Abstract
Animals show various behaviors in response to environmental chemicals. These behaviors are often plastic depending on previous experiences. Caenorhabditis elegans, which has highly developed chemosensory system with a limited number of sensory neurons, is an ideal model for analyzing the role of each neuron in innate and learned behaviors. Here, we report a new type of memory-dependent behavioral plasticity in Na+ chemotaxis generated by the left member of bilateral gustatory neuron pair ASE (ASEL neuron). When worms were cultivated in the presence of Na+, they showed positive chemotaxis toward Na+, but when cultivated under Na+-free conditions, they showed no preference regarding Na+ concentration. Both channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) activation with blue light and up-steps of Na+ concentration activated ASEL only after cultivation with Na+, as judged by increase in intracellular Ca2+. Under cultivation conditions with Na+, photoactivation of ASEL caused activation of its downstream interneurons AIY and AIA, which stimulate forward locomotion, and inhibition of its downstream interneuron AIB, which inhibits the turning/reversal behavior, and overall drove worms toward higher Na+ concentrations. We also found that the Gq signaling pathway and the neurotransmitter glutamate are both involved in the behavioral response generated by ASEL.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Animals have acquired various types of behavioral plasticity during their long evolutionary history. Caenorhabditis elegans prefers odors associated with food, but plastically changes its behavioral response according to previous experience. Here, we report a new type of behavioral response generated by a single gustatory sensory neuron, the ASE-left (ASEL) neuron. ASEL did not respond to photostimulation or upsteps of Na+ concentration when worms were cultivated in Na+-free conditions; however, when worms were cultivated with Na+, ASEL responded and inhibited AIB to avoid turning and stimulated AIY and AIA to promote forward locomotion, which collectively drove worms toward higher Na+ concentrations. Glutamate and the Gq signaling pathway are essential for driving worms toward higher Na+ concentrations.
Introduction
Animals sense a variety of environmental stimuli and change their behavioral responses according to previous experiences to eventually maximize their chance of survival. To uncover the versatile roles of sensory systems for sensing environmental stimuli, storing memory, and executing learned behaviors, it is important to examine the characteristics of neurons and molecules and delineate the interactions between them (Bargmann, 2006).
The nematode Caenorhabditis elegans has only 302 neurons in an adult hermaphrodite body, which have been well described, and possesses many genes homologous to those expressed in vertebrate brains, making it an ideal model organism for functional analysis of the nervous system (White et al., 1986). It also has a highly developed chemosensory system that enables it to detect a wide variety of sensory cues (volatile and water-soluble) associated with food, danger, or other animals. It is known that the pair of ASE neurons is responsible for sensing water-soluble chemicals, demonstrated by the fact that simultaneous ablation of all amphid and phasmid neurons except ASE spares chemotaxis (Bargmann and Horvitz, 1991). The ASE neuron class consists of a bilaterally symmetrical pair, ASE-left (ASEL) and ASE-right (ASER), both of which are involved in chemotaxis, but sense different sets of ions. ASEL responds to Mg2+, Li+, and Na+, whereas ASER responds to Br−, I−, and Cl− (see Fig. 1a; Ortiz et al., 2009).
C. elegans not only recognizes ions, but also memorizes and learns to move toward or away from certain concentrations of ions according to previous experience. When nematodes are grown in medium containing NaCl and food, they show attraction to NaCl via ASE neurons (Kunitomo et al., 2013). However, when they are starved with NaCl, the chemotaxis to NaCl falls dramatically or even becomes negative to avoid NaCl (Tomioka et al., 2006; Adachi et al., 2010).
Two behavioral mechanisms have been reported for salt chemotaxis: klinokinesis, in which worms change the direction of locomotion quickly with pirouettes (sharp turns) (Pierce-Shimomura et al., 1999) and klinotaxis, in which worms gradually curve toward higher (or lower) salt concentrations (Iino and Yoshida, 2009). In klinokinesis, worms increase their rate of pirouettes either when salt concentration decreases (driving positive chemotaxis) or when salt concentration increases (driving negative chemotaxis). ASEL and ASER neurons have lateralized functions: the ASEL neuron is stimulated by increased salt concentration and promotes forward locomotion and the ASER neuron is stimulated by decreased salt concentration and promotes either forward or turning behaviors (see Fig. 1a; Suzuki et al., 2008; Kunitomo et al., 2013). The Gq/diacylglycerol (DAG)/protein kinase C (PKC) pathway acts in ASER to promote migration to higher salt concentrations, possibly by augmenting the AIB response (Kunitomo et al., 2013). It was also reported that ASER-evoked curving toward lower concentrations is mediated by AIY interneurons (Satoh et al., 2014).
In contrast, the role and properties of the ASEL neuron in salt chemotaxis plasticity has not yet been examined thoroughly, although our previous study showed that, together with ASER, ASEL is important for NaCl chemotaxis learning (Adachi et al., 2010). To keep balance between external and internal conditions in the body, C. elegans, much like flies (Zhang et al., 2013) and mammals (Chandrashekar et al., 2010), prefers relatively low NaCl concentrations (Saeki et al., 2001; Bargmann, 2006). ASE neurons play the main role in salt chemotaxis at low concentrations of NaCl (<200 mm; Hukema et al., 2008). It has been also reported that, when ASEL senses a high external concentration of salt (750 mm), it releases INS-6 to AWC neurons to recruit them to act as interneurons for ASEL (Leinwand and Chalasani, 2013). Here, we investigate Na+ chemotaxis systematically and reveal that ASEL generates a new type of memory-dependent behavioral plasticity in Na+ chemotaxis. ASEL showed no response after Na+-free cultivation, but was activated after cultivation with Na+, released glutamate to activate AIY and AIA to stimulate forward movement, inhibited AIB to prevent turning behavior, and ultimately drove worms to higher concentrations of Na+.
Materials and Methods
Strains and culture.
All animals used in this study were hermaphrodites and were cultivated at 20°C under standard conditions (Brenner, 1974) except one strain. This strain (SRS281; Table 1) was cultivated at 25°C as described by Kocabas et al., 2012 and Satoh et al., 2014. All C. elegans strains were derived from the wild-type strain Bristol N2. All strains used for optogenetics carried the lite-1(ce314) mutation to diminish the innate avoidance of blue light (Edwards et al., 2008; Ward et al., 2008; Liu et al., 2010; Li et al., 2014). Fast-growing E. coli NA22 was used as food source in behavioral assays and chemotaxis assays to avoid starvation (Tomioka et al., 2006; Adachi et al., 2010; Kunitomo et al., 2013). OP50 was used for imaging experiments because animals cultivated with NA22 tended to exhibit high background fluorescence in the intestine (Oda et al., 2011; Kunitomo et al., 2013). The C. elegans strains and transgenic lines used in this study are listed in Table 1. ASEL-, AWC-, AIY-, AIA- and AIB-ablated animals were described previously (Table 1; Adachi et al., 2010; Yoshida et al., 2012; Kunitomo et al., 2013; Satoh et al., 2014).
Chemotaxis assay and tracking assay.
To evaluate animals' behaviors on an Na+ gradient, a modified chemotaxis assay was used. Test plates were prepared as shown in Figure 1b. The background agar was 8.5 cm in diameter and 1.76 mm in thickness (10 ml of solution was poured) and included 100 mm NH4Cl, pH 6.0, and 2% agar in chemotaxis buffer (25 mm potassium phosphate, pH 6.0, 1 mm CaCl2, 1 mm MgSO4). On top of the plate, two cylindrical agar blocks, each 14.5 mm in diameter and 5.3 mm in thickness, including either 100 mm NaCl (A site) or 100 mm NH4Cl (B site) and 2% agar in chemotaxis buffer, were placed for 24 h at 20°C and removed just before assay. This procedure created a concentration gradient on the test plate with a Na+ concentration of 45 mm at point A, 5 mm at point C, and 0 mm at point B and a NH4+ concentration of 65 mm at point A, 95 mm at point C, and 100 mm at point B based on a numerical simulation of diffusion (Iino and Yoshida, 2009; Kunitomo et al., 2013). NH4+ is mainly sensed by AWC neurons (Frøkjær-Jensen et al., 2008) and our results using an AWC-ablated strain showed that the NH4+ ion was not responsible for the chemotaxis observed in this study (see Fig. 1c). Therefore, this assay format is viable for testing Na+ chemotaxis. Animals were grown to young adults on standard nematode growth medium (NGM), and then transferred to NGM plates either without Na+ or with 100 mm Na+ for preassay cultivation for 6 h. One hundred to 200 animals were washed out from the plates and placed at the center of test plates (C point) (see Fig. 1b). One microliter each of 0.5 m NaN3 was spotted on points A and B of test plates just before the start of each assay. Animals were allowed to move for 45 min and the plates were chilled at 4°C before counting. Animals within a 2 cm radius from the center of each agar block were considered to be attracted and those within a 1 cm radius from the start point were excluded from the total number of animals to calculate the chemotaxis index (CI) as follows: The tracking assay was performed as described previously (Iino and Yoshida, 2009; Yoshida et al., 2012; Kunitomo et al., 2013) with some modifications. Thirty to 50 animals were washed out from culture plates with 100 mm NaCl and placed at the center of test plates and then chemotaxis was tested as described above (see Fig. 1b). Images of the whole test plate were captured for 15 min at 1 frame/s. Individual animals were tracked on the images and pirouette events were detected. Na+ concentration on the test plate was estimated by numerical simulation as above and pirouette frequency was calculated for each 0.1 mm/s bin of the time derivative of Na+ concentration (dC/dT) (Iino and Yoshida, 2009). Bins with fewer than 20 data points were omitted from analyses. Data from 150–600 s were used to calculate the pirouette frequency. The experiment was repeated independently 13 times.
Optogenetic stimulation and behavioral assay.
Behavioral response to photostimulation was quantified as described previously (Kunitomo et al., 2013) with some modifications. Briefly, young adults grown on standard NGM plates were further cultivated overnight under various salt concentrations supplemented with 10 μm all-trans retinal (ATR; Sigma-Aldrich). Worms cultivated under various salt concentrations without ATR were used as controls. Their behavior was tested on retinal-free chemotaxis plates without a salt gradient. Pulses of blue light (peak wavelength 470 nm; 0.2 mW/mm2) of a fixed length between 1 and 60 s, depending on the experiment, were delivered by a ring-shaped light-emitting diode after 2 min of recording without light. For each animal, light pulses were applied five times, with 80 s intervals between each pulse. Locomotion was monitored by the multiworm tracking system. The nonforward probability was calculated as the ratio of animals that were performing pirouettes, sharp turns, or pauses (collectively called nonforward) at each time point. At least six independent experiments were performed for each condition. The average nonforward probability in a 21 s window before stimulation (typically, time −30 s to −10 s) was set as NS (for “no stimulation”); the average nonforward probability around the peak value in a 5 s window (3 s for AIY#1; see Figs. 3g, 5d,e) during stimulation [typically, time 3–7 s (2–4 s for AIY#1)] was set as DS (for “during stimulation”); therefore, the change of nonforward probability during stimulation equals NS subtracted from DS.
Calcium imaging upon photoactivation.
Plasmids for germline transformation were generated by the Gateway system (Invitrogen) in which the LR reaction using Gateway LR Clonase (Invitrogen) was used to combine the promoter sequence on an entry vector and the open reading frame of the gene of interest on a destination vector. Details of this method have been described previously (Matsuki et al., 2006; Kunitomo et al., 2013). pENTR-gcy-7p, pENTR-npr-9p, pENTR-ttx-3p, pENTR-ins-1(short)p have also been described previously (Matsuki et al., 2006; Tomioka et al., 2006; Yoshida et al., 2012; Kunitomo et al., 2013). pDEST-RGECO was a gift from Prof. Takeshi Ishihara (Kyushu University) and pDEST-RCaMP2 was kindly provided by Dr. Keiko Gengyo-Ando (Saitama University, Japan), Prof. Junichi Nakai (Saitama University, Japan), and Prof. Haruhiko Bito (The University of Tokyo). Transgenic strains were generated by microinjection of the following DNA mixtures to a hermaphrodite germline: pG[gcy-7p::RCaMP2] (76.7 ng/μl); pG[lin-44::gfp] (19.5 ng/μl), pG[ttx-3p::RCaMP2] (72.5 ng/μl); pG[lin-44::gfp] (13.0 ng/μl), pG[ins-1(short)p::RGECO] (94.2 ng/μl); pG[lin-44::gfp] (26.1 ng/μl), pG[npr-9p::RCaMP2] (56.9 ng/μl); pG[lin-44::gfp] (18.4 ng/μl). We found no obvious defects in the calcium response to salt concentration changes in these animals. Animals were cultivated on standard NGM until they were young adults and then were further cultivated overnight on NGM without or with Na+ (50 mm). Then, an animal was immobilized in a microfluidic device (Chronis et al., 2007) with imaging solutions (containing 25 mm potassium phosphate, pH 6.0, 1 mm CaCl2, 1 mm MgSO4, 0.02% gelatin, 50 mm NaCl for worms cultivated without Na+ and 100 mm NaCl for worms cultivated with 50 mm Na+). Tetraethyl ammonium chloride (TEA, 20 mm) or 3,4 diaminopyridine (3,4-DAP, 1 mm) was added to the imaging solutions in the K+ channel blocker experiments (Jospin et al., 2002; Jones et al., 2011). Optical stimulation and imaging were performed on a fluorescent microscope system (Olympus) described previously (Satoh et al., 2014) with modifications. Briefly, we used an upright microscope (BX51; Olympus) equipped with a halogen light source (U-LH100IR) and with a UplanApo 40× objective [numerical aperture (NA), 0.85], 530/50 nm band-path excitation filter, and DV2 photometrics (DV2-cube, T565 lpxr; Olympus), a motorized stage (HV-STU02-1; HawkVision), a CCD camera (GRAS-03K2M-C; Point Gray Research), and a blue LED (470 nm, PE-100; CollLED) for worm observation and stimulation at 4 frames/s (exposure time: 50 ms). In each experiment, frame recording for calcium imaging was initiated at a fixed time of 5 min after removal of animal from preassay cultivation plates. Individual animals were subjected to a single recording and replaced for each experiment. Regulated photostimulation was started after 20 s of recording and consisted of 20 cycles of 150 ms stimulations with 350 ms intervals (10 s in total). Only fluorescence images obtained during the 350 ms intervals were used for quantification. Average fluorescence intensity of the neurons (cell body for ASEL; dendrites for AIB, AIY, and AIA) was calculated from the region of interest (ROI), which was moved to track the object using the Track Objects function of MetaMorph software (Molecular Devices), followed by subtraction of the average intensity of the background region. The average fluorescence in a 10 s window (typically, time 11–20 s; 41–80 frame) was set as F0 and the fluorescence intensity relative to F0 (F/F0) was calculated for a series of images. For each figure, F/F0 was averaged for all animals at each time point. The average F/F0 in a 5 s window before stimulation (typically, time 3–7 s) was set as NS; the average F/F0 in a 5 s window around the peak value during stimulation (typically, time 13–17 s; 16–10 s for AIB; see Fig. 4b) was set as DS. The F/F0 change during stimulation equals DS subtracted by NS. We note that, in some experiments (see Figs. 3e–g, 5c,d), the baselines of nonforward probability were different between conditions of ATR (−) and ATR (+). It appeared that the baselines of nonforward probability in AIY-channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) and AIA-ChR2 animals decreased and those in AIB-ChR2 increased under ATR (+) conditions. This might be because ChR2 worms were exposed to weak light (ambient light) throughout the experiments, which may have reduced the baseline of nonforward probability in AIY-ChR2 and AIA-ChR2 animals and increased the baseline of nonforward probability in AIB-ChR2 animals.
Calcium imaging upon Na+ stimulation.
Calcium imaging was performed as described previously (Kunitomo et al., 2013) with some modifications. Strain JN611 (see details in Table 1) was used in this study. Briefly, animals were cultivated on standard NGM plates until they were young adults and were then further cultivated on NGM plates with or without 50 mm NaCl overnight. One animal was then immobilized in a microfluidic device (described above) and NaCl concentration steps were delivered to its nose tip by switching the imaging solutions (solutions 1–4), as described below. Solution 1 contained 50 mm NaCl and 50 mm NH4Cl; solution 2 contained 60 mm NaCl and 40 mm NH4Cl; solution 3 contained 50 mm NH4Ac; and solution 4 contained 40 mm NH4Ac. All solutions also contained 25 mm potassium phosphate, pH 6.0, 1 mm CaCl2, 1 mm MgSO4, and 0.02% gelatin and were supplemented with glycerol to adjust the osmolarity to 350 mOsm. TEA (20 mm) and 3,4-DAP (1 mm) were added to the imaging solutions in K+ channel blocker experiments (Jospin et al., 2002; Jones et al., 2011). Time-lapse images were acquired with a DMI-6000B microscope (Leica) equipped with an HCX-PL-APO 63× objective (NA, 1.40), N2.1 filter set (a combination of 515∼560 nm band-path excitation filter and 580 nm dichromatic mirror; Leica), and the ImagEM EM-CCD camera (Hamamatsu) at 2 frames/s. The recording was started 5 min after fixation of animals in the microfluidic device. To shift Na+ and NH4+ concentrations, solution 1 was first used for the imaging buffer, switched to solution 2 after 25 s of recording, and then returned to solution 1 after 50 s recording. To switch NH4Ac concentrations, solution 3 was first used for the imaging buffer, switched to solution 4 after 25 s of recording, and then returned to solution 3 after 50 s recording. Fluorescence intensity in an ROI around the ASEL cell body was determined by correcting the position of ROI using Track Objects function of MetaMorph software (Molecular Devices), followed by subtracting the average intensity of a background region. The average fluorescence in a 10 s window (typically, time 11–20 s, 21–40 f) was set as F0 and the fluorescence intensity relative to F0 (F/F0) was calculated for a time series of images. For traces, a time series of F/F0 values were determined from one animal per experiment and averaged for all animals at each time point.
Confocal imaging of ChR2 expression.
Anesthetized hermaphrodite adults were imaged on a 5% agar pad. Cultivation of the JN611 strain was the same as the above section (“Calcium imaging upon Na+ stimulation”). Images of ChR2::Venus-expressing ASEL cell bodies (slice spacing of 0.5 μm) were acquired with a Leica SP5 confocal microscope using a 63×/1.30 objective. The fluorescence measurements of worms cultivated with and without Na+ were performed using the same parameter settings for excitation intensity, exposure time, and detector gain. For average ChR2::Venus fluorescence intensity of ASEL cell body, we determined an average pixel intensity of Z-stack images of ASEL cell bodies after subtraction of an average intensity of the background. For ChR2::Venus fluorescence intensity of ASEL membrane, a line was drawn across the ASEL cell body of a Z-slice image, which was located at the center of the ASEL cell body. The peak intensity of the ASEL membrane was determined by subtracting the maximum fluorescence intensity on the line by the minimum fluorescence intensity on the line.
Statistical analysis.
Statistical analyses were performed using GraphPad Prism 5 software. Two-tailed t test was performed to compare the differences between two conditions of one strain (see Figs. 1c,d, 5f) and between ATR (+) and ATR (−) (see Figs. 7b, 8b,c). If there were more than two conditions, one-way ANOVA was performed first to determine whether there was significant difference among the conditions. In cases where there was a significant effect of conditions, one of the following post hoc tests was performed: Dunnett's test was performed to test the differences between wild-type and mutants or cell-ablation lines (see Figs. 1c, 2, 3d); t test with Bonferroni's correction was performed to test the differences between ATR (+) and ATR (−) (see Figs. 1f, 3d–g, 4e, 6b) or test conditions (see Fig. 6b); Bonferroni's multiple-comparisons test (see Fig. 6a,c,d) or Tukey's test (see Fig. 7c) was performed to test many or all conditions in one experiment. Two-way ANOVA was used to test the effects of two factors, cultivation conditions, and strains (see Fig. 1c) or test conditions with the addition of ATR (see Figs. 1f, 3e–h, 5a–e). In these cases, only ATR (+) results were tested.
Results
ASEL generates memory-dependent behaviors in Na+ chemotaxis
To test chemotaxis to Na+, we conducted a chemotaxis assay using an agar plate format with a Na+ concentration gradient, but without a gradient for the counter-ion (Fig. 1b; see Materials and Methods for details). The chemotaxis assay revealed that, after cultivation in Na+-free conditions, worms showed no Na+ concentration preference but, after cultivation in the presence of 100 mm NaCl, worms showed positive Na+ chemotaxis (i.e., they migrated to higher Na+ concentrations; Fig. 1c). Detailed behavioral analysis revealed that, after cultivation with 100 mm Na+, worms showed a higher frequency of pirouette turns when Na+ concentration decreased, whereas the frequency of pirouettes was lower when Na+ concentration increased, indicating that worms are more likely to engage in forward movement behavior when sensing increasing concentrations of Na+ (Fig. 1d). Although ASEL is known to be the major Na+-sensing neuron, ASER also contributes to Na+ chemotaxis to a lesser extent (Fig. 1a; Ortiz et al., 2009). This minor influence on Na+ chemotaxis complicates analysis of downstream neural circuits and, because of this, we decided to focus on ASEL's role in Na+ chemotaxis by using optogenetics combined with behavioral assays and calcium imaging. Transgenic worms in which ChR2 was expressed only in ASEL were exposed to blue-light after preassay cultivation at different Na+ concentrations and their behavioral response was quantified by using the worm-tracking system. Because ASEL is activated by an increase in Na+ concentration, optogenetic activation of ASEL mimics an increase of Na+ concentrations. We evaluated the behavioral response by quantifying the nonforward probability, which was calculated as the ratio of animals that were performing pirouettes, sharp turns, or pauses at each time point. In other words, nonforward probability = 1 − the rate of forward locomotion. When worms were cultivated with Na+ and tested on plates at any Na+ concentration (including absence of Na+), the nonforward probability decreased (forward locomotion was stimulated) during blue-light stimulation of ASEL and increased after blue-light illumination was turned off (Fig. 1e,f). These results are similar to the previous observation of behavioral responses to step concentration changes of NaCl (Miller et al., 2005), consistent with the above results on pirouette frequency (Fig. 1d), and can generate chemotaxis toward a higher salt concentration through the klinokinesis mechanism. These results were consistent with the observation that, during the Na+ chemotaxis assay (Fig. 1b), worms that were cultivated with Na+ migrated to a higher concentration (Fig. 1h). Extending the stimulation time caused no change in worms' behavioral patterns (Fig. 1g), which was also the case in earlier reports on photoactivation of AIY (Li et al., 2014) and AWA (Larsch et al., 2015). After cultivation at 50 or 100 mm Na+, worms showed larger forward locomotion responses upon ASEL photoactivation on test plates with 100 mm Na+ compared with those with 50 or 0 mm Na+ (Fig. 1f).
AIB is required for Na+ chemotaxis after cultivation with Na+
AIB, AIY, and AIA are the first-layer interneurons that receive synaptic inputs from ASEL (White et al., 1986; Fig. 1a), but the role of each neuron in Na+ chemotaxis had not yet been examined. AIB-ablated animals showed significantly reduced Na+ chemotaxis when worms were cultivated at 100 mm Na+ (Fig. 2a), suggesting that AIB is required for transmission of sensory signals from ASEL. In the previous report, NaCl chemotaxis was affected minimally in AIB-ablated animals, whereas klinokinesis was reduced significantly (Kunitomo et al., 2013). Conversely, although AIA- or AIY-ablated animals showed reduced locomotion activity (Fig. 2c), their chemotaxis to Na+ was not affected severely (Fig. 2a). AIA/AIY double-ablated animals showed a defect in Na+ chemotaxis (Fig. 2b), suggesting that AIA and AIY may be involved in ASEL-induced chemotaxis. It is also possible, however, that the observed Na+ chemotaxis defect in the AIY/AIA double mutant was caused by reduced locomotion ability (Fig. 2c,d).
AIB, AIY, and AIA are all required for stimulation of forward locomotion by ASEL after cultivation with Na+
AIB is activated upon down-stepping of the NaCl concentration through the off-response neuron ASER (Oda et al., 2011; Kunitomo et al., 2013). Because AIB was required for Na+ chemotaxis, we investigated whether AIB is required for the behavioral response generated by ASEL activation. Behavioral response upon stimulation of ASEL was diminished significantly in AIB-ablated animals (Fig. 3a,d). It was therefore suggested that AIB was required for stimulation of forward locomotion upon activation of ASEL by ChR2. AIY is an important interneuron for controlling multiple behaviors and is involved in regulating reversal and gradual turning (Kocabas et al., 2012; Satoh et al., 2014). Behavioral responses after ASEL photoactivation were completely eliminated in AIY-ablated animals: there was no decrease of nonforward probability during ASEL photoactivation or increase of nonforward probability after termination of ASEL stimulation (Fig. 3b,d). Furthermore, similar to ASEL, AIA is reportedly activated in response to up-steps of NaCl concentrations, implying that AIA receives excitatory inputs from ASEL either directly or indirectly (Oda et al., 2011). We found that the change of nonforward probability was significantly smaller or eliminated when ASEL was stimulated by ChR2 in AIA-ablated animals (Fig. 3c,d), indicating that AIA is required for the behavioral response to ASEL activation. Therefore, ASEL requires all first-layer interneurons, AIB, AIY, and AIA, for generating behavioral responses.
AIB promotes turning behavior, whereas AIY and AIA promote forward locomotion
Interneurons AIB, AIY, and AIA have synapses with many of the sensory neurons, including ASER, AWC, and ASEL, all of which are implicated in chemotaxis to chemical cues (Chalasani et al., 2007, 2010; Kunitomo et al., 2013; Satoh et al., 2014). AIB showed increased calcium levels upon odor removal (Chalasani et al., 2007) and decreased NaCl concentration (Kunitomo et al., 2013) and stimulated turning behavior (Fig. 1a). Behavioral assays showed that the nonforward probability increased during AIB stimulation by ChR2 in many conditions of Na+ concentrations (Fig. 3e) and the magnitude of the response was not related to test concentrations or cultivation concentrations. In contrast, AIY was reported to show increased calcium level upon odor addition and decreased calcium level upon odor removal (Chalasani et al., 2007) and AIY activation promoted forward locomotion and inhibited turning (Fig. 1a; Li et al., 2014). For stimulation of AIY, we used two strains that express ChR2 in AIY (strain #1 and #2; see Materials and Methods). When AIY was stimulated by ChR2, the nonforward probability decreased in both strains during AIY stimulation and increased after termination of AIY stimulation in most cultivation conditions (Fig. 3g,h). It was also reported that AIA is activated by the addition of odor (Chalasani et al., 2010) and that activated AIA inhibits turning behavior (Fig. 1a; Larsch et al., 2015). Our results revealed that, when AIA was stimulated by ChR2, the behavioral response was similar to that of AIY stimulation: the nonforward probability decreased during stimulation of AIA by ChR2 and increased after termination of AIA stimulation in most conditions of cultivation Na+ concentrations (Fig. 3f), Therefore, behavioral response to ASEL stimulation was similar to AIY stimulation or AIA stimulation but opposite to AIB stimulation, suggesting a positive relationship between ASEL and AIY/AIA but a negative relationship between ASEL and AIB.
ASEL inhibits AIB and activates AIY and AIA after cultivation with Na+
We used a combination of photostimulation with ChR2 and calcium monitoring with RCaMP2 (Inoue et al., 2015) or RGECO (only for AIA) (Zhao et al., 2011) to observe directly the response of each neuron to activation of ASEL (Fig. 4a–e; see Materials and Methods for details of experimental setup). We first confirmed that the calcium content of ASEL was increased upon ASEL stimulation (Fig. 4a,e). The calcium level of AIB was decreased significantly upon ASEL photostimulation, indicating that AIB was inhibited by ASEL (Fig. 4b,e). AIY's calcium content was increased during photoactivation of ASEL (Fig. 4d,e), revealing that AIY was activated by ASEL. In addition, some variation of calcium levels in AIB and AIY were observed before photostimulation of ASEL, possibly because of the spontaneously activating characteristics of these neurons (Chalasani et al., 2007; Gordus et al., 2015). Similar to AIY, AIA's calcium content was increased upon ASEL stimulation, indicating that AIA was also activated by ASEL (Fig. 4c,e). Even under cultivation conditions without ATR, blue light stimulation decreased calcium levels of AIY and ASEL. Although we used strains with the lite-1 background for optogenetic experiments, lite-1 mutations were reported to diminish but not completely eliminate worms' response to blue light stimulation, suggesting that intrinsic response to blue light remains in these strains to some extent (Edwards et al., 2008; Ward et al., 2008; Li et al., 2014; Liu et al., 2010). Therefore, judging from the difference between ATR (+) and ATR (−), all three first-layer interneurons respond to ASEL photoactivation: AIB was inhibited by ASEL, which is expected to inhibit turning behavior, whereas AIY and AIA were activated by ASEL to possibly promote forward locomotion; therefore, all three interneurons may contribute to driving worms to higher Na+ concentrations (Fig. 4f). AIA and AIY, together with AIB, mediate ASEL-generated behavioral response after cultivation with Na+.
Cellular basis of behavioral plasticity caused by cultivation with or without Na+
In the chemotaxis assay, we observed that, when worms were cultivated in Na+-free conditions, they had no Na+ concentration preference (Fig. 1c). This is not due to general deficits in sensory or motor functions because the same cultivation conditions do not affect chemotaxis to odorants (Kunitomo et al., 2013). To determine the underlying mechanism for modulation, we tested the behavioral response to ASEL stimulation after Na+-free cultivation. When worms were cultivated in Na+-free conditions, they showed no behavioral response to ASEL photostimulation in the presence of Na+ (Fig. 5a). We tested which ion, Na+ or Cl−, changes the behavioral response to ASEL stimulation when present during preassay cultivation. We used NaAc instead of NaCl as a salt included in either the cultivation or test plates. As a result, worms displayed similar behavioral responses upon ASEL activation after NaCl or NaAc cultivation: increased forward movement during stimulation. Likewise, replacing NaCl with NaAc in test plates did not cause a significant difference (Fig. 6a,b), indicating that Na+ is responsible for the plasticity in the behavioral response to ASEL stimulation.
The next question was whether Na+ is sensed directly by ASEL during preassay cultivation to generate behavioral plasticity. To answer this, we made use of the dyf-11 mutant, which has deformed cilia and therefore cannot sense water-soluble chemicals (Kunitomo and Iino, 2008). When worms were cultivated with Na+, reduced turning behavior was no longer observed in dyf-11 mutants when ASEL was stimulated by ChR2 and this defect was rescued by dyf-11 expression only in ASEL (Fig. 6c). When worms were cultivated in Na+-free conditions, they showed increase of nonforward probability upon ASEL photoactivation in the dyf-11 mutant and this response was suppressed by dyf-11 expression in ASEL neurons (Fig. 6d). These results indicated that ASEL alone could generate Na+-dependent plasticity (behavioral response only after cultivation with Na+).
Conversely, when AIB, AIY, or AIA was stimulated by ChR2, wild-type worms cultivated in Na+-free conditions showed similar responses to those cultivated with Na+ (Figs. 3e–h, 5b–e). This implied that the difference in the behavioral responses after Na+-containing and Na+-free cultivation conditions was attributed to neuronal responses upstream of first-layer interneurons, possibly in ASEL. In fact, ASEL showed no calcium response upon ASEL photostimulation after Na+-free cultivation (Fig. 5f). This is likely the reason that there was no behavioral response to ASEL stimulation after Na+-free cultivation and no Na+ concentration preference after cultivation in Na+-free conditions (Fig. 5g).
Inhibition of K+ channel rescued ASEL's response after cultivation without Na+
Due to the unexpected lack of calcium response of ASEL after photoactivation, we investigated whether ChR2 was still present in the ASEL neuron after cultivation without Na+ by observing ChR2 expression under a confocal microscope. We found that ChR2 tagged with Venus was still present and localized to the cell membrane of ASEL after cultivation without Na+ (Fig. 7a) and the fluorescence intensity of ChR2::Venus in the ASEL cell body and membrane was not different from that in animals that were cultivated with Na+ (Fig. 7b). This result revealed that expression and localization of ChR2 in ASEL was not affected by the presence or absence of Na+ in cultivation conditions.
Next, we investigated ASEL's calcium responses to Na+ concentration changes after cultivation without Na+. ASEL showed no calcium response when Na+ concentration was changed from 50 to 60 mm after cultivation without Na+; whereas, after cultivation with Na+, the calcium level of ASEL increased when the Na+ concentration was changed from 50 to 60 mm (Fig. 7c,e). Because the concentration of the counter ion, NH4+, also changed upon Na+ concentration change during imaging assays, we tested the effect of NH4+ concentration changes on ASEL's calcium response. Consistent with the previous report showing that a 10 mm concentration change of NH4+ did not cause a calcium response in ASEL (Suzuki et al., 2008), ASEL showed no response to a 10 mm down-step of NH4+ after cultivation with or without Na+ (Fig. 7c).
The above observations suggested that, after cultivation without Na+, ASEL was not activated in response to cation influx by either ChR2 or a sensory input. This could be due to activation of a shunting function and likely candidates for this function are K+ channels (Daoudal and Debanne, 2003). We therefore used K+ channel blockers to determine whether any of them could rescue ASEL's response to Na+ concentration changes after cultivation without Na+. The results showed that ASEL's calcium level was increased in response to 10 mm Na+ up-step (Fig. 7c) or ASEL photostimulation (Fig. 7d) even after cultivation without Na+ when the K+ channel blockers TEA and 3,4-DAP were added to the perfusion buffer. Therefore, K+ channels play a role in making ASEL resistant to excitatory stimuli after cultivation without Na+, which may be mitigated by K+ channel blockers to allow ASEL depolarization by up-steps of Na+ concentrations or photostimulation (Fig. 7e).
eat-4 and egl-30 in ASEL are involved in ASEL-triggered behavioral response
eat-4, which encodes a vesicular glutamate transporter, is necessary for glutamatergic transmission in C. elegans (Lee et al., 1999; Rand et al., 2000; Lee et al., 2008). eat-4 is also expressed in ASE neurons (Serrano-Saiz et al., 2013), which implies that ASE neurons release glutamate onto downstream interneurons. An eat-4 mutation eliminated the behavioral response upon ASEL activation after cultivation with Na+. Moreover, cell-specific knockdown of eat-4 in ASEL by RNA interference caused smaller behavioral responses than those seen in wild-type animals during ASEL stimulation (Fig. 8a), suggesting that glutamate is used as a neurotransmitter in ASEL for the behavioral responses.
egl-30, which encodes an ortholog of the α subunit of heterotrimetric G-protein Gq, regulates locomotory movements positively (Brundage et al., 1996; Lackner et al., 1999; Adachi et al., 2010). The Gq/DAG/PKC pathway modulates NaCl chemotaxis and counteracts phophatidylinositol 3-kinase signaling (Tomioka et al., 2006; Adachi et al., 2010; Kunitomo et al., 2013). Conversely, manipulation of the Gq signaling pathway in ASEL has only a marginal effect on chemotaxis to NaCl (Adachi et al., 2010). We therefore investigated whether the Gq signaling pathway regulates ASEL-dependent chemotaxis. egl-30(pe914), a gain-of-function mutation, is characterized by hyperactive locomotion (Tomioka et al., 2006; Adachi et al., 2010). In the transgenic strain in which egl-30(pe914) was expressed in ASEL, the behavioral response was not observed during or after stimulation of ASEL after cultivation with Na+ (Fig. 8b), suggesting that egl-30 negatively regulated the response to ASEL stimulation. After Na+-free cultivation, the transgenic worms showed a small increase of turning behavior during stimulation of ASEL by ChR2 (Fig. 8c). These results also implied that egl-30 regulates Na+ chemotaxis, and possibly the behavioral plasticity in Na+ chemotaxis, negatively.
Discussion
Unexpectedly, we found that ASEL generates a novel type of memory-dependent behavioral plasticity in Na+ chemotaxis: when worms were cultivated with Na+, they migrated to higher Na+ concentrations (Figs. 1c, 4f), whereas when cultivated in the absence of Na+ and placed in an Na+-containing environment, they showed no Na+ concentration preference (Figs. 1c, 5g). Behavioral responses to optogenetic activation of the ASEL neuron approximately recapitulated the chemotaxis responses: worms showed no response when they were transferred from Na+-free cultivation to Na+-containing conditions (Fig. 5a), whereas they showed reduction of turning frequency upon activation of ASEL after cultivation with Na+ (Fig. 1e,f). Calcium imaging upon photoactivation or Na+ stimulation provided mechanistic insights into this new type of Na+ chemotaxis plasticity: ASEL showed no calcium response upon photostimulation or up-step of Na+ concentrations after worms were cultivated in Na+-free conditions (Fig. 5f). As one of the mechanisms for learning and memory, changes in neuronal excitability are well documented, for example, for long-term changes in piriform cortex (Saar and Barkai, 2009), amygdala (Sehgal et al., 2014), and hippocampus in rodents (Gruart et al., 2012). In these events, voltage-gated and leak cation channels are often involved. We found that the calcium response of ASEL was restored in the presence of the K+ channel inhibitors TEA and 3,4-DAP (Fig. 7c,d). There are several possible explanations for these observations. First, cultivation without Na+ may cause hyperpolarization of ASEL neuron at the resting states through opening of K+ channels, making it more resistant to activating stimuli. Second, the voltage-dependent K+ channel may open when extracellular cations flow into ASEL through ChR2 upon ASEL photostimulation and export intracellular cation in ASEL to keep ASEL's resting membrane potential, which in turn makes ASEL more difficult to be excited. Third, more K+ ions may leak out of ASEL to shunt the effect of stimulation, which is also considered as reduction in input resistance. No matter which possibility is true, our current findings might be the first discovery of change in intrinsic excitability as a mechanism of cellular plasticity mechanism in neuronal cells in C. elegans.
The neural circuits functioning downstream of ASEL are in contrast to those of the well studied AWC neuron, which mediate chemotaxis to odor (Chalasani et al., 2007, 2010). Both AWC neurons and ASE neurons are glutamatergic (Chalasani et al., 2007; Serrano-Saiz et al., 2013) and, as seen in AWC neurons, we found that glutamatergic neurotransmission from ASEL plays an important role in behavioral response for Na+ chemotaxis (Fig. 8a). AWC neurons are activated by odor removal similar to ASER activation by decrease in ion concentration (OFF sensory response), whereas ASEL is activated by the increase in salt concentration (ON sensory response; Chalasani et al., 2007; Suzuki et al., 2008). Furthermore, all of these sensory neurons send synaptic outputs to the interneurons AIA, AIY, and AIB. However, in contrast to ASEL, AWC releases glutamate to inhibit AIY and AIA while activating AIB to stimulate odor chemotaxis and local search behavior (Chalasani et al., 2007, 2010). Similar to AWC, ASER, also an OFF neuron, activates the interneuron AIB to migrate to higher salt concentrations in salt chemotaxis (Kunitomo et al., 2013). Conversely, ASEL works inversely of AWC in that it activates AIA and AIY and inhibits AIB. Overall, these interneurons are expected to promote migration to higher Na+ concentrations given that ASEL shows an ON response after cultivation with Na+. Our study provides a very interesting cue to understanding what differentiates ON and OFF sensory neurons even though they use the same interneurons for behavioral response and similar neural circuits for regulating chemotaxis. It will be necessary to investigate glutamate receptors acting on the interneurons in the ASEL neural circuit for Na+ chemotaxis.
There are multiple behavioral mechanisms and multiple sensory neurons involved in chemotaxis. The calcium level of ASEL was increased by ∼10% as a response to 10 mm up-step of Na+ (Fig. 7c) and ∼20% in response to photostimulation (Fig. 5f). However, during chemotaxis, worms sense much smaller changes (<0.06 mm/s) in Na+ concentrations (Fig. 1d), so the contribution of interneurons might be different. Although ASEL, along with ASER, contributes to both klinokinesis and klinotaxis (Iino and Yoshida, 2009), we only examined the effect of ASEL activation on turning behavior, which is the main component of klinokinesis. AIB, AIY, and AIA are all important interneurons involved in the klinokinesis mechanism (Iino and Yoshida, 2009; Kunitomo et al., 2013). Activation or ablation of each of these neurons affects reversal/turning behaviors (Gray et al., 2005; Kocabas et al., 2012; Kunitomo et al., 2013; this study). AIA laser-ablated worms show a severe defect in the klinokinesis mechanism (Iino and Yoshida, 2009), AIB receives synaptic input from AIA, and both interneurons are involved in klinokinesis (Iino and Yoshida, 2009; Kunitomo et al., 2013). AIB sends synaptic outputs to command interneurons AVA, AVB, and AVE, which regulate forward/reversal locomotion through synaptic outputs to body wall motor neurons, and also to RIM interneurons, which are important for regulation of reversal (Piggott et al., 2011). AIZ interneurons, which receive inputs from AIY, are essential interneurons for the klinokinesis mechanism (Iino and Yoshida, 2009) and are also connected to the command interneurons both directly and indirectly. In addition, head motor neurons are important for the turning behavior. RIA neurons, which receive synaptic inputs from both AIY and AIZ and are connected to SMD and RMD motor neurons, are also important for the klinokinesis mechanism. Therefore, concerted actions of the primary interneurons AIB, AIY, and AIA must be a driving force regulating the klinokinesis mechanism to generate chemotaxis to Na+ and other ions and odors.
Our results indicated that the Gq signaling pathway can act in both ASEL and ASER to regulate behavioral plasticity. Upregulation of the Gq signaling pathway in ASER causes worms' attraction to higher NaCl concentrations, not only in starved conditions (Adachi et al., 2010), but also in well fed conditions (Kunitomo et al., 2013), suggesting that the Gq/PKC pathway has a function in driving worms to higher salt concentrations (Adachi et al., 2010; Kunitomo et al., 2013). Conversely, we showed that activation of egl-30 in ASEL eliminated the promotion of forward movement upon ASEL stimulation after cultivation with Na+ (Fig. 8b) and caused weak turning during ASEL stimulation after Na+-free cultivation (Fig. 8c), suggesting that the Gq signaling pathway may negatively regulate attraction to Na+ after cultivation at high Na+ concentrations. In view of the lateralized characteristics of ASE neurons, in which ASEL neurons are activated by an increase of NaCl concentration whereas ASER neurons are activated by a decrease of NaCl concentration (Suzuki et al., 2008), it is interesting that the Gq signaling pathway modulates attraction to salts in opposite ways in ASEL and ASER neurons. It would be necessary to investigate the involvement of components of the Gq signaling pathway, such as DAG and ttx-4/pkc-1, in ASEL for Na+ chemotaxis (Brundage et al., 1996; Sieburth et al., 2007; Adachi et al., 2010; Kunitomo et al., 2013).
This is the first report to analyze systematically the characteristics of ASEL, which plays an important role in Na+ chemotaxis. The neural mechanisms underlying Na+ chemotaxis plasticity is different from NaCl chemotaxis plasticity mediated by ASER, in which worms get attracted to NaCl when grown with salt and food (Kunitomo et al., 2013) but avoid NaCl when starved with NaCl (Tomioka et al., 2006; Adachi et al., 2010). It also differs from odor chemotaxis plasticity, meaning that, when animals are kept with a certain odor without food, they no longer show attraction to that odor (Colbert and Bargmann, 1995), and from concentration-dependent odor chemotaxis, in which odor-sensing neurons switch between high-concentration odor avoidance and low-concentration odor attraction behaviors (Yoshida et al., 2012). All of these chemosensory behaviors are mediated by overlapping neural circuits and our results will extend the platform for further understanding the versatile actions of the small neural circuit of C. elegans.
Footnotes
This work was supported by Grants-in-Aid for Innovative Area “Memory dynamism” (25115010) and “Comprehensive Brain Network” (221S0003) and a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (B) (26291069) to Y.I.; “Mesoscopic Neurocircuitry” to H.K. from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology of Japan; and by “The Salt Science Foundation” (No. 1635) to H.K. We thank Takeshi Ishihara for R-GECO; Keiko Gengyo-Ando, Junichi Nakai, and Haruhiko Bito for R-CaMP2; Olympus for help in setting up the optogenetics-imaging system used in this research; Manami Kanamori for performing microinjections of R-CaMP2 and R-GECO; and Yu Toyoshima for discussion of optogenetics-imaging experiments.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Yuichi Iino, The University of Tokyo, Science Building No.3, Rm. 224, 2-11-16 Yayoi, Bunkyo-ku, Tokyo 113-0032, Japan. iino{at}bs.s.u-tokyo.ac.jp