Abstract
In chronic pain, the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is deactivated and mPFC-dependent tasks such as attention and working memory are impaired. We investigated the mechanisms of mPFC deactivation in the rat spared nerve injury (SNI) model of neuropathic pain. Patch-clamp recordings in acute slices showed that, 1 week after the nerve injury, cholinergic modulation of layer 5 (L5) pyramidal neurons was severely impaired. In cells from sham-operated animals, focal application of acetylcholine induced a left shift of the input/output curve and persistent firing. Both of these effects were almost completely abolished in cells from SNI-operated rats. The cause of this impairment was an ∼60% reduction of an M1-coupled, pirenzepine-sensitive depolarizing current, which appeared to be, at least in part, the consequence of M1 receptor internalization. Although no changes were detected in total M1 protein or transcript, both the fraction of the M1 receptor in the synaptic plasma membrane and the biotinylated M1 protein associated with the total plasma membrane were decreased in L5 mPFC of SNI rats. The loss of excitatory cholinergic modulation may play a critical role in mPFC deactivation in neuropathic pain and underlie the mPFC-specific cognitive deficits that are comorbid with neuropathic pain.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) undergoes major reorganization in chronic pain. Deactivation of mPFC output is causally correlated with both the cognitive and the sensory component of neuropathic pain. Here, we show that cholinergic excitation of commissural layer 5 mPFC pyramidal neurons is abolished in neuropathic pain rats due to a severe reduction of a muscarinic depolarizing current and M1 receptor internalization. Therefore, in neuropathic pain rats, the acetylcholine (ACh)-dependent increase in neuronal excitability is reduced dramatically and the ACh-induced persisting firing, which is critical for working memory, is abolished. We propose that the blunted cholinergic excitability contributes to the functional mPFC deactivation that is causal for the pain phenotype and represents a cellular mechanism for the attention and memory impairments comorbid with chronic pain.
Introduction
Working memory and attention impairments accompany chronic pain in patients (Moriarty et al., 2011; Baker et al., 2016) and in animals (Pais-Vieira et al., 2009a; Ren et al., 2011), and animal models of persistent pain are characterized by functional deactivation of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) (Metz et al., 2009; Neugebauer et al., 2009; Pais-Vieira et al., 2009b; Ji et al., 2010; Lee et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2015; Kelly et al., 2016), although the mechanisms of the deactivation remain largely unexplored. The mPFC receives and integrates synaptic inputs from sensory areas (Vertes et al., 2007) and limbic regions (Vertes, 2006), which impart emotional salience onto peripheral stimuli, and modulatory afferents from regions such as the nucleus basalis of the basal forebrain (Mesulam et al., 1983), which provide cholinergic input important for cue detection and “allocation of processing resources” (Sarter and Bruno, 1997). The more ventral subregions of the mPFC, the prelimbic (PL) and infralimbic (IL) cortices, receive hippocampal (Jay et al., 1989; Parent et al., 2010), amygdalar, and thalamic glutamatergic inputs (Krettek and Price, 1977; Vertes et al., 2007). Cholinergic afferents can activate excitatory networks in both layer 2/3 (L2/3) (Vidal and Changeux, 1993) and L5 (Guillem et al., 2011) and the disruption of cholinergic signaling in the mPFC has been shown to adversely affect attentional processing and working memory tasks (Granon et al., 1995). Pyramidal neurons in L5, which provide the main output of the mPFC, receive these multiple inputs and create the reciprocal connections thought to underlie working memory (Goldman-Rakic, 1995). Cortical cholinergic modulation is related to attention and working memory (Croxson et al., 2011; Hasselmo and Sarter, 2011) and is mediated by both nicotinic and muscarinic receptors (Mansvelder et al., 2006; Arroyo et al., 2014). In the mPFC, activation of postsynaptic M1 receptors modulates pyramidal cell excitability through several mechanisms. It mediates a slow membrane depolarization (Haj-Dahmane and Andrade, 1996; Kurowski et al., 2015) that enhances temporal summation (Carr and Surmeier, 2007) and increases neuronal firing frequency (Satake et al., 2008). M1 activation also facilitates a slow afterdepolarization (sADP) (Haj-Dahmane and Andrade, 1997; 1999; Lei et al., 2014) which promotes persistent firing (McQuiston and Madison, 1999). Acetylcholine (ACh) release in the mPFC correlates with temporally discrete tasks such as successful cue detection (Parikh et al., 2007) and with the persistent activity necessary for spatial working memory (Jung et al., 1998; Romanides et al., 1999). Interestingly, these executive functions, which require cholinergic modulation of the medial prefrontal circuitry, are impaired in different models of chronic pain (Pais-Vieira et al., 2009a; Ji et al., 2010; Ren et al., 2011).
Because of the fundamental role of cholinergic modulation in attention and memory tasks (Klinkenberg and Blokland, 2011; Arnsten and Rubia, 2012), we hypothesized that the increase in neuronal excitability induced by cholinergic modulation is impaired in the pain condition; if so, this may represent both a mechanism for cortical deactivation and a cellular substrate for the mPFC-dependent cognitive impairments. To assess this hypothesis, we used ex vivo slices of rat PL mPFC to compare the effects of cholinergic modulation in control animals and in the spared nerve injury (SNI) model of neuropathic pain 1 week after neuropathic lesion. We focused on L5 pyramidal cells because these cells provide the main output to the nucleus accumbens, the deactivation of which plays a causal role in the neuropathic pain phenotype (Lee et al., 2015). We found that the increased intrinsic excitability of L5 pyramidal neurons induced by M1 receptor activation in sham animals was virtually abolished in SNI animals. Although the total amount of M1 protein was unaffected, both the level of M1 protein expressed at cell surface and the fraction associated with the synaptic plasma membrane (SPM) were reduced, suggesting receptor internalization as a mechanism for the disruption of cholinergic modulation in neuropathic pain.
Materials and Methods
Protocols.
All experiments followed protocols approved by the Northwestern University Center for Comparative Medicine.
SNI model.
Twenty-one- to 24-d-old male Sprague Dawley rats were anesthetized using gas anesthesia (isoflurane 2–3% and 30% N2O, 70% O2). The left sciatic nerve was exposed at the level of the trifurcation of the peroneal, tibial, and sural branches. Both the peroneal and tibial nerves were tightly ligated at 2 separate points ∼3 mm apart using #6 sutures (Decosterd and Woolf, 2000). Sterile scissors were used to make cuts within both suture points and the excised 3 mm nerves were removed; the sural nerve was left intact. The skin was then sutured and treated with antibiotic ointment. Animals were placed under a heating element until they regained consciousness. A second group of animals received a sham surgery. In this case, the nerves were exposed but left untouched.
Behavioral testing.
Seven days after SNI/sham surgery, tactile withdrawal responses were measured by mechanically stimulating the left hindpaw (in the area corresponding to sural nerve innervation) using von Frey hairs. Animals were placed in a cage with a wire grid floor within an isolated room and allowed to habituate for a minimum of 20 min. Filaments (Stoelting) of increasing force were applied to the plantar surface of the hindpaw for a maximum of 6 s. Paw withdrawal during application was recorded as a positive response. Fifty percent response thresholds were calculated according to the method of Chapman et al. (1998). SNI surgery induced a robust allodynic response in all animals (Fig. 1).
Tactile threshold is markedly reduced in the injured paw of SNI rats 1 week after surgery. Before surgery (week 0), there was no significant difference in the tactile threshold of the right and left hindpaws of sham (14.47 ± 3.5 g and 12.2 ± 3.0 g, respectively, n = 19) or SNI animals (15.8 ± 4.8 g and 14.97 ± 3.8 g, respectively, n = 14). One week after surgery (week 1), tactile thresholds in sham animals were 12.7 ± 2.6 g on the right paw contralateral to the surgery site and 8.6 ± 1.4 g on the left paw. In SNI animals, no significant change in threshold was detected in the right hindpaw (10.4 ± 2.8 g), whereas a significant drop was observed in the left hindpaw ipsilateral to the surgery site (0.4 ± 0.1 g, p = 0.002).
Patch-clamp recordings in acute slices.
Twenty-eight- to 31-d-old male Sprague Dawley rats that had previously undergone either SNI or sham surgery were anesthetized with an intraperitoneal injection of ketamine and xylazine (100 and 20 mg/kg). The animals were then perfused with an ice-cold N-methyl-d-glucamine (NMDG) solution containing the following (in mm): 92 NMDG, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 30 NaH2CO3, 20 HEPES, 25 glucose, 2 thiourea, 5 Na l-ascorbate, 2 Na-pyruvate, 0.5 CaCl2, and 10 MgCl2, pH 7.32–7.4 (titrated with 4 n HCl and saturated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2). The brain was removed from the skull in ice-cold NMDG solution and cut just caudal to bregma. Then, 300-μm-thick coronal slices were cut using a vibro-slicer (Leica VT-1200), stored for ∼20 min at 35°C, and allowed to recover at room temperature (22–24°C) for at least 30 min in NMDG solution (saturated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2). For recordings, slices were transferred to a recording chamber and continuously superfused with ACSF containing the following (in mm): 125 NaCl, 25 NaHCO3, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 25 glucose, 1.2 CaCl2, and 1 MgCl2 saturated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2. Bath temperature was kept between 30°C and 32°C using a TC-324B control unit (Warner Instruments). L5 pyramidal neurons of the mPFC contralateral to the site of surgery were visualized using an upright microscope (Olympus) with oblique illumination and video microscopy using a digital camera (DVC). L5 cells were identified according to location, size, and shape and were filled with biocytin for subsequent histological processing. Because the mPFC is heterogeneous, we made an effort to ensure that the cell populations from sham and SNI animals were homogenous. This was achieved by monitoring cell location and cell-firing pattern. All cells analyzed in this study were located between 525 and 1159 μm from pia in the mPFC (as assessed using online imaging of patched neurons). In addition, the locations of all of the neurons used for current-clamp experiments and a subset of those for the voltage-clamp experiments were also confirmed by post hoc examination of biocytin-filled cells. These neurons are plotted relative to distance from pia and apex and shown in Figure 2A. For somata of cells from sham-operated rats, the main distance from pia and apex was 726.4 ± 22 μm and 2227 ± 61.6 μm, respectively (n = 31); neurons from SNI rats were located 685.6 ± 22 μm from pia and 2315.7 ± 57 μm from apex (n = 22). L5 mPFC neurons comprise two main populations, one projecting to the pons and one to the contralateral cortex (COM). These two populations can be separated on the basis of their firing properties (Dembrow et al., 2010). According to these parameters, all cells used for our current-clamp recordings could be classified as COM because they exhibited accommodating firing patterns and were all capable of firing with 100 pA depolarizing current injections from a membrane potential of −65 mV (Fig. 2B). Cells recorded in voltage clamp, however, could not be classified functionally because the internal solution contained Cs-methanesulfonate and QX-314. However, we found no evidence for cellular heterogeneity with regard to ACh response between L5 pyramidal neurons. All recordings were performed using an Axopatch 200B amplifier. Signals were filtered at 10 kHz and sampled at 20 kHz for current-clamp recordings and filtered at 5 kHz and sampled at 10 kHz for voltage-clamp recordings. Traces presented in the figures represent single sweeps or the average of up to three sweeps. Pipettes were pulled from WPI glass (PG10–165) using a horizontal puller (P97; Sutter Instruments) and were filled with an internal solution consisting of either Cs-methanesulfonate for voltage-clamp experiments containing the following (in mm): 138 Cs-methanesulfonate, 10 HEPES, 0.1 EGTA, 4 NaCl, 2 MgCl2, 2 ATP, 0.3 GTP, and 5 QX, along with biocytin (1 mg/ml), pH 7.33 with CsOH or, for current-clamp experiments: 140 K-gluconate, 10 HEPES, 0.1 EGTA, 8 NaCl, 2 MgCl2, 2 ATP, and 0.3 GTP, along with biocytin (1 mg/ml), pH 7.35 with KOH. Pipette resistances in the working solutions ranged from 3 to 6 MΩ, yielding series resistances of 10–30 MΩ. Pipettes of comparable tip size were used for focal application of ACh (0.1 or 1 mm) dissolved in a HEPES (Sigma-Aldrich)-buffered extracellular solution, pH ∼7.32 with NaOH, and containing blockers of fast excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission.
To characterize the effect of ACh on neuronal excitability, we used a picospritzer (Parker) to deliver short pulses (100 ms) of ACh to the soma of pyramidal neurons from animals that had received either the SNI or sham surgery. ACh pulses were delivered at 60 s intervals to allow recovery from receptor desensitization. In control conditions, with fast synaptic transmission pharmacologically blocked (20 μm DNQX, 50 μm APV, and 100 μm picrotoxin), 2-s-long current injections of 50, 100 and 150 pA were delivered to evoke trains of action potentials. Input/output (I/O) curves were calculated by measuring the number of spikes elicited in response to a 2 s depolarizing current step delivered either in control conditions or 3 s after a 100 ms pulse of somatically applied ACh. The curves were then fitted with a Boltzmann function of the following form:
where I is the injected current, I1/2 is the current injection at which the value of the function is 0.5, and k is the slope factor.
Drug stock solutions.
DNQX (20 mm) and APV (50 mm) were dissolved in water and stored at −30°C. Picrotoxin (50 mm) was dissolved in dimethylsulfoxide (DMSO) and stored at 4°C. ACh (100 mm), CdCl2 (1 m), and Ba2+ (1 m) were prepared in water and stored at 4°C. Atropine (2 mm), pirenzepine (10 mm), and methoctramine (1 mm) were prepared in water and stored at −30°C. Fresh working solutions were prepared on the day of the experiment. DNQX, APV, picrotoxin, atropine, and ACh were from AbCam; pirenzepine was from R&D Research; all other chemicals were from Sigma-Aldrich.
Post hoc anatomical visualization of patched cells.
After the patch-clamp recordings, cells were fixed for 2–4 h at room temperature in 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 m phosphate buffer (PB, pH 7.4) and then stored at 4°C in PBS, pH 7.4, until processing. Biocytin-filled cells were processed using a standard 3,3-diaminobenzidine (DAB) protocol. Slices were rinsed in PBS, treated with hydrogen peroxide for 20–30 min, and permeabilized with 2% Triton X-100 for 1 h. They were then incubated overnight at 4°C in PBS containing 1% avidin-biotinylated horseradish peroxidase complex (ABC; Vector Labs). After the primary incubation, slices were washed with PBS, developed by 0.1% DAB (Sigma-Aldrich) exposure for ∼5 min, and washed again multiple times in fresh PBS before being mounted on a slide and embedded with Mowiol (Sigma-Aldrich). Cell location was quantified based on the distances of the cell body from the pia and distance from the apex of the cortical section.
qRT-PCR.
Coronal slices of mPFC were cut in ice-cold ACSF; the L5 contralateral to SNI was dissected out, frozen in liquid nitrogen, and stored until being processed for PCR. RNA was extracted using a Qiagen RNeasy RNA extraction kit (#74136); DNA contamination was prevented by first using a column that binds DNA while allowing RNA to flow through. RNA was reverse transcribed into cDNA using Roche's First Strand cDNA Synthesis kit using both oligo dT and random hexamer primers according to the manufacturer's protocol (#04897030001). RNA yield and quality were confirmed with Nanodrop analysis.
qRT-PCR was performed using a Roche Lightcycler 480 (LC480) with SYBR Green I Master Mix (#04707516001), primers (0.4 μm), and mPFC cDNA. All genes of interest were normalized to the GAPDH reference gene. Reactions consisted of a 5 min hot start incubation at 95°C, followed by 45 cycles of 10 s at 95°C, 10 s at 60°C, and 10 s at 72°C. Melting temperature analysis demonstrated a single peak for each gene product. Although most primers were intron spanning, cDNA-negative and reverse transcriptase-negative controls were done for all genes of interest. As recommended in published guidelines for qRT-PCR methods, all data were efficiency corrected (Bustin et al., 2010). Standard curves were obtained for each primer pair to calculate reaction efficiency for each gene product using progressive dilutions of cDNA. All data were efficiency corrected using Roche LC480 software and the delta delta Ct method (Livak and Schmittgen, 2001) (Schmittgen and Livak, 2008). Within the recommendations of the MIQE qRT-PCR guidelines (Bustin et al., 2010), a reference gene can be validated by running another reference gene against it. Therefore, to validate GADPH as a stable reference gene, tubulin was run relative to GAPDH for all samples and no significant differences in tubulin were detected when run against GAPDH (data not shown). Statistical analyses were done using a two-way ANOVA test, with Fisher LSD for post hoc analysis. The following primers were used: acetylcholinesterase (NM_172009.1, f-5′ ttaatgtgtggacaccataccc 3′, r-5′ tgcataagtcgctgagcaaa 3′), cholinergic muscarinic receptor 1 (NM_080773.1, f-5′ gcctacagctggaaggaaga 3′, r-5′ cggaggatgtgagggactc 3′), TRPC4 (NM_001083115.1, f-5′ ccatgatcagagaggcaaaaa 3′, r-5′ gctagaaatgtcttgctttagttcc 3′), TRPC5 (NM_080898.2, f-5′ caactcctaccagctcattgc 3′, r-5′ ggctggggatgatgttga 3′), TRPC6 (NM_053559.1) f-5′ gcagctgttcaggatgaaaac 3′, r-5′ acattcagcccatatcattccta 3′), TRPM4 (NM_001136229.1, f-5′ acttggaaccagtgcgactt 3′, r-5′ aaacaggccaggagtcagc 3′), TRPM5 (NM_001191896.1, f-5′ atccttcaccgccaacttct 3′, r-5′ gcagaggggtccctgagt 3′), GAPDH (f-5′ ctgcaccaccaactgcttag 3′, r-5′ tgatggcatggactgtgg 3′), α tubulin 1B (f-5′ cttctaacccgtagctatcatgc 3′, r-5′ gccatgttccaggcagtag 3′), and Nav1.9 (f-5′ gcgaagacttcataatgtgtgg 3′, r-5′ cacgtagaaccattgggaca 3′).
Total protein level and subcellular brain fractionation.
Biochemical fractionation was performed following standard methods as described previously (Hallett et al., 2008; Sanz-Clemente et al., 2010). Briefly, the contralateral L5 mPFC was microdissected and each sample contained tissue obtained from three different mice. Samples were then homogenized in ice-cold buffer (10 mm Tris, pH 7.5, 1 mm EDTA) containing 0.32 m sucrose, protease inhibitors (#11836145001; Roche) and phosphatase inhibitors (mixture 2 and 3, #P5726 and #P0044; Sigma-Aldrich). Samples were centrifuged for 10 min to 1000 × g at 4°C to remove nuclei and large debris and, after removing an aliquot (total homogenate), the supernatant was subsequently centrifuged to 10,000 × g. Pellet (synaptosomes) was resuspended in hypoosmotic buffer (10 mm Tris, pH 8.8, 1 mm EDTA), briefly sonicated, and incubated at 4°C for 10 min with gentle rotation. Samples were centrifuged 30 min at 25,000 × g to collect SPMs in the pellet. SPMs were dissolved in PBS containing 1% SDS; the protein concentration in total homogenate and SPM samples was quantified using the BCA assay (Pierce). Ten micrograms of proteins per sample were separated by SDS-PAGE and analyzed by immunoblotting.
Surface biotinylation in acute cortical slices.
Three-hundred-micrometer acute cortical slices were prepared from sham or SNI rats in ice-cold ACSF (0.5 mm Ca2+, 7 mm Mg2+), incubated for 30 min in ice-cold ACSF containing 0.5 mg/ml biotin (EZ-link-Sulfo-NHS-LC-Biotin; Thermo Scientific), and equilibrated with 95% O2, 5% CO2. After two washes with ice-cold oxygenated ACSF containing 50 mm NH4Cl and two washes with ice-cold oxygenated ACSF, L5 mPFC was dissected out and frozen in liquid nitrogen. Again, each sample contained tissue obtained from three different mice. Samples were homogenized as above and centrifuged at 1000 × g for 10 min at 4°C; 10% Triton X-100 was then added to the supernatant to obtain a final Triton X-100 concentration of 1% and the sample tubes were inserted in a vertical wheel to provide continuous rotation and incubated for 10–15 min at 4°C. The samples were centrifuged for 45 min at 100,000 × g at 4°C to remove insoluble material and, after taking an aliquot (input), the supernatant was incubated with rotation for 2 h at 4°C with 20 μl of NeutrAvidin Agarose Resin (Thermo Scientific). After 3 washes with PBS 1% Triton X-100, recovered material was subjected to immunoblotting.
Immunoblotting.
Proteins were separated in 9% acrylamide gels by SDS-PAGE and transferred to a 0.45 μm PVDF membrane. Membranes were blocked in TBS-T (TBS with 0.1% Tween 20) containing 5% (w/v) nonfat skim milk before incubation with primary antibodies in TBS-T. After 3 washes with TBS-T, membranes were incubated with HRP-conjugated anti-mouse or anti-rabbit secondary antibodies (1:10,000; GE Healthcare). Signal was detected in an Azure c300 imager after incubation 5 min with SuperSignal West Pico Chemiluminescent Substrate (Thermo Scientific) and intensity was quantified using ImageJ software. The antibodies used were obtained from Santa Cruz Biotechnology (anti-actin #sc47778, RRID:AB_626632), Millipore (anti-M1 muscarinic ACh receptor antibody #AB5164, RRID:AB_91713; anti-GluA2 #MAB397), and Invitrogen (anti-Transferrin Receptor, clone H68.4, #13-6800).
Results
ACh enhancement of L5 pyramidal neuron firing is blunted in SNI animals
In the brain, ACh acts as a neuromodulator and regulates intrinsic excitability and synaptic transmission. We investigated the effect of ACh on the response of pyramidal cells to depolarizing current injections in brain slices from control and SNI animals. Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were obtained from L5 pyramidal neurons in the PL region of the rat mPFC (Fig. 2). In control conditions (no ACh), cells from sham and SNI animals showed similar firing patterns and responded to 100 pA depolarizing current injections (2 s) with train of action potentials at a frequency of 5.1 ± 1.4 Hz for cells from sham and 6.0 ± 1.1 Hz for SNI (10 and 9 cells, respectively), whereas 50 pA injections seldom elicited firing (Fig. 3A,B, black traces). Focal application of ACh, however, had dramatically different effects in the two experimental groups. In sham animals, ACh (1 mm, 100 ms, at the soma) strongly increased cellular excitability by causing a leftward shift in the I/O curve (Fig. 3A,B, gray traces; arrows indicate the time of ACh application). In sham animals, ACh increased the number of spikes elicited by 50 pA depolarizing current injection by ∼10 fold, from 0.7 ± 0.7 Hz to 6.8 ± 2.2 Hz (10 cells). Fitting the averaged I/O curve with a Boltzmann function revealed that, in cells from sham animals, ACh caused an ∼55 pA leftward shift in the midpoint of the curve from 99.5 ± 8.1 pA in control conditions to 45.0 ± 6.3 pA after ACh application (p = 0.006; Fig. 3C). Surprisingly, the ACh effect was almost completely eliminated in pyramidal neurons from SNI animals, in which no effect on either the I/O curve or the total number of spikes was detected (Fig. 3D).
Location and firing properties of the recorded mPFC L5 pyramidal neurons. A, All current-clamp (and a subset of voltage-clamp) recorded cells were filled with biocytin; the positions were then mapped onto an image of the mPFC (Paxinos and Watson, 1998) and plotted as a function of distance from pia (x-axis) and distance from apex (y-axis). Cell bodies of sham neurons were located at a distance of 726.4 ± 22 μm from the pia and 2227 ± 61.6 μm from the apex (n = 31). SNI neurons were located 685.6 ± 22 μm from pia and 2315.7 ± 57 μm from apex (n = 22), in the prelimbic region of the rat mPFC. B, Left, Current-clamp recordings in slices from sham and SNI rats. Middle, Cells from both sham and SNI animals exhibited accommodation, as evidenced by interspike interval ratios (first interspike interval divided by the average interval) of 0.2 ± 0.02 and 0.29 ± 0.07 (n = 10, 9, respectively) and a time-dependent reduction in instantaneous firing frequency in response to a current injection of 150 pA (13.21 ± 1.51 Hz at 1 s, and 11.53 ± 1.12 Hz at 2 s, paired t test p = 0.01; n = 19; right).
ACh increases pyramidal cell excitability in sham, but not SNI, animals. A, Voltage responses recorded from L5 pyramidal neurons in acute slices in the presence of blockers of fast synaptic transmission (100 μm picrotoxin, 20 μm DNQX, 50 μm APV); cells were stimulated with 2 s current injections of either 50 or 100 pA in control conditions (black) and after a 1 mm, 100 ms focal application of ACh (black; arrow indicates the time of ACh application) in a slice from a sham animal. At rest, both neurons were held at −70 mV. B, In slices from SNI animals, the same stimulation protocol failed to increase cellular excitability in response to a 50 pA current injection and actually decreased excitability in response to a 100 pA current injection (gray) compared with control conditions (black). C, In sham rats, ACh application induced a leftward shift in the I/O curve. Fitting the data points with Boltzmann equations showed that ACh shifted the midpoint from 99.5 ± 8.1 pA to 45 ± 6.3 pA (n = 10). D, ACh failed to shift the I/O curve of pyramidal neurons from SNI animals (gray) (I1/2 = 104.9 ± 7.8 pA, in control and 120.9 ± 14.2 pA in ACh, in SNI cells, n = 9).
The differential effect of ACh on sham and SNI neurons included another important functional modulation. Consistent with previous findings showing that ACh elicits an sADP following a train of action potentials (Haj-Dahmane and Andrade, 1999; Lei et al., 2014), ACh elicited an sADP in L5 neurons from both sham and SNI animals (Fig. 4). However, in cells from SNI rats, the ACh-dependent sADP was significantly smaller (5.5 ± 1.4 mV versus 10.6 ± 0.8 mV in sham; 9 and 7 cells, respectively, p = 0.01; Fig. 4B). In addition, we found that, in slices from sham animals, focal application of ACh (1 mm, 100 ms) 3 s before a 2-s-long depolarizing current injection often caused the firing response to last well beyond the end of the current stimulus (6 of 11 neurons; Fig. 4C,D). In slices from SNI animals, however, this prolonged firing response was observed in only one of nine cells (Fig. 4D) and, in the only SNI cell in which it was detected, it was limited to a single spike after the end of the stimulus. On average, the time of the last observed spike relative to the end of the step current injection was +2918 ± 1219 ms in sham and −15 ± 156 ms in SNI cells (p = 0.04; Fig. 4D). Therefore, ACh-induced persistent firing is virtually abolished in the SNI condition.
ACh-mediated persistent firing is abolished in SNI animals. A, Whole-cell, current-clamp traces of an ACh-dependent sADP response immediately after a 100 pA, 2-s-long current injection (inset) in a cell from a sham (black) and a 150 pA, 2-s-long current injection from a SNI (gray) rat. B, Average of the maximum peak sADP voltage response in sham and SNI animals (10.55 ± 0.8 mV and 5.5 ± 1.4 mV, n = 7 and 9, respectively, p = 0.01). C, Top trace, Current-clamp response of a mPFC pyramidal neuron to a 150 pA, 2 s current injection in control conditions. Bottom trace, Voltage response of an mPFC pyramidal neuron to 1 mm, 100 ms focal application of ACh (large arrow), followed by a 150 pA, 2 s current injection. Note the persistence of action potentials (small arrows) after the end of the current step. D, Left, Time, relative to the end of the current step (time 0, dashed line), of the last recorded action potential after focal application of ACh plotted for both sham and SNI animals. Note that, in 55% of the tested pyramidal neurons from sham animals, persistent activity was observed for several seconds after the end of the current step. Right, Average time, in seconds, of the last recorded action potential relative to step repolarization after focal application of ACh in sham and SNI animals (2.7 ± 1.1 s and −0.015 ± 0.16 s, respectively, n = 11, 9 p = 0.036).
Prolonged depolarization induced by ACh in L5 mPFC pyramidal neurons is reduced in SNI animals
The persistent firing in response to short applications of ACh is intriguing. To investigate the mechanisms underlying this phenomenon, we performed current-clamp recordings to measure the voltage response evoked by ACh application (1 mm, 100 ms, delivered at 60 s intervals to allow for the recovery of the response) to the soma of pyramidal neurons from animals that had received either the SNI or sham surgery.
Focal ACh application to L5 prelimbic pyramidal neurons elicited a slow-activating, long-lasting net depolarizing response in both sham and SNI animals (Fig. 5A). The initial resting membrane potential was −70 mV (if necessary, a small holding current was injected). We observed no difference in the resting membrane potential between sham and SNI animals (data not shown). The peak positive voltage responses were reached ∼3 s after the ACh pulses and decayed with a time constant of 9.1 ± 2.3 s (10 cells from sham rats). In 50% of sham and 80% of SNI neurons tested, the depolarizing voltage response was preceded by a hyperpolarizing response (Fig. 5A, bottom). Interestingly, neurons from SNI animals exhibited, on average, a significantly larger hyperpolarizing response and a significantly reduced depolarizing response compared with their sham counterparts (Fig. 5B). The average peak depolarizing response to 1 mm ACh was 7.47 ± 0.95 mV in sham animals and 3.5 ± 0.8 mV in SNI (n = 11, 9 respectively, p = 0.01), whereas the average hyperpolarizing response was −0.42 ± 0.2 mV in sham animals and −2.28 ± 0.3 mV in SNI (n = 11, 9 p = 0.02). Consistent with recent findings (Kelly et al., 2016), the membrane resistance of SNI cells was larger compared with sham (at −70 mV, it was 231 ± 19 MΩ in SNI vs 176 ± 14.0 MΩ in sham animals, 10 and 9 cells, respectively, p = 0.03); therefore, the observed reduction in the depolarizing voltage responses cannot be attributed to a reduced membrane resistance in the SNI cells. We also tested whether the net excitatory responses in control animals depend on the particular ACh concentration used or the application duration. Reducing the ACh concentration 10-fold, from 1 mm to 100 μm, resulted in a reduced, but still depolarizing, voltage response (3.48 ± 0.7 mV, n = 10, recorded in cells from naive animals; Fig. 5D, black). Reducing the duration of application from 100 to 50 ms further reduced the observed response (2.3 ± 0.5 mv, n = 7, naive, 100 μm ACh; Fig. 5D, gray), although it remained depolarizing. Accordingly, 100 μm ACh applications (100 ms) effectively increased the firing frequency of L5 pyramidal neurons, causing a leftward shift in the I/O curve (Fig. 5E) similar to that caused by 1 mm ACh.
ACh induced depolarization is blunted in SNI animals. A, Voltage responses of L5 pyramidal neurons to 100-ms-long focal applications of ACh (1 mm, at the soma) in slices from sham and SNI animals. The bath temperature was ∼31°C and the resting potential −70 mV, in both cells. The resting membrane potential was kept at −70 mV in all cells (if necessary, a small holding current was injected). In some cells, the ACh-dependent depolarization was preceded by a hyperpolarization (bottom, gray). B, Left, When present, the average hyperpolarizing responses to ACh (1 mm) were significantly different in sham animals (black, −0.42 ± 0.19 mV, 11 cells) and SNI animals (gray, −2.28 ± 0.34 mV, 9 cells, p = 0.017). Right, Average depolarizing voltage responses to ACh (1 mm) were significantly different between sham (black, 7.47 ± 0.95 mV, n = 11) and SNI animals (gray, 3.50 ± 0.78 mV, n = 9, p = 0.005). C, Membrane resistance in SNI cells was larger compared with sham (231.0 ± 19 MΩ and 176.08 ± 14.0 MΩ, respectively, n = 9 and 10, respectively, p = 0.03). D, Responses evoked by focal application of 100 μm ACh were 3.48 ± 0.7 mV after 100 ms applications (10 cell from naive animals) and 2.3 ± 0.5 mV in response to 50 ms applications (7 cells from naive animals). The gray dotted bar shows, for comparison, the depolarizing response observed in sham animals using 1 mm ACh. Note that no hyperpolarizing component was detectable with the reduction in ACh concentration and duration. E, Focally applied ACh (100 μm) caused an increase in the firing frequency (in response to a 100 pA current injection it was 6.1 ± 0.7 Hz in control versus 10.1 ± 0.9 Hz in 100 μm ACh, p = 0.02; 4 cells from naive animals).
ACh-evoked current is mediated by the M1 receptor
In the brain, ACh can activate two types of receptors: ionotropic nicotinic and metabotropic muscarinic receptors, which mediate currents with very different properties (McCormick and Prince, 1985; Nathanson, 1987; McGehee and Role, 1995). The nature of the slow-activating, long-lasting inward current in mPFC pyramidal neurons is still controversial because it was previously suggested to depend on the closure of G-protein-coupled potassium channels (Carr and Surmeier, 2007), the opening of muscarinic receptor-coupled cationic channels (Haj-Dahmane and Andrade, 1996; Kurowski et al., 2015), or the activation of α4β2 subunit-containing nicotinic receptors (Guillem et al., 2011). We designed voltage-clamp experiments to determine the properties of the ACh-induced depolarizing current in L5 pyramidal cells of sham and SNI animals; for these recordings, we maximized the neuronal response to ACh by using 1 mm ACh focally applied for 100 ms. In addition, because the hyperpolarizing component of the evoked response was completely blocked when the intracellular solution contained Cs, we isolated the depolarizing component by using a Cs-methanesulfonate-based intrapipette solution. Bath application of ACh (1 mm, at −70 mV) elicited a slow, inward current in sham and SNI animals (Fig. 6A). Consistent with our current-clamp results, the inward current recorded in neurons from SNI animals was smaller compared with sham (−36.5 ± 5.2 pA vs −89.5 ± 9.6 pA, respectively, 10 cells in each group; p = 0.0003; Fig. 6B). The magnitude of the reduction was similar when the current was normalized to cell capacitance (0.25 ± 0.03 vs 0.65 ± 0.09 pA/pF, respectively; p = 0.001), thereby ruling out the possibility that the reduction was simply due differences in cell size (Kelly et al., 2016). Eliciting the current with short, focal somatic ACh applications yielded results similar to those obtained with bath application (Fig. 6C,D). We then used a pharmacological approach to identify the type of cholinergic receptor involved. After applying ACh (1 mm) focally for 100 ms and recording a control response, we bath applied the nonspecific muscarinic ACh receptor antagonist atropine (2 μm). When the bath solution was completely exchanged, we reapplied the ACh and measured the response. Atropine blocked 96.3 ± 2.2% of the ACh-evoked current in sham and 89.2 ± 8.6% in SNI animals (Fig. 7A,B), showing that activation of a muscarinic receptor mediates the response in both experimental groups. We further identified the nature of the receptor involved by testing the effect of the M1 muscarinic receptor antagonist pirenzepine (Hammer et al., 1980) and the M2 antagonist methoctramine (Gulledge and Kawaguchi, 2007). Because the sensitivity to atropine was the same in sham and SNI, these recordings were only performed in slices from sham or naive rats, in which the larger currents allow more reliable quantification of the drug effects. We found that pirenzepine at either 10 μm (93.5 ± 5.6% block n = 5, sham) or 2 μm (82.03 ± 13.2% block, n = 3, naive; Fig. 7C,D) produced a current block similar to that by atropine, showing that the cholinergic inward current is almost completely mediated by M1-subunit-containing muscarinic receptors. Consistent with these data, the M2 antagonist methoctramine did not produce any detectable effect on the current (the ACh response after 5 min of bath-applied 1 μm methoctramine was 96 ± 0.02% of control, n = 5 cells; Fig. 7E).
ACh-evoked currents in L5 pyramidal neurons are markedly reduced in mPFC of SNI rats. A, Whole-cell, current traces in sham (left) and SNI (right) pyramidal neurons voltage clamped at −70 mV after bath application of 1 mm ACh (bar). The bath solution contained 100 μm picrotoxin to block fast, inhibitory synaptic currents. B, Average peak current responses evoked by bath application of 1 mm ACh in sham (−89.5 ± 9.6 pA, n = 10) and SNI (−36.5 ± 5.2 pA, n = 10, p = 0.0003) animals. When individual peak current responses are normalized to the capacitance of the cell, the difference between sham and SNI responses remains significant (0.70 ± 0.09 pA/pF and 0.25 ± 0.03, n = 10 and 10, respectively, p = 0.0014). C, Current traces in sham (left) and SNI (right) neurons voltage clamped at −70 mV after a 1 mm, 100 ms picospritzer application of ACh (arrow). Bath and focal application solution contained blockers of fast excitatory (50 μm APV and 20 μm DNQX) and inhibitory synaptic currents (100 μm picrotoxin). D, Average peak current responses evoked by 1 mm ACh in sham (−32.57 ± 1.98 pA, n = 27) and SNI (−18.81 ± 2.69 pA, n = 18, p = 0.0015) animals.
Depolarizing cholinergic current in mPFC neurons is mediated by the M1 receptor. A, Traces represent peak current responses to focal application of 1 mm ACh (arrow) in control conditions (black) and in the presence of 2 μm bath-applied atropine (gray) in a mPFC neuron from sham (left) and SNI (right) animals. Voltage was −70 mV. B, Atropine blocked the ACh-activated current (the block of the peak current was 96.32 ± 2.17% in sham and 89.2 ± 8.6% in SNI animals (n = 6 for each group). C, Current responses to 1 mm focally applied ACh (arrow) in control conditions (black) and after bath application of either 2 μm pirenzepine (gray) or 10 μm pirenzepine (gray). D, Summary of the effect of either 2 or 10 μm pirenzepine on the ACh-elicited current. Ten micromolar pirenzepine blocked 93.54 ± 5.62% (p = 0.002; 5, cells from sham animals) of the current. Similarly, 2 μm pirenzepine blocked 82.03 ± 13.21% (p = 0.01; n = 3, naive animals) of the peak control current. E, Depolarizing current evoked by ACh (1 mm) was not affected by bath application of the M2 blocker methoctramine (1 μm; the current was 96 ± 0.02% of that in control; n = 5 cells from naive animals).
What is the mechanism mediating the reduction of the depolarizing muscarinic current in SNI animals?
Muscarinic receptors are expressed throughout the cortex and may be coupled to diverse ion channels through G-protein- and calcium-dependent biochemical cascades. In the hippocampus, activation of muscarinic receptors of CA1 pyramidal neurons results in a net depolarizing response thought to be mediated by the inhibition of potassium currents, specifically the M-current, as well as by activation of cationic currents (Halliwell and Adams, 1982; Fisahn et al., 2002). Similar heterogeneity has been reported in cortical pyramidal neurons. In our preparation, the depolarizing response was not mediated by potassium channels because it persisted when using a Cs-based pipette solution and when the extracellular solution contained barium (0.5 mm, data not shown; 3 cells). This finding is consistent with previous reports suggesting that the muscarinic current in the same brain area is mediated by nonspecific cationic channels (Haj-Dahmane and Andrade, 1997; Lei et al., 2014). The precise identity of the subunits mediating this current in L5 of the PL cortex is still unclear. Yan et al. (2009) showed that the expression level of TRPC5 and TRPC6 subunits modulates the current mediating the sADP in L3–5 pyramidal neurons of the mPFC, whereas Zhang et al. (2011) suggested that TRPC4/5 subunits are critical mediators of the cholinergic plateau potentials in the entorhinal cortex. Lei et al. (2014) examined the subunit composition of the muscarinic sADP in L5 pyramidal neurons of the mouse mPFC, the same area as our study, and concluded that it is largely mediated by TRPM5 and TRPM4 subunits. More recently, Kurowski et al. (2015) reported that, in L5 mPFC neurons of young (18–22 d) rats, M1 muscarinic receptors are coupled to Nav1.9 channels that mediate a sustained depolarizing current, similar to the one described here. Therefore, multiple channels appear to be coupled to the M1 receptor in these cells, with TRPM channels likely involved in the sADP and Nav1.9 in the persistent depolarizing response. To identify the mechanism causing the loss of cholinergic modulation, we used qRT-PCR to determine the expression levels of the M1 muscarinic receptor, TRPC4, TRPC5, TRPC6 TRPM4, TRPM5, and Nav1.9, the subunits most likely to mediate the coupled currents, in L5 of the rat mPFC. In addition, because an increase in acetylcholinesterase may reduce ACh availability and thus cause a decrease in muscarinic current, we also studied the expression of this transcript. We found that M1, TRPC4, TRPC5, TRPC6, TRPM4, Nav1.9, and acetylcholinesterase were all expressed, whereas TRPM5 did not produce a signal above the background. Next, we investigated whether an altered expression of any of these transcripts may account for the current reduction in SNI animals by comparing their expression levels in L5 of the mPFC contralateral to the peripheral lesion. Similar to the electrophysiological recordings, this analysis was performed 1 week after SNI/sham surgery. Compared with sham, no significant change in transcript level of any of these target genes was detected in the mPFC tissue obtained from SNI rat brains (contralateral to surgery, 14 sham and 16 SNI rats; Fig. 8A). Therefore, the smaller current amplitude in SNI is unlikely to be caused by a reduction of either M1 or the potentially coupled candidate channel transcripts. Finally, we confirmed by immunoblotting that SNI surgery does not modify the total M1 protein level in L5 mPFC (contralateral to surgery) from sham and SNI rats (Fig. 8B,C).
Level of M1 mRNA and total protein is unaffected in L5 mPFC of SNI rats. A, qRT-PCR was performed on cDNA from tissue isolated from L5 of the mPFC contralateral to the surgery site to measure the expression of several transcripts potentially involved in the reduction of cholinergic modulation in SNI animals. No significant decrease was observed for any of the transcripts investigated. B, Representative blots showing the total expression of M1 and the AMPAR subunit GluA2 protein in L5 mPFC of sham and SNI animals. C, Total amount of protein in mPFC L5 of SNI rats normalized to actin and compared with expression level in sham (dotted line); similar to the PCR data, no difference was found in M1 expression in SNI relative to sham.
Both ligand-gated channels and G-protein-coupled receptors can be strongly modulated through mechanisms of desensitization and trafficking. In the case of muscarinic receptors, this modulation can lead to clathrin-mediated endocytosis of the receptor and eventual degradation (Tsuga et al., 1998). Biochemical techniques can be used to measure what fraction of a protein is bound to the plasma membrane and what fraction is associated with internal membranes. Therefore, we first collected L5 mPFC tissue (contralateral to peripheral injury site) from sham and SNI rats 1 week after surgery and performed a subcellular fractionation protocol to isolate the SPM fraction, followed by Western blot analysis using an M1 antibody. Once more, the total amount of M1 protein in mPFC L5 was unaffected by SNI surgery (Fig. 9A,B); the fraction bound to the plasma membrane, however, was significantly reduced in tissue from SNI rats (the SPM/homogenate fraction in SNI was 81.2 ± 5.3% of that in sham; 7 groups each, 3 hemispheres per group; Fig. 9A,B). To further support this mechanistic interpretation, we performed surface biotinylation in acute mPFC slices from a different group of rats (12 sham and 12 SNI). In agreement with the fractionation data, we found that the surface expression level of M1 in isolated contralateral mPFC L5 tissue (fraction biotinylated and recovered with NeutrAvidin beads) was significantly reduced in SNI rats (83.5 ± 2.1% of that in sham; Fig. 9C,D). As a control for our fractionation and biotinylation experiments, we analyzed the surface expression level of the AMPA subunit GluA2, which was not affected by SNI surgery. Therefore, our results strongly suggest that the reduction in muscarinic current in the contralateral PL mPFC of SNI animals is due, at least in part, to M1 receptor internalization.
Surface expression of M1 muscarinic receptor is reduced in L5 mPFC of SNI animals. Representative blots (A) showing the total expression (Homg) and fraction associated with SPM of the indicated proteins in L5 mPFC. B, Bar chart showing the significant decrease of M1 receptor in the SPM fraction (81.2 ± 5.3% of sham level; n = 7 and 7, 3 animals per sample, p = 0.017). C, Slice biotinylation experiment showing the total (Input) and surface expressed (Surface) fraction of the indicated proteins. The cytosolic protein actin was evaluated as a control. D, Quantification of the data in C showing a significant reduction in the levels of surface-expressed M1 receptor in SNI rats (83.5 ± 2.1% of sham level; 4 samples in each condition, 3 animals per sample, p = 0.029).
Discussion
Our data show that, 1 week after the onset of peripheral neuropathy (SNI model), cholinergic modulation is dramatically reduced in L5 pyramidal neurons of the contralateral PL cortex of SNI animals. We demonstrate that a cationic current apparently mediated by M1 receptor activation is reduced in L5 pyramidal neurons from SNI animals. The muscarinic nature of the cholinergic depolarizing response in these cells is consistent with a recent study (Hedrick and Waters, 2015) showing that L5 pyramidal neurons of the mPFC almost completely lack nicotinic responses. The downregulation of this muscarinic current virtually eliminates the ability of ACh to increase intrinsic neuronal excitability and to induce sustained firing. Although we observed no changes in the gene expression of any of the cationic channels potentially coupled to the M1 receptor, we found a significant reduction in the plasma-membrane-bound fraction of M1 protein in SNI animals a mere 7 d after surgery. This finding suggests that M1 receptor internalization may mediate this functional impairment, although the apparent disproportion between the large functional effect and the change (∼20%) in M1 surface expression suggests that other mechanisms are also likely to be involved. Finally, we propose that the impairment in cholinergic modulation contributes to the general functional mPFC deactivation associated with chronic neuropathic pain (Ji and Neugebauer, 2011; Lee et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2015).
Disruption of cholinergic signaling in the mPFC may underlie higher-level cognitive impairment associated with neuropathic pain
Recent studies support the idea that chronic pain is associated with major reorganization of the nervous system. This includes nociceptors and the spinal cord (Woolf and Salter, 2000; Sandkühler, 2009), but also many supraspinal structures. In the SNI model, synaptic transmission is potentiated in the anterior cingulate cortex as early as 1 week after the neuropathic lesion (Xu et al., 2008) and functional and morphological changes are found throughout the limbic system (Metz et al., 2009; Mutso et al., 2012; Chang et al., 2014), with the PFC appearing to be particularly involved. Functional imaging studies show hypoactivity (Gündel et al., 2008) and decreased levels of excitatory neurotransmitters (Grachev and Apkarian, 2001) in the mPFC of chronic pain patients. Similar cortical deactivation is also observed in animal studies, in which the pain-associated mPFC inhibition is thought to be driven by hyperactivity of the amygdalar inputs to the mPFC, which results in a group 1 metabotropic glutamate receptor-mediated increase in local GABAergic activity (Ji et al., 2010; Ji and Neugebauer, 2011). Our data support this general picture and add an important piece of evidence by showing that, 1 week after neuropathic injury, ACh-evoked intrinsic excitability of L5 pyramidal neurons was dramatically decreased in SNI animals due to the loss of muscarinic modulation, further supporting the idea of a pain-associated deactivation of this brain area soon after neuropathic injury.
Even more importantly, because of the well established role of cholinergic signaling in attention and memory processes (Hasselmo and Sarter, 2011; Zhou et al., 2011; Yoshida et al., 2012), it is tempting to hypothesize that the loss of cholinergic signaling has a causal role in the deficits in attention and working memory that have been reported in human patients and in animal models of neuropathic pain. Patients with chronic pain show impaired decision making and disrupted attention and working memory (Apkarian et al., 2004; Dick and Rashiq, 2007). Attentional processing is thought to involve the integration of multiple modes of input within a discrete temporal window. In the mPFC, pyramidal neurons of L5 receive glutamatergic, sensory input from the thalamus in parallel with cue-specific, cholinergic input from nucleus basalis (Parikh et al., 2007). Successful cue detection, which requires ACh release in the mPFC and is sensitive to muscarinic receptor antagonism, is adversely affected in a rodent model of chronic pain (Pais-Vieira et al., 2009a). Interestingly, these deficits persist even when the hypersensitive peripheral areas and acute pain responses are mitigated by lidocaine, further emphasizing the role of supraspinal reorganization. Because previous work showed that a cholinergic nonspecific cationic current is critical for the persistent firing that is believed to serve as a memory trace in neurons of the temporal lobe (Yoshida et al., 2012), the reduced muscarinic response and the lack of an ACh-dependent modulation of the I/O curve that we observed in SNI animals could result in the missing of relevant cues and the ultimate disruption of mPFC-dependent attentional processing. Consistent with this hypothesis, the persistent “reverberant” firing that we found in sham animals, in which neurons continue to fire for tens of seconds after a brief application of ACh, was virtually nonexistent in SNI animals. Therefore, the reduced muscarinic current and loss of ACh-evoked persistent firing in SNI rats may represent the cellular background for the cognitive deficits associated with chronic pain. This scenario is consistent with the known impairment of working memory by pharmacological antagonism of muscarinic receptors (Zhou et al., 2011).
Is the loss of cortical muscarinic modulation causal for pain chronification?
It was reported recently that direct optogenetic activation of the mPFC output reduces allodynia in SNI animals (Lee et al., 2015). Similar results were obtained by optogenetic inhibition of mPFC GABAergic neurons (Zhang et al., 2015). These findings support the idea that, in the pain state, the mPFC is functionally deactivated and match the observation that, in a rodent model of arthritis, enhanced amygdalar inputs inhibit mPFC pyramidal neurons through activation of interneurons (Ji et al., 2010). Intriguingly, according to their firing pattern, the cells investigated in this study are commissurally projecting neurons; therefore, the impaired cholinergic modulation that decreases the contralateral excitability may also contribute to the decreased glutamatergic tone (Kelly et al., 2016). Therefore, in the pain condition, different synergistic network activity modulations lead to cortical deactivation that modulates the allodynia. The impaired cholinergic modulation described here provides another mechanism contributing to the overall deactivation of the mPFC. This deactivation, however, is part of a larger and more complex reorganization that involves the entire limbic system. At an early time point (∼1 week after injury), the extent of the reorganization is astoundingly large. It involves impaired plasticity in the hippocampus (Ren et al., 2011; Mutso et al., 2012), increased intrinsic excitability of indirect pathway spiny neurons of the nucleus accumbens shell (Ren et al., 2016), increased amygdala activation (Ji et al., 2010), and decreased glutamatergic tone in the mPFC (Kelly et al., 2016). Although this is a very complex picture, and one that is likely to change at later time points (Ren et al., 2011; Chang et al., 2014), it depicts a relatively consistent scenario in which the negative valence limbic system (amygdala–mPFC) is enhanced. It is interesting that systemic administration of donepezil, an acetylcholinesterase inhibitor, was reported to decrease allodynia in SNI rats, although apparently through an M2-dependent mechanism and at a later time point (Ferrier et al., 2015).
Therefore, the picture that emerges is complex and evolving. To further complicate matters, we have to keep in mind that, even at this early time point, whereas the sign of the mPFC activity related to pain is clear at the system level (deactivation of the PL output), this is likely the result of multiple cellular mechanisms. For example, L2/3 pyramidal neurons of the mPFC appear to receive increased excitatory input in pain conditions (Metz et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2015), but this is counterbalanced by decreased intrinsic excitability that leads to the inability of these neurons to generate action potentials and to cortical deactivation despite increased inputs (Wang et al., 2015). Similarly, different cortical layers and individual pyramidal cells within a single layer may be differentially regulated (e.g., dendritic complexity of L5 pyramidal cells of SNI animals is decreased in L3–4, but not in L1–2 or L6 (Kelly et al., 2016)) and the role of the many inhibitory interneurons may further complicate the picture. Therefore, this represents a typical example in which a relatively well defined functional fingerprint at the macroscopic (organ) level, such as that resolvable with fMRI studies, appears to be the result of a myriad of complex and possibly contrasting events at the cellular/molecular level.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (Grants NS064091, DE022746, and AG041225).
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Marco Martina, M.D., Ph.D., Department of Physiology, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, 303 E. Chicago Avenue, Chicago, IL 60611. m-martina{at}northwestern.edu