Abstract
μ-Opioid receptor agonists are widely used within the contemporary treatment of pain, but abrupt opioid suspension, even after short-term infusion, can paradoxically increase the sensitivity to noxious stimuli, a phenomenon that has been, for example, reported after application of the fast-acting μ-opioid receptor agonist remifentanil. To investigate the mechanisms underlying the effects of discontinuation of remifentanil application on pain processing in the human CNS, we analyzed neuronal responses to thermal stimuli before and after a short-term infusion of remifentanil (30 min 0.1 μg/kg body weight/min) compared with control in the brain, brainstem, and spinal cord in drug-naive male volunteers using fMRI. Subsequent to remifentanil suspension, we observed reduced heat pain thresholds and increased neuronal responses in pain-encoding as well as in key regions of the descending pain-modulatory system, such as the periaqueductal gray matter, the nucleus cuneiformis, and the rostral ventromedial medulla. Moreover, the spinal pain-related multivoxel activity pattern showed an opioid-specific change after drug suspension. Importantly, remifentanil suspension increased the functional coupling between the nucleus cuneiformis and the rostral anterior cingulate cortex, and the coupling strength between the rostral anterior cingulate cortex and the nucleus cuneiformis correlated negatively with the individual pain threshold after opioid suspension. These findings demonstrate that, already subsequent to a short-term infusion of the μ-opioid receptor agonist remifentanil, signaling in the descending pain-modulatory system is fundamentally altered and that these changes are directly related to the behavioral sensitivity to pain.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Opioids are widely used in modern medicine, but, in addition to their known side effects, it is increasingly recognized that opioids can also increase sensitivity to pain subsequent to their use. Using the fast-acting μ-opioid receptor agonist remifentanil and fMRI in healthy male volunteers, this study demonstrates how signaling changes occur along the entire descending pain-modulatory pathway after opioid discontinuation and how these alterations are closely linked to increased behavioral pain sensitivity. Particularly by revealing modified responses in pain-modulatory brainstem regions that have been previously demonstrated to be causally involved in acute opioid withdrawal effects in rodents, the data provide a plausible neuronal mechanism by which the increased sensitivity to pain after opioid suspension is mediated in humans.
Introduction
Therapeutically applied opioids exert their pharmacodynamic properties through an interaction with the complex endogenous opioid system shifting the balance between pronociception and antinociception and further homeostatic functions on a cellular, systems, and behavioral level toward a “protective” hypoalgesic state (Fields, 2004; Pasternak and Pan, 2013). However, abrupt discontinuation of opioid application, even after short-term application, has been shown to potentially bring an opposite state about, which is characterized by reduced pain thresholds and increased sensitivity to noxious stimulation (Rossbach, 1880; Mao, 2002; Angst et al., 2003; Koppert et al., 2003a,b; Angst and Clark, 2006; M. Lee et al., 2011), indicating the intricacy of the interaction between exogenously applied opioids and endogenous systems (Célèrier et al., 2001).
While the degree of manifestation and its relevance under clinical conditions are still the subject of investigation (Angst and Clark, 2006; Fishbain et al., 2009; Fletcher and Martinez, 2014; Volkow and McLellan, 2016; Yu et al., 2016), basic research studies consistently showed the existence of this class of phenomena under controlled laboratory conditions (Vanderah et al., 2000; Célèrier et al., 2001; Mao, 2002; Angst and Clark, 2006). With regard to the underlying mechanisms that can explain increased sensitivity to pain after opioid suspension, several explanations have been suggested (Vanderah et al., 2000; Gardell et al., 2002; Brown and Russell, 2004; Wang et al., 2005; Simonin et al., 2006; Drdla et al., 2009; Heinl et al., 2011; Chu et al., 2015; Roeckel et al., 2016). One prominent hypothesis is based on an altered equilibrium between ascending nociceptive signals and descending pain-modulatory systems (Kaplan and Fields, 1991; Heinricher et al., 2001; Vanderah et al., 2001a,b; Bie et al., 2003; Ossipov et al., 2003; Drdla et al., 2009; Wanigasekera et al., 2011). While several studies examined direct effects of opioid application in the human brain (Petrovic et al., 2002; Wise et al., 2002, 2004; Leppä et al., 2006; Atlas et al., 2012), demonstrating an involvement of descending pain-modulatory systems in opioid analgesia in humans, potential changes in the functionality of this system subsequent to opioid application have not been investigated in detail in humans so far. Hence, the current study aimed at addressing this hypothesis.
Investigating this hypothesis requires an experimental approach that is sensitive to neuronal responses in pain-encoding and pain-modulatory systems and to their dependence on neurochemical processes. To this end, we realized two pharmacological fMRI experiments using thermal pain stimuli and a short-term infusion of the μ-opioid receptor agonist remifentanil (0.1 μg/kg body weight/min for 30 min). In the first fMRI experiment (n = 32), BOLD responses were assessed in the brain and brainstem. In the second experiment (n = 30), responses were measured in the spinal cord. Both experiments compared BOLD responses in a between-group design between a baseline condition before opioid application and the state after remifentanil suspension with a precisely matched saline control condition to account for confounding effects, such as time (Fig. 1A). We implemented an injury-free stimulation protocol using slowly ascending thermal ramps (applied to the left forearm) starting from a physical intensity causing a slight warm sensation up to a painful level (Fig. 1B). This approach allowed us to disentangle BOLD signal changes related to the warm and painful period of thermal stimulation, to assess heat pain thresholds (HPTs), and to depict the temporal dynamics of BOLD signal changes along the entire neuraxis during the transition from nonpainful to painful perception and to finally analyze the effects of remifentanil suspension on these parameters. The results reveal opioid discontinuation-related effects at all levels of the CNS and elucidate how BOLD signal changes in the involved brain regions and altered functional coupling in the descending pain-modulatory system relate to the behavioral increase in pain sensitivity after discontinuation of remifentanil.
Materials and Methods
Experiment 1: brain fMRI
Participants.
A total of 38 healthy male volunteers (26.5 ± 1.2 years of age, mean ± SEM) were acquired and found to be eligible to participate in the first fMRI experiment investigating opioid suspension-related effects in the brain.
To avoid possible gender differences (Bodnar and Kest, 2010; Niesters et al., 2010; C. W. Lee and Ho, 2013), we only studied male volunteers. Exclusion criteria were abnormal HPT at the site of stimulus application, any history of neurological disease, including any history of pain syndrome, any other serious acute or chronic illness, current medication, intolerance to remifentanil, and any pain event within the last week. All participants underwent a medical examination and gave written consent before the experiment. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Medical Council of Hamburg and followed the ethical guidelines of the International Association for the Study of Pain. Participants were randomly assigned to either remifentanil or a saline group. Data of 6 participants had to be excluded (due to discomfort within the scanner and premature termination of the experiment, 2 participants; incomplete ratings, 2 participants; technical malfunction, 2 participants), leaving 32 complete datasets (remifentanil group: n = 16). Groups did not differ significantly regarding age, body weight, initial HPT, depression score, and pain vigilance and awareness score (Table 1). Aftereffects of remifentanil and remifentanil effects on participant's mood can be obtained from Tables 2–5.
Experimental procedures.
To study the effects of suspension from acute remifentanil application in the CNS, we used two pharmacological groups (remifentanil and saline, randomized single-blind protocol) and two experimental sessions (before and after application of the test substance; Fig. 1). We implemented a new thermal stimulation protocol that allowed us to obtain information regarding the intensity component of the pain perception, the HPT, and the dynamic development of the BOLD signal over time. To this end, we applied during each session 13 ascending thermal ramps (Fig. 1) with a Peltier-Thermode (TSA-II, Medoc) to the left radial forearm (dermatome C6), starting at baseline temperature of 32°C and rising over 30 s with a constant slope of 0.5°C per second up to 47°C. Pilot experiments had shown that this slope and peak temperature are well tolerable for almost all healthy young adults. At the beginning of each thermal ramp (i.e., shortly before the thermal stimulus becomes noticeable), a white crosshair that was displayed in the center of the screen during the entire intertrial interval changed its color into red to indicate the onset of the new trial to the participant and to avoid surprise. This crosshair stayed red until a visual analog scale (VAS) was presented (see below).
During the first, seventh, and 13th trial, participants had to indicate by pressing a button when their HPT was reached following the instructions of the quantitative sensory testing protocol of the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (Rolke et al., 2006). However, in contrast to a typical method-of-limits assessment of the HPT, the temperature did not return to baseline after the participant indicated the HPT, but further increased until the end of the thermal ramp was reached, hence causing a painful period for some seconds. We only assessed the HPT during three trials, which allowed us to estimate the threshold for the intermediate trails (see Behavioral data analysis) and to avoid button-press-related motor activity in the CNS during the remaining trials. Subsequent to each thermal ramp, after a short pause of 6 s, participants had to rate the level of pain present during the painful phase of that trial using a VAS (endpoints labeled with 0 = “no pain” and 100 = “unbearable pain”) within a period of 12 s. Finally, a variable intertrial interval (60 ± 5 s) followed during which a white crosshair was presented. Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems) was used for stimulus control, recording of scanner pulses, and behavioral responses.
Remifentanil application.
Access to a cubital vein was established before the first scanning session. After the first fMRI session, participants received an intravenous infusion of remifentanil (0.1 μg/kg body weight/min; Ultiva, GlaxoSmithKline) or the same amount of 0.9% saline solution for 30 min outside the scanner. This remifentanil dose is a clinically relevant and falls within the dose range that is required for the relief of acute severe clinical pain (Bowdle et al., 1996; Volmanen et al., 2002). While experimental evidence indicates that higher remifentanil doses elicit stronger effects on pain after discontinuation (Guignard et al., 2000; Koppert et al., 2003a), the chosen dose still exhibits a low risk of unwanted drug effects and has been shown to elicit reliable effects on pain after discontinuation (Koppert et al., 2003a,b).
Remifentanil has a fast onset and likewise a rapid offset/washout due to hydrolysis by nonspecific plasma esterases with a half-life time of a few minutes, and the plasma concentration 30 min after infusion can be estimated as almost zero (Wise et al., 2002; Angst et al., 2003). During the infusion period, all participants received 3 L oxygen per minute via a respiratory mask, and ECG, SpO2, and noninvasive blood pressure were monitored. The infusion with the test substance was followed by a 1 h break before the second scanning session started. Pilot experiments had suggested that behavioral effects might increase after this period. In accordance with the very short half-life period of remifentanil, shortly after suspension of the infusion, physiological parameters of all participants were within normal range.
MRI data acquisition.
fMRI data were acquired on a 3 tesla system (Magnetom Tim Trio, Siemens) equipped with a 32-channel head coil. Forty transversal slices (slice thickness, 2 mm; gap, 1 mm) were acquired in each volume (repetition time: 2.4 s, echo time: 25 ms, flip angle: 80°, FOV: 216 × 216 mm2, matrix: 108 × 108; GRAPPA with acceleration factor 2, and 48 reference lines) using a T2*-weighted gradient-echo EPI sequence. Slice orientation was tilted by ∼°30. We also acquired high-resolution (1 × 1 × 1 mm3 voxel size) T1-weighted images for each subject using a 3D MPRAGE sequence (repetition time 2.3 s, echo time 2.98 ms, readout flip angle 9°, inversion time 1.1 s, FOV 192 × 240 × 256 mm3).
fMRI data preprocessing.
fMRI data preprocessing and statistical analyses were performed using SPM12 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London). The first five EPI volumes of each session were discarded to exclude T1-saturation effects. Functional images were corrected for time differences in slice acquisition time and motion (6 df). Structural T1-weighted images were coregistered to the functional images and subsequently SPM12-segmented. The obtained normalization parameters were applied to the functional images, which were resampled with a resolution of 2 × 2 × 2 mm3. Finally, functional images were smoothed with an 8 mm FWHM isotropic Gaussian kernel.
Because we specifically hypothesized brainstem structures to be involved in the mechanisms underlying opioid suspension-related effects, we used a second preprocessing strategy for the preprocessing of the brainstem that is specifically optimized for this region. To minimize the effect of movements of cortical regions on the motion correction procedure of the brainstem, the cost function of the realignment algorithm was weighted by an automatically created cylindrical binary mask that only included the brainstem and directly surrounding tissues (Napadow et al., 2006). Within this mask, highly variant regions (i.e., CSF) were excluded, to minimize the influence of strong-intensity fluctuations on the motion-correction procedure and to base the image registration only on brainstem movements. For the brainstem, EPI images were resampled after normalization with a resolution of 1 × 1 × 1 mm3 and smoothed with a 4 mm FWHM isotropic Gaussian kernel (Beissner, 2015).
Statistical analysis: behavior.
All behavioral data were analyzed in MATLAB version 8.4 (The MathWorks). We calculated the mean and SEM of pain ratings and HPTs for each session and group. To test for effects of remifentanil on behavioral pain measures, we compared differences of the mean pain ratings (based on all 13 trials) and thresholds (based on the first, seventh, and 13th trial) of the first and the second session between groups using a two-sample t test. Results were considered significant at p ≤ 0.05 (one-tailed based on the a priori hypothesis). To assign painful and warm periods in trials where the HPT was not assessed, we estimated the time point of the HPT based on the arithmetic mean of the HPT of the seventh and the 13th trials. This approach is based on a pilot experiment (n = 10) where participants had to indicate their HPTs during all 13 trials. The chosen approach yielded the least sum of error squares between estimated and recorded thresholds compared with other interpolations techniques.
Statistical analysis: fMRI.
The statistical fMRI data analysis was performed using a GLM approach. Analyses were performed separately for the imaging data preprocessed with the brain and the brainstem optimized preprocessing approach. For structures below the diencephalon, we report results from analyses that used the brainstem-adjusted preprocessing procedure. To identify brain regions responding to the warm and painful phase of the ascending thermal stimulation, the analysis of the imaging data commenced by specifying a basis model at the single-subject level implementing the warm and the painful phase of the ascending thermal ramps as separate box-car regressors, separately for trails during which subjects indicated their HPT via button press (first, seventh, and 13th trials) or not (second to the sixth and the eighth to the 12th trials). Only β estimations of the latter regressors were used for subsequent analyses to ensure that these analyses do not depict any button-press-related motor activity. The model also specified a box-car regressor for the VAS rating, a linear increasing regressor for the duration of each thermal ramp, and finally a session constant for each session, thus comprising altogether 12 regressors. Box-car functions were subsequently convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function as implemented in SPM12, and data were subjected to temporal high-pass filtering with a cutoff period of 128 s. After model estimations, β images corresponding to the warm and the painful phase of trials without button presses were raised to a second-level random-effects analysis.
To capture the temporal dynamics of signal changes in response to the thermal stimulation protocol, we also set up a first-level analysis using a finite impulse response set as the basis function using a window length of 36 s (locked to the onset of each thermal ramp) and 15 bins (1 bin = 2.4 s). Trials during which subjects performed button presses and trials without button presses were again modeled separately. Each first-level design matrix of this model thus comprised 62 regressors (2 sessions × 2 conditions × 15 bins plus 2 sessions constants). Data were likewise subjected to temporal high-pass filtering with a cutoff period of 128 s. After model estimations, we defined contrasts for the time periods before and after exceeding the HPT. As the HPT naturally varies across participants, our linear contrast vector was defined as the fixed time window during which all participants were below or above the HPT to ensure that the group analysis of the nonpainful phase is not confounded by pain responses and vice versa. Contrast images corresponding to the warm and the painful period were then raised to a second-level random-effects analysis using a SPM-flexible factorial design considering pharmacological condition as a group factor. To illustrate the peristimulus time course in brain region, we also specified a similar second-level analysis incorporating the β weights for all bins.
To identify brain regions that are related to intraindividual variations of pain intensity perception and potentially modulated by opioid suspension, we also specified a first-level analysis model that included the trialwise individual pain ratings as a parametric modulator of the regressor representing the nonpainful and painful phase of stimulation respectively. The model was otherwise identical to our basis model (see above).
To further test for a modified signaling in the descending pain-modulatory network caused by opioid suspension, we used the nucleus cuneiformis (NCF) and the periaqueductal gray matter (PAG) (which were revealed by the opioid suspension specific interaction contrast) as seed voxels for functional connectivity analyses. We set a sphere of 6 mm diameter around the respective coordinate and extracted the mean time series for each subject for each session. These time series were then used as regressors in subject-specific design matrices. To remove stimulus-related linear trends from the data, we also included a linear increasing regressor for the duration of each thermal ramp in the design matrices. The resulting β images were subsequently raised to the second-level analysis, where we observed a significant pharmacological interaction effect for the analyses using the left and the right NCF as the seed voxel in the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC). To better account for symmetry in our analysis, we subsequently also extracted the time series around the observed rACC coordinates and correlated the two time series. The resulting correlation coefficients for each session for each subject were Fisher z-transformed. Differences in the correlation strength between sessions were compared between groups using a two-sample t test. To test for the functional relevance of the findings, we correlated on the individual level the coupling strength with the HPT. As both vectors contained extreme values, we report Spearman's rank correlation coefficient. To test for group differences, we Fisher z-transformed the correlation coefficients (Myers and Sirois, 2006) and calculated z statistics. Finally, we used a similar analysis to test for a correlation between the individual nucleus cuneiformis–rostral ventromedial medulla (RVM) coupling strength and the HPT.
Results were considered significant at p < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons using the FWE rate based on the Gaussian random field theory. Based on our hypothesis of an altered signaling in the descending pain-modulatory system after opioid suspension, we defined the rACC, the PAG, the NCF, and the RVM as a priori regions of interest (ROIs). Correction was based on peak coordinates obtained from previous studies on pain processing and pain modulation. The rACC was corrected using a sphere of 12 mm radius; brainstem regions were corrected using a sphere of 6 mm radius. Outside a priori ROIs, FWE correction was performed for all voxels. We also report uncorrected p values with a threshold of p < 0.001. Findings in the brain are reported in square brackets stating x, y, z coordinates in the MNI space. For illustration purposes, statistical maps of the brain are thresholded at p < 0.005 uncorrected for multiple comparisons.
Experiment 2: high-resolution fMRI of the cervical spinal cord
Participants.
A total of 34 healthy male volunteers (26.5 ± 1.2 years of age) participated in the second experiment investigating opioid suspensions effects at the spinal level. Inclusion and exclusion criteria and ethical guidelines were the same as in Experiment 1 (see above). Data of 4 participants had to be excluded due to premature termination of the experiment (2 participants) and image artifacts (2 participants), leaving 30 complete datasets (remifentanil group: n = 15). Both groups did not differ significantly regarding age, body weight, initial HPT, depression score, and pain vigilance and awareness score (Table 1). Aftereffects of remifentanil and remifentanil effects on participant's mood can be again obtained from Tables 2–5. The experimental procedures and the drug application were identical to the first experiment.
MRI data acquisition.
fMRI data were acquired on the same system as Experiment 1, equipped with a 12-channel head coil and an additional 4-channel neck coil with the target region of the spinal cord being centered in the magnet's isocenter (EPI parameter: repetition time 1040 ms, echo time: 34 ms, flip angle: 60°, FOV: 132 × 130 mm2, matrix: 110 × 108; GRAPPA acceleration factor of 2, and 24 reference lines).
In short, we acquired 12 slices positioned approximately perpendicular to the spinal cord, using a slice thickness of 3.5 mm and an in-plane resolution of 1.2 × 1.2 mm2. Saturation pulses were applied anterior and posterior to the target region to avoid ghosting. To minimize signal intensity variations along the z-axis caused by magnetic susceptibility differences in the spinal canal, a slice-specific z-shim gradient momentum was used (Finsterbusch et al., 2012; Sprenger et al., 2012). We also acquired T2*-weighted anatomical images of the spinal target region using a Multiple Echo Data Image Combination sequence (12 transversal slices, slice thickness 3.5 mm, 0.57 × 0.57 mm2 in-plane resolution, repetition time 473 ms, echo time 22 ms, flip angle 20°, FOV 256 × 184 mm2, GRAPPA, acceleration factor 2, and 24 reference lines). Finally, we acquired high-resolution (1 × 1 × 1 mm3) T1-weighted anatomical images using a 3D-MPRAGE sequence (repetition time 2.3 s, echo time 3.43 ms, readout flip angle 9°, inversion time 1.1 s, FOV 192 × 240 × 256 mm3).
Physiological data acquisition.
To allow for retrospective physiological noise correction, the pulsoximetric pulse-curve (left index finger) and respiratory chest movements (using a respiratory belt) were measured using a clinical monitoring unit (Expression, In vivo). Data were recorded together with the scanner pulses using Spike2 software (Cambridge Electronic Design) to ensure timing accuracy.
fMRI data preprocessing.
After the first 5 EPI volumes were discarded, we corrected spinal EPIs for motion by using the same motion correction procedure that we used for the brainstem (see above).
We then spatially normalized the T1-weighted structural images following an approach similar to the one described by Geuter and Büchel (2013). After the subject-specific first-level analysis (see below), we applied the previously determined normalization parameters to the β images and resampled images to a resolution of 0.75 × 0.75 × 0.75 mm3. Finally, β images were smoothed using a 3D isotropic Gaussian kernel with an FWHM of 2 mm.
Statistical analysis: behavior.
The behavioral data analysis was identical to Experiment 1.
Statistical analysis: fMRI.
For the statistical analysis of the spinal data, we followed the same basic analysis approach on the single-subject and group level that we used for the analysis of the brain data (see above). Given that the influence of physiological noise is much stronger at the spinal level compared with the brain (Kong et al., 2012; Fratini et al., 2014; Eippert et al., 2017), we additionally corrected for this potential confound. Similar to previous studies (Eippert et al., 2009b; Sprenger et al., 2012, 2015), we used the selective averaging approach described by Deckers et al. (2006) to generate regressors representing cardiac and respiratory effects (using 10 bins for cardiac and respiratory effects, respectively) that were subsequently included in the subject-specific first-level design matrices. We also corrected for CSF-related signal fluctuations in the spinal EPI time series (Brooks et al., 2008). Therefore, we extracted the time courses of voxels from the spinal canal whose variance lay in the top 10th percentile and generated 1–3 regressors explaining 95% of the variance of these time series by using a principal component analysis. These regressors were likewise included in the first-level design matrices.
Results were considered significant at p < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons using the FWE rate. Approaches adjusting p values to control FWE rates based on the Gaussian random field theory are not suitable for very small cylindrical structures, such as the spinal cord (Worsley et al., 1992; Brooks, 2014). Similar to a previous study (Sprenger et al., 2015), we therefore used nonparametric permutation tests (Nichols and Holmes, 2002) using the Statistical Non-Parametric Mapping toolbox (SnPM13; http://warwick.ac.uk/snpm) to report FWE-corrected p values in the spinal cord. Estimations were based on 5000 permutations (Nichols and Holmes, 2002).
Based on observations from previous spinal fMRI studies (Eippert et al., 2009b; Sprenger et al., 2012, 2015), which consistently revealed BOLD responses caused by painful stimulation in the dermatome C6 in the ipsilateral dorsal horn of the spinal segment C6, we defined this region as a priori ROI. For this region, the multiple-comparison correction was restricted to a sphere of a 5 mm radius centered on the average coordinate from the previous studies. Outside this region, FWE correction was performed for all voxels.
In addition to the GLM analysis, we also performed a multivoxel pattern analysis (MVPA) using a linear support vector machine to decode the saline and remifentanil condition based on the corresponding spinal activity patterns as determined by the individual GLM contrast images. The MVPA was restricted to a region that showed in accordance with the functional neuroanatomy of the nociceptive system a significant BOLD response to the painful phase of thermal stimulation in the ipsilateral dorsal horn of the spinal segment C6 (thresholded at p < 0.05 uncorrected). We used a leave-one-subject-out cross-validation and compared the average accuracy across subjects to chance (50%) using a nonparametric permutation test. Finally, we conducted a multivoxel pattern searchlight analysis over the entire measured volume with a radius of 4 mm that was otherwise identical to the initial MVPA.
Results
Effects subsequent to remifentanil short-term infusion on behavior and BOLD responses to ascending thermal ramps in the brain
We first tested whether our experimental protocol resulted in a behaviorally measurable aftereffect of remifentanil suspension on pain perception. Both experiments yielded very comparable behavioral results (Fig. 2). The latter phase of thermal stimulation provoked a clearly painful sensation across all participants that remained approximately stable over trials and did not significantly differ between groups before application of the test substance (two-sample t test: Experiment 1, t(30) = 0.16, p = 0.88; Experiment 2, t(28) = −0.48, p = 0.63). We observed no significant effect of remifentanil suspension on pain intensity ratings (two-sample t test, Session 2 − Session 1: Experiment 1, t(30) = −0.93, p = 0.18; Experiment 2, t(28) = −0.02, p = 0.49). However, in both independent experiments, remifentanil suspension caused a clear effect on HPTs. While thresholds did not differ between both groups during the first experimental session (two-sample t test: Experiment 1, t(30) = −0.5, p = 0.63; Experiment 2, t(28) = −0.1, p = 0.93), we observed a decreased HPT in the second session after remifentanil application (Experiment 1, −0.37 ± 0.3°C; Experiment 2, −0.32 ± 0.4°C) and an increased HPT following saline application (Experiment 1, 0.25 ± 0.1°C; Experiment 2, 0.56 ± 0.2°C), leading to a significant pharmacological interaction effect in both experiments (two-sample t test: Experiment 1, t(30) = 2.2, p = 0.02; Experiment 2, t(28) = 1.9, p = 0.04).
Next, we analyzed BOLD responses to the thermal stimuli in the brain separately for the nonpainful and painful period of stimulation and whether these responses would be modulated after remifentanil infusion. We identified a number of brain regions in response to thermal stimulation that have been previously shown to typically exhibit increased levels of activity during the processing of warm and painful stimuli by a large body of evidence (Apkarian et al., 2005; Duerden and Albanese, 2013). For the painful phase of stimulation this comprises regions, such as the bilateral insula, the anterior cingulate cortex, the primary and secondary somatosensory cortex (Fig. 3; Table 6).
We subsequently tested whether BOLD responses in the brain were modulated subsequent to opioid application using t tests. After remifentanil suspension, we observed increased responses to the painful phase of stimulation (Session 2 − Session 1) that were stronger compared with the control group in the posterior insula (−40, −20, 12, t(120) = 3.47, punc.< 0.001), the contralateral thalamus (8, −22, 10, t(120) = 3.88, punc.< 0.001) and the amygdala (26, −12, −18, t(120) = 3.35, punc.< 0.001; Fig. 4). The reverse contrast did not reveal any significant results. Likewise, our analysis revealed no significant differences between groups regarding the nonpainful phase.
Remifentanil suspension-related effects on descending pain-modulatory circuits
Based on the hypothesis that, subsequent to remifentanil application, the processing of pain-related signals would be altered in brainstem structures well known to play a pivotal role in various aspects of pain modulation, including opioid action (Basbaum and Fields, 1984; Millan, 2002; Mason, 2012) and amplification of nociceptive signals (Ji et al., 2003; Ren and Dubner, 2007; M. C. Lee et al., 2008), we next analyzed this region using a brainstem-optimized image preprocessing approach. Following remifentanil suspension, we observed increased BOLD responses to the painful phase of stimulation that were significantly stronger elevated compared with the control group in a region that can be most likely attributed to the RVM (presumably the nucleus raphe magnus and the adjacent nucleus reticularis gigantocellularis; 0, −36, −43, t(120) = 4.01, p = 0.001; Fig. 4A). The reverse contrast did not reveal any significant differences.
More importantly, with regard to mechanisms underlying the observed effect of remifentanil suspension on the pain threshold, we subsequently analyzed the period before reaching the HPT. This analysis showed increased BOLD responses after remifentanil suspension compared with control bilaterally in the midbrain reticular formation's NCF (right: 9, −26, −13, t(120) = 3.36, p = 0.04; left: −12, −27, −12, t(120) = 3.75, p = 0.01; Fig. 5) and in two portions of the PAG matter (ventrolateral PAG, −2, −29, −11, t(120) = 3.92, p = 0.01; caudal portion of the ventromedial PAG, 1, −36, −24, t(120) = 3.79, punc.< 0.001).
A subsequent analysis revealed that, after remifentanil application, parametric trial-by-trial responses during the nonpainful period correlated positively with the behavioral pain ratings in the previously identified NCF, but not in any of the other experimental sessions (right: 10, −28, −14, t(60) = 2.88, p < 0.05; left: −7, −26, −13, t(60) = 2.63, p < 0.05; Fig. 5C). At both peaks, parametric responses were significantly stronger after remifentanil suspension compared with the saline control condition (right: t(60) = 2.1 p < 0.05; left: t(60) = 2.13, p < 0.05).
Altered functional connectivity in the descending pain-modulatory system predicts remifentanil offset-related increase in pain sensitivity
As μ-opioid receptor agonists unfold part of their analgesic properties by acting on the pain-modulatory system (Barton et al., 1980; Fields, 2004; Haghparast et al., 2008), changing the functional connectivity within this system during exogenous opioid application (Petrovic et al., 2002) and opioid-dependent endogenous analgesia (Petrovic et al., 2002; Eippert et al., 2009a; Sprenger et al., 2012), and previous studies suggesting altered functionality of this system to be crucially linked to opioid related increase in pain sensitivity (Kaplan and Fields, 1991; Heinricher et al., 2001; Vanderah et al., 2001a,b; Ossipov et al., 2003), we next directly investigated whether such connectivity changes can be observed in humans subsequent to remifentanil short-term application.
Our functional connectivity analysis revealed significant functional coupling between the right NCF and the rACC (−2, 42, −2) that exhibited an opioid-suspension-related pharmacological interaction effect. During the baseline session, we observed a similar functional coupling between the right NCF and the rACC in both groups (saline group, r' = 0.16 ± 0.03; remifentanil group, r' = 0.13 ± 0.03). However, while NCF-rACC coupling showed a reduction during the second session in the saline group (r' = 0.06 ± 0.04), NCF-rACC coupling increased after remifentanil application (r' = 0.18 ± 0.03), leading to a significant pharmacological interaction effect (t(30) = 2.24, p = 0.02; Fig. 6A–C).
With regard to the functional relevance of this finding, we observed that, after remifentanil short-term infusion, the individual strength of this coupling showed a strong negative correlation with the HPTs (r = −0.7, p = 0.004; Fig. 6D); that is, the stronger the individual opioid-offset related NCF-rACC coupling, the lower the HPT. This correlation was significantly more negative compared with the saline group (z = 2.6, p = 0.01).
Similarly to the right NCF, we observed functional coupling between the left NCF and the rACC (−16, 46, 2) that likewise exhibited a pharmacological interaction effect (t(30) = 3.5, p < 0.001).
If rACC-NCF coupling is involved in a pain facilitating mechanism after opioid application, it can be expected that NCF activity subsequently also predicts activity in the RVM, which receives direct fiber input from the NCF (Edwards, 1975; Zemlan and Behbehani, 1988) and takes a key position in descending pain modulation (Millan, 2002; Heinricher et al., 2009). In line with this hypothesis, we also observed functional coupling between the NCF and the RVM that increased after remifentanil suspension (remifentanil group session 1, r' = −0.08 ± 0.02, remifentanil group session 2, r' = 0.13 ± 0.03), but the strength of coupling displayed no pharmacological interaction effect (t(30) = 0.03, p > 0.05). However, similar to the rACC-NCF coupling, the individual strength of the right NCF-RVM coupling correlated negatively with the HPT after remifentanil application (r = −0.56, p = 0.03), and this correlation was significantly stronger compared with the saline control group (z = 2.1, p = 0.02).
We also observed functional coupling between the PAG and the rACC that was increased after remifentanil suspension. However, the pharmacological interaction failed significance (t(30) = 1.4, p = 0.09).
BOLD responses subsequent to remifentanil short-term application at the spinal level
We finally investigated neuronal responses at the spinal level using high-resolution fMRI of the human cervical spinal cord. To localize the core region of nociceptive processing in the spinal cord in response to signals originating from painful stimulation to the left radial forearm (dermatome C6), we first determined BOLD responses to the painful phase of stimulation before application of the test substance across all participants. In accordance with the functional neuroanatomy of the nociceptive system, we observed a marked BOLD response in the dorsal horn of the spinal segment C6 ipsilateral to the side of stimulation (t(112) = 8.48, p < 0.001; Fig. 7A–C). No further significant clusters and no negative responses were detected within the spinal cord. We subsequently analyzed the effect of opioid suspension on BOLD responses at this site. We observed no significant pharmacological interaction effect. Also, extending our analysis to the entire measured volume did not reveal a significant modulation of BOLD responses by remifentanil suspension.
To further pursue the assumption of an opioid evoked modulation of neuronal activity in the spinal cord, we subsequently analyzed the ROI in the spinal cord that showed a BOLD response to painful stimulation in our previous analysis with an MVPA. For this analysis, we used a linear support vector machine classifier and a leave-one-subject-out cross-validation to decode the pharmacological condition based on the spinal activity patterns in response to the painful phase of stimulation. While the MVPA was not able to decode the pharmacological condition during the baseline session (average accuracy 56.7%, not significant), it was successfully able to decode the remifentanil suspension and the saline condition better than chance after application of the test substance (average accuracy 83.3%, p = 0.005). An MVPA based on the activity patterns of the nonpainful phase of stimulation was not able to decode the pharmacological condition.
To demonstrate the specificity of our ROI-based finding regarding the involved neuronal pathway, we subsequently conducted a searchlight MVPA across the entire measured volume, which quantitatively measures the degree to which spatial activation patterns predict the pharmacological condition. This analysis showed the highest values at the initially determined site of the BOLD response to painful stimulation in the dorsal horn of the spinal segment C6 (Fig. 7D).
Discussion
Performing two fMRI experiments with optimized protocols for the respective CNS regions allowed us to study remifentanil-induced neuronal changes along the entire central pain pathway. Remifentanil discontinuation lowered HPTs and increased BOLD responses to painful stimulation in brain regions that exhibit high μ-opioid receptor density and that are crucially involved in nociceptive processing, such as the thalamus, posterior insula, amygdala, and RVM. Already before reaching a painful level of stimulation, remifentanil suspension increased neuronal responses in the PAG and NCF. Opioid discontinuation increased functional connectivity between NCF and rACC, and both the strength of rACC-NCF and NCF-RVM connectivity correlated negatively with the individual HPT after application of remifentanil. Finally, we observed an opioid-suspension-specific modulation of the spinal multivoxel activity pattern. Our results thus delineate specific opioid-dependent alterations in pain-encoding and pain-modulatory systems in humans after short-term application of remifentanil that are directly related to the behavioral sensitivity to pain.
Experiments in both animal and human volunteers have demonstrated increased sensitivity to pain after exposure to opioids across many different nociceptive setups and routes of administration. Whereas most human studies investigated the effect of opioid suspension in models of preexisting hyperalgesia (Petersen et al., 2001; Angst et al., 2003; Hood et al., 2003), the current study extends previous observations by showing that short-term infusion of remifentanil affects HPTs and central pain processing after opioid discontinuation in an injury-free experimental model without presensitizing the skin. Under these experimental conditions, remifentanil caused a reliable effect on HPTs, whereas heat pain ratings were not altered. This observation is interesting as it highlights a partial independence of the underlying CNS mechanisms and is in line with human studies showing no effect (Angst et al., 2003; Hood et al., 2003) of opioid suspension on thermal pain ratings but on other experimental parameters.
Altered signaling after opioid suspension in the brain and brainstem
In the brain, we observed increased BOLD responses during painful stimulation subsequent to remifentanil discontinuation in the thalamus, amygdala, and posterior insula. All three regions are involved in nociceptive processing, belong to the regions exhibiting a high opioid receptor density (Henriksen and Willoch, 2008), and are directly modulated during μ-opioid receptor agonist application (Wise et al., 2002; Leppä et al., 2006; Atlas et al., 2012). A series of animal studies indicated the paraventricular thalamus and the amygdala to be related to the highly aversive withdrawal symptoms after prolonged opioid consumption (Gracy et al., 2001; Watanabe et al., 2002; Zhu et al., 2016). However, given that short-term opioid exposure generated only weak effects on mood and parameters of well-being after discontinuation, it appears unlikely that the observed BOLD changes in the amygdala reflect fear, aversiveness, or stress effects related to pain.
In the brainstem, we observed BOLD responses in the RVM that were increased after opioid application during painful stimulation. The RVM is well established to be an essential source of bulbospinal inhibitory and facilitatory modulation of nociceptive inputs receiving numerous inputs from other brainstem regions, such as the PAG and the NCF (Millan, 2002). Importantly, our finding is in agreement with a series of conclusive rodent studies showing a crucial contribution of this region to the occurrence of opioid-induced increased pain sensitivity (Bederson et al., 1990; Kaplan and Fields, 1991; Bie et al., 2003; Neubert et al., 2004). These studies demonstrated a strong increase in on-cell activity and silencing of off-cell activity after naloxone-precipitated opioid withdrawal, thus displaying an inverted picture of acute opioid effects, subsequently leading to a descending facilitation of pain (Vanderah et al., 2001b).
Because we did not record BOLD responses during remifentanil application, we cannot know which direct effects remifentanil exerted on the neuronal circuits showing altered signaling after discontinuation. However, previous studies have shown that the used and even lower doses of remifentanil dose cause marked behavioral analgesia and both reduced neuronal responses to noxious stimuli in pain-related brain regions and altered functional coupling with the brainstem (Petrovic et al., 2002; Wise et al., 2002, 2004; Koppert et al., 2003a,b).
To capture neuronal mechanisms underlying the decreased HPTs, we also analyzed the time period before the threshold. After opioid discontinuation, we observed BOLD responses in the PAG and the bilateral NCF that were exclusively elevated before the HPT. The NCF is an important brainstem site for pain-modulatory processes (Gray and Dostrovsky, 1983; Richter and Behbehani, 1991), involved in the mediation of acute opioid effects (Haghparast et al., 2007, 2008; Ronaghi et al., 2011), negative and positive expectation effects (Keltner et al., 2006; Wager et al., 2007), and most notably in central sensitization processes (Zambreanu et al., 2005; M. C. Lee et al., 2008; Wanigasekera et al., 2016), including opioid withdrawal (Wanigasekera et al., 2011). The NCF receives ample input from spinal lamina I neurons (McMahon and Wall, 1985; Hylden et al., 1986) but also strong input from the PAG, which in turn receives inputs from the rACC, hypothalamus, and amygdala (Mantyh, 1982; An et al., 1998; Floyd et al., 2000). On the other hand, the NCF mainly projects to the RVM, which exerts direct control over dorsal horn neurons (Fields et al., 1995; Zhuo and Gebhart, 1997). Interestingly, we observed the BOLD increase after opioid withdrawal in the NCF directly before the HPT, which is reminiscent of the fact that on-cells fire shortly before the occurrence of nociceptive withdrawal reflexes (Heinricher et al., 1987; Haws et al., 1989). Similar to our study, Wanigasekera et al. (2011) observed heightened BOLD responses in the NCF in a subset of individuals that showed behavioral pain increase after short-term remifentanil exposure. The authors additionally reported a negative correlation between withdrawal-related rise in NCF activity and changes in pain scores pointing at a possible inhibitory role of the NCF in responders. In contrast, our data indicate a predominantly pain-facilitatory role. Such a facilitatory influence of the NCF after opioid withdrawal appears plausible in view of the response characteristics of on-cells (Haws et al., 1989; Pan et al., 1990; Neubert et al., 2004), electrophysiological features (Haghparast et al., 2010), and a mainly excitatory effect of the NCF on the RVM (Richter and Behbehani, 1991).
Opioid suspension and spinal cord processing of thermal stimuli
In accordance with previous studies, we were able to detect robust pain-related BOLD responses in the ipsilateral spinal cord with the strongest response centered in deeper portions of the dorsal horn. The activation of heat-sensitive nociceptors by a noxious thermal stimulus induces a considerably stronger response in the spinal cord compared with a non-noxious warm stimulus (Coghill et al., 1991, 1993), potentially even triggering NMDA receptor unblocking from Mg2+ after repetitive stimulation (Dickenson and Sullivan, 1990; Sandkühler, 2009). In line with the present results, previous spinal fMRI studies reported only weak or absent BOLD responses after non-noxious thermal stimulation (Brooks et al., 2002; Summers et al., 2010; Weber et al., 2016), but frequently responses to noxious stimulation (Kolesar et al., 2015). Pain-related BOLD responses in the spinal cord showed a marked decrease during the second experimental session. Reduced responses to repeated thermal stimuli have been previously reported in multisession experiments (Letzen et al., 2014; Quiton et al., 2014), and similar phenomena are generally recognized in context of stimulus repetition and are presumably related to firing-rate adaptation and synaptic depression (Grill-Spector et al., 2006; Nickel et al., 2014). In the present study, this assumption is supported by the observation that, during the second session, also BOLD responses in the brain were weaker in the saline group. Given the considerably higher level of physiological noise in spinal fMRI (Fratini et al., 2014), the detection of responses to thermal stimuli during the second session and consequently of opioid-induced neuronal changes was therefore hampered at this level. However, using a more sensitive multivariate approach allowed us to successfully decode the pharmacological condition based on the spinal activity patterns after application of the test substance. This was not possible for the baseline session, hence indicating an opioid-suspension-induced modulation of neuronal activity.
Effects of remifentanil suspension on functional connectivity
We finally attempted to functionally link structures within the descending pain-modulatory system by using functional connectivity analyses. Our analysis revealed significantly enhanced functional connectivity between NCF and rACC after remifentanil discontinuation. Moreover, we observed enhanced functional connectivity between rACC and PAG, but without significant pharmacological interaction. Given that the NCF receives input from the rACC mainly indirectly via connections with the PAG that in turn receives direct input from the rACC, it appears plausible that the functional connectivity between rACC and NCF is structurally mediated via the PAG. There is a large body of evidence showing rACC–brainstem connectivity to be an important functional pathway mediating influences of opioid-receptor-rich forebrain regions to phylogenetically old brainstem sites that exert direct control over the spinal cord in different types of endogenous analgesia (Petrovic et al., 2002; Valet et al., 2004; Eippert et al., 2009a). Although it is known that this pathway can be recruited by both exogenous opioid application and cognitive/motivational factors, our study now shows that abrupt opioid suspension most likely sets the functionality of this system into a pronociceptive mode. In support of this view, we could show that the opioid-withdrawal-enhanced functional connectivity within the descending pain-modulatory system is of functional relevance, as both the individual strength of the rACC-NCF and of the NCF-RVM coupling correlated negatively with the HPT, thus linking the signaling within the most rostral portions of the descending pain-modulatory system with the RVM, which directly controls spinal nociceptive transmission.
In conclusion, our study indicates remifentanil-suspension-related changes along the entire central pain pathway, particularly in key structures of the descending pain-modulatory system. Both neuronal signaling within and functional connectivity between these structures corresponded with the behavioral pain sensitivity. Our findings thus demonstrate a crucial involvement of this system in increased sensitivity to pain subsequent to opioid application in humans.
Footnotes
C.S. was supported by German Research Foundation SP-1668. C.B. was supported by German Research Foundation SFB 936 Project A06 and SFB TRR 134. C.B. and C.S. were supported by European Research Council ERC-2010-AdG_20100407.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Christian Sprenger, Department of Systems Neuroscience, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Martinistrasse 52, 20246 Hamburg, Germany. c.sprenger{at}uke.de