fields (FEFs) are two of the best-studied areas in the primate brain. Surprisingly, little is known about what happens in the SC when the FEF is temporarily inactivated. Here, we show that temporary FEF inactivation decreases all aspects of functionally related activity in the SC. This combination of techniques also enabled us to relate changes in SC activity to concomitant increases in saccadic reaction time (SRT). Although stochastic accumulator models relate SRT increases to reduced rates of accumulation or increases in threshold, such changes were not observed in the SC. Instead, FEF inactivation delayed the onset of saccade-related accumulation, emphasizing the importance of this parameter in biologically plausible models of saccade initiation.
Introduction
How does the brain commit to a voluntary action? The oculomotor system that moves our line of sight provides a model system for study of this question. The primate frontal eye fields (FEFs) and intermediate layers of the superior colliculus (iSC) are two of the most studied oculomotor structures. Within each, saccade-related activity peaks around the time of saccade initiation (for review, see Gandhi and Katnani, 2011; Schall, 2015). Such patterns conform well to stochastic accumulator models relating neural activity to saccade initiation (Hanes and Schall, 1996; Dorris et al., 1997; Paré and Hanes, 2003; Ratcliff et al., 2003; Ding and Gold, 2012). Despite this work, simple yet fundamental questions about the oculomotor system remain unresolved. For example, what happens to iSC activity when the FEF is suddenly compromised, and how do changes in iSC activity relate to concomitant increases in saccadic reaction times (SRTs; Peel et al., 2014)? Answering these questions would not only advance basic understanding of communication within the oculomotor system, but can also test the neural implementation of stochastic accumulator models for saccade initiation. However, such answers are surprisingly hard to predict for a variety of reasons.
First, these structures are highly interconnected by monosynaptic corticotectal projections (Leichnetz et al., 1981; Komatsu and Suzuki, 1985) and by polysynaptic descending (e.g., through the basal ganglia or other cortical structures), ascending [e.g., via the thalamus or pulvinar (Sommer and Wurtz, 2004; Berman et al., 2009; Crapse and Sommer, 2009)], and callosal pathways (Pandya and Vignolo, 1971); the FEF and iSC also project directly to the brainstem saccadic burst generator (Raybourn and Keller, 1977; Schnyder et al., 1985; Huerta et al., 1986). Second, the functional content of signals relayed from the FEF to the superior colliculus (SC), and indeed between many different oculomotor areas, span the sensorimotor continuum (Segraves and Goldberg, 1987; Everling and Munoz, 2000; Sommer and Wurtz, 2000; Wurtz et al., 2001; Helminski and Segraves, 2003). Third, since stochastic accumulator models predict that saccades occur when activity increases above a fixed threshold, then increases in SRTs should relate to decreases in the rate at which activity accumulated and/or the baseline level of activity. However, oculomotor thresholds may not be fixed (Jantz et al., 2013) and can paradoxically decrease for longer SRTs (Heitz and Schall, 2012). Also, other parameters, such as the onset of accumulation (Pouget et al., 2011), the speed of perceptual evaluation (Shankar et al., 2011), or the period of integration (Heitz and Schall, 2012), may also affect when the oculomotor system commits to a saccade. Thus, recording iSC activity during inactivation of the FEF can not only address the contribution of the FEF to iSC activity, but can also test whether the observed profiles of iSC activity accompanying increased SRTs match those predicted by contemporary stochastic accumulator models of saccade initiation.
Materials and Methods
Subjects and surgical procedures.
Four male monkeys (Macaca mulatta, monkeys M, G, D, and O, weighing 8.7, 11.1, 9.8, and 8.6 kg respectively) were used in these experiments. All training, surgical, and experimental procedures conformed to the policies of the Canadian Council on Animal Care and the National Institutes of Health on the care and use of laboratory animals, and were approved by the Animal Use Subcommittee of the University of Western Ontario Council on Animal Care. We monitored the monkeys' weights daily and their health was closely supervised by university veterinarians.
Each monkey underwent two surgeries to permit cryogenic inactivation of the FEF and extracellular recordings from the iSC. In the first surgery, we implanted either unilateral (right side only; monkeys M and G) or bilateral cryoloops into the arcuate sulcus using surgical procedures previously described (Lomber et al., 1999; Peel et al., 2014). Briefly, we performed a small 2.25 cm2 craniotomy above the spur of the arcuate sulcus, and implanted two customized, stainless steel cryoloops (each 5–8 mm in length and 3 mm in depth) into the arcuate sulcus (Fig. 1A), which permitted cooling of tissue adjacent to the superior and inferior arms of the arcuate sulcus. Cryoloop temperatures of 3°C silence postsynaptic activity in tissue ≤1.5 mm away without influencing axonal propagation of action potentials (Lomber et al., 1999). Thermal surface imaging of the exposed tissue during surgery revealed that cryoloop cooling did not spread to adjacent gyri. In this manuscript, we only performed unilateral FEF inactivation and alternated the side of cooling on separate days in monkeys D and O. In the second surgery, we positioned a recording chamber over a 19-mm-diameter craniotomy to permit a surface normal approach to either iSC (Rezvani and Corneil, 2008).
Experimental procedures.
Head-restrained monkeys were placed in front of a rectilinear grid of 500+ red LEDs covering ±35° of the horizontal and vertical visual field. We conducted experiments in a dark, sound-attenuated room and sampled each monkey's eye position using a single, chair-mounted eye tracker at 500 Hz (EyeLink II, SR Research). Behavioral tasks were controlled via customized real-time LabView programs running on a PXI controller (National Instruments) at a rate of 1 kHz.
Extracellular activity was recorded on a Multichannel Acquisition Processor data acquisition system (Plexon) via tungsten microelectrodes (impedance, 0.5–3.0 MΩ at 1 kHz; FHC). Action potential waveforms surpassing a user-defined threshold were amplified, low-cut filtered, sorted, and stored at 40 kHz. All neurons were recorded ∼1 mm or more below the surface of the SC, in locations where electrical stimulation (300 Hz, 100 ms, biphasic cathodal-first pulses with each phase 0.3 ms in duration) evoked saccades with currents <50 μA. In conjunction with most recorded neurons exhibiting delay-related or saccade-related activity, recorded neurons were most likely contained with the intermediate, rather than superficial, layers of the SC, but we cannot completely rule out this possibility. We subsequently confirmed the isolation of single-unit neurons off-line throughout cooling using both sorted and unsorted action potential waveforms, and when possible ensured that the functional definition of a given neuron was maintained before and after FEF inactivation.
Upon isolating an iSC neuron, we mapped the response field for contralateral visually guided saccades. Across the 239 isolated neurons in our sample, response field centers were located at an eccentricity of 11.6 ± 4.7° (range, 4–25°) and at an angle relative to the horizontal axis of 12.8 ± 30.5° (range, −90 to 90°; Fig. 1B). After response field mapping, we attempted to collect a dataset consisting of a precooling, pericooling, and postcooling session (60 correct trials each), which required maintaining isolation for ∼20 min. We largely followed a previously described procedure for cryogenic FEF inactivation (Peel et al., 2014), although to facilitate neuronal isolation through the entire dataset, we did not implement a 3 min transition between cooling sessions. Following the completion of the precooling session, chilled methanol was pumped through the lumen of the cryoloops, decreasing the cryoloop temperature. Once the cryoloop temperature was stable at 3°C, we began the pericooling session. Upon finishing the pericooling session, we turned off the cooling pumps, which allowed the cryoloop temperature to rapidly return to normal. When the cryoloop temperature reached 35°C, we began the postcooling session. Although saccadic behavior and iSC activity rapidly recovered after rewarming, the postcooling sessions may have contained residual effects of cooling. We controlled for this and other time-dependent factors by combining trials from precooling and postcooling sessions into the FEF warm condition. For ∼15% of all datasets, isolation of an iSC neuron was lost after completion of the pericooling session. We excluded postcooling trials from these sessions. Nonetheless, the effects of cooling in these sessions (based on comparing pericooling to precooling activity) were similar to the other 85% of datasets where isolation was maintained throughout the postcool session.
Behavioral tasks.
Monkeys performed visually or memory-guided saccades after a delayed response period. Following a variable fixation period (750–1000 ms), where monkeys maintained fixation within a radius of ±3° of a central cue, a peripheral cue appeared in the periphery. The fixation window was purposely set to be large, as FEF inactivation can shift the average fixation position slightly (fixation position can shift by ∼0.5° on average, although the ranges of fixation positions adopted before and during FEF inactivation still overlap; Peel et al., 2016). In 79% of our datasets, the peripheral cue could appear either in or diametrically opposite to the center of the response field of an isolated SC neuron. In the remaining 21% of datasets, we collected data from intermixed visually or memory-guided saccades, using peripheral cues placed only in the neuron's response field. Peripheral cues were either extinguished after 250 ms or remained on for memory-guided or visually guided saccades, respectively. To receive a liquid reward, monkeys were required to maintain fixation throughout a fixed delay period of 1000 ms and to generate a saccade toward a target window (70% of the peripheral cue's visual angle) when the central cue was extinguished. This large target window was necessary because FEF inactivation increased saccadic error, particularly for memory-guided saccades (Peel et al., 2014). The use of a fixed delay period increases anticipation of central cue disappearance, which presumably would be related to iSC activity late in the delay period.
Consistent with previous reports (Peel et al., 2014, 2016; Kunimatsu et al., 2015), large-volume unilateral FEF inactivation increased contraversive and occasionally ipsiversive saccadic reaction times (Fig. 1C) and decreased the accuracy and peak velocity of contraversive saccades. However, FEF inactivation had only a marginal effect on the monkeys' ability to perform either task, with error rates increasing at most by 14%.
Neuron classification.
iSC neurons were classified functionally according to a variety of response characteristics on acceptable trials (Fig. 1D). To quantify neuronal activity, we convolved spike times on individual trials with a spike-density function that mimics an excitatory postsynaptic potential (rise time of 1 ms, decay time of 20 ms, kernel window of 100 ms; Thompson et al., 1996). We confirmed that all our results were the same if we convolved neural activity with a 10 ms Gaussian.
For visual activity, we defined acceptable trials as those where the monkey maintained fixation of the central cue for the entire delay period for either visually guided or memory-guided saccades, and generated their first saccade toward the target as determined using a velocity criterion of 30°/s. Then, using a Poisson analysis described previously (Hanes et al., 1995), we checked whether neurons exhibited a visually related response. Briefly, we compared the actual number of spikes within a time window to the number of spikes predicted by a Poisson distribution based on spiking activity across the entire trial. To calculate the latency of visual response within a trial, we used the time of the first burst of spikes greater than chance between 30 and 120 ms after cue onset; the visual latency of a given neuron was then derived by averaging the latency of detected single-trial visual responses across ≥8 trials. In addition, we ensured that neurons with a visually related response had mean firing rates in the 50 ms interval after the average visual latency significantly greater than baseline activity integrated in the last 200 ms before cue onset (p < 0.05, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Basso and Wurtz, 1998; McPeek and Keller, 2002). Visual activity was defined as the difference between these firing rates. We also calculated the peak magnitude of the visual response minus the baseline activity.
For delay-period and build-up activity, we applied the same criteria, but also removed any trial with anticipatory saccades (i.e., reaction time <60 ms after fixation cue offset; ∼12% of trials). Neurons displayed delay-period activity if the mean firing rates in the last 100 ms of the delay period were ≥5 spikes/s above baseline activity (i.e., 200 ms before cue onset; p < 0.05, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Basso and Wurtz, 1998; McPeek and Keller, 2002). The magnitude of delay-period activity was then calculated as the difference in firing rates between these intervals. Neurons with build-up activity had mean firing rates 100–200 ms before saccade onset significantly greater than the preceding 100 ms (p < 0.05, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Anderson et al., 1998).
Finally, for saccadic activity, we used the same trials as those for the analysis of delay-period and build-up activity, but we additionally removed any trial where the monkey blinked during the first saccade (∼11% of all trials in this subset). We subsequently removed any dataset with <8 acceptable saccades into the response field of an isolated iSC neuron either before, during, or after FEF inactivation (∼9% of all datasets were removed). Neurons exhibited saccadic activity if the mean perisaccadic firing rates (defined as 8 ms before saccade onset to 8 ms before its end) were significantly greater than the last 100 ms of the delay period (p < 0.05, Wilcoxon signed-rank test), and if the increase in perisaccadic activity above baseline activity in the 200 ms before cue onset exceeded 50 spikes/s (Munoz and Wurtz, 1995; McPeek and Keller, 2002). Saccadic activity was defined as the difference between mean perisaccadic firing rates and baseline firing rates. We also calculated the peak magnitude of saccade-related activity minus baseline activity.
Matched saccade analysis.
To examine iSC activity associated with saccade initiation and generation across FEF inactivation, the actual saccades being compared must be as similar as possible. Otherwise, any differences in saccade-related activity could be due to the generation of a saccade of different metrics, given the spatial coding of saccade metrics in the iSC, or different peak velocity, given potential relationships between the vigor of iSC activity and peak saccade velocity (Waitzman et al., 1991; Stanford et al., 1996; Katnani and Gandhi, 2012). To avoid these confounds, we matched for each neuron each FEF cool trial with one trial from a set of corresponding FEF warm trials containing similar saccade metrics and kinematics (Fig. 1E). We specified that any such matched saccades had to have horizontal and vertical displacements within 1.5° and peak velocities within 50°/s. If an FEF cool trial matched with multiple FEF warm trials, we selected, with replacement, the closest match with the lowest-ranked differences in three variables (horizontal displacements, vertical displacements, and peak velocities), and occasionally by radial displacement for ties. Importantly, this ranking procedure ensured that most matches usually had differences in horizontal and vertical displacement much less than 1° (mean ± SD: 0.06 ± 0.5° and −0.01 ± 0.5°, respectively), and <10°/s in peak velocity (−4 ± 20°/s; Fig. 1F). Using this matching procedure, we matched 80% of FEF cool trials with a corresponding FEF warm trial recorded from the same neuron. Unless otherwise noted, we matched saccades for both metrics and kinematics for all analyses on saccade-related activity, and only analyzed saccade-related activity in neurons where we could match ≥5 trials in each of the FEF warm and FEF cool conditions (96% of neurons with saccade-related activity met this criterion). To assess the variability inherent to this procedure with and without FEF inactivation, we also performed the same matched-saccade analysis using only FEF warm trials.
Detection of the onset of saccade-related accumulation.
To investigate the neuronal correlates of SRT increases during FEF inactivation, we derived various parameters of a rise-to-threshold model (i.e., onset of accumulation relative to the go-cue, baseline, threshold, accumulation rate) from saccade-related iSC neurons. To detect the onset time of saccadic accumulation on a trial-by-trial basis, we implemented a piecewise two-piece linear regression of presaccadic iSC activity, modifying an approach described previously (Cashaback et al., 2013; Goonetilleke et al., 2015). The objective of this analysis was to find the two linear regressions that best fit the convolved iSC activity before saccade onset; the onset of saccade-related accumulation was taken as the spike time closest to the inflection point between these two linear fits. The first linear regression is based on activity from 100 ms before the go-cue (offset of fixation cue) to a candidate inflection point; the second linear regression is based on activity from this candidate inflection point to the peak of presaccadic activity. Candidate inflection points are tested at each millisecond within this window before peak presaccadic activity, and the onset of activity coincided with the time of the spike closest to the inflection point that minimized the summed squared error between convolved iSC activity and the two linear regressions. Note that the slope of the first linear regression was not required to be zero. Hence, it could capture any delay-period or build-up activity the precedes saccade-related accumulation.
Of the 7524 total trials (3762 matches) matched from 193 neurons included in this analysis, we discarded a small percentage of trials (16%) that did not reach certain criteria due to the trial-by-trial variability in saccade-related activity. Of these total trials, we removed 7% of trials with no inflection point (i.e., the minimum summed squared error occurred at the start or end of testing window), and 6% of trials where accumulation was not maintained until saccade threshold (i.e., activity decreased after the inflection point). Moreover, we also discarded 1 and 2% of total trials for when onset times occurred before the go-cue or after saccade threshold, respectively. Because we obtained identical results with or without removing a small fraction of trials with onsets before the go-cue, this suggests that anticipation of the go-cue did not influence our results. These criteria left us with a total of 5420 trials from 2710 pairs of matched trials (2083 pairs from ipsilesional iSCs, and 627 pairs from contralesional iSCs). Importantly, the r2 values of 0.63 ± 0.24 (mean ± SD) produced by this two-piece linear regression were substantially larger than those arising from a single linear fit over the same time range (0.34 ± 0.21). Further, the r2 values changed by <2% during FEF inactivation (0.63 ± 0.25 for FEF warm trials to 0.61 ± 0.25 for FEF cool trials). To ensure that the first linear regression sufficiently captured delay-period or build-up activity before the inflection point, we also performed similar analyses using larger window sizes starting before go-cue (200, 350, and 500 ms) or convolution functions (e.g., a 10 ms Gaussian). We also tried fitting the activity before the inflection point with a quadratic function. All of these alternative analyses produced robust r2 values of >0.59 and had similarly detected onset of accumulation values (median difference <1 ms across all matched trials) compared with the two-piece piecewise linear-regression analysis. Although r2 values did significantly increase with the quadratic fit (as expected given the use of an additional term), we deemed that fitting the first portion of the data with a quadratic curve was justified only when this increased the r2 value by 10% and significantly reduced the residuals (F test with p < 0.01; Nagy and Corneil, 2010). Using such criteria, a quadratic fit was deemed necessary on only 15% of all matched trials, and changed onset times by only 6 ± 27 ms (mean ± SD) across all matched trials. We also implemented an alternative Poisson-based analysis of burst onset (Hanes et al., 1995) to verify our main results. For this alternative Poisson-based analysis, we only included trials where we could detect a burst of spikes after the go-cue and before saccade onset, with the onset time coinciding with the burst of spikes nearest to saccade onset. We obtained similar results using this alternative onset detection method (see Results).
Determining accumulation rate, baseline, and threshold.
We also derived the accumulation rate, baseline, and threshold from saccade-related iSC neurons. For the accumulation rate, we simply took the slope of the second linear regression running between the inflection point and the peak presaccadic activity. Because this calculation of accumulation rate hinges on this calculation of onset time, we also derived the accumulation rate in another way by finding the slope of the line when activity crossed the 30 and 70% points of peak presaccadic activity. This latter calculation of accumulation rate is independent of the two-piece linear regression, but yielded similar results. For baseline activity, we took the average level of activity in the 100 ms window before the offset of the fixation cue. For threshold activity, we calculated the activity 8–18 ms before saccade onset (Jantz et al., 2013; Johnston et al., 2014), which is based on the minimum amount of time for the iSC to influence the brainstem circuits regulating saccade onset (Miyashita and Hikosaka, 1996).
Finally, we conducted two further analyses to examine how combinations of the changes in the parameters of SC saccade-related activity related to SRT differences during FEF inactivation. First, we calculated the time-to-reach-threshold as the difference in the threshold and baseline activities divided by the accumulation rate, and directly related this combined measure to SRT differences using similar millisecond units. Second, because these parameters may covary, we examined the change in each parameter [onset of accumulation (ΔO), baseline (ΔB), threshold (ΔT), accumulation rate (ΔA)] in isolation by regressing such changes against the timing residuals (ε) of a multiple linear regression consisting of all other parameters (Hewitt et al., 2015). This approach removed the variability associated with interactions among the other parameters, so that we could examine how well a single parameter directly contributed to SRT differences. For example, to evaluate ΔO independently of ΔB, ΔT, and ΔA, we used the following Equation 1 to compute the ε term, which represents the remaining temporal variability between ΔSRT and model parameters after everything but ΔO is removed: ε(x) = ΔSRT − α1 − βBΔB(x) − βTΔT(x) − βAΔA(x).
After calculating the timing residuals, we then used a linear regression to fit the final parameter (ΔO) against the ε(x) term as shown in the following Equation 2: r(x) = ε(x) − α2 − βoΔO(x).
The goodness-of-fit (r2) measure taken from the residuals r(x) of this linear regression represents how well changes in onset of accumulation explains SRT differences across matched trials, after removing any variability associated with interactions among the other parameters.
Experimental design and statistical analysis.
To quantify the effects of FEF inactivation on visual and delay-period iSC activity, we performed two-sample Wilcoxon rank-sum tests to find statistical differences within individual neurons at p < 0.05, and we used paired Wilcoxon signed-rank tests to uncover statistical differences across the neuronal population at p < 0.05. Because we only analyzed saccadic activity from paired trials having similar saccade metrics and kinematics, we performed paired Wilcoxon signed-rank tests on both individual neurons and a population of neurons to ensure that differences in saccadic activity reached significance at p < best to SRT increases in more 0.05. We also correlated changes in saccade-related activity during FEF inactivation to concomitant SRT increases. To do this, we performed a regression analysis where we fitted a linear regression to examine how changes in iSC activity predicted SRT differences across the neuron population or all matched trials. We interpreted a significant correlation if the probability associated with the F statistic (the mean regression sum of squares divided by the mean error sum of squares) was <0.05.
Results
We recorded activity from either the ipsilesional or contralesional iSC before, during, and after large-volume unilateral cryogenic FEF inactivation while monkeys performed delayed visually or memory-guided saccades to cues placed in or opposite to the center of the neuron's response field (see Materials and Methods; Fig. 1). We only cooled the cryoloop in the inferior arm of the arcuate sulcus, which provided an estimated volume of inactivation of 90 mm3 in the anterior bank of the arcuate sulcus. Previously, we showed that cooling only the inferior arm cryoloop produced behavioral deficits ∼70% of the magnitude produced by cooling both loops together (Peel et al., 2014).
The delayed nature of the behavioral tasks used here, requiring the animals to temporarily withhold a saccade to a persistent or remembered peripheral visual cue until the offset of the central fixation point, provides the opportunity to quantify the effects of FEF inactivation on different aspects of functionally defined iSC activity. Accordingly, in the first half of the results, we describe the effects of FEF inactivation on iSC activity during visual, delay-period, build-up, and saccade-related epochs. In the second half of the results, we focus on how changes in presaccade activity in the iSC during FEF inactivation relate to the parameters derived from a stochastic accumulator model. While such models have not customarily been applied to delayed saccade tasks, they provide a framework for determining whether changes in baseline, rate of rise, threshold, or onset of accumulation best predicted the associated increases in SRT.
We recorded 239 neurons (178 ipsilesional, 61 contralesional to FEF inactivation) from the caudal iSC, maintaining isolation before and during FEF inactivation, and usually (85%) after rewarming. Of these neurons, 107 (45%) exhibited visual activity, 147 (62%) had delay-period activity, 60 (25%) had build-up activity, 155 (65%) had saccade-related activity, and 22 neurons (9%) did not meet any of our classification criteria. Consistent with previous work (Peel et al., 2014, 2016), unilateral FEF inactivation increased SRT toward contraversive cues and usually toward ipsiversive cues. Because FEF inactivation also impaired the accuracy and peak velocity of contraversive saccades, we performed a matched-saccade analysis to ensure analysis of saccade-related iSC activity during the production of equivalent saccades. Such matching is crucial to avoid confounds related to the generation of a different saccade or to differing degrees of saccade-related drive onto the brainstem burst generator (Yoshida et al., 1999).
Responses of iSC neurons before FEF inactivation
Our sample of iSC neurons exhibited visual, delay-period, and saccade-related activity, the latencies and magnitudes of which concurred well with previous reports of iSC activity (Munoz and Wurtz, 1995; Basso and Wurtz, 1998; McPeek and Keller, 2002). Before FEF inactivation, neurons with visual activity had response latencies of 56 ± 9 ms (mean ± SD; range, 42–99 ms), firing rates of 77 ± 39 spikes/s, and peak magnitudes of 106 ± 54 spikes/s. Eighty percent of these 107 visual neurons also exhibited delay-period (71%), build-up (11%), and/or saccade-related activity (59%). Based on these results and our recording approach (see Materials and Methods), we surmise that most visual neurons resided in the intermediate rather superficial layers of the SC. However, the small subset of visual-only neurons (21 total, 14 from the ipsilesional iSC) may have been located in the superficial SC. For neurons exhibiting saccade-related activity, we found firing rates of 160 ± 86 spikes/s and peak magnitudes of 225 ± 108 spikes/s during visually guided saccades, and corresponding activities of 124 ± 60 and 175 ± 71 spikes/s during memory-guided saccades. Eighty-three percent of 155 saccade-related neurons also exhibited visual (41%), delay-period (68%), and/or build-up (28%) activity.
We now turn to the effects of FEF inactivation on iSC activity. Where possible, we controlled for time-dependent factors by combining precooling and postcooling trials into the FEF warm condition, and compared this to trials when the FEF was inactivated (the FEF cool condition). Equivalent results were obtained if we excluded the 15% of our sample where we were not able to record postcooling data.
FEF inactivation reduced but did not delay ipsilateral iSC visual responses
We first examined the effect of FEF inactivation on the magnitude and timing of visual responses in the iSC. Figure 2A shows an example neuron recorded from the ipsilesional iSC before, during, and after FEF inactivation. This neuron exhibited delay-period activity, since it also remained active at a lower rate when the cue remained on, and also exhibited saccade-related activity (data not shown). In the FEF warm condition, visual activity commenced 45 ± 6 ms after cue onset (determined by a Poisson burst analysis; see Materials and Methods) and averaged 71 ± 23 spikes/s for the subsequent 50 ms. FEF inactivation did not alter the visual burst onset latency (45 to 47 ms, p = 0.99, z = −0.01, Wilcoxon rank sum test), but decreased visual activity from 71 to 52 spikes/s (p = 0.06, z = 1.89, Wilcoxon rank sum test).
Across our sample, FEF inactivation consistently decreased visual activity in the ipsilesional (p < 0.01, z = −2.95, Wilcoxon signed-rank test, ∼3 ± 2% decrease, mean ± SE, in all 89 neurons, and ∼24 ± 3% decrease in the 21 neurons exhibiting significantly decreased visual activity), but not contralesional iSC (3 ± 4% increase; p = 0.62, z = 0.50, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Fig. 2). A statistical comparison of changes in the magnitude of the visual response across the ipsilesional and contralesional iSC did not reach significance (p = 0.07, z = 1.79, Wilcoxon rank sum test), perhaps because of the small sample size of contralesional iSC neurons. FEF inactivation did not alter visual response latencies across either ipsilesional (increase and SE of <1 ms, p = 0.93, z = 0.09, Wilcoxon signed-rank test) or contralesional iSC neurons (increase and SE of <1 ms, p = 0.84, z = 0.20, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Fig. 2C). When present, FEF inactivation also did not alter activity in the 200 ms preceding cue onset in either the ipsilesional (13 ± 10% increase, p = 0.87, z = 0.16, Wilcoxon signed-rank test) or contralesional (2 ± 24% decrease, p = 0.19, z = 1.36, Wilcoxon signed-rank test) iSC across our sample, nor in the subset of 21 ipsilesional iSC neurons (7 ± 14% decrease, p = 0.78, z = 0.28, Wilcoxon signed-rank test) exhibiting significantly reduced visual activity.
The effect of FEF inactivation on the magnitude of visual responses depended on the functional classification of the recording neuron, being more pronounced in ipsilesional iSC neurons that also displayed delay-related and/or saccade-related activity (5 ± 2% decrease, p < 0.01, z = −2.65, Wilcoxon signed-rank test, n = 75), compared with the putative superficial SC neurons, which only exhibited a visual response (3 ± 10% increase, n = 14; Fig. 2D). Although FEF inactivation increased the response magnitude of visual-only neurons, this increase was not significant (p = 0.15, z = −1.48, Wilcoxon signed-rank test) given a small sample size and a high degree of variability between neurons. In terms of response latency, and in contrast to the effects of FEF inactivation on response magnitude, we did not observe any influence of FEF inactivation on visual latency of SC neurons when subdivided into different functional classifications (all differences, <1 ms; p = 0.98 and 0.77, z = −0.03 and 0.28, Wilcoxon signed-rank tests on visual-only and multiple-response neurons, respectively; Fig. 2D).
FEF inactivation decreased delay-period activity in ipsilesional iSC neurons
FEF inactivation reduced delay-period activity in ipsilesional iSC neurons both in the presence (visually guided saccades) and absence (memory-guided saccades) of peripheral cues. This result is shown for two representative ipsilesional iSC neurons (Fig. 3A,B). In the memory-guided saccade task, the neuron shown in Figure 3A displayed modest delay-period activity of 13 ± 4 spikes/s in the last 100 ms period before fixation cue offset, which ramped up until saccade onset. FEF inactivation effectively abolished this delay-period activity (decreasing from 13 to 0.2 spikes/s; p < 0.0001, z = 4.90, Wilcoxon rank sum test). The visuomotor neuron shown in Figure 3B was recorded during interleaved visually and memory-guided saccades, and exhibited far greater delay-period activity when the cue was present (73 ± 6 spikes/s) than absent (12 ± 2 spikes/s). FEF inactivation decreased delay-period activity both when cues were present (73 to 45 spikes/s; p < 0.0001, z = 4.41, Wilcoxon rank sum test) or absent (12 to 5 spikes/s; p < 0.05, z = 2.23, Wilcoxon rank sum test). Across our sample, FEF inactivation robustly reduced delay-period activity in both saccade tasks in the ipsilesional iSC (Fig. 3C; visually guided, cyan, p < 0.0001, z = −4.07, 31% of 77 neurons exhibited significant decreases; memory-guided, magenta, p < 0.0001, z = −4.67, Wilcoxon signed rank test, 40% of 45 neurons exhibited significant decreases). The effects of FEF inactivation were the same on ipsilesional neurons with (squares) or without (circles) build-up activity before saccade onset, but proportionally larger during the memory-guided (34 ± 7% decrease in all 45 neurons, or 68 ± 6% decrease in the 18 neurons with significant decreases) versus visually guided (18 ± 4% decrease in all 77 neurons, or 51 ± 4% decrease in the 24 neurons with significant decreases) saccade task. In contrast, we observed no effect of FEF inactivation on delay-period activity in our sample of contralesional iSC neurons (Fig. 3D; p = 0.84 and 0.65, z = −0.20 and −0.45, Wilcoxon signed-rank tests for memory-guided and visually guided, respectively). The lack of changes on contralesional iSC neurons during FEF inactivation is also supported by FEF inactivation producing larger decreases of delay-period activity in the ipsilesional compared with the contralesional iSC (p < 0.01 and 0.1, z = 2.76 and 1.37, Wilcoxon rank sum tests for memory-guided and visually guided, respectively).
As previously mentioned, FEF inactivation had a stronger effect on visual neurons that also exhibited delay-related or saccade-related activity. We found a similar pattern for the 113 ipsilesional iSC neurons with delay-period activity: 40 of 42 neurons exhibiting significantly reduced delay-period activity during FEF inactivation also exhibited either visual (62%), build-up (21%), or saccade-related activity (71%). Likewise, FEF inactivation consistently decreased visual (p < 0.001, z = 3.57, Wilcoxon signed-rank test) and saccade-related activity before both visually (p < 0.001, z = −3.59, Wilcoxon signed rank test) and memory-guided saccades (p < 0.05, z = −2.29, Wilcoxon signed-rank test) in this subset of 40 neurons.
FEF inactivation reduced saccade-related activity of ipsilesional iSC neurons
Next, we examined the FEF's contribution to saccade-related activity in the ipsilesional and contralesional iSC. For this analysis, saccades generated during FEF inactivation must be matched as closely as possible for metrics and velocity, otherwise any changes in saccade-related activity could simply arise from the generation of a different saccade. We matched saccades within 1.5° of horizontal and vertical displacement and within 50°/s of radial peak velocity, although we typically found matches well within these limits using a ranking procedure (see Materials and Methods; Fig. 1F). Despite the generation of effectively equivalent saccades, FEF inactivation decreased ipsilesional saccade-related activity in ipsilesional iSC neurons. This result is shown for one neuron in Figure 4A, where activity for visually guided saccades decreased significantly from 301 ± 85 to 263 ± 52 spikes/s (p < 0.001, z = 3.61, Wilcoxon signed-rank test). Across our sample, FEF inactivation consistently decreased saccade-related activity for ipsilesional iSC neurons (≥5 matches) for visually or memory-guided saccades (each p < 0.0001, z = −5.37 and −5.88, Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, respectively; Fig. 4B). The proportional decrease caused by FEF inactivation was greater for memory-guided versus visually guided saccades, both in terms of how much activity decreased (20 ± 3 and 11 ± 2% decrease, respectively) and in the proportion of neurons exhibiting significantly changed activity (69% of 59 neurons and 58% of 77 neurons, respectively).
Another way of analyzing the effects of FEF inactivation on saccade-related iSC activity is to directly compare activity for all matched saccades, pooled across all recorded neurons. The result of this analysis is shown in Figure 4C (top row), where each point represents saccade-related activity for a matched saccade generated during the FEF cool versus FEF warm condition. The clustering of points below the line of unity in the top row of Figure 4C, as well as the rightward skew of the blue histograms, reinforces how FEF inactivation decreases ipsilesional iSC saccade-related activity. As a control, we performed a similar saccade-matching procedure using FEF warm trials. While there is considerable scatter in this analysis around the line of unity (Fig. 4C, bottom row), the resulting distributions of the red histograms were not skewed away from zero.
Finally, while FEF inactivation significantly decreased saccade-related activity in some contralesional iSC neurons (20 and 29% of neurons for visually and memory-guided, respectively), such effects were less consistent across our sample (Fig. 4D; 3 ± 5% decrease for visually guided, p = 0.71, z = −0.37; 11 ± 5% decrease for memory-guided, p < 0.05, z = −2.14, Wilcoxon signed-rank tests). Further, FEF inactivation decreased saccade-related activity significantly more for ipsilesional versus contralesional iSC neurons during both the memory-guided saccade task (p < 0.05, z = −2.35, Wilcoxon rank sum test), but not the visually guided saccade task (p = 0.07, z = −1.82, Wilcoxon rank sum test).
Summary of effects of FEF inactivation on iSC activity
The results up to this point have focused on how FEF inactivation decreased visual, delay-period, and saccade-related activity in the downstream iSC, particularly in the ipsilesional side. In Figure 5, we summarize the impact of FEF inactivation on all aspects of functionally defined activity in the ipsilesional iSC by presenting average spike-density functions across our sample, separated where appropriate for visually and memory-guided saccades. Presenting the data in this way emphasizes the greater impact of FEF inactivation on memory-guided saccades, which is consistent with previous behavioral (Peel et al., 2014) and neurophysiological (Sommer and Wurtz, 2000) findings of a larger contribution of FEF to working memory, and also makes comparisons with other studies easier (Koval et al., 2011; Johnston et al., 2014).
Delays in the onset of iSC saccade-related accumulation predicted bilateral SRT increases
One of the intriguing features in Figures 4 and 5 is that saccade-related activity decreases during FEF inactivation, despite concomitant increases in SRT. Were SRT increases to be related to saccade threshold, then SC activity should have increased upon FEF inactivation. To reconcile these observations, we next examined how FEF inactivation altered the parameters of iSC presaccadic activity derived from a modified stochastic accumulator model (i.e., baseline and threshold activity, onset and rate of accumulation; see Materials and Methods), and determined which of these parameters best predicted the accompanying SRT increases. Within this model, increased SRTs could arise from one or some combination of decreases in baseline activity or rate of accumulation, or from increases in onset of accumulation or saccade threshold.
We first focus on the onset of accumulation. To find the onset of accumulation for each trial, we used a piecewise two-piece linear-regression method (Fig. 6A; see Materials and Methods). This method systematically tests within a window for the inflection point between two linear regressions (one for baseline and one for the rise in activity before saccade onset), with the onset of accumulation coinciding with the inflection point that minimizes the sum-of-square values for the two linear regressions against the convolved spike-density function.
The influence of FEF inactivation on the onset of accumulation (black ticks) and SRT (black circles) is shown for a representative neuron in Figure 6B, showing single-trial activity for one pair of matched saccades (Fig. 6B, top row; inset shows the position and velocity profiles for this match), and across all matches recorded from this neuron (Fig. 6B, bottom). Across all matched trials for this neuron, FEF inactivation increased the average onset of accumulation from 142 ± 16 to 162 ± 22 ms after fixation cue offset (p < 0.0001, z = −3.91, Wilcoxon signed-rank test) and significantly increased the average SRT of matched memory-guided saccades from 207 ± 20 to 238 ± 29 ms (p < 0.0001, z = −4.39, Wilcoxon signed-rank test). Thus, in this example, FEF inactivation delayed onset of iSC accumulation and increased SRT.
Across our sample, FEF inactivation delayed the onset of iSC accumulation in the ipsilesional iSC for memory-guided (Fig. 6C; 12 ± 3 ms increase, p < 0.001, z = 3.53, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; onsets from individual trials were averaged within neurons for this analysis) saccades, but not for visually guided saccades (5 ± 3 ms increase, p = 0.20, z = 1.29, Wilcoxon signed-rank test). We surmise that the lack of a significant increase for visually guided saccades may relate both to the smaller accompanying changes in SRT with this task and to our stringent saccade-matching analyses, as significant increases in accumulation onset were observed in the absence of saccade matching for both visually guided (increases of 16 ± 3 ms, p < 0.0001, z = 4.82, Wilcoxon signed-rank test) and memory-guided saccades (increases of 17 ± 3 ms, p < 0.0001, z = 4.81, Wilcoxon signed-rank test). More importantly, as shown in Figure 6C, increases or decreases in the onset of accumulation during FEF inactivation generally corresponded to a similar increase (e.g., closed circles clustering above the line of unity in Fig. 6C) or decrease (e.g., open circles clustering below the line of unity in Fig. 6C) in SRT, respectively. To analyze this more closely, we plotted the change in the onset of accumulation versus the change in SRTs on a neuron-by-neuron basis, and determined the variance explained by a linear correlation (Fig. 6D). Interestingly, we found robust correlations across ipsilesional iSC neurons for both visually and memory-guided saccades (r2 values of 0.72 and 0.55, p < 0.0001 for each, F statistics of 175.28 and 58.64, respectively), with slopes near 1.0 (0.82 and 0.69, respectively) and intercepts near 0 ms (4 and 11 ms, respectively). Notably, we also observed similar patterns in how changes in the onset of accumulation within the contralesional iSC predicted the accompanying changes in SRT (r2 = 0.44, p < 0.01, F = 12.83 for visually guided saccades, and r2 = 0.60, p < 0.001, F = 23.68 for memory-guided saccades). We verified our findings using various approaches to detect onset time (see Methods and Materials), including an alternative Poisson-based analysis. Results using this Poisson-based analysis agreed with the two-piece linear-regression method, with FEF inactivation delaying the onset of accumulation in the ipsilesional iSC (increase of 6 ± 4 ms, p = 0.13, z = 1.51, n = 71 for visually guided saccades, and increase of 11 ± 5 ms, p < 0.0001, z = 4.15, n = 54 for memory-guided saccades, Wilcoxon signed-rank tests), and the changes in onset remained strongly correlated with SRT differences (r2 = 0.53, p < 0.0001, F = 78.41, m = 0.53, y-intercept = 6 for visually guided saccades, and r2 = 0.64, p < 0.0001, F = 93.56, m = 0.61, y-intercept = 11 for memory-guided saccades). Thus, on a neuron-by-neuron basis, changes in the onset of accumulation of saccade-related iSC activity predicted accompanying changes in SRT in an almost one-to-one manner.
FEF inactivation altered other parameters in the ipsilesional iSC neurons, but such changes did not predict accompanying SRT increases
SRT increases with FEF inactivation could also be related to increases in threshold activity or to decreases in baseline activity or rate of accumulation. While FEF inactivation did affect these parameters, the variability or direction of changes in neural activity did not relate as well to accompanying changes in SRT. For instance, FEF inactivation decreased the accumulation rate (4.6 ± 1.1 to 3.5 ± 1.1 spikes/s2, p < 0.0001, z = 4.70, Wilcoxon signed-rank test) and threshold activity (210 ± 40 to 179 ± 48 spikes/s, p < 0.01, z = 2.90, Wilcoxon signed-rank test) in an exemplar neuron (Fig. 7A, arrow; same neuron as in Fig. 6B). Across our sample of ipsilesional iSC neurons, FEF inactivation consistently decreased baseline (Fig. 7B; decrease of 2 ± 1 spikes/s, p < 0.01, z = −2.60, Wilcoxon signed-rank test) and threshold activity (Fig. 7C; decrease of 11 ± 3%, p < 0.001, z = −3.34, Wilcoxon signed-rank test) before memory-guided saccades, but not before visually guided saccades (p = 0.77 and 0.13, z = −0.29 and −1.52, Wilcoxon signed-rank tests for baseline and threshold activity, respectively). FEF inactivation did not influence the rate of accumulation in either task (Fig. 7D; p = 0.76 and 0.06, z = −0.30 and −1.85, Wilcoxon signed-rank tests for visually and memory-guided tasks, respectively). Although changes in the rate of accumulation or baseline would be consistent with a neuronal mechanism that increases SRT during FEF inactivation, it is also important to consider interactions among parameters and how these collective changes could quantitatively relate to SRT increases.
To examine how well such parametric changes related to accompanying changes in SRT, we used a standard accumulator model that allowed us to extract the time-to-reach-threshold, computed from the measured values for baseline, threshold, and accumulation rate [(threshold − baseline activity)/(accumulation rate); see Materials and Methods]. Accumulator models are most commonly applied to speeded, rather than delayed, response tasks, but nevertheless such models provide a framework in which to relate changes in iSC activity to accompanying changes in SRT. Note that this time-to-reach-threshold value does not directly incorporate the onset of accumulation; effectively this model assumes that neural activity accumulates from baseline at some fixed time after a go-cue. The results of this analysis are shown on a neuron-by-neuron basis in Figure 7E,F. While the time-to-reach-threshold in the ipsilesional iSC marginally increased with FEF inactivation for visually guided saccades (1 ± 1 ms increase, p < 0.05, z = 2.25, Wilcoxon signed-rank test), we observed no consistent effects on memory-guided saccades (p = 0.17, z = 1.37, Wilcoxon signed-rank test), and a linear regression of such changes against concomitant changes in SRT revealed no correlation in the ipsilesional iSC for either visually (p = 0.35, F = 0.89, r2 value of 0.01) or memory-guided saccades (p = 0.28, F = 1.20, r2 value of 0.02). Moreover, any changes in the time-to-reach-threshold for the contralesional iSC did not relate at all to associated changes in ipsiversive SRT (p = 0.55 and 0.97, F = 0.38 and 0.001 for visually and memory-guided saccades, respectively). Thus, even when incorporating the three parameters of baseline, threshold, and accumulation rate, such changes do not fully account for the accompanying changes in SRT in the ipsilesional iSC.
Changes in the onset of accumulation best explained SRT differences across matched saccades, even without FEF inactivation
We extended the above analyses to the level of individually matched saccades, which enabled us to test how changes in onset time correlated with a larger distribution of SRT changes. For each matched saccade pair, extracted either across FEF inactivation (Fig. 8A, top left subplot) or only from trials without FEF inactivation (Fig. 8A, bottom left subplot) from the ipsilesional iSC, we derived both a change in the onset of accumulation and a change in SRT. We pooled trials across saccade tasks, since we found largely equivalent results for visually or memory-guided saccades (r2 values of 0.62 and 0.42, F = 1731.2 and 622.8, respectively, for relationship of changes in onset time vs SRT differences with FEF inactivation). Note that this procedure often matches trials where SRT decreased upon FEF inactivation, due to the overlap between response-time distributions when the FEF was or was not inactivated. Regardless, the changes in SRT and onset of accumulation remained highly correlated, regardless of whether the matched pairs were being compared across FEF inactivation or not (p's <0.0001, F = 2201.4 and 4684.1, r2 values of 0.53 and 0.51, slopes of 0.65 and 0.59, and y-intercepts of 8 and 0 ms, respectively). In contrast, comparison of the change in the time-to-reach-threshold (computed from the baseline, rate of accumulation, and threshold) versus the change in SRT for matched-saccade pairs revealed much weaker relationships (Fig. 8A, right columns). Moreover, we found similarly poor relationships from a time-to-reach-threshold computed from a completely independent measure of accumulation rate (r2 values of 0.10 and 0.11 for with or without FEF inactivation, respectively; see Materials and Methods).
We repeated these analyses independently for each of the parameters of baseline, rate of accumulation, and threshold. As shown by r2 values in Figure 8B, changes in these single parameters were also very poor predictors of accompanying changes in SRT. In contrast, consideration of the onset of accumulation either alone or in conjunction with all other parameters (i.e., adding the onset time of accumulation to the time-to-reach-threshold) greatly increased the relationship between changes in iSC activity and changes in SRT (Fig. 8B). Perhaps just as importantly, these observations held regardless of whether the matched pairs were compared across FEF inactivation or not (Fig. 8B, blue, red bars, respectively), or from contralesional iSC (data not shown). Comparable results were also obtained when we only matched saccades for metrics and not kinematics (data not shown).
Finally, to address how well a single parameter could independently explain SRT changes, even when allowing for correlation among other parameters, we adopted a multiple linear-regression approach (Hewitt et al., 2015) within the framework of a rise-to-threshold model of saccade initiation (see Materials and Methods). The objective of this analysis was to determine how much an individual parameter could independently explain the remaining variance following a multiple linear regression that correlates all the other parameters to SRT. As shown in Figure 8C, the onset of accumulation best accounted for the remaining variance from a multiple linear regression consisting of the other three parameters, reaffirming the importance of this parameter.
Summary of results
Reversible inactivation of a large volume of the unilateral FEF decreased all aspects of ipsilesional iSC activity, consistent with a general loss of excitatory input. The magnitude of such decreases in iSC activity depended both on the functional content of the signal, with greatest decreases for saccade-related activity, and on the inferred depth of the neuron within the SC, with greater decreases in visually related activity on those neurons also displaying delay-related and saccade-related activity. Such results largely conform both with the preferential distribution of frontal projections to intermediate and deeper layers of the iSC (Tigges and Tigges, 1981) and with antidromic studies of the functional content of corticotectal neurons (Segraves and Goldberg, 1987; Sommer and Wurtz, 2000; Helminski and Segraves, 2003). The absence of any effect of FEF inactivation on the latency of the first spike following visual-stimulus onset is also consistent with evidence that visual responses in the iSC require the integrity of the retinogeniculostriate pathway passing through the magnocellular laminae of the LGN (Schiller et al., 1979); perhaps not too surprisingly given the short response latencies, the FEF is also not a critical node in the pathway mediating the initial timing of the visual response in the iSC. Finally, by fitting the profiles of saccade-related iSC activity to a stochastic accumulator model, we showed that delays in the onset of saccade-related accumulation in either iSC, rather than systematic changes in either saccade threshold, baseline, or the rate of accumulation, best explained concomitant changes in SRT in the delayed saccade task, emphasizing the need to consider the onset of accumulation as a parameter in neurophysiologically inspired models of saccade initiation.
Discussion
FEF inactivation reduces excitatory input to the ipsilesional iSC without disinhibiting the contralesional iSC
The functional content of cortical signals relayed directly to iSC has been well characterized using antidromic identification (Segraves and Goldberg, 1987; Everling and Munoz, 2000; Sommer and Wurtz, 2000; Wurtz et al., 2001; Helminski and Segraves, 2003). However, due to other polysynaptic pathways, such as through the basal ganglia or other cortical areas, inactivation studies like ours are required to causally assess the collective influence of the FEF on iSC activity. The general agreement between our results and those that would have been predicted by antidromic studies alone is encouraging, reaffirming the FEF's role in providing excitatory input to the ipsilateral iSC, particularly for those neurons displaying saccade-related activity.
Long-range interactions between different regions of the iSC or between the FEFs are thought to be inhibitory (Munoz and Istvan, 1998; Schlag et al., 1998). Could the general reduction in ipsilesional iSC activity during FEF inactivation and the associated increases in response time arise from disinhibition of the contralesional FEF and/or iSC or from a shift toward increased activity of fixation neurons in the rostral iSC (Munoz and Wurtz, 1993; Dorris and Munoz, 1995)? While we did not record from the rostral iSC or contralesional FEF, several observations argue against these interpretations. First, the magnitude of contralesional iSC activity neither increased nor decreased, contrary to what would have been expected from disinhibition or increased rostral iSC activity, respectively. Second, unilateral FEF inactivation decreased the peak velocity and prevalence of cue-related microsaccades in both directions (Peel et al., 2016), which is more consistent with decreasing, rather than increasing, levels of rostral iSC activity (Hafed et al., 2009). Third, although FEF inactivation decreased the magnitude of visual responses on ipsilesional saccade-related iSC neurons, the latency of such responses was unchanged (Fig. 2). In contrast, during paradigms associated with increased rostral iSC activity, visual responses in the caudal iSC are both reduced in magnitude and delayed in onset latency (Marino et al., 2012).
Although FEF inactivation reduced ipsilesional iSC activity and delayed the onset of accumulation across our sample, there were instances where FEF inactivation had little to no effect on ipsilesional iSC activity, or decreased rather than increased the onset of accumulation. There are a number of potential reasons for this variability. It may be that not every iSC neuron we recorded receives input from the FEF, direct or otherwise. Further, any impact of FEF inactivation may be masked by compensatory inputs from other structures that converge on the same neuron. To our knowledge, there is no data that speak to this level of anatomical and physiological detail within the oculomotor network. Additionally, given the topographically aligned nature of projections from the FEF to the iSC (Sommer and Wurtz, 2000), it is possible that we occasionally recorded from iSC locations unaffected by FEF inactivation. The question of spatial register may also explain why FEF inactivation occasionally decreased both SRTs and the onset of accumulation (Fig. 6D, points in the lower-left quadrant). We did not find any spatial dependency in how much FEF inactivation affected iSC activity or behavior (e.g., larger cooling effects did not stem from particular parts of the visual field), but this analysis was hampered by the variability in loop location across monkeys and hemispheres.
A bilateral influence of the unilateral FEF on SRT and the onset of saccade-related accumulation in the iSC
The failure to observe disinhibition in the contralesional iSC is surprising given that focal pharmacological inactivation of the FEF facilitates ipsiversive oculomotor behaviors (Sommer and Tehovnik, 1997; Dias and Segraves, 1999; Wardak et al., 2006). Studies using focal microstimulation (Schlag et al., 1998; Seidemann et al., 2002) or paired bilateral recordings (Cohen et al., 2010) have also supported a view wherein the two FEFs compete in a push–pull fashion. In contrast, large-volume FEF inactivation tends to delay rather than facilitate ipsiversive oculomotor behaviors (Fig. 1; Peel et al., 2014, 2016; Kunimatsu et al., 2015), and although ipsiversive SRT increases were of lower magnitude and more idiosyncratic than the increases in contraversive SRTs, when present such SRT increases related best to delays in the onset of saccade-related accumulation in the corresponding iSC.
The behavioral and neurophysiological results produced by unilateral cryogenic inactivation of the FEF, reported here and elsewhere (Peel et al., 2014, 2016), also differ from that seen following unilateral cryogenic inactivation of the adjacent dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). As reported by Koval et al. (2011) and Johnston et al. (2014), unilateral cryogenic inactivation of the DLPFC via cooling loops implanted within the caudal principal sulcus shortens the response times of ipsiversive saccades and increases preparatory, visual-related, and saccade-related activity in the contralesional iSC. Thus, unlike what we observed following unilateral inactivation of the FEF, unilateral cryogenic inactivation of the nearby DLPFC does produce results consistent with disinhibition via a push–pull mechanism. Importantly, since the cooling loops implanted in the FEF or DLPFC were placed within the arcuate or caudal principal sulci respectively, it is unlikely that they inactivated overlapping volumes of tissue on the gyral crowns.
New perspectives on saccade initiation
FEF inactivation altered how iSC activity relates to saccade initiation in a manner that provides new insights into how the oculomotor brainstem initiates a saccade. One fundamental observation is that less saccade-related activity is emitted by ipsilateral iSC neurons during FEF inactivation, even for saccades matched closely for metrics and kinematics. Clearly, at the level of single iSC neurons, the saccade threshold can vary for movements to the center of the response field (Fig. 7). This observation complements observations of changes in the saccade threshold in the iSC (Everling et al., 1999; Jantz et al., 2013) or FEF (Heitz and Schall, 2012) during different tasks or cognitive sets. Heitz and Schall (2012) reconciled observations of increased SRTs despite decreasing thresholds by proposing a leaky integrator mechanism where saccade-related spikes are integrated over time to produce an invariant level of cumulative activity, and our results of decreasing thresholds and rates of accumulation upon FEF inactivation show that a similar mechanism may apply to the iSC. The decreased activation of iSC neurons for movements to the center of the response field could also be offset by increased activity from other off-center iSC neurons, extending the notion of saccade threshold within the iSC to forms of population coding envisaged for other aspects of saccade control (for review, see Gandhi and Katnani, 2011). It is also possible that an overall decrease in iSC activity could be offset by increasing activity in other areas that project to the brainstem burst generator, such as the fastigial nucleus (Noda et al., 1990), to produce an overall equivalent input to the brainstem burst generator. Understanding the contribution of these other cell populations, either within or outside of the iSC, is presumably required to fully explain SRT changes with FEF inactivation.
Previous evidence for fixed thresholds in the FEF (Hanes and Schall, 1996; Brown et al., 2008) and iSC (Paré and Hanes, 2003) fit well with rise-to-threshold (Carpenter and Williams, 1995; Reddi and Carpenter, 2000; Lo and Wang, 2006; Carpenter et al., 2009) and drift-diffusion models (Ratcliff et al., 2003, 2007) of saccade initiation, strengthening contentions that these models provided useful descriptions of neural activity. However, by embracing a greater subset of experimental tasks or conditions, the work of Heitz and Schall (2012, 2013) and Jantz and colleagues (2013) revealed that contemporary stochastic accumulator models failed to predict observed profiles of FEF or iSC activity. More broadly, delays in the onset of movement-related activity in the FEF and/or iSC, rather than changes in threshold or rate of accumulation, relate best to SRT increases in more difficult visual-search paradigms (Woodman et al., 2008), or to post-error increases in SRT within a stop-signal paradigm (Pouget et al., 2011). A recent review has emphasized the importance of incorporating the onset of activity within studies of decision making, particularly in tasks requiring top–down regulation (Teichert et al., 2016). Our inactivation findings, as well as our comparison of matched saccades even without FEF inactivation, extend these findings by demonstrating the importance of the onset of saccade-related activity in the iSC in delayed-response tasks, and by showing that such onset is governed at least in part by inputs from the FEF. It is becoming increasingly clear that the onset of saccade-related activity is a relevant metric that affects SRT. Future studies incorporating FEF inactivation and iSC recording during the performance of more unconstrained tasks requiring either a speeded response or target selection and decision making in light of multiple competing options are necessary to see whether our results generalize more widely; it may well be that variations in accumulation rate are critical determinants of SRT in such tasks. The results in the current report will serve as a benchmark for such efforts.
Footnotes
This work was supported by operating grants from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research to B.D.C. (MOPs: 93796, 123247, and 142317) and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council (NSERC; RGPIN-311680). T.R.P. was supported by an Ontario Graduate Scholarship and T.R.P. and S.D, were supported by funding from an NSERC Collaborative Research and Training Experience Program grant.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Brian D. Corneil, Robarts Research Institute, 1151 Richmond Street N, London, ON N6A 5B7, Canada. bcorneil{at}uwo.ca