Abstract
Mutations in the cereblon (CRBN) gene cause human intellectual disability, one of the most common cognitive disorders. However, the molecular mechanisms of CRBN-related intellectual disability remain poorly understood. We investigated the role of CRBN in synaptic function and animal behavior using male mouse and Drosophila models. Crbn knock-out (KO) mice showed normal brain and spine morphology as well as intact synaptic plasticity; however, they also exhibited decreases in synaptic transmission and presynaptic release probability exclusively in excitatory synapses. Presynaptic function was impaired not only by loss of CRBN expression, but also by expression of pathogenic CRBN mutants (human R419X mutant and Drosophila G552X mutant). We found that the BK channel blockers paxilline and iberiotoxin reversed this decrease in presynaptic release probability in Crbn KO mice. In addition, paxilline treatment also restored normal cognitive behavior in Crbn KO mice. These results strongly suggest that increased BK channel activity is the pathological mechanism of intellectual disability in CRBN mutations.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Cereblon (CRBN), a well known target of the immunomodulatory drug thalidomide, was originally identified as a gene that causes human intellectual disability when mutated. However, the molecular mechanisms of CRBN-related intellectual disability remain poorly understood. Based on the idea that synaptic abnormalities are the most common factor in cognitive dysfunction, we monitored the synaptic structure and function of Crbn knock-out (KO) animals to identify the molecular mechanisms of intellectual disability. Here, we found that Crbn KO animals showed cognitive deficits caused by enhanced BK channel activity and reduced presynaptic glutamate release. Our findings suggest a physiological pathomechanism of the intellectual disability-related gene CRBN and will contribute to the development of therapeutic strategies for CRBN-related intellectual disability.
Introduction
Human intellectual disability, also widely known as mental retardation, is a psychiatric disease characterized by intelligence quotient scores lower than 70 and cognitive impairments such as decreases in language development, memory, and problem-solving skills (Greenspan and Woods, 2014). Intellectual disability is thought to be caused by many different genetic etiological factors. Among them, a nonsense (c.1274C>T) or missense (c.1171T>C) mutation in the CRBN gene, located on chromosome 3 (3p26.3) exon 11, is suspected to cause intellectual disability in humans (Higgins et al., 2004, 2008; Sheereen et al., 2017). Deletion or microduplication of 3p26.3 including CRBN also results in cognitive and behavioral impairment (Dijkhuizen et al., 2006; Papuc et al., 2015).
CRBN is a target of the drug thalidomide (Millrine and Kishimoto, 2017). Thalidomide was originally developed as a sedative, but was found to be severely teratogenic, upon which its use for sedation was discontinued (Vargesson, 2015). However, thalidomide and its derivatives (lenalidomide and pomalidomide) have recently been widely prescribed for multiple myeloma, myelodysplastic syndrome, and autoimmune diseases (Bartlett et al., 2004; Millrine and Kishimoto, 2017). The mechanism of CRBN function has been intensely investigated in the past decade. CRBN forms a CRL4CRBN E3 ubiquitin ligase complex with DNA-binding protein 1 (DDB1), cullin-4A (CUL4A), and regulator of cullins 1 (ROC1) as substrate receptors (Fischer et al., 2014; Liu et al., 2014). Thalidomide inhibits CRL4CRBN E3 ubiquitin ligase activity after binding to CRBN (Ito et al., 2010; Fischer et al., 2014; Liu et al., 2014). A large body of studies have identified the substrates of CRL4CRBN E3 ubiquitin ligase and the include AMP kinase (Lee et al., 2011), casein kinase 1A1 (Krönke et al., 2015), IKZF1 and 3 (Krönke et al., 2014; Lu et al., 2014; Zhu et al., 2014), MEIS2 (Fischer et al., 2014), glutamine synthetase (Nguyen et al., 2016, 2017), CLC channels (Hohberger and Enz, 2009; Ronstedt et al., 2015), and BK channels, which are large-conductance calcium-activated potassium channels (Jo et al., 2005; Liu et al., 2014). However, despite the fact that the study of CRBN has evolved from its identification as a mild intellectual disability causal factor, the cellular and molecular mechanisms of cognitive impairment caused by CRBN mutations remain unclear.
Crbn is widely expressed in the postmitotic period in most parts of the rodent brain, including the hippocampus, cerebral cortex, and cerebellum (Aizawa et al., 2011). We sought to investigate whether CRBN mutations cause defects in the maintenance of synaptic structure and function, which are closely related to cognitive function. Here, we studied the synaptic and behavioral function of CRBN and CRBN-related intellectual disability using Crbn mutant animal models. Crbn knock-our (KO) mice, a loss-of-function animal model for Crbn, have normal brain development, but exhibit impaired cognitive function similar to CRBN-mutated human intellectual disability patients (Higgins et al., 2004; Dijkhuizen et al., 2006; Papuc et al., 2015; Sheereen et al., 2017). Research into synaptopathy and channelopathy has determined the molecular mechanisms of cognitive disorders and identified genes and factors associated with neuropsychiatric diseases such as fragile X mental retardation and autism spectrum disorder (Vaillend et al., 2008; Pavlowsky et al., 2012). We applied this approach to Crbn mutant animal models and found that they had reduced neurotransmitter release at excitatory synapses because of increased BK channel activity.
Materials and Methods
Experimental animals
All mice were housed in an animal facility with a specific pathogen-free barrier under a 12 h light/dark cycle. Mice were allowed access to food and water ad libitum. Wild-type (WT) and Crbn KO male mice (Lee et al., 2013) from the C57BL/6 background were used. All experiments were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Seoul National University and the Gwangju Institute of Science and Technology Animal Care and Use Committee. Drosophila strains were raised on a standard yeast, sugar, and agar medium at 25°C. The WT strain was w1118 unless otherwise noted. CRBNex1 was generated by P-element imprecise excision of the GS14825 line (Kyoto Drosophila Genetic Resource Center, #205969). Df(3R)BSC621 (a deficiency uncovering the CRBN locus, #25696) and C155-GAL4 (pan-neuronal driver, #458) were obtained from Bloomington Stock Center. The CRBN cDNA clone LD28592 was purchased from the Drosophila Genomics Resource Center (Indiana University, Bloomington, IN). UAS-Myc-CRBN and UAS-Myc-CRBNG552X were generated by microinjection of pUAST vector-cloned DNA into w1118 embryos.
Behavior tests
Passive avoidance test.
The passive avoidance test was performed using 10- to 11-week-old (WT vs KO experiment) male mice. The observer was blinded to the genotype or pharmacological treatment of the animal. The passive avoidance apparatus consisted of a light and dark chamber separated by a guillotine door. The floor of the dark chamber was made of stainless-steel grids. During habituation, mice were allowed to freely explore the box for 5 min with the door open and were then returned to their home cage. For conditioning, after 24 h, the mice were placed into the light chamber and the sliding door was closed when both hindlimbs of the mice had entered into the dark chamber. Then, an electric foot shock (0.25 mA, 2 s) was delivered via the floor grids. Ten seconds later, the mice were returned to their home cage. Tests were performed 24 h after conditioning and the latency time required for mice to enter the dark chamber was measured with a 300 s cutoff.
Novel object recognition test.
The novel object recognition test was performed using 10- to 11-week-old male mice. The mice were first habituated in a large plastic chamber (22 cm wide × 27 cm long × 30 cm high) for 7 min at 24 h intervals. After 2 d (training trials), the mice were presented with 2 objects and allowed to explore freely for 7 min. In the testing trial performed 24 h later, a novel object was substituted for 1 of the 2 objects (the familiar objects) and the mice were scored for recognition over a 7 min period. Object recognition was defined as spending time oriented toward the object at a distance of 1 cm or less, touching the object, or sniffing with the nose. All of the objects were cleaned with ethanol after each session to remove odor cues. The observer was blinded to the genotype of the animal. Exploration times were recorded and discrimination time was calculated as follows: (exploration time with novel object − exploration time with familiar object)/(total exploration time).
Elevated plus maze (EPM) test.
The EPM test was performed using 10- to 11-week-old male mice. The EPM apparatus was a plus-shaped maze elevated 60 cm above the floor. It consisted of two closed arms surrounded by 30-cm-high opaque walls and two open arms (110 × 110 cm). Each mouse was placed in the center (5 × 5 cm) of the maze facing one of the closed arms and allowed to explore the space for 15 min. The movement of mice was tracked and recorded via an overhead video camera and further analyzed by real-time video-tracking computer software (Smart Version 2.5; Panlab). Time spent in the closed arms, center, and open arms was measured. The maze was cleaned with 70% ethanol and wiped with paper towels between each trial.
Drug preparation and in vivo injection
Paxilline (Tocris Bioscience) was dissolved to a 25 or 10 mm concentration in dimethylsulfoxide (DMSO) and then diluted 1:10 or 1:2000 in saline (the concentration of DMSO was 10% or 0.05%, v/v). Paxilline were injected at 30 mpk or 3 μg/kg body weight intraperitoneally. Ten percent DMSO was used for vehicle treatment.
Primary neuronal culture and transfection
Primary hippocampal neurons were isolated from the early postnatal day 0 (P0) to P1 WT and Crbn KO mice brains. Hippocampi were digested with trypsin (Invitrogen, 15090) and DNase (Sigma-Aldrich, DN25). Dissociated neurons were plated on poly-l-lysine coated coverslips at 3 × 105 cells per well. Neurons were maintained in neurobasal-A medium (Invitrogen, 10888-022) supplemented with B-27 (Invitrogen, 17504044), GlutaMAX-I (Invitrogen, 35050-061), and 1% penicillin/streptomycin in a humidified 5% CO2 incubator at 37°C. Two days after plating, 2.5 μm cytosine arabinoside was added to inhibit the non-neuronal cells. Twice a week thereafter, half of the medium was exchanged with fresh maintenance medium. Cultured hippocampal neurons were transfected with DNA constructs (human wild CRBN and R419X) at 7 d in vitro (DIV7) using calcium phosphate. Experiments were performed at DIV9-14.
Spine morphology analysis
For the detailed spine morphology observation, we labeled cultured neurons using DiI (Invitrogen, D-282). Twenty DIV-cultured neurons were incubated with DiI (5 μg/ml) for 30 min at 37°C. The stained cells were quickly washed in 1× PBS and fixed using 4% PFA. The fluorescence images were acquired using a Fluoview FV 1000 confocal laser-scanning microscope equipped with 100× and 60× oil-immersion objectives and capable of an additional 4× zoom. Spine density and morphology were analyzed using ImageJ software. Spine density was quantified based on morphological categories as described previously (Pak et al., 2001).
Nissl staining
P2 mice brains were harvested and immersed in 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 m phosphate buffer (PFA) for 2 d at 4°C and then transferred to PBS buffer for 1 d at 4°C. Three-month-old mice were anesthetized with 20% urethane and then perfused transcardially with 4% PFA. After cardiac perfusion, whole brains were removed and postfixed for 1 d. For cryosections, brains (both P2 and 3 months) were cryoprotected with 30% sucrose for 2 d and coronal sections of 40 μm were cut with a cryostat (Leica) and stained with 0.1% cresyl violet for Nissl staining.
Immunohistochemistry of Drosophila larval neuromuscular junction (NMJs)
Wandering third-instar larvae were dissected in Ca2+-free HL3.1 saline (Feng et al., 2004) and fixed in Bouin's fixative for 7 min. Larvae were washed with PBS containing 0.1% Triton X-100 (PBT) and blocked in 5% BSA in PBT for 1 h. Samples were incubated with FITC-conjugated anti-HRP (Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories) at 1:200 for 2 h at room temperature. Fluorescence images of NMJs labeled with anti-HRP were acquired with an FV300 laser-scanning confocal microscope (Olympus) and accompanying FLOUVIEW software using a Plan Apo 40× 0.90 numerical aperture (NA) objective. All quantifications were performed at NMJ 6/7 in A2 segments. For analysis of NMJ morphology, confocal sections were maximally projected. Quantification of bouton number was performed as described previously (Lee et al., 2016).
qRT-PCR analysis
Adults of WT and CRBNex1 were collected and RNA was isolated using QIAGEN's RNeasy Mini Kit. Total RNA (1 μg) was reverse-transcribed by using M-MLV reverse transcriptase (Promega) to generate cDNA for qRT-PCR (Bio-Rad CFX96 Real-Time PCR Detection System and SYBR Green) using 500 nm primer concentrations and 2 ng of cDNA templates. The following primers were used to amplify Drosophila CRBN cDNA: 5′-GCAATTGGAGGACCAACAGT-3′ and 5′-ACATGTCACTGCTGGGATGA-3′. The relative values were calculated using the ΔΔCT method via normalization to α-tubulin mRNA levels.
Electrophysiology
Slice preparation.
Using 3- to 5-week-old (both sexes) or 10- to 12-week-old (male) WT and Crbn KO mice, transverse hippocampal slices (300 μm for whole-cell recordings or 400 μm for extracellular field potential recordings) were prepared as described previously (Yang et al., 2011; Choi et al., 2015). After decapitation, brains were rapidly removed and placed in ice-cold, oxygenated (95% O2 and 5% CO2), low-Ca2+/high-Mg2+ dissection buffer containing the following (in mm): 5 KCl, 1.23 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 10 dextrose, 0.5 CaCl2, 10 MgCl2, and 212.7 sucrose. Slices were transferred to a holding chamber in an incubator containing oxygenated (95% O2 and 5% CO2) artificial CSF (ACSF) containing the following (in mm): 124 NaCl, 5 KCl, 1.23 NaH2PO4, 2.5 CaCl2, 1.5 MgCl2, 26 NaHCO3, and 10 dextrose at 28–30°C for at least 1 h before recording.
Extracellular field potential recordings.
After recovery, slices were transferred to a recording chamber where they were perfused continuously with oxygenated ACSF (28–30°C) at a flow rate of 2 ml/min. CA1 field EPSPs (fEPSPs) were evoked by Schaffer collateral (SC) stimulation (0.2 ms current pulses) using a concentric bipolar electrode. Synaptic responses were recorded with ACSF-filled microelectrodes (1–3 MΩ) and were quantified as the initial slope of fEPSP in CA1. Recordings were performed using an AM-1800 microelectrode amplifier (A-M Systems), a PG 4000A stimulator (Cygnus Technology), and a SIU-90 isolated current source (Cygnus Technology). Baseline responses were collected at 0.07 Hz with a stimulation intensity that yielded a 40–60% maximal response. NMDAR-dependent LTP was induced by high-frequency stimulation (HFS, 100 Hz for 1 s) or four episodes of theta burst stimulation (TBS) at 10 s intervals. TBS consisted of 10 stimulus trains delivered at 5 Hz; each train consisted of four pulses at 100 Hz. NMDAR-dependent LTD was induced by low-frequency stimulation (LFS; 900 pulses at 1 Hz for 900 s). Long-lasting LTP (L-LTP) was induced by 4 trains of HFS separated by 5 min intervals. Group I mGluR-dependent LTD was induced by 10 min bath-application of 100 μm (R,S)-3,5-DHPG (Tocris Bioscience). Data from slices with stable recordings (<5% change over the baseline period) were included in the analysis. All data are presented as mean ± SEM normalized to the preconditioning baseline (at least 20 min of stable responses). The experimenters were blinded to mouse genotypes. IGOR software (Wavemetrics) was used for digitizing and analyzing the responses.
Whole-cell recordings.
After recovery, slices were transferred to a recording chamber where they were perfused continuously with ACSF gassed with 95% O2/5% CO2 at a flow rate of 2 ml/min. Slices were equilibrated for 5 min before the recordings and all of the experiments were performed at 23–25°C. All recordings were performed in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons identified by their size and morphology. Recordings were obtained using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices) under visual control with differential interference contrast illumination in an upright microscope (Olympus, BX51WI). Patch pipettes (4–6 MΩ) were filled with the following (in mm): 130 Cs-MeSO4, 0.5 EGTA, 5 TEA-Cl, 8 NaCl, 10 HEPES, 1 QX-314, 4 ATP-Hg, 0.4 GTP-Na, 10 phosphocreatine-Na2, 0.1 spermine [for the miniature ESPC (mEPSC), excitatory ESPC (eEPSC), and eIPSC experiments], 130 CsCl, 1.1 EGTA, 2 MgCl2, 0.1 CaCl2, 10 NaCl, 10 HEPES, 2ATP-Na (for mIPSC experiments), 135 K-gluconate, 8 NaCl, 10 HEPES, 2 ATP-Na, and 0.2 GTP-Na (for calcium-activated potassium current experiments) (Aoki and Baraban, 2000) at a pH of 7.4 and 280–290 mOsm. The extracellular recording solution consisted of ACSF supplemented with picrotoxin (100 μm), TTX (1 μm), and DL-AP5 (50 μm) for mEPSC experiments, picrotoxin (100 μm) for the eEPSC experiment, TTX (1 μm), CNQX (20 μm) and DL-AP5 (50 μm) for the mIPSC experiment, CNQX (20 μm) and DL-AP5 (50 μm) for the eIPSC experiment, and TTX (1 μm) for measurement of calcium-activated potassium currents. Evoked synaptic responses were elicited by SC stimulation (0.2 ms current pulses) using a concentric bipolar electrode placed 200–300 μm in front of the postsynaptic pyramidal cell. All eEPSC and eIPSC recordings were conducted in an external solution containing elevated divalent cations (4 mm Ca2+ and Mg2+) to reduce network excitability and prevent polysynaptic responses upon stimulation (Clem and Huganir, 2010). Only cells with an access resistance <20 MΩ and an input resistance >100 MΩ were studied. The cells were discarded if the input or the access resistance changed >20%. Data were acquired and analyzed using pClamp 10.2 (Molecular Devices). Signals were filtered at 2 kHz and digitized at 10 kHz with Digidata 1440A (Molecular Devices).
Drosophila NMJ recordings Two-electrode voltage-clamp (TEVC) recordings of wandering third-instar female Drosophila larvae NMJ were obtained from ventral longitudinal muscle 6 in segment A3–A4 at room temperature as described previously with modifications (Karr et al., 2009). All dissections and recording were performed in HL3.1 saline containing the following (in mm): 70 NaCl, 5 KCl, 4 MgCl2, 5 trehalose, 115 sucrose, and 5 HEPES (Feng et al., 2004). Larval dissection was performed in Ca2+-free saline to minimize muscle contraction. TEVC recording was performed with 2 mm Ca2+. Recording electrodes were filled with 3 m KCl, which had a resistance of 10–15 MΩ. Recordings were made from cells with an initial resting membrane potential between −60 and −70 mV at a holding potential of −70 mV with a Geneclamp 500 amplifier (Molecular Devices). The cut segmental nerve was stimulated with a glass suction electrode at a suprathreshold level (5 mA) for 0.2 ms. Signals were filtered at 10,000 Hz, acquired with Axoscope version 10.2 software (Molecular Devices), and analyzed with Clampfit version 10.2 software (Molecular Devices). EPSC amplitudes were measured from peak to the baseline immediately before EPSC onset.
Live imaging and analysis of neurotransmitter release with vGlut1-pHluorin
To measure vesicle release, neurons were transfected with vesicular glutamate transporter-pHluorin (vG-pH) as described previously (Ariel and Ryan, 2010; Kim and Ryan, 2013). Coverslips were mounted in a stimulation chamber with laminar flow perfusion on the stage of a custom-built laser-illuminated epifluorescence microscope. Live images were acquired with an iXon Ultra 897 (Andor Technology, DU-897U-CS0-#BV) back-illuminated EM CCD camera. A diode-pumped OBIS 488 laser (Coherent) shuttering by synchronizing the TTL on/off signal from the EMCCD camera during acquisition was used as a light source. Fluorescence excitation/emission and collection were achieved using a 40× (1.3 NA) Fluar Zeiss objective lens with 500–550 nm emission and 498 nm dichroic filters (Chroma) for pHluorin. Action potentials were evoked by passing a 1 ms current pulse through platinum-iridium electrodes from an isolated current stimulator (World Precision Instruments). Neurons were perfused in saline-based buffer containing the following (in mm): 119 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 2 CaCl2, 2 MgCl2, 25 HEPES, and 30 glucose, along with 10 μm CNQX and 50 μm AP5 (adjusted to pH 7.4). All experiments were performed at 30°C. For image analysis, ImageJ was used with the plugin Time-series analyzer. vGlut1-pH+ boutons were selected as a region of interest (oval, diameter: 8 pixels). Fluorescence traces were analyzed with Origin Pro-8.0.
Brain slice biotinylation assay
Biotinylation assays were performed using hippocampal slices as described previously (Gabriel et al., 2014). Hippocampal slices (300 μm thick) were prepared from 5- to 7-week-old WT and male Crbn KO mice. The slices were incubated for 40 min at 31°C in oxygenated ACSF. To biotinylate the cell surface proteins, slices were treated with sulfo-NHS-SS-biotin (Thermo Scientific, 21328) in ACSF under gentle shaking at 4°C for 45 min. The slices were then incubated twice at 4°C for 25 min in quenching solution (100 mm BSA in ACSF). The quenched slices were lysed in 400 μl of ice-cold RIPA buffer containing protease inhibitor mixture (Sigma-Aldrich, 11836153001) and incubated for 1 h on ice. The lysates were then centrifuged at 14,000 × g for 15 min at 4°C and supernatant was collected. Protein concentrations were determined using the BCA Protein Assay Kit (Thermo Scientific, 23225). To isolate biotin-labeled proteins, 200 μg lysates were added to 40 μl of streptavidin-agarose beads (Thermo Scientific, 29200) and rotated at 4°C overnight. The beads were washed with lysis buffer and eluted with 2× Laemmli sample buffer. Surface (biotin-labeled) and input (10% of total, 20 μg) proteins were analyzed via Western blot.
Western blot analysis
Hippocampal slices from 3- to 5-week-old WT and CRBN KO male mice were prepared using the same method of slice preparation as for electrophysiology. Samples were homogenized in ice-cold lysis buffer (T-PER tissue protein extraction reagent; Thermo Scientific, 78510) combined with a protease inhibitor mixture (Sigma-Aldrich, P8340). Homogenates were centrifuged at 13,000 rpm for 30 min at 4°C and supernatants were stored at −80°C. Protein concentrations were measured using the BCA Protein Assay Kit (Thermo Scientific, 23225). Equal amounts (30 μg) of protein were loaded into the lanes of a SDS polyacrylamide gel (10%). Separated proteins were then transferred onto PVDF membranes, blocked with 5% skim milk in TBST for 1 h at room temperature, and incubated overnight with primary antibodies for immunoblotting. Membranes were washed with TBST and then incubated with HRP-conjugated secondary antibodies for 1 h. HRP was detected using SuperSignal West Pico Chemiluminescent substrate (Thermo Scientific, 34080) and a Bio-Image Analyzer (Bio-Rad ChemiDoc MP). Membranes were stripped with Restore Western blot Stripping Buffer (Thermo Scientific, 21059) and reprobed for β-actin.
Antibodies
The following primary antibodies were used: GluA1 (Abcam, ab31232, 1:500), GluA2 (Millipore, MAB397, 1:500), GluN1 (Thermo Scientific, PA3-102, 1:1000), GluN2A (Santa Cruz Biotechnology, sc-1468, 1:1000), GluN2B (Santa Cruz Biotechnology, sc-1469, 1:1000), synapsin1 (Millipore, AB1543, 1:500), synaptophysin (Santa Cruz Biotechnology, sc-9116, 1:1000), vGluT1 (Millipore, MAB5502, 1:500), GAD67 (Millipore, MAB5406, 1:5000), vGAT (Millipore, AB5062P, 1:500), PSD95 (Thermo Scientific, MA1-046, 1:2000), gephyrin (Santa Cruz Biotechnology, sc-6411, 1:1000), BK channel (BD Biosciences, 611249, 1:500), and β-actin (Santa Cruz Biotechnology, sc-47778, 1:1000). HRP-conjugated anti-mouse (Abcam, ab6728, 1:10,000), anti-rabbit (Abcam, ab6802, 1:10,000), and anti-goat (Jackson Immunoresearch Laboratories, 805-035-180, 1:10,000) IgG H&Ls were used as secondary antibodies.
Statistical analysis
Data analyses and graphical display were performed with SigmaPlot version 11.0 software (Systat Software). All displayed values represent the mean ± SEM. Significant differences between groups were determined using independent or paired Student's t tests or Mann–Whitney U test and multiple comparisons were performed using two-way ANOVA.
Results
Crbn KO mice show normal brain and synaptic morphology
We first tested whether the cognitive impairments of Crbn mutants were due to abnormal neurodevelopment because the inhibitory target of thalidomide is CRBN and thalidomide causes abnormal limb development. When Nissl-stained brain sections of WT and Crbn KO mice were examined, however, no appreciable anatomical abnormalities were detected in P2 or 3-month-old mice (Fig. 1A). Morphological changes in synaptic structure generally affect the cognitive function of mammalian brains (Kasai et al., 2010). However, we found no significant differences in the density (Fig. 1C; t(63) = −0.489, p = 0.626), head-width (Fig. 1D; t(63) = −1.626, p = 0.109), or length of dendritic spines (Fig. 1E; t(63) = 1.378, p = 0.173) between WT and Crbn KO neurons. Analysis revealed that the density of the four different types of dendritic spines (mushroom (t(63) = −1.484, p = 0.143), thin (t(63) = 1.066, p = 0.291), stubby (t(63) = −0.472, p = 0.639), and branched (t(63) = −0.0355, p = 0.972)) was not significantly affected by Crbn deficiency (Fig. 1F). These results demonstrated that lack of CRBN did not affect the macroscopic or microscopic architecture of the mouse brain significantly and strongly suggested that the defects in learning and memory found in Crbn KO mice were unlikely to be related to brain structural abnormalities.
Crbn KO mice have normal brain architecture and spine morphology. A, Nissl stains of WT and Crbn KO whole brain sections (top) and hippocampus (bottom) at P2 and 3 months. Scale bar, 1 mm (top) and 0.5 mm (bottom). B, Images of primary cultured hippocampal neurons from WT (left) and Crbn KO (right) mice labeled with DiI at 20 DIV (Scale bar, 50 μm). The morphology of the dendritic spines is shown in a high-magnification view in the bottom panel. Scale bar, 10 μm. C–E, No differences in total spine density (C), total spine head-width (D), or total spine length (E) were seen between WT and Crbn KO neurons. F, No differences in spine densities of the different morphological categories (mushroom, thin, stubby, or branched types) were seen between WT and Crbn KO neurons. n = 37 WT neurons; n = 28 Crbn KO neurons. All data shown are mean ± SEM. n.s., Not significant by independent Student's t test.
Crbn KO mice show normal long-term synaptic plasticity but reduced excitatory neurotransmitter release
We next characterized synaptic function in Crbn KO animals. We found that basal excitatory synaptic transmission measured by the amplitude of evoked eEPSCs against stimulus intensity (SI) was decreased (Fig. 2A; SI at 0.04 mA: t(39) = 2.893, p = 0.006; SI at 0.06 mA: t(39) = 2.568, p = 0.014; SI at 0.08 mA: t(39) = 2.453, p = 0.019; SI at 0.10 mA: t(39) = 2.267, p = 0.029; SI at 0.15 mA: t(39) = 2.177, p = 0.036; SI at 0.20 mA: t(39) = 2.358, p = 0.023), but basal inhibitory synaptic transmission measured by the amplitude of eIPSCs against SI (Fig. 2A; p > 0.05 at all SIs) in hippocampal SC-CA1 synapses of Crbn KO mice. Crbn KO mice and WT mice showed no differences in mEPSCs Fig. 2B; amplitude: t(37) = 0.782, p = 0.439; frequency: t(37) = 1.056, p = 0.298) or mIPSCs (Fig. 2C; amplitude: t(47) = 0.397, p = 0.693; frequency: t(47) = −0.341, p = 0.734) in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. We compared the expression levels of several synaptic proteins that are important for maintaining and regulating synaptic structure and function in the hippocampal tissues of WT and Crbn KO mice. Levels of most synaptic proteins, including glutamate receptor subunits GluA1 (t(7) = 0.511, p = 0.625), GluA2 (t(7) = −0.0187, p = 0.986), GluN1 (t(7) = −0.103, p = 0.921), GluN2A (t(7) = −0.0724, p = 0.944), GluN2B; excitatory (PSD95, t(7) = 0.376, p = 0.718) and inhibitory (gephyrin, t(7) = 0.200, p = 0.847) postsynaptic proteins, and excitatory (synaptophysin, t(7) = −0.688, p = 0.513;, vGluT1, t(7) = 0.0498, p = 0.962) and inhibitory (GAD67 (t(7) = 0.0838, p = 0.936) presynaptic proteins, were identical in WT and Crbn KO mice (Fig. 2D). Crbn KO mice showed slightly increased synapsin I expression (t(7) = −3.089, p = 0.018) and decreased vGAT expression (Fig. 2D; t(7) = 0.371, p = 0.05); however, the physiological relevance and mechanism of these changes were not clear.
Basal synaptic transmission in SC-CA1 synapses and synaptic protein profiles of Crbn KO mice hippocampus. A, Left, representative traces of evoked EPSCs (eEPSCs) and evoked IPSCs (eIPSCs) recorded with holding potentials of −70 and 0 mV in hippocampal SC-CA1 synapses from WT (black trace) or Crbn KO (red trace) mice. Right, input/output relationship of eEPSCs (middle) or eIPSCs (right) from WT (black bar) and Crbn KO mice (red bar). Crbn KO mice showed reduced eEPSC amplitudes than WT mice, but eIPSCs were similar between two groups. n = 21, 3 (21 cells from 3 WT mice); n = 20, 3 (KO). B, Representative traces (left), amplitude (middle), and frequency (right) of AMPAR-mEPSCs in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons of WT (black trace) and Crbn KO mice (red trace). Amplitude and frequency of AMPAR-mEPSCs were not significantly different between WT and Crbn KO mice. n = 19, 4 WT; n = 20, 5 KO. C, Representative traces (left), amplitude (middle), and frequency (right) of mIPSCs in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons of WT (black trace) and KO mice (red trace). Amplitude and frequency of mIPSCs were not significantly different between WT and Crbn KO mice. n = 27, 3 WT; n = 22, 3 KO. D, Hippocampal homogenates from 3- to 5-week-old WT and Crbn KO mice were immunoblotted to evaluate the levels of several synaptic proteins. All protein signals were normalized relative to β-actin levels before comparisons between genotypes were made. Synapsin I was increased and vGAT was decreased significantly in Crbn KO mice, but the expression levels of other synaptic proteins were similar between two groups. n = 4 WT; n = 5 KO. All data shown are mean ±SEM. **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05. n.s., Not significant by independent Student's t test.
Long-term synaptic plasticity such as LTP or LTD is a cellular and/or molecular mechanism of learning and memory and is impaired in Crbn KO mice (Neves et al., 2008). Therefore, we tested whether Crbn KO mice show impaired long-term synaptic plasticity. Three- to 4-week-old Crbn KO mice exhibited normal induction of NMDAR-dependent TBS-LTP (Fig. 3A; t(53) = 0.155, p = 0.877) and protein synthesis-dependent L-LTP (Fig. 3B; t(18) = 0.402, p = 0.877). The induction of LTP in 10- to 12-week-old Crbn KO animals was also intact (Fig. 3C; t(21) = 0.340, p = 0.737). We confirmed that Crbn KO mice also had normal NMDAR-dependent LFS-LTD (Fig. 3D; t(19) = 0.793, p = 0.438) and 100 μm DHPG-induced mGluR-dependent LTD (Fig. 3E; t(11) = 1.649, p = 0.127).
Crbn KO mice exhibit normal long-term synaptic plasticity in hippocampal SC-CA1 synapses. A, B, No differences were seen in LTP in hippocampal SC-CA1 synapses from 3- to 5-week-old Crbn KO mice; NMDAR-dependent early-phase TBS-LTP (n = 27 cells from 7 WT mice; n = 28 cells from 8 Crbn KO mice, compared with last 10 min average after tetanic stimulation) (A) and L-LTP induced by four repeated HFS (HFS × 4) with 5 min intervals (n = 11, 7 (WT); n = 9, 6 (KO) compared with last 10 min average after tetanic stimulation) (B). C, No differences in NMDAR-dependent TBS-LTP in hippocampal SC-CA1 synapses from 10- to 12-weeks old WT and Crbn KO mice (n = 13, 8 WT; n = 10, 5 KO compared with last 10 min average after tetanic stimulation). D, E, No differences in long-term depression in hippocampal SC-CA1 synapses from 3- to 4-week-old Crbn KO mice; NMDAR-dependent LFS-LTD (n = 13, 3 WT; n = 8, 4 KO) compared with last 10 min average after tetanic stimulation (D) and group I mGluR-dependent LTD (mGluR-LTD) induced by 10 min bath-application of 100 μm (R,S)-3,5-DHPG (n = 7, 3 WT; n = 6, 2 KO compared with last 10 min average after tetanic stimulation) E, Left, fEPSP slope was plotted before and after stimulation. Middle, Sample traces represent fEPSPs at 1 min before (gray is WT, pink is KO) and 1 or 3 (only for L-LTP experiment) h after (black is WT, red is KO) conditioning stimulation. Right, Magnitudes of long-term synaptic plasticity were calculated by comparing the average slopes of fEPSPs recorded during the last 10 min with those recorded before stimulation. All data shown are mean ± SEM. n.s., Not significant by independent Student's t test.
However, Crbn KO mice showed an increased paired-pulse ratio (PPR) in excitatory [Fig. 4A; interstimulus interval (ISI) at 50 ms: t(26) = −3.933, p < 0.001; ISI at 100 ms: t(26) = −2.349, p = 0.027; ISI at 200 ∼ 800 ms: p > 0.05] but not inhibitory synaptic transmission (Fig. 4B; ISI at 20 ∼ 800 ms: p > 0.05), implying decreased probability of release (Pr) from CRBN-lacking presynaptic excitatory neurons. We confirmed decreased Pr in excitatory (Fig. 4C; p < 0.05 at the second ∼ 20th stimulus) but not inhibitory synapses (Fig. 4D; p > 0.05 at the second ∼ 20th stimulus) with altered short-term plasticity (STP) induced by 20-pulse train stimulation. The results suggest that cognitive impairment in Crbn KO mice is caused by reduced presynaptic excitatory neurotransmitter release, not by alterations in long-term synaptic plasticity.
Excitatory, but not inhibitory, presynaptic neurotransmitter release is reduced in hippocampal SC-CA1 synapses of Crbn KO mice. A, B, Left, Representative traces of PPR of eEPSCs (A) or eIPSCs (B) in hippocampal SC-CA1 synapses from WT (black trace) or Crbn KO mice (red trace). Right, eEPSC-PPR or eIPSC-PPR was plotted against ISIs. n = 13, 2 WT; n = 15, 3 KO (for the eEPSC-PPR experiment), n = 12, 2 WT; n = 12, 2 KO (for the eIPSC-PPR experiment). eEPSC-PPR was increased in Crbn KO mice and eIPSC-PPR was similar between two groups. C, D, Left, Representative traces of altered 20-pulse train stimulation-induced responses of eEPSCs (C) or eIPSCs (D) in hippocampal SC-CA1 synapses from WT (black trace) or Crbn KO mice (red trace). Right, Relative amplitudes of eEPSCs or eIPSCs were plotted against stimulus number. STP in excitatory synaptic transmission induced by 20 pulses at 20 Hz stimulation was increased in Crbn KO mice (second ∼ 20th stimulus, *p < 0.05). n = 8, 2 WT; n = 8, 2 KO for the eEPSC-STP experiment; n = 13, 2 WT; n = 12, 2 KO for the eIPSC-STP experiment; ***p < 0.001.
Drosophila Crbn loss-of-function mutants show decreased probability of neurotransmission release not rescued by the overexpression of the CRBN G552X mutant
Crbn KO mice showed decreased neurotransmitter Pr in excitatory synapses without detectable structural phenotypes. We confirmed the changes in excitatory Pr in other species by testing CRBN function in third-instar NMJ synapses in Drosophila (Fig. 5A), which are glutamatergic excitatory synapses (Davis and Müller, 2015). The trans-heterozygous loss-of-function mutants, CRBNex1/Df, bear a copy of the CRBNex1 allele and a deficiency (Df) allele for Drosophila Crbn (CG3925) (Fig. 5B–D). CRBNex1/Df mutants showed normal synaptic structures similar to those observed in WT and Crbn KO neurons (Fig. 5E). We observed normal amplitude (t(25) = −1.652, p = 0.11) and normal frequency (t(25) = −1.975, p = 0.062) of the miniature excitatory junctional currents (mEJCs) (Fig. 5F), and normal amplitude of the evoked EJCs (eEJCs, Fig. 5G; t(25) = 0.553; p = 0.58) in CRBNex1/Df mutants. Notably, CRBNex1/Df mutants had obviously increased PPR (Fig. 5H; ISI at 10 ms: t(24) = −4.935, p < 0.001; ISI at 25 ms: t(24) = −4.604, p < 0.001; ISI at 50 ms: t(24) = −4.730, p < 0.001; ISI at 100 ms: t(24) = −4.614, p < 0.001) and altered 20-pulse train stimulation-induced STP (Fig. 5I; p < 0.001 at the third ∼ sixth stimulus; p < 0.01 at the seventh stimulus; p < 0.05 at eighth ∼ 10th stimuli). These Drosophila results were highly consistent with the presynaptic neurotransmitter release phenotypes seen in Crbn KO mice.
Drosophila Crbn mutants have decreased probability of neurotransmitter release. A, Domain structures of human CRBN and its Drosophila ortholog with percentage identity between their corresponding domains. CRBN C-like domain is shown in dark yellow. The single asterisks denote substitution mutations of CRBN used in this study. B, Genomic map of Drosophila CRBN locus. The exons of CRBN are indicated by boxes and the coding regions are colored black. The 1160 bp deleted regions (3R10048848∼10050008) for CRBN null mutants (CRBNex1) are also presented. C, Genomic PCR analyses in WT WT, heterozygous CRBN mutants (CRBNex1/+) using the C primer set in B. D, qRT-PCR analysis for CRBN using the D primer set in B. E, Representative confocal images of anti-HRP labeled Drosophila Crbn mutants third-instar larval NMJ 6/7. Scale bar, 50 μm. Quantification of total bouton number as a percentage of the control. n = 9 WT; n = 8 (CRBNex1/Df). F, Spontaneous mEJCs of the NMJ synapses in Drosophila Crbn mutant. Representative traces (left), mEJC amplitude (middle), and mEJC frequency (right). n = 12 WT; n = 17 CRBNex1/Df. G, eEJC in Crbn mutant. Representative traces (left) and eEJC amplitude (right). n = 12 WT; n = 15 (CRBNex1/Df). H, Decreased PPR in Crbn mutant larvae. Representative traces (left) and PPR is plotted against indicated ISIs (right). n = 15 WT; n = 11 CRBNex1/Df. I, STP induced by 20-pulse train stimulation in Crbn mutant flies. Representative traces (left) and the amplitudes of eEJCs are plotted against stimulus number (right). n = 20 WT; n = 11 CRBNex1/Df; ***p < 0.001.
Next, we evaluated whether impaired presynaptic release could be rescued by the reintroduction of WT CRBN. Using a pan-neuronal C155-GAL4 driver and UAS-Crbn lines, we specifically overexpressed WT CRBN in neurons with a CRBNex1/Df background (C155-GAL4/+; UAS-Myc-CRBN/+; CRBNex1/Df) and tested presynaptic release phenotypes. The overexpression of not only WT CRBN, but also the Drosophila CRBN G552X mutant did not affect the amplitudes of mEJCs (Fig. 6A; amplitude: F(2,34) = 1.056, p = 0.359) or eEJCs (Fig. 6B; amplitude: F(2,34) = 0.767, p = 0.473) but mEJC frequency in the overexpression of CRBN G552X mutant was decreased (Fig. 6A; frequency: F(2,34) = 4.264, p = 0.022). However, the overexpression of WT CRBN successfully rescued PPR (Fig. 6C; ISI at 10 ms: F(2,34) = 6.637, p = 0.004; ISI at 25 ms: F(2,34) = 4.055, p = 0.026; ISI at 50 ms: F(2,34) = 2.474, p = 0.099; ISI at 100 ms: F(2,33) = 0.493, p = 0.614), and 20-pulse train stimulation-induced STP (Fig. 6D; p < 0.001 at the fifth stimulus, p < 0.01 at the sixth, seventh, and ninth stimulus, p < 0.05 at the second, fourth, and 11th stimulus, p > 0.05 at the third, eighth, 10th, and 12th ∼ 15th stimulus) in CRBNex1/Df mutants to the levels seen in C155-GAL4 control flies. These rescue effects were not found in flies overexpressing the Drosophila CRBN G552X mutant, which mimics the human CRBN R419X pathogenic mutant (Fig. 6C,D). Together, these results strongly suggest that the presynaptic functions of CRBN are highly conserved from flies to humans and that presynaptic defects are caused not only by lack of Crbn expression, but also by the CRBN nonsense mutation that is observed in human mild intellectual disability patients (Higgins et al., 2004, 2008).
Decreased probability of neurotransmitter release in Drosophila Crbn mutants is not rescued by the overexpression of the pathogenic CRBN mutant. A, Representative traces (left) of spontaneous mEJCs in the following genotypes: C155-GAL4/+ (control), C155-GAL4/+; UAS-Myc-CRBNWT/+; CRBNex1/Df (CRBN WT rescue) and C155-GAL4/+; UAS-Myc-CRBNG552X/+; CRBNex1/Df (CRBN GX rescue). Mean amplitude (middle) and frequency (right) of spontaneous mEJCs are shown. n = 11 control; n = 8 CRBN-WT rescue; n = 18 CRBN-GX rescue. B, Representative eEJC traces (left) and mean amplitude of eEJCs (right) in CRBN rescue animals. n = 11 control; n = 8 CRBN-WT rescue; n = 18 CRBN-GX rescue. C, Overexpression of CRBN WT rescued the increased PPR (10 and 25 ms ISI) at Crbn mutant synapses but CRBN GX did not. Representative traces (left) and PPR is plotted against indicated ISIs (right). n = 11 control; n = 8 CRBN-WT rescue; n = 18 CRBN-GX rescue. D, 20-pulse train stimulation-induced STP at Crbn mutant synapses is rescued by presynaptic CRBN-WT expression but not by CRBN-GX. Representative traces (left) and the amplitudes of eEJCs are plotted against stimulus number (right). n = 10 control; n = 8 CRBN-WT rescue; n = 15 CRBN-GX rescue. All data shown are mean ± SEM. ***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05. n.s., Not significant by independent Student's t test.
Crbn KO mice show increased BK channel activity-induced synaptic and behavioral dysfunction
Since a previous report in which we established that CRBN binds to the C terminus of the BK channel α subunit and inhibits surface expression of the BK channel (Jo et al., 2005), an interaction between CRL4CRBN and the BK channel, which promotes ubiquitination of the BK channel and blocks membrane exposure via ER retention of the BK channel, has been reported (Liu et al., 2014). Therefore, we hypothesized that a Pr decrease in Crbn KO mice might be due to BK channel hyperactivity. We measured calcium-activated potassium currents (IK(Ca)) by applying brief depolarization from the holding potential (−50 mV) under a TTX-including external solution (Aoki and Baraban, 2000). As a result, Crbn KO mice showed approximately twofold higher IK(Ca) than WT mice, but treatment with 10 μm paxilline, a BK channel blocker, in Crbn KO mice normalized IK(Ca) to the extent that it became similar to the current in neurons from WT mice (Fig. 7A; genotype: F(1,94) = 12.931, p < 0.001; treatment: F(1,94) = 14.585, p < 0.001; genotype × treatment: F(1,94) = 6.580, p = 0.012). This suggests that BK channel activity is significantly increased in Crbn KO mice. To test the idea, we measured BK channel expression in the plasma membrane. We analyzed the amount of actin for control as well. We found that the expression of the BK channel in the plasma membrane was increased in Crbn KO mice (Fig. 7B; Input: p = 0.099; Biotinylation: p = 0.006), suggesting that increased BK current in Crbn KO is due to increased BK channel expression in the plasma membrane. We next tested whether increased BK channel activity in Crbn KO mice induces excitatory Pr changes. Paxilline eliminated the increase in PPR (Fig. 7C; genotype: F(1,46) = 9.152, p = 0.004; treatment: F(1,46) = 14.677, p < 0.001; genotype × treatment: F(1,46) = 6.202, p = 0.016) and 20-pulse train stimulation-induced responses observed in Crbn KO mice and established similar levels to those seen in WT mice (Fig. 7D; p < 0.01 at all responses to the second ∼ 20th stimulus between WT + vehicle vs KO + vehicle, p < 0.05 at all responses to the second ∼ 20th stimulus between KO + vehicle vs KO + paxilline). We found that paxilline treatment itself did not changes intrinsic properties such as membrane capacitance, input resistance, and resting membrane potential in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons in either WT or Crbn KO mice (Table 1). We confirmed our findings with iberiotoxin, another BK channel blocker. Our results clearly showed that iberiotoxin decreased PPR elevation in Crbn KO (Fig. 7E; genotype: F(1,57) = 12.321, p < 0.001; treatment: F(1,57) = 7.045, p = 0.010; genotype × treatment: F(1,57) = 6.896, p = 0.011) and 20-pulse train stimulation-induced responses (Fig. 7F; p < 0.05 at all responses to the second ∼ seventh, 10th, 13th, 16th, and 20th stimulus between WT + vehicle vs KO + vehicle, p < 0.05 at all responses to the second ∼ 20th stimulus between KO + vehicle vs KO + iberiotoxin).
Reduced excitatory presynaptic function in Crbn KO mice is rescued by a BK channel blocker. A, Increased IK(Ca) in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons in Crbn KO mice were selectively reduced by paxilline (a pan-BK channel blocker, 10 μm). n = 29, 3 (29 cells from 3 animals) for WT mice with vehicle (WT + vehicle); n = 17, 3 for KO mice with vehicle (KO + vehicle); n = 29, 3 (WT + paxilline); n = 29, 3 (KO + paxilline). B, Increased surface expression of BK channels in Crbn KO mice. Hippocampal slices prepared from WT and Crbn KO mice were used for steady-state biotinylation of surface BK channels. Input (10%) of total lysates are shown on the left and the biotinylated surface BK channels are shown on the right. Actin was used as a control. Quantification of surface BK channel levels in WT and Crbn KO mice. Data are normalized to WT and presented as mean ± SEM. n = 4, **p < 0.01 (on independent Student's t test). C, Paxilline (10 μm) negates the increased PPR (50 ms inter stimulus interval) at Crbn KO SC-CA1 synapses. n = 15, 3 WT + vehicle; n = 16, 3 KO + vehicle; n = 14, 3 WT + paxilline; n = 16, 3 KO + paxilline. D, Paxilline (10 μm) restores increased activity-dependent plasticity during repetitive stimulation (20 pulses at 20 Hz) at Crbn KO SC-CA1 synapses. n = 20, 5 WT + vehicle; n = 16, 6 KO + vehicle; n = 13, 3 WT + paxilline; n = 11, 3 KO + paxilline. E, Iberiotoxin (100 nm) negates the increased PPR (50 ms interstimulus interval) at Crbn KO SC-CA1 synapses. n = 12, 2 WT + vehicle; n = 14, 2 KO + vehicle; n = 18, 2 WT + iberiotoxin; n = 17, 2 KO + iberiotoxin. F, Iberiotoxin (100 nm) restores increased activity-dependent plasticity during repetitive stimulation (20 pulses at 20 Hz) at Crbn KO SC-CA1 synapses. n = 14, 2 WT + vehicle; n = 16, 2 KO + vehicle; n = 18, 2 WT + iberiotoxin; n = 21, 2 KO + iberiotoxin; *p < 0.05; ***p < 0.001 or ###p < 0.001.
Comparison of the intrinsic properties of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons from WT and Crbn KO mice
Human mutant form of Crbn, R419X, results in decreased probability of neurotransmission
Next, we tested the synaptic effects of the intellectual disability-related human mutant form of CRBN, R419X. For this purpose, we cultured hippocampal neurons from Crbn KO and overexpressed R419X mutant CRBN or WT CRBN and then measured spine morphology, BK channel activity, and probability of neurotransmitter release. First, overexpression of R419X mutant CRBN and WT CRBN did not induce detectable changes in neuronal structure (Fig. 8A). However, the BK channel activity of Crbn KO neurons with WT CRBN expression was significantly lower than that of the untransfected control neurons from Crbn KO mice, whereas Crbn KO neurons with R419X mutant CRBN expression showed a relatively small decrease in BK channel activity (Fig. 8B; WT rescue, t(18) = 4.215, p < 0.001; R419X rescue, t(19) = 0.869, p = 0.396). This suggests that increased BK channel activity in Crbn KO neurons was reduced by WT CRBN expression, but not by R419X mutant CRBN expression. Next, we tested the influence of the R419X mutant on release probability in Crbn KO hippocampal neurons. To measure synaptic transmission at presynaptic terminals directly, we used a high-fidelity optical imaging system combined with vG-pH assay in primary cultured hippocampal neurons. We monitored vG-pH responses of hippocampal neurons to 100 action potential (AP) at 10 Hz in WT and Crbn KO neurons. The release probability in Crbn KO neurons was significantly suppressed compared with WT neurons ([100AP]WT = 20.07 ± 1.02, [100AP]Crbn KO = 6.89 ± 1.32). This defect was completed restored by reexpressing Crbn WT cDNA in Crbn KO neurons ([100AP]resWT in Crbn KO = 21.17 ± 2.44). In addition, neurons with R419X mutant CRBN expression failed to exhibit release probability recovery to the level of WT ([100AP]resR419X in Crbn KO = 8.95 ± 1.72), suggesting that CRBN is indeed involved in presynaptic excitatory neurotransmitter release (Fig. 8C; F(3,26) = 20.19, p < 0.001). Interestingly, the size of readily releasable pool (RRP) among the four different conditions (WT, Crbn KO, CRBN WT rescue, CRBN R419X rescue) was indistinguishable (Fig. 8E; F(3,23) = 0.126, p = 0.944), implying that CRBN does not control RRP size.
Human R419X CRBN expression rescue decreased the probability of release in Crbn KO mice. A, Representative images of WT and Crbn KO neurons. Primary cultured hippocampal neurons were transfected with cytosol GFP (EGFP only) and fixed 14∼20 DIV. B, Representative IK(Ca) traces in untransfected (black) and transfected (red) hippocampal neurons cultured from Crbn KO mice. Overexpression of CRBN WT reduced the increased IK(Ca) in hippocampal cultured neurons in Crbn KO mice but overexpression of CRBN R419X did not. n = 11 untransfected Crbn KO cells; n = 9 WT CRBN transfected Crbn KO cells (WT-Rescue); n = 11 untransfected Crbn KO cells; n = 10 R419X CRBN transfected Crbn KO cells (R419X-Rescue). C, Left, Representative traces of vG-pH response to 100 AP stimuli from WT, Crbn KO, WT-Rescue, and R419X-Rescue neurons (normalized to the NH4 Cl peak signal). Right, Mean value of 100 AP response from WT (n = 8 cells), Crbn KO (n = 8 cells), WT-Rescue (n = 7 cells), and R419X-Rescue (n = 7 cells). ***p < 0.001. D, Representative fields of vG-pH fluorescence images at rest, ΔF100, and NH4Cl application from WT, Crbn KO, WT-Rescue, and R419X-Rescue, respectively. E, Left, Representative traces of vG-pH response to 20 AP stimuli at 100 Hz to discriminate readily releasable pool (RRP). The arrow indicates a short plateau representing RRP position (Ariel and Ryan, 2010). Right, Mean RRP size from WT (5.64 ± 1.28; n = 7 cells), Crbn KO (5.23 ± 0.68; n = 6 cells), WT-Rescue (5.84 ± 0.64; n = 7 cells), and R419X-Rescue (5.94 ± 0.61; n = 7 cells).
Paxilline restored impaired cognitive behavior in Crbn KO mice
Finally, we investigated whether behavioral defects in Crbn KO mice could be restored by treatment with the BK blocker paxilline. We found that paxilline treatment rescued the abnormal context-dependent associative memory of Crbn KO mice, as measured by the passive avoidance test (Fig. 9A). We also observed that Crbn KO mice showed less preference for new objects, as calculated by exploration time (WT_Vehicle, t(7) = 4.778, p = 0.002; KO_Vehicle, t(7) = 0.871, p = 0.413; WT_Paxilline, t(7) = 3.883, p = 0.006; KO_Paxilline, t(7) = 4.244, p = 0.004) and discrimination index (vehicle, t(14) = 2.492, p = 0.026; Paxilline, t(14) = 0.980, p = 0.344), and paxilline treatment successfully reversed this defect (Fig. 9B). Interestingly, Crbn KO mice showed hyperanxious behavior in the EPM test (time spent in open arm: vehicle, t(16) = 4.485, p < 0.001; paxilline, t(14) = 3.924, p = 0.001; time spent in close arm: vehicle, t(16) = 2.529, p = 0.022; paxilline, t(14) = 2.194, p = 0.046), a behavior that paxilline did not affect (Fig. 9C). Our results strongly suggested that Crbn KO mice exhibit abnormally increased BK channel activity-induced decreased excitatory presynaptic release, which is closely related to cognitive dysfunction.
Paxilline treatment rescued abnormal cognitive behaviors in Crbn KO mice. A, Paxilline treatment rescued the abnormal behavior of Crbn KO mice in the passive avoidance test. n = 7 WT with vehicle; n = 6 KO with vehicle; n = 7 WT with paxilline; n = 6 KO with paxilline. B, Paxilline treatment rescued the abnormal behavior of Crbn KO mice in the novel object recognition test. Left, Exploration time. Right, Discrimination index. n = 8 WT with vehicle; n = 8 KO with vehicle; n = 8 WT with paxilline; n = 8 KO with paxilline. C, Paxilline treatment did not affect the hyperanxious behavior of Crbn KO mice in the EPM test. Left, Time spent in the open arm. Right, Time spent in the closed arm. n = 9 WT with vehicle; n = 9 KO with vehicle; n = 8 WT with paxilline; n = 8 KO with paxilline. All data are presented as mean ± SEM. ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, n.s., not significant for comparisons within genotype; n.s., not significant for comparisons within treatment by two-way ANOVA with Holm–Sidak's multiple-comparisons test.
Discussion
In this study, we found that CRBN mutation induces intellectual disability by inducing hyperfunction of BK channels and subsequent decreases in presynaptic neurotransmitter release. CRBN-mediated modulation of BK channel activity has been reported in primary cultured neurons in previous studies on CRBN function (Jo et al., 2005; Liu et al., 2014). However, although the CRBN-BK channel interaction was expected to affect cognitive function, no direct evidence was available at the synaptic or behavioral levels. Our study of synapses in ex vivo brain slices from a Crbn-KO animal model provided evidence for the cellular and molecular mechanism of synaptic dysfunction in Crbn KO animals. Our results demonstrated that BK channel expression in the plasma membrane is elevated in Crbn KO mice, resulting in BK channel hyperfunction and subsequent reduction of presynaptic release. The RRP results indicated that the loss of function of CRBN does not affect the presynaptic machinery involved in the maintenance of synaptic pools. Furthermore, the evidence in support of this BK channel mechanism as a causal contributor to cognitive deficits in the Crbn KO mice was strengthened by the successful rescue of both synaptic and cognitive deficits upon administration of a BK channel blocker.
The role of K+ channels in synaptic and cognitive function has garnered wide interest (Lee and Jan, 2012). In particular, Ca2+-activated K+ channels are typical K+ channels that modulate the regulation and maintenance of neuronal function. In particular, altered BK channel activity has recently been suggested to cause cognitive impairment. For instance, human patients with FMR1 missense mutations and Fmr1 KO animals show decreased BK channel activity and increased presynaptic neurotransmitter release (Deng et al., 2013; Myrick et al., 2015). Genetic upregulation of BK channel activity also recovered neuronal and synaptic defects induced by FMRP loss in mice (Deng and Klyachko, 2016). In contrast, we found that Crbn KO mice exhibited increased BK channel activity and a subsequent decrease in excitatory Pr, whereas a BK channel blocker normalized synaptic and behavioral function in Crbn KO mice. Therefore, our results suggest that balanced BK channel activity is critical for synaptic and cognitive function. Further studies are required to determine whether hyperactivity of the BK channel causes intellectual disability. To date, no cognitive defects have been reported from a human harboring gain-of-functional mutations in the BK channel (Du et al., 2005; Li et al., 2018). However, it should be noted that such mutations accompanied increased Ca2+ sensitivity (D434G mutation) (Du et al., 2005) or increased open channel duration and probability (N995S mutation) (Li et al., 2018), which might be different from the changes caused by elevated expression of the native BK channel. It will be interesting to investigate thoroughly the functional and behavioral consequences caused by specific mutations that change BK channel characteristics. Nonetheless, based on the findings of BK channel hyperfunction in Crbn KO animals, our study provides a possible therapeutic strategy for ameliorating CRBN-related intellectual disability.
One of the concerns regarding experiments conducted with conventional or conditional Crbn KO mice is whether the complete genetic deletion of the gene encoding CRBN in Crbn KO mice is a valid model of CRBN-associated intellectual disability in human patients. To address this question, we applied two approaches. First we used a Drosophila model with neuron-specific Crbn overexpression in Crbn KO flies using both WT and G552X mutant CRBN. Drosophila CRBN G552X mutation mimics the human CRBN R419X mutation, which causes mild intellectual disability. We first confirmed that presynaptic release defects were exhibited in Crbn KO flies (CRBNex1/Df). In addition, the defects were rescued by reintroduction of WT CRBN into Crbn KO flies, but not by reintroduction of the G552X mutant CRBN. We also used cultured hippocampal neurons from Crbn KO mice expressing human WT or R419X mutant CRBN to demonstrate that the reduced Pr in Crbn KO neurons is restored by reintroduction of WT CRBN, but not by R419X mutant CRBN. These results suggest that presynaptic Pr decrease can be induced by either complete loss of CRBN expression or CRBN mutation (i.e., R419X in humans and G552X in Drosophila).
There are some conflicting points between previous studies and our results. Crbn forebrain-specific conditional KO mice showed impaired learning and memory but normal anxiety-like behavior (Rajadhyaksha et al., 2012). In this study, we observed that Crbn KO mice have cognitive defects with hyperanxious behavior (Fig. 9). Interestingly, the BK channel blocker rescued altered behaviors in the passive avoidance test and a novel object recognition test, but did not affect hyperanxious behavior. We suspect that this dissimilarity originates from the different CRBN expression pattern in the brains of different mouse models. For example, CRBN is still expressed in the cerebellum in Crbn forebrain-specific conditioned KO mice (Rajadhyaksha et al., 2012) and the cerebellum is important to not only motor but also mood behavior (Phillips et al., 2015). Our results suggest that the CRBN mutation effect in those anxiety-related brain regions is due to a BK channel-independent mechanism. It is not yet clear whether CRBN-mutated human intellectual disability patients show anxiety disorders, but mood disorders in intellectual disability are an important issue (Hurley, 2006). Further investigation is needed to understand other defective phenotypes in Crbn KO mice and CRBN-mutated human intellectual disability patients beyond cognitive issues. Another conflict is from our results of low BK channels activity in hippocampus from WT mice. We found that BK channel blockers paxilline and iberiotoxin did not affect calcium-activated potassium currents in CA1 pyramidal neurons or PPR and 20 Hz train responses in hippocampal SC-CA1 synapses of WT mice. We assumed that one of main reasons is the expression pattern of BK channel in the hippocampus. Immunostaining studies showed that α subunit of BK channels detected by slo1 antibody were rarely expressed in CA1 regions, although mossy fiber axons from granule cells of dentate gyrus and stratum oriens containing basal dendrites from CA3 pyramidal neurons showed high levels of expression of BK channels in rat or mouse hippocampus (Misonou et al., 2006; Sailer et al., 2006).
Together, our results show that Crbn KO animals with cognitive deficits exhibit normal synaptic morphology and plasticity, but presynaptic dysfunction in glutamate release mediated by increased BK channel activity. A large body of recent work suggests that cognitive impairment is caused by synaptopathy (Bhakar et al., 2012; Wallace et al., 2012) and channelopathy (Guglielmi et al., 2015). Therefore, our study using a Crbn KO animal model that displayed all of these dysfunctions will contribute to future investigations of intellectual disability.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea (Grants 2016-M3C7A1905481 and 2016-R1A2B4006811 to S.Y.C.), the Korea Healthcare Technology Research and Development Project (Grant HI13C1412 to C.S.P.), and GIST (GIST Research Institute Grant 2017 to C.S.P.). We thank Ms. Hui-Yeon Ko and Dr. Hojae Lee for participation in early experiments.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to either of the following: Dr. Chul-Seung Park, School of Life Sciences, Gwangju Institute Science and Technology, Gwangju 61005, Republic of Korea, cspark{at}gist.ac.kr; or Dr. Se-Young Choi, Department of Physiology, Seoul National University School of Dentistry, Seoul 03080, Republic of Korea, sychoi{at}snu.ac.kr