Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Visual Working Memory Is Independent of the Cortical Spacing Between Memoranda

William J. Harrison and Paul M. Bays
Journal of Neuroscience 21 March 2018, 38 (12) 3116-3123; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2645-17.2017
William J. Harrison
1Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, United Kingdom, and
2Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for William J. Harrison
Paul M. Bays
1Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, United Kingdom, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Paul M. Bays
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The sensory recruitment hypothesis states that visual short-term memory is maintained in the same visual cortical areas that initially encode a stimulus' features. Although it is well established that the distance between features in visual cortex determines their visibility, a limitation known as crowding, it is unknown whether short-term memory is similarly constrained by the cortical spacing of memory items. Here, we investigated whether the cortical spacing between sequentially presented memoranda affects the fidelity of memory in humans (of both sexes). In a first experiment, we varied cortical spacing by taking advantage of the log-scaling of visual cortex with eccentricity, presenting memoranda in peripheral vision sequentially along either the radial or tangential visual axis with respect to the fovea. In a second experiment, we presented memoranda sequentially either within or beyond the critical spacing of visual crowding, a distance within which visual features cannot be perceptually distinguished due to their nearby cortical representations. In both experiments and across multiple measures, we found strong evidence that the ability to maintain visual features in memory is unaffected by cortical spacing. These results indicate that the neural architecture underpinning working memory has properties inconsistent with the known behavior of sensory neurons in visual cortex. Instead, the dissociation between perceptual and memory representations supports a role of higher cortical areas such as posterior parietal or prefrontal regions or may involve an as yet unspecified mechanism in visual cortex in which stimulus features are bound to their temporal order.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Although much is known about the resolution with which we can remember visual objects, the cortical representation of items held in short-term memory remains contentious. A popular hypothesis suggests that memory of visual features is maintained via the recruitment of the same neural architecture in sensory cortex that encodes stimuli. We investigated this claim by manipulating the spacing in visual cortex between sequentially presented memoranda such that some items shared cortical representations more than others while preventing perceptual interference between stimuli. We found clear evidence that short-term memory is independent of the intracortical spacing of memoranda, revealing a dissociation between perceptual and memory representations. Our data indicate that working memory relies on different neural mechanisms from sensory perception.

  • cued recall
  • short-term memory
  • visual crowding
  • visual working memory

Introduction

Although a focus of research for decades, the neural basis of working memory storage is still disputed (Serences, 2016; Xu, 2017). Recent neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that items in memory can be decoded from activity in human primary visual cortex (V1). Whereas the amplitude of the blood-oxygenation-level-dependent (BOLD) signal within V1 is not predictive of a remembered stimulus, patterns of activity across voxels can predict memoranda reliably (Harrison and Tong, 2009; Serences et al., 2009). For example, in a study by Harrison and Tong (2009), observers viewed two sequentially presented oriented gratings and were cued to hold one item in memory so that they could later compare it with a test grating. These investigators found that the remembered stimulus orientation could be decoded from patterns of activity within V1 during the retention interval. They concluded that visual cortex retains information about features in working memory. Similar studies have found that activity patterns within early visual cortex are specific to only the task-relevant feature of multifeature objects (Serences et al., 2009) and that the precision of decoding diminishes with increasing numbers of memoranda (Emrich et al., 2013; Sprague et al., 2014).

These findings among others have led some researchers to conclude that memory storage mechanisms are located within the sensory neural systems involved in processing the stimulus attributes, a proposal termed the sensory recruitment hypothesis (Pasternak and Greenlee, 2005; Emrich et al., 2013; Sreenivasan et al., 2014; Serences, 2016). This hypothesis is appealing in part because visual cortex is thought to be one of the few brain areas with sufficient processing power to represent objects with the level of detail observed in short-term memory (for review, see Serences, 2016). However, it is not clear how visual cortex could maintain memory representations while simultaneously processing new incoming information nor how the different perceptual experiences of seeing versus remembering are accounted for by this hypothesis.

In contradiction to the sensory recruitment hypothesis, Bettencourt and Xu (2016) found that target features could not be decoded from early visual cortex when distractors were presented during the memory retention period, but that such distractors had no impact on behavioral performance. These investigators could decode activity reliably within a region of parietal cortex to predict the target stimulus regardless of whether a distractor was presented, suggesting an important role of that area in short-term memory. It remains contentious, therefore, whether visual cortex plays a necessary role in short-term memory maintenance (Xu, 2017).

In the present study, we tested whether the fidelity with which memoranda are stored is affected by the neural resources available within early visual cortex by varying the intracortical spacing of items. When items are presented simultaneously in the absence of working memory demands, their intracortical spacing is the primary constraint on their perceptual discriminability. Nearby stimuli “crowd” each other and the zone of crowding is determined by the distance between stimuli in retinotopic cortex (Pelli and Tillman, 2008; Pelli, 2008). Visual crowding occurs when the cortical spacing between visual objects prevents a distinct target representation in early visual cortex (Pelli, 2008; van den Berg et al., 2010; Anderson et al., 2012; Kwon et al., 2014; Chen et al., 2014) or results in pooling of stimulus representations at later levels of the visual hierarchy (Freeman and Simoncelli, 2011). If short-term memory of items presented in spatial isolation is maintained via the recruitment of the same sensory areas involved in the encoding of those features, then we should see worse memory performance for items that are closer together in visual cortex, and therefore share more neural resources, than for items with greater intracortical spacing.

Materials and Methods

Experiment 1 overview

We investigated whether log-scaling of visual cortex affects short-term memory by having observers remember three items on each trial arranged according to one of two spatial configurations and, using a method of adjustment, report the orientation of the item indicated by a probe. Within a trial, items were aligned along either the tangential axis or the radial axis and thus had greater or lesser intracortical spacing, respectively (see Fig. 1A,B). Importantly, in each configuration, one item appeared at 10° eccentricity directly to the right of fixation, so targets at this location were matched in all aspects except for the intracortical spacing between memoranda within the same trial. We thus focus analyses only on target items at this location, although all locations were probed equally often so as to encourage participants to store all items in short-term memory. Finally, we ensured that our data were not confounded by perceptual interference (Yeshurun et al., 2015) by presenting items sequentially and with sufficient durations and interstimulus intervals to negate such perceptual effects.

Participants.

Ten people participated in Experiment 1 (mean age 24 ± 3.07; 5 male, 5 female). All had typical color vision and normal or corrected-to-normal acuity and were naive to the purposes of the experiment. All observers gave written informed consent and were paid £10/h for their participation. The study was approved by the University of Cambridge Psychology Research Ethics Committee.

Experimental setup.

Participants sat in a head and chin rest positioned 57 cm from an ASUS LCD monitor. The resolution of the monitor was 1920 × 1200 within an area that was 44.8 × 28 cm with no pixel interpolation. Stimulus colors were selected after measuring the luminance of each color channel of the monitor with a spectrophotometer. Fixation was monitored online with an EyeLink 1000 (SR Research) recording at 500 Hz, calibrated once before each testing session and recalibrated as required throughout the experiment (see below). The experiment was programmed with the Psychophysics Toolbox Version 3 (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997) and Eyelink Toolbox (Cornelissen, Peters, and Palmer, 2002) in MATLAB (The MathWorks).

Stimuli.

On each trial, three randomly oriented bars (2° × 0.2° of visual angle) were presented sequentially and each was uniquely colored red, green, or blue. Colors were matched in luminance (26.2 cd/m2) and the order in which they appeared and their screen position were randomized across trials. A white fixation spot was displayed in the center of the screen throughout stimulus presentation and the memory delay period. All stimuli were presented on a black background (luminance < 1 cd/m2).

Within a trial, stimulus positions were arranged either tangentially or radially with respect to the point of fixation (see Fig. 1B). In both conditions, one item was centered on the horizontal meridian 10° right of fixation. In the radial condition, the two other items were positioned 2° left or right of the central item such that they were arranged along the horizontal meridian. In the tangential condition, one item was positioned 2° above the central item and the other was positioned 2° below the central item such that they were arranged orthogonal to the horizontal meridian. Although never presented simultaneously, the interstimulus spacing meant that their positions did not overlap. The order in which a stimulus was presented at each position was randomized across trials.

Procedure.

A typical trial sequence is shown in Figure 1C. At the start of each trial, an observer had to maintain fixation within a 2° region of the fixation spot for 500 ms for the trial to proceed. If fixation remained outside of this region for >2 s, then the eye tracker was recalibrated. Once correct fixation was registered, there was an additional variable delay between 250 and 750 ms (uniformly distributed). Stimuli were then presented sequentially in either a tangential or radial arrangement (see Fig. 1B). The stimulus duration and interstimulus interval were 500 ms. After the offset of the third stimulus, there was a 500 ms delay period, after which a probe circle (2° diameter) appeared centered on the location previously occupied by one of the three items, cueing the observer to report the orientation of that item using the mouse. Once any movement of the mouse was recorded, a response bar replaced the probe circle and followed the orientation designated by the mouse position relative to the bar center. The response bar had the same dimensions as the target item, but its orientation was randomized at the start of each response period and remained on screen until the observer clicked the mouse button to confirm their response.

During pilot testing with white stimuli, we noted that it was difficult to attribute clearly the probe circle to one memory item based on location alone, particularly for the radial condition. This is most likely due to the well known compression of perceptual space in peripheral vision (White et al., 1992; McGraw and Whitaker, 1999), so during Experiment 1, the probe circle and response bar also matched the color of the target item. Participants were informed that all items were equally likely to be the target. The target appeared equally as often across temporal order and location. There were 324 trials consisting of 18 repetitions for each target combination (three target locations for each of two spatial arrangements and three temporal orders).

If gaze position deviated by >2° from the fixation spot during stimulus presentation, the interstimulus interval, or the delay period, then the message, “Don't look away from the fixation point until it's time to respond,” appeared for 2 s and the trial restarted with newly randomized stimulus orientations. Each testing session took ∼1 h. After 50% of trials were completed, the observers were requested to take a short break, but were also instructed that they could rest at other times as they required.

Experimental design and analyses.

All comparisons in this experiment were within-subjects. Only trials in which the target item was positioned 10° to the right of fixation were analyzed. For items at this location, we compared memory performance across radial and tangential conditions with two measures collapsed across temporal order. We first analyzed the variability of report errors by calculating the circular SD of reports for each condition for each observer. These values were compared across conditions with a Bayesian t test using JASP software. We used the default Cauchy prior width of 0.707, but all results reported below were robust to standard alternate prior widths. Alongside Bayes factors, we provide Student's t test results.

In a second analysis, we assessed whether there was an influence of intracortical spacing on observers' reports using a probabilistic model of working memory performance. This was the “swap” model introduced by Bays et al. (2009) in which observers' responses are attributed to a mix of noisy reports centered on the target orientation, noisy reports centered on nontarget items, and a uniform lapse rate (see also Zhang and Luck, 2008). The details of this model have been described extensively previously (Bays et al., 2009; Gorgoraptis et al., 2011). The model has three free parameters: precision of reports, proportion of swap errors, and proportion of guesses. Parameters were estimated by maximum likelihood using code available online (http://www.paulbays.com/code/JV10; Bays et al., 2009).

We compared two versions of the model: a full model in which a separate set of parameters was used for radial and tangential conditions and a restricted model in which a single set of parameters was used for both conditions. To compare which of the two models best described the data, we used the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) summed across participants. To further test whether the models accounted for the data differentially, we submitted the differences in individuals' AIC scores to Bayesian and Student's t tests.

Experiment 2 overview

Experiment 2 was designed to ensure that the physical spacing between memoranda would result in competing representations within V1. For each participant in Experiment 2, we first measured the critical spacing of crowding, which is the area within which crowding occurs (Pelli and Tillman, 2008). We then tested observers' memory for memoranda presented sequentially within versus beyond their critical spacing. Moreover, we tested whether there was a correlation between critical spacing and memory performance, which could arise if working memory is related to individual differences in cortical surface area (Schwarzkopf et al., 2011). To increase statistical power and to assess the correlation between critical spacing and memory performance, we greatly increased the sample size compared with Experiment 1.

Each participant first completed a crowding task in which we found the inter-item distance at which their ability to recognize a target reached threshold level, which we take as the critical spacing of crowding. A participant's basic task was to identify the orientation of a bar surrounded by a circle flanked on either side by distractors (see Fig. 3A). Target and distractors were briefly presented in the upper peripheral visual field and trial-by-trial variations in inter-item spacing were controlled by an adaptive procedure. The participant reported the target orientation by clicking on one of three response options shown around the point of fixation (a three-alternative forced-choice task). After finding their critical spacing, the participant then completed a memory experiment in which three randomly oriented bars were presented in sequence in one of two spatial configurations (see Fig. 4A). Within each trial, memoranda were presented across a spatial range equal to either 0.75 or 1.5 times their critical spacing, corresponding to “crowded” and “uncrowded” conditions, respectively. As in Experiment 1, there was a common screen position for one item in each condition and we analyzed only memory performance for this stimulus position. Therefore, any differences in performance across conditions could only be driven by differences in intracortical spacing of memoranda.

Participants.

Twenty-one participants took part in Experiment 2 (mean age 30.14 ± 8.69 years; 8 male, 13 female), one had also participated in Experiment 1 and all other details were as per the previous experiment. Two participants did not complete the experiment due to problems tracking their eyes and their data were excluded from analyses, leaving a final sample size of 19.

Experimental setup.

All details were as per Experiment 1.

Stimuli.

Stimuli were bars (0.85° × 0.04°) centered in a circle with a diameter matching the bar length and a width of 0.04° (see Figs. 3A, 4A). Three of such stimuli were displayed in each trial of both the crowding experiment and the memory experiment and were uniquely colored. We chose three colors equally spaced in CIE L*a*b* color space, approximating red (L* = 74, a* = 34.6, b* = 20), green (L* = 74, a* = −28.3, b* = 28.3), and blue (L* = 74, a* = −28.3, b* = −28.3) hues. Colors were assigned randomly to the three stimuli on each trial. A white fixation spot was displayed in the center of the screen throughout stimulus presentation and the memory delay period. All stimuli were presented on a black background.

In the crowding task, three oriented stimuli were presented simultaneously on each trial (see Fig. 3A). The target orientation was random and the distractors' orientations were selected randomly from a uniform distribution that excluded orientations within 22.5° of the target orientation. Stimuli were centered 8.5° above fixation and arranged tangentially relative to fixation. The center stimulus was the target and the others were distractors. As described below, the target–distractor distance was controlled via a staircase. Response stimuli were target and distractors in a neutral hue (gray) appearing in random positions but equally spaced on the border of an imaginary circle (radius = 1.7°) around the screen center (see Fig. 3A). When response stimuli were on screen, observers could move a standard mouse arrow that appeared in the screen center. In the memory experiment, memoranda were of the same dimensions as the target and distractors in the crowding experiment, were each randomly assigned the colors described above, but were presented sequentially in random order. Stimulus orientations in the memory experiment were randomized with no restrictions.

Procedure.

A typical trial sequence of the crowding task is shown in Figure 3A. Each trial began after fixation compliance as per Experiment 1. Target and distractors appeared for 500 ms. After a 500 ms delay, response stimuli and the response arrow appeared centered at fixation and observers moved the arrow with the mouse and clicked on which stimulus they thought matched the target orientation. Observers were instructed that the target was always the central item on every trial and that one response item matched its orientation exactly. No other instruction was given explicitly regarding the distractor response items, but if a participant asked about them, then the experimenter told them that one item matched the target and the other two response items matched the distractors. The next trial immediately followed each mouse click that fell within the border of a stimulus and that stimulus was taken as their response.

The distance between the target and each distractor was controlled on each trial via an adaptive procedure, QUEST (Watson and Pelli, 1983), which was set to find the target–distractor spacing at which performance reached 67% accuracy (the midpoint of the psychometric function for a 3AFC task). We ran two randomly interleaved staircases of 36 trials each. For each QUEST procedure, we set the initial midpoint of the psychometric function (μ, see below) to two different levels to probe the asymptotes of the fitted function. These values, based on pilot observations, were set to 3.4° and 1.7°. These different QUEST parameters have the added advantage that the participant initially experiences relatively difficult and easy trials early on during testing. Furthermore, we allowed the target–distractor distance to vary only in steps of 0.21° during this threshold task. The procedure took ∼7 min. Whereas there was inevitably a working memory component to the crowding task, only the central element needed to be held in memory, so performance in this task indexes crowding occurring in sensory processing due to the simultaneously presented flankers rather than in memory.

The memory experiment was conducted in the same session as the crowding task and is shown in Figure 4A. Fixation compliance was performed as above and then each memory item was shown in random order with a duration, interstimulus interval, and delay period of 500 ms. Memoranda were shown in one of two spatial configurations either spaced to fall within or beyond the critical spacing of crowding, as measured during the preceding task. After the delay period, a circle (diameter = 0.85°, width = 0.04°) matching the color and location of one memory item was displayed, prompting the observer to report that item's orientation using the mouse. After the first mouse movement was registered, a response bar appeared within the circle so that the entire response stimulus matched the target dimensions. Observers then reported the target orientation as per Experiment 1 and the next trial began. Fixation errors and breaks were dealt with as described for Experiment 1. The crowding task and memory experiment took between 1 and 1.5 h per observer. The number of trials per stimulus combination was as described in Experiment 1.

Experimental design and statistical analyses.

We pooled data across staircases in the crowded task and used the least-squares method to fit the Weibull function specified by Watson and Pelli (1983) (see Fig. 3B). We modified the function to have three free parameters, μ, σ, and g, corresponding to the midpoint of the psychometric function, the slope, and the lapse rate, respectively. We took an observer's critical spacing to be μ, which was bound between 0.85° and 8.5°, the lower of which ensured incomplete overlap of stimulus positions in the memory experiment for participants with very small crowding zones. Note that the lower bound was reached by only 2 of 19 participants and none reached the upper bound (see Fig. 3C), so this restriction is unlikely to have affected the results. The slope, σ, was bound between 0 and infinity and the lapse rate, g, was bound between 0 and 0.05 as recommended by Watson and Pelli (1983) (see also Wichmann and Hill, 2001).

All comparisons in the memory experiment were within-subjects. We performed the same analyses of report variability and model fitting as per Experiment 1, but now with the conditions “crowded” and “uncrowded” to indicate trials in which memoranda were presented within or beyond the critical spacing of crowding, respectively. Importantly, these analyses were restricted to only memory items presented at the same screen position in both conditions so that performance was matched in all aspects except for the spatial arrangement of memoranda. We further tested for a relationship between cortical spacing and short-term memory with correlational analyses. We performed both a Bayesian Pearson correlation and linear regression using JASP to determine whether memory performance, regardless of crowding level, could be predicted by critical spacing. We again restricted data to only trials in which the memory item was presented directly above fixation. For the Bayesian correlation, we used the default stretched β prior width of 1, but results of this analysis were robust to various prior widths.

Results

Experiment 1

Perceptual resolution in peripheral vision is constrained by the distance between objects in V1. As visual eccentricity increases, fewer visual neurons are available to process a constantly sized input and this relationship is approximately logarithmic (Duncan and Boynton, 2003; Pelli, 2008). This log-scaling of visual cortex causes greater perceptual interference when multiple items are presented along a radial axis from the fovea compared with a tangential axis (Toet and Levi, 1992; Pelli and Tillman, 2008). In Experiment 1, we tested whether working memory is similarly influenced by the cortical spacing between memoranda (Fig. 1A). Observers were required to remember three sequentially presented, randomly oriented bars arranged either radially or tangentially relative to the point of fixation (Fig. 1B). At the end of each sequence, observers' memory of orientation was tested for a single item indicated by a location and color probe (Fig. 1C) and responses were made by manually adjusting a response bar to match the cued item. To control for non-memory-related differences across conditions, such as visual acuity, we analyzed memory performance only for targets positioned at 10° to the right of fixation in each condition. These stimuli were matched in all aspects except their spatial context.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Experiment 1 design. A, Differences in cortical spacing in peripheral vision. Top row, Screen coordinates of stimuli in peripheral vision with respect to the point of fixation (black spot). Bottom row, Inter-item spacing after cortical transformation. Such a cortical representation of space occurs in V1, which is hypothesized to maintain memory representations. Cortically transformed coordinates are normalized to the central target position. Green spots and purple diamonds represent radial and tangential spatial arrangements of stimuli, respectively. Note that, although stimuli are equally spaced in screen coordinates across conditions, radially arranged stimuli have less intracortical spacing than tangentially arranged stimuli. B, Stimulus design. Memoranda were randomly oriented colored bars presented sequentially along either the radial or tangential axis. Note that the center stimulus in each condition occupies the same screen (and therefore cortical) location. C, Example trial sequence. Observers fixated a white spot while memoranda were presented in sequence. Following a delay after the presentation of the third item, a probe was shown matching the color and location of one item chosen at random, cueing observers to move the mouse to report the remembered orientation of that item. A response bar appeared within the circle after the first mouse movement was detected, allowing observers to make their response using a method of adjustment.

Figure 2 summarizes observers' report errors for memoranda presented within a radial or tangential spatial configuration. As shown in Figure 2A, the circular SD did not differ consistently between configurations. Indeed, a Bayesian paired-samples t test found weak to moderate evidence in favor of there being no difference between conditions (B01 = 2.97; t(9) = 0.45, p = 0.66). These data provide evidence against the hypothesis that short-term memory is worse when memoranda are more closely spaced in visual cortex.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Results of Experiment 1. A, Report variability for each condition. Filled circles show the mean circular SD of reports for radial (green) and tangential (purple) configurations. Colored lines show individual participants' data. Error bars indicate ±1 SE. B, Differences in report variability across conditions. The black datum shows the mean difference and the colored data show individual difference scores, with colors corresponding to lines in A. C, Error distributions and model fit. Frequency of errors for the radial and tangential conditions are expressed as probability densities, with colors as in A. Data are shown for 16 equally spaced bins in the range [−π, π]. The solid blue line shows predictions of the best-fitting model, in which we assume memory is independent of the configuration of stimuli (shaded area indicates ±1 SE).

Figure 2C shows the distribution of errors in each condition. The solid line shows the fit of a model in which we assumed that memory performance factors are independent of the arrangement of stimuli. This model was a better fit to the data than the model in which cortical spacing could influence memory performance [summed ΔAIC = 29.5; 8 of 10 participants; Bayesian paired-samples t test: B10 = 3.60; t(9) = 2.83, p = 0.02; maximum likelihood (ML) parameter values, mean (SE): precision = 5.21 (0.25); swaps = 0.10 (0.01); guesses = 0.14 (0.02)]. This analysis further supports a dissociation between intracortical spacing and memory performance.

Finally, we ruled out the possibility that, although memory for the central item was unaffected, cortical spacing may have influenced the flanking memoranda that were excluded from the preceding analyses. We therefore repeated the above analyses, but included only trials in which the probed item was not in the central position. We first collated data across the remaining probe locations for each condition. We again found that there was no difference in circular SD between radial and tangential conditions (B01 = 2.89; t(9) = 0.52, p = 0.62). The model in which we assume working memory is independent of cortical spacing was also the superior model (summed ΔAIC = 34.4; 9 of 10 participants).

Experiment 2

In Experiment 1, we manipulated intracortical spacing of memoranda by presenting items along a radial or tangential visual axis relative to fixation. We found positive evidence that performance was the same across conditions (Fig. 2A). These results suggest that visual short-term memory does not have the properties of visual crowding that characterize retinotopic sensory areas that encode features. It is possible, however, that the stimulus arrangements that we selected were not appropriately scaled to produce overlapping cortical mnemonic representations. To address this possibility, we conducted a second experiment in which we used a psychophysical approach to tailor intracortical spacing of memoranda individually for each participant.

We tested whether the cortical spacing of memoranda affects short-term memory by presenting items sequentially either within or beyond the critical spacing of crowding. Critical spacing was found for each participant in a perceptual crowding task in which we used an adaptive staircase to find the target–distractor distance at which they could identify a target orientation at threshold level (Fig. 3A). Results from two example participants who performed differently at this task are shown in Figure 3B. Figure 3C shows the critical spacing estimates for all observers and the median for the group. Critical spacing estimates span a nearly fourfold range and such between-subjects variability has been reported previously (Petrov and Meleshkevich, 2011; Greenwood, Szinte, Sayim, and Cavanagh, 2017). To control for between-subjects crowding variability in the memory experiment, and therefore to control for cortical spacing variability across participants, we adjusted the spatial range of memoranda in the memory experiment to be either 0.75 times (“crowded”) or 1.5 times (“uncrowded”) an observer's critical spacing.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Design and results of the crowding task. A, Example trial sequence. After fixating a white spot, three stimuli were presented in the upper visual field. An observer's task was to identify the orientation of the center stimulus and report its orientation by clicking on the matching stimulus in gray in the subsequent display. The distance between target and distractors on each trial was controlled via an adaptive procedure. B, Example results and psychometric functions. Differently colored data show results for two differently performing observers. Solid lines show Weibull functions fit to each dataset. Dashed black lines and colored X symbols show the midpoint of the function and corresponding critical spacing estimates, respectively, for each participant. C, Estimated critical spacing for 19 observers. The median critical spacing is shown as the black datum and individual participants' values are shown in various colors. Estimates corresponding to the psychometric functions in B are shown as X symbols. Data have been jittered randomly on the x-axis to minimize overlap. Error bars indicate ±1 SE.

Results from the memory experiment are shown in Figure 4, B–E. We first compared observers' report variability for the crowded and uncrowded conditions (Fig. 4B). These data are summarized as difference scores in Figure 4C. Rather than finding an effect of crowding on response SD, a Bayesian paired-samples t test found moderate evidence in favor of there being no difference between conditions (BF01 = 4.21; t(18) = 0.051, p = 0.96).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Experiment 2 design and results. A, Example trials of the crowded and uncrowded conditions. Observers fixated a white spot and viewed a sequence of randomly orientated memoranda that appeared within (crowded condition) or beyond (uncrowded condition) the critical spacing of their upper visual field, as indicated by the white dotted circle (shown for illustration only). After the final delay period, a probe appeared at one of the memorandum locations and observers reported the target orientation at this location using a method of adjustment (see Materials and Methods). B, Report variability for each condition. Data are shown as in Figure 2A. Colored lines indicating each observer's performance match colors in Figure 3C. C, Differences in report variability across conditions. Data are shown as in Figure 2B. D, Error distributions and model fit. Green and purple points show crowded and uncrowded conditions, respectively. Data are shown as described in Figure 2C. The model assuming memory performance is independent of cortical spacing (blue line) was again a better fit to the data than the model assuming an influence of cortical spacing, which has been omitted to increase visibility. E, Relationship between critical spacing and memory performance. No correlation between report variability pooled across conditions and critical spacing was found. Solid line indicates regression line of best fit.

Figure 4D shows the distribution of report errors averaged across observers, with green and purple data showing crowded and uncrowded conditions, respectively. We tested whether memory performance across conditions is better described by a model in which cortical spacing influences performance or a model in which working memory is independent of cortical spacing of memoranda. The blue line in Figure 4D shows the model that is independent of cortical spacing, which was a better fit than the alternate model [summed ΔAIC = 52.46; 16 of 19 participants; Bayesian paired-samples t test: B10 = 150.2; t(18) = 2.83, p < 0.001; ML parameter values, mean (SE): precision = 5.62 (0.16); swaps = 0.04 (0.002); guesses = 0.34 (0.01)]. Although there is a higher probability density of uncrowded trials than crowded trials in the central bin (Fig. 4D; BF10 = 6.61), 16 bins were arbitrarily chosen for display purposes and there would have been evidence against such a difference between conditions had we selected, for example, 15 bins (BF10 = 0.43). The analysis of variability and model fitting above are based on raw (unbinned) data, so they are not influenced by arbitrary designation of bin size.

Figure 4E shows the results of the correlational analysis in which we investigated whether there was a relationship between observers' critical spacing and memory performance. A Bayesian correlation pairs test found moderate evidence that there is no relationship (r = 0.015, BF01 = 3.52). Similarly, a linear regression that uses critical spacing to predict report error found a slope of only 0.007 (t = 0.062, p = 0.951), indicating that there is no relationship between critical spacing and working memory performance.

As with Experiment 1, we again ruled out the possibility that cortical spacing may have influenced the flanking memoranda that were excluded from the preceding analyses. We repeated the above analyses including only trials in which the probed item was not in the central position, collapsing data across the remaining probe locations for each condition. In support of the results above, we found that there was no difference in circular SD between crowded and uncrowded conditions (B01 = 4.08; t(18) = 0.27, p = 0.79). Finally, the model in which we assume working memory is independent of cortical spacing was superior (summed ΔAIC = 51.86; 16 of 19 participants).

Discussion

We investigated whether the cortical spacing between sequentially presented memoranda affects observers' ability to hold those items in memory. In Experiment 1, we manipulated intracortical spacing by arranging memoranda either radially or tangentially relative to the fovea (Fig. 1). In Experiment 2, we tailored the intracortical spacing of memoranda to each observer by first quantifying their critical spacing of crowding (Fig. 3) and then presented memory items within or beyond this region (Fig. 4). Across both experiments, we found positive evidence that working memory performance is independent of the cortical distance between memoranda. Although the strength of evidence in each experiment was only moderate, the combined evidence across experiments is assessed by the product of the individual Bayes factors: that is, 12.5, which is substantial.

Our study provides clear evidence of a dissociation between perceptual coding and memory coding within a very short period after stimulus offset. Cortical distance in retinotopically organized visual cortex can account for a wide variety of perceptual phenomena such as visual acuity (Duncan and Boynton, 2003), shape perception (Michel et al., 2013), subjective experience of size (Schwarzkopf et al., 2011), and visual crowding (Pelli, 2008). In the present study, however, we have shown that memory representations of nonspatial features are independent of their V1 sensory representations. We know from our data that the emergence of dissociated representations occurs within the timeframe of the target duration and interstimulus interval (1 s). This time course thus places an upper bound on the transfer of retinotopic sensory representations to other neural systems involved in working memory.

This result sheds light on previous psychophysical studies that have found errors in working memory due to spatially proximal memoranda. Pertzov et al. (2014) and Ahmad et al. (2017) found that memory for nonspatial features was worse when memoranda were presented sequentially at overlapping or similar screen locations than when memoranda were presented at spatially separate screen locations. However, the timing used in these experiments would have likely produced perceptual interference sometimes referred to as “temporal crowding” (Yeshurun et al., 2015). Such perceptual interference would degrade the encoding of memoranda due to their persistent overlapping cortical representations. Indeed, the nature of errors in these previous studies of working memory are consistent with those in visual crowding paradigms with minimal working memory demands (Ester et al., 2014; Harrison and Bex, 2015, 2017). The combination of target duration and interstimulus interval used by Pertzov et al. (2014) (500 ms) thus sets a lower bound on the time required to transform a sensory signal into a memory representation.

Our results raise several important challenges for the hypothesis that working memory representations are maintained via the same sensory neurons that encode the features of memoranda (Serences, 2016). Previous studies in which a remembered feature is decoded from activity within V1 typically analyze activity within voxels corresponding to the spatial location of the memory item (Serences et al., 2009; Harrison and Tong, 2009). Because our data reveal that sensory representations are independent of memory representations, these decoding analyses must either be decoding nonsensory neurons that are nonetheless tuned to the memoranda feature dimension, which we think is unlikely, or reflect an influence from other areas. Other brain regions implicated in memory maintenance include prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex (Courtne et al., 1998; Todd and Marois, 2004; Christophel et al., 2012; Bettencourt and Xu, 2016). In prefrontal cortex in particular, neurons display activity during memory delays that encodes stimulus locations and features (Goldman-Rakic, 1995; Wimmer et al., 2014; Mendoza-Halliday and Martinez-Trujillo, 2017; Murray et al., 2017; but see Lara and Wallis, 2014). These areas are part of a distributed network involved in working memory and the role of V1 in this network remains to be fully understood (D'Esposito, 2007; D'Esposito and Postle, 2015; Christophel et al., 2017).

Another alternative is that working memory is maintained via the recruitment of sensory neurons well beyond the initial sensory representation (Ester et al., 2009). According to this “neural outsourcing” proposal, the memory representation of a stimulus might be shifted to neurons that normally encode sensory stimulation in some other part of the visual field. However, it has yet to be clarified how visual features with overlapping sensory representations are allocated to other sensory regions in a way that prevents memory interference and how a mapping is maintained between outsourced representations and their original locations in the visual field.

Bays (2014) recently proposed a neural resource model of working memory based on population coding, which can account for changes in memory precision as a function of the number of memoranda. A key feature of this model is that a fixed amount of neural activity (i.e., spiking) must be shared among all memory items. Increasing set size, therefore, decreases the neural resource available for each item, resulting in a loss of memory precision. The property of maintaining a fixed level of population activity is termed normalization: it has been described as a canonical neural computation, which is implemented in many different neural systems using varied mechanisms (Carandini and Heeger, 2011). To reproduce observed effects of set size accurately, the normalization in the model must operate globally; that is, not be limited to particular regions of the visual field or particular stimulus feature values (Bays, 2015). The present results are in agreement with this in that they confirm that there is no cost of spatial proximity of memoranda as might be expected from a purely local form of normalization.

Neurophysiological evidence consistent with global normalization has been found in prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices, areas that have been implicated as playing an important role in working memory maintenance (for review, see Bays, 2015). Although inspired by properties of visual neurons, the neural resource model is agnostic as to the neural locus of working memory representations because population coding is a common mechanism of representation observed throughout the brain (Pouget, Dayan, and Zemel, 2000), including prefrontal cortex (Wimmer et al., 2014; Murray et al., 2017). Nonetheless, one possible interpretation consistent with the present findings is that, in the case of visual working memory representations, normalization occurs within networks in which neurons are not strictly topographically organized.

Although neural models of short-term memory can account for a broad range of human performance, we are not aware of any model that can account for our result. In a recent study, Schneegans and Bays (2017) presented strong evidence in favor of a model in which nonspatial features are combined with spatial location via a conjunctive population code. This extension of the neural resource model correctly predicted their empirical observation that, when memoranda are presented simultaneously, observers were more likely to confuse items in working memory (“swap” errors) when the cued memory item was close to distractors than when distractors were relatively distant from the cued item.

This model is also consistent with the results of Tamber-Rosenau et al. (2015), who found that the frequency of swap errors for simultaneously presented memoranda depends on the degree of perceptual crowding. Because visual crowding increases positional uncertainty (Harrison and Bex, 2017), a conjunctive code that binds spatial location with orientation will produce more swap errors under strongly crowded conditions than weakly crowded conditions, as was observed by Tamber-Rosenau et al. (2015). The Schneegans and Bays' (2017) model therefore suggests an important role of location in binding nonspatial features when items are presented simultaneously, but leaves open the question of how to account for the present findings with sequentially presented memoranda. It is possible that nonspatial features can be bound according to a conjunctive code that links features with their temporal order, but neurophysiological evidence for such a model is scarce. Accounting for the lack of spatial interactions between sequentially presented memoranda represents a challenge for future modeling efforts.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust and the National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia (C.J. Martin Fellowship APP1091257 to W.J.H.).

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to William J Harrison, Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK. willjharri{at}gmail.com

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Ahmad J,
    2. Swan G,
    3. Bowman H,
    4. Wyble B,
    5. Nobre AC,
    6. Shapiro KL,
    7. McNab F
    (2017) Competitive interactions affect working memory performance for both simultaneous and sequential stimulus presentation. Sci Rep 7:4785. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-05011-x pmid:28684800
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Anderson EJ,
    2. Dakin SC,
    3. Schwarzkopf DS,
    4. Rees G,
    5. Greenwood JA
    (2012) The neural correlates of crowding-induced changes in appearance. Curr Biol 22:1199–1206. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.063 pmid:22658599
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Bays PM
    (2014) Noise in neural populations accounts for errors in working memory. J Neurosci 34:3632–3645. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3204-13.2014 pmid:24599462
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Bays PM
    (2015) Spikes not slots: noise in neural populations limits working memory. Trends Cogn Sci 19:431–438. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.06.004 pmid:26160026
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Bays PM,
    2. Catalao RF,
    3. Husain M
    (2009) The precision of visual working memory is set by allocation of a shared resource. J Vis 9:7.1–11. doi:10.1167/9.10.7 pmid:19810788
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Bettencourt KC,
    2. Xu Y
    (2016) Decoding the content of visual short-term memory under distraction in occipital and parietal areas. Nat Neurosci 19:150–157. doi:10.1038/nn.4174 pmid:26595654
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Brainard DH
    (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis 10:433–436. doi:10.1163/156856897X00357 pmid:9176952
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Carandini M,
    2. Heeger DJ
    (2011) Normalization as a canonical neural computation. Nat Rev Neurosci 13:51–62. doi:10.1038/nrn3136 pmid:22108672
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Chen J,
    2. He Y,
    3. Zhu Z,
    4. Zhou T,
    5. Peng Y,
    6. Zhang X,
    7. Fang F
    (2014) Attention-dependent early cortical suppression contributes to crowding. J Neurosci 34:10465–10474. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1140-14.2014 pmid:25100582
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Christophel TB,
    2. Hebart MN,
    3. Haynes JD
    (2012) Decoding the contents of visual short-term memory from human visual and parietal cortex. J Neurosci 32:12983–12989. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0184-12.2012 pmid:22993415
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Christophel TB,
    2. Klink PC,
    3. Spitzer B,
    4. Roelfsema PR,
    5. Haynes JD
    (2017) The distributed nature of working memory. Trends Cogn Sci 21:111–124. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.12.007 pmid:28063661
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Cornelissen FW,
    2. Peters EM,
    3. Palmer J
    (2002) The eyelink toolbox: eye tracking with MATLAB and the psychophysics toolbox. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers 34:613–617. doi:10.3758/BF03195489
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Courtney SM,
    2. Petit L,
    3. Maisog JM,
    4. Ungerleider LG,
    5. Haxby JV
    (1998) An area specialized for spatial working memory in human frontal cortex. Science 279:1347–1351. doi:10.1126/science.279.5355.1347 pmid:9478894
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. D'Esposito M
    (2007) From cognitive to neural models of working memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences 362:761–772. doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.2086 pmid:17400538
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. D'Esposito M,
    2. Postle BR
    (2015) The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Annu Rev Psychol 66:115–142. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015031 pmid:25251486
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Duncan RO,
    2. Boynton GM
    (2003) Cortical magnification within human primary visual cortex correlates with acuity thresholds. Neuron 38:659–671. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00265-4 pmid:12765616
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Emrich SM,
    2. Riggall AC,
    3. Larocque JJ,
    4. Postle BR
    (2013) Distributed patterns of activity in sensory cortex reflect the precision of multiple items maintained in visual short-term memory. J Neurosci 33:6516–6523. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5732-12.2013 pmid:23575849
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Ester EF,
    2. Serences JT,
    3. Awh E
    (2009) Spatially global representations in human primary visual cortex during working memory maintenance. J Neurosci 29:15258–15265. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4388-09.2009 pmid:19955378
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Ester EF,
    2. Klee D,
    3. Awh E
    (2014) Visual crowding cannot be wholly explained by feature pooling. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 40:1022–1033. doi:10.1037/a0035377 pmid:24364703
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Freeman J,
    2. Simoncelli EP
    (2011) Metamers of the ventral stream. Nat Neurosci 14:1195–1201. doi:10.1038/nn.2889 pmid:21841776
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Goldman-Rakic PS
    (1995) Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron 14:477–485. doi:10.1016/0896-6273(95)90304-6 pmid:7695894
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Gorgoraptis N,
    2. Catalao RF,
    3. Bays PM,
    4. Husain M
    (2011) Dynamic updating of working memory resources for visual objects. J Neurosci 31:8502–8511. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0208-11.2011 pmid:21653854
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    1. Greenwood JA,
    2. Szinte M,
    3. Sayim B,
    4. Cavanagh P
    (2017) Variations in crowding, saccadic precision, and spatial localization reveal the shared topology of spatial vision. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 114:E3573–E3582. doi:10.1073/pnas.1615504114 pmid:28396415
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  24. ↵
    1. Harrison SA,
    2. Tong F
    (2009) Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas. Nature 458:632–635. doi:10.1038/nature07832 pmid:19225460
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Harrison WJ,
    2. Bex PJ
    (2015) A unifying model of orientation crowding in peripheral vision. Curr Biol 25:3213–3219. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.052 pmid:26628010
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Harrison WJ,
    2. Bex PJ
    (2017) Visual crowding is a combination of an increase of positional uncertainty, source confusion, and featural averaging. Sci Rep 7:45551. doi:10.1038/srep45551 pmid:28378781
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Kwon M,
    2. Bao P,
    3. Millin R,
    4. Tjan BS
    (2014) Radial-tangential anisotropy of crowding in the early visual areas. J Neurophysiol 112:2413–2422. doi:10.1152/jn.00476.2014 pmid:25122703
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Lara AH,
    2. Wallis JD
    (2014) Executive control processes underlying multi-item working memory. Nat Neurosci 17:876–883. doi:10.1038/nn.3702 pmid:24747574
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. McGraw PV,
    2. Whitaker D
    (1999) Perceptual distortions in the neural representation of visual space. Exp Brain Res 125:122–128. doi:10.1007/s002210050667 pmid:10204765
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Mendoza-Halliday D,
    2. Martinez-Trujillo JC
    (2017) Neuronal population coding of perceived and memorized visual features in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Nat Commun 8:15471. doi:10.1038/ncomms15471 pmid:28569756
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Michel MM,
    2. Chen Y,
    3. Geisler WS,
    4. Seidemann E
    (2013) An illusion predicted by V1 population activity implicates cortical topography in shape perception. Nat Neurosci 16:1477–1483. doi:10.1038/nn.3517 pmid:24036915
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Murray JD,
    2. Bernacchia A,
    3. Roy NA,
    4. Constantinidis C,
    5. Romo R,
    6. Wang XJ
    (2017) Stable population coding for working memory coexists with heterogeneous neural dynamics in prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 114:394–399. doi:10.1073/pnas.1619449114 pmid:28028221
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    1. Pasternak T,
    2. Greenlee MW
    (2005) Working memory in primate sensory systems. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:97–107. doi:10.1038/nrn1603 pmid:15654324
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Pelli DG
    (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis 10:437–442. doi:10.1163/156856897X00366 pmid:9176953
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Pelli DG
    (2008) Crowding: a cortical constraint on object recognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol 18:445–451. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2008.09.008 pmid:18835355
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Pelli DG,
    2. Tillman KA
    (2008) The uncrowded window of object recognition. Nat Neurosci 11:1129–1135. doi:10.1038/nn.2187 pmid:18828191
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Pertzov Y,
    2. Husain M
    (2014) The privileged role of location in visual working memory. Atten Percept Psychophys 76:1914–1924. doi:10.3758/s13414-013-0541-y pmid:24027033
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Petrov Y,
    2. Meleshkevich O
    (2011) Asymmetries and idiosyncratic hot spots in crowding. Vision Res 51:1117–1123. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2011.03.001 pmid:21439309
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Pouget A,
    2. Dayan P,
    3. Zemel R
    (2000) Information processing with population codes. Nat Rev Neurosci 1:125–132. doi:10.1038/35039062 pmid:11252775
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Schneegans S,
    2. Bays PM
    (2017) Neural architecture for feature binding in visual working memory. J Neurosci 37:3913–3925. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3493-16.2017 pmid:28270569
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    1. Schwarzkopf DS,
    2. Song C,
    3. Rees G
    (2011) The surface area of human V1 predicts the subjective experience of object size. Nat Neurosci 14:28–30. doi:10.1038/nn.2706 pmid:21131954
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Serences JT
    (2016) Neural mechanisms of information storage in visual short-term memory. Vision Res 128:53–67. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2016.09.010 pmid:27668990
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Serences JT,
    2. Ester EF,
    3. Vogel EK,
    4. Awh E
    (2009) Stimulus-specific delay activity in human primary visual cortex. Psychol Sci 20:207–214. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02276.x pmid:19170936
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Sprague TC,
    2. Ester EF,
    3. Serences JT
    (2014) Reconstructions of information in visual spatial working memory degrade with memory load. Curr Biol 24:2174–2180. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.07.066 pmid:25201683
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Sreenivasan KK,
    2. Curtis CE,
    3. D'Esposito M
    (2014) Revisiting the role of persistent neural activity during working memory. Trends Cogn Sci 18:82–89. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.001 pmid:24439529
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Tamber-Rosenau BJ,
    2. Fintzi AR,
    3. Marois R
    (2015) Crowding in visual working memory reveals its spatial resolution and the nature of its representations. Psychol Sci 26:1511–1521. doi:10.1177/0956797615592394 pmid:26270073
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Todd JJ,
    2. Marois R
    (2004) Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature 428:751–754. doi:10.1038/nature02466 pmid:15085133
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Toet, A,
    2. Levi DM
    (1992) The two-dimensional shape of spatial interaction zones in the parafovea. Vision Research, 32:1349–1357. pmid:1455707
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. van den Berg R,
    2. Roerdink JB,
    3. Cornelissen FW
    (2010) A neurophysiologically plausible population code model for feature integration explains visual crowding. PLoS Comput Biol 6:e1000646. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000646 pmid:20098499
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Watson AB,
    2. Pelli DG
    (1983) QUEST: a bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Percept Psychophys 33:113–120. doi:10.3758/BF03202828 pmid:6844102
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. White JM,
    2. Levi DM,
    3. Aitsebaomo AP
    (1992) Spatial localization without visual references. Vision Res 32:513–526. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(92)90243-C pmid:1604838
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Wichmann FA,
    2. Hill NJ
    (2001) The psychometric function: I. fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit. Percept Psychophys 63:1293–1313. doi:10.3758/BF03194544 pmid:11800458
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Wimmer K,
    2. Nykamp DQ,
    3. Constantinidis C,
    4. Compte A
    (2014) Bump attractor dynamics in prefrontal cortex explains behavioral precision in spatial working memory. Nat Neurosci 17:431–439. doi:10.1038/nn.3645 pmid:24487232
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Xu Y
    (2017) Reevaluating the sensory account of visual working memory storage. Trends Cogn Sci 21:794–815. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2017.06.013 pmid:28774684
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Yeshurun, Y.,
    2. Rashal, E., and
    3. Tkacz-Domb, S
    (2015) Temporal crowding and its interplay with spatial crowding. J Vis 15: pii: 11. doi:10.1167/15.3.11 pmid:25788705
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. ↵
    1. Zhang W,
    2. Luck SJ
    (2008) Discrete fixed-resolution representations in visual working memory. Nature 453:233–235. doi:10.1038/nature06860 pmid:18385672
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 38 (12)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 38, Issue 12
21 Mar 2018
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Visual Working Memory Is Independent of the Cortical Spacing Between Memoranda
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Visual Working Memory Is Independent of the Cortical Spacing Between Memoranda
William J. Harrison, Paul M. Bays
Journal of Neuroscience 21 March 2018, 38 (12) 3116-3123; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2645-17.2017

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Visual Working Memory Is Independent of the Cortical Spacing Between Memoranda
William J. Harrison, Paul M. Bays
Journal of Neuroscience 21 March 2018, 38 (12) 3116-3123; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2645-17.2017
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • cued recall
  • short-term memory
  • visual crowding
  • visual working memory

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Expectation cues and false percepts generate stimulus-specific activity in distinct layers of the early visual cortex Laminar profile of visual false percepts
  • Acute ethanol modulates synaptic inhibition in the basolateral amygdala via rapid NLRP3 inflammasome activation and regulates anxiety-like behavior in rats
  • Haploinsufficiency of Shank3 in mice selectively impairs target odor recognition in novel background odors
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Prostaglandin E2 induces long-lasting inhibition of noradrenergic neurons in the locus coeruleus and moderates the behavioral response to stressors
  • Detection of spatially-localized sounds is robust to saccades and concurrent eye movement-related eardrum oscillations (EMREOs)
  • Rewarding capacity of optogenetically activating a giant GABAergic central-brain interneuron in larval Drosophila
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.