Abstract
Rapid plasticity of layer (L) 2/3 inhibitory circuits is an early step in sensory cortical map plasticity, but its cellular basis is unclear. We show that, in mice of either sex, 1 d whisker deprivation drives the rapid loss of L4-evoked feedforward inhibition and more modest loss of feedforward excitation in L2/3 pyramidal (PYR) cells, increasing the excitation-inhibition conductance ratio. Rapid disinhibition was due to reduced L4-evoked spiking by L2/3 parvalbumin (PV) interneurons, caused by reduced PV intrinsic excitability. This included elevated PV spike threshold, which is associated with an increase in low-threshold, voltage-activated delayed rectifier (presumed Kv1) and A-type potassium currents. Excitatory synaptic input and unitary inhibitory output of PV cells were unaffected. Functionally, the loss of feedforward inhibition and excitation was precisely coordinated in L2/3 PYR cells, so that peak feedforward synaptic depolarization remained stable. Thus, the rapid plasticity of PV intrinsic excitability offsets early weakening of excitatory circuits to homeostatically stabilize synaptic potentials in PYR cells of sensory cortex.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Inhibitory circuits in cerebral cortex are highly plastic, but the cellular mechanisms and functional importance of this plasticity are incompletely understood. We show that brief (1 d) sensory deprivation rapidly weakens parvalbumin (PV) inhibitory circuits by reducing the intrinsic excitability of PV neurons. This involved a rapid increase in voltage-gated potassium conductances that control near-threshold spiking excitability. Functionally, the loss of PV-mediated feedforward inhibition in L2/3 pyramidal cells was precisely balanced with the separate loss of feedforward excitation, resulting in a net homeostatic stabilization of synaptic potentials. Thus, rapid plasticity of PV intrinsic excitability implements network-level homeostasis to stabilize synaptic potentials in sensory cortex.
Introduction
Parvalbumin (PV) inhibitory circuits are highly plastic to altered sensory experience and contribute to receptive field plasticity in sensory cortex (Froemke, 2015). PV circuit plasticity is evident after just 1–2 d of visual or auditory deprivation or 3 d of whisker deprivation, and thus is an early step in cortical plasticity. In layer (L) 2/3, deprivation reduces PV-mediated inhibition, which can increase sensory-evoked responses in pyramidal (PYR) cells and enable subsequent Hebbian plasticity (Gambino and Holtmaat, 2012; Donato et al., 2013; Kuhlman et al., 2013; Li et al., 2014; Froemke, 2015; Resnik and Polley, 2017). Disinhibition may also implement rapid homeostasis to stabilize and maintain mean PYR firing rate for several days before the onset of ocular dominance or whisker map plasticity (Hengen et al., 2013; Li et al., 2014; Barnes et al., 2015; Gainey and Feldman, 2017; Turrigiano, 2017).
Despite its functional importance, the cellular mechanisms for rapid disinhibition are incompletely understood. Best studied are structural changes, including the loss of inhibitory synapses and structural remodeling of GABAergic axons (Marik et al., 2010; Keck et al., 2011; Chen et al., 2012; van Versendaal et al., 2012; Chen and Nedivi, 2013). Deprivation can also functionally weaken excitatory input synapses and inhibitory output synapses of PV cells (Maffei et al., 2004; Jiao et al., 2006; House et al., 2011; Kuhlman et al., 2013; Xue et al., 2014; Sun et al., 2016). In contrast, whether experience alters the intrinsic excitability of PV neurons has been less clear. Molecular pathways exist for activity-dependent regulation of PV intrinsic excitability, but whether experience engages these mechanisms to rapidly alter intrinsic excitability in vivo is unknown (Li et al., 2011; Dehorter et al., 2015). Mechanisms for PV circuit plasticity have been mostly studied after long-term activity manipulation (days or weeks; Jiao et al., 2006; Sun, 2009; House et al., 2011; Kätzel et al., 2011; Xue et al., 2014), and, thus, the mechanisms that underlie rapid disinhibition remain murky.
We examined the mechanisms for rapid disinhibition in L2/3 of somatosensory cortex (S1). In rats, 5–7 d of whisker deprivation reduces feedforward and recurrent inhibition in L2/3 PYR cells, mediated by weakening excitatory synapses onto PV neurons (House et al., 2011; Shao et al., 2013). Disinhibition also occurs in rats after 3 d of deprivation, where it is thought to homeostatically stabilize whisker-evoked firing in L2/3 PYR cells, but the mechanisms for this rapid disinhibition are unknown (Li et al., 2014). Here, we examined rapid disinhibition after 1 d of whisker deprivation in mice, which matches the briefest period of deprivation known to induce disinhibition in visual cortex (Kuhlman et al., 2013). One day of deprivation robustly weakened feedforward L4-L2/3 inhibition in L2/3 PYR cells, due to reduced L2/3 PV neuron spiking. Unlike in prior studies of PV circuit plasticity, this was not due to reduced synaptic drive onto PV cells, but instead was caused by rapid reduction of PV intrinsic excitability. This was mediated by increased voltage-activated potassium currents that elevated the PV spike threshold, reducing near-threshold excitability. Functionally, rapid disinhibition preserved net L4-evoked peak synaptic responses in L2/3 PYR cells, suggesting that the rapid plasticity of PV intrinsic excitability acts to homeostatically stabilize sensory responses in the PYR network during ongoing changes in sensory use.
Materials and Methods
Procedures were approved by the University of California, Berkeley Animal Care and Use Committee and followed National Institutes of Health guidelines. PV-IRES-Cre mice (catalog #008069, The Jackson Laboratory; https://www.jax.org/strain/008069; Hippenmeyer et al., 2005) were crossed with Cre-dependent TdTomato reporter (Ai14) mice (catalog #007914, The Jackson Laboratory; https://www.jax.org/strain/007914; Madisen et al., 2010) to generate PV-Cre;tdTomato offspring. Layer 4 optogenetics experiments were performed in Scnn1a-Tg3-Cre mice (https://www.jax.org/strain/009613). Mice were housed as litters in standard cages. For whisker deprivation, the right D-row whiskers (D1–D6 and gamma) were plucked under transient isoflurane anesthesia, 24 ± 2 h before slice preparation. Sham-plucked littermates underwent anesthesia but not plucking.
Slice preparation
Postnatal day 18 (P18) to P21 mice of either sex were anesthetized with isoflurane and decapitated. Brain slices were prepared using a Leica VT1200S vibratome in chilled, oxygenated, low-sodium, low-calcium Ringer's solution (in mm: 85 NaCl, 75 sucrose, 25 d-(+)-glucose, 4 MgSO4, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 Na2HPO4, 0.5 ascorbic acid, 25 NaHCO3, and 0.5 CaCl2, 320 mOsm). Cortical slices (350 μm) were cut from the left hemisphere in the “across-row” plane, oriented 50° toward coronal from the midsagittal plane and 35° from vertical. Using this plane, each slice contains one column from each whisker row A–E, and within-column circuits are largely preserved (Finnerty et al., 1999; Allen et al., 2003). Slices were transferred to standard Ringer's solution (in mm: 119 NaCl, 26.2 NaHCO3, 11 d-(+)-glucose, 1.3 MgSO4, 2.5 KCl, 1 NaH2PO4, 2.5 CaCl2, 300 mOsm) for 30 min at 30°C and then were kept at room temperature until recording (0.5–7 h).
Slice electrophysiology and L4-evoked synaptic responses.
Recordings were made at 30–31°C in standard Ringer's solution. Barrel columns were identified by transillumination at 4×, and visually guided patching was performed using infrared differential interference contrast optics at 40×. L2/3 PYR cells were identified by soma shape, and were located ∼100–240 μm below the L1–L2 boundary, within ∼100 μm tangentially of column center. PV neurons were identified by tdTomato fluorescence (530–550 nm bandpass excitation, 575–625 nm emission; Dage-MTI camera). PYR cell recordings were made in PV-Cre;tdTomato or C57BL/6 mice (https://www.jax.org/strain/000664).
All recordings were made in L2/3 of D-row barrel columns, with the NMDA receptor antagonist 50 μm d-AP5 in the bath. Whole-cell recording was performed with 3–5 MΩ pipettes using a Multiclamp 700B Amplifier (Molecular Devices) with 2 kHz low-pass filtering and 7–10 kHz digitization. L4-evoked synaptic responses were elicited using 200 μs current pulses delivered via a bipolar stimulating electrode (115 μm tip spacing, FHC) placed in the center of the D barrel in L4. The interstimulus interval was 10 s. For input–output curves, the EPSC threshold (Eθ) was defined as the minimal stimulation current that evoked an EPSC in five consecutive sweeps. For L2/3 PYR cells, the Eθ was determined individually for each cell. For PV cells, Eθ was defined as the average Eθ measured for two cocolumnar L2/3 PYR cells.
Voltage-clamp recordings of synaptic currents used cesium gluconate internal solution (in mm: 108 d-gluconic acid, 108 CsOH, 20 HEPES, 5 tetraethylammonium (TEA)-Cl, 2.8 NaCl, 0.4 EGTA, 4 MgATP, 0.3 NaGTP, 5 BAPTA, and 5 QX-314 bromide, pH 7.2, 295 mOsm). The holding potential (Vhold) was corrected for the liquid junction potential (12 mV). Series resistance (Rseries) was monitored in each sweep and was compensated by 40–80%. Cells whose input resistance (Rinput) changed >30% throughout recording were excluded from analysis. PYR cells with a resting membrane potential (Vrest) that was more depolarized than −60 mV were discarded. L4-evoked EPSCs and IPSCs were separated by recording at Vhold = −68 and 0 mV, respectively, which correspond to the chloride equilibrium potential (ECl) and the glutamate receptor reversal potential (EAMPA). Currents were converted to conductances using standard methods (Wehr and Zador, 2003; House et al., 2011). Integrated synaptic conductance was quantified in a window 3–23 ms after the stimulus.
Current-clamp recordings were made using K gluconate internal solution (mm: 116 K gluconate, 20 HEPES, 6 KCl, 2 NaCl, 0.5 EGTA, 4 MgATP, 0.3 NaGTP, and 5 Na2phosphocreatine, pH 7.2, 295 mOsm). Series resistance artifacts were corrected by bridge balance. In PV cells, postsynaptic potentials (PSPs) were measured from a baseline of −68 mV, the mean Vrest of L2/3 PV neurons. PSP amplitude was quantified in a 1.8 ms window at the peak. In PYR cells, we measured L4-evoked PSPs from Vrest or from an estimated synaptic membrane potential (Vsyn) of −55 mV achieved by somatic current injection. For the latter case, because cells in current clamp are not spatially isopotential, we performed a separate calibration experiment to estimate the difference between somatic membrane potential (Vsoma) and Vsyn during somatic current injection in these conditions. To do this, we recorded using modified K gluconate internal with Nernst potential for chloride of −50 mV, and found that somatic depolarization to −44.3 ± 1.3 mV was necessary to reverse pharmacologically isolated GABAA-mediated IPSPs in current clamp (n = 4 cells). Thus, during somatic current injection, Vsoma was ∼5 mV more depolarized than Vsyn. Thus, in the main experiments, we depolarized Vsoma to −50 mV to achieve an estimated Vsyn of −55 mV.
Juxtacellular (loose-seal) recordings in PV cells were made using K gluconate internal. Spikes were detected in voltage-clamp mode with Vhold continuously adjusted to maintain a 0 pA holding current. Spike probability was calculated over 10 sweeps for each L4 stimulus intensity.
Spontaneous miniature IPSCs (mIPSCs) were recorded in voltage clamp at Vhold = 0 mV, and isolated using 50 μm d-AP5, 10 μm NBQX, and 100 μm saclofen, and 0.1 μm tetrodotoxin (TTX) in the bath. Rseries compensation was not used for mIPSC recordings. Analysis was performed by semiautomatic template matching in Axograph X (Axograph Scientific). The detection threshold was 5 pA. A minimum of 300 events was analyzed per cell. mIPSC analysis was performed blind to experimental condition.
L4 optogenetic stimulation.
Scnn1a-Cre mice were injected at P2–P3 with AAV2.9-CAGGS-Flex-ChR2-tdTom-WPRE-SV40 virus into S1 (catalog #V1345, Penn Vector Core, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA). Scnn1a-Cre mice express Cre in L4 excitatory cells (Adesnik and Scanziani, 2010; Madisen et al., 2010), and we verified L4 expression using AAV2.9-CAGGS-flex-tdTomato viral reporter (catalog #V1682, Penn Vector Core; data not shown). Slices were prepared at P18–P22. The D column was readily identifiable by L4 fluorescence, which revealed the barrel pattern. A 443 nm laser (40 mW; catalog #DL445-040, CrystaLaser) was connected via optic fiber to the microscope epifluorescence arm and projected through a 4× objective to form a 238-μm-diameter spot. This was centered over the D barrel in L4. Laser power at the slice was calibrated using a power meter (catalog #1916-R, Newport). We patched L2/3 PYR cells in the D column, and synaptic responses were measured in voltage clamp (as above, except that Rseries was not compensated). Two millisecond light pulses were applied at a 10 s interstimulus interval to activate L4 neurons and elicit synaptic responses in L2/3 PYR cells in the D column. For optogenetic input–output curves, Eθ was defined as the minimal laser power that evoked an EPSC in at least 5 of 10 consecutive sweeps and was determined individually for each PYR cell. Other experimental parameters were identical to the L4 electrical stimulation experiments.
To ensure sufficient ChR2 expression level in each slice, we required that tdTomato fluorescence in L4 surpass a minimum brightness (200 intensity units), measured in a 350-μm-diameter circular region centered on the L4 D barrel, using identical excitation power and camera settings each day. We also quantitatively measured functional ChR2 expression level in L4 using the “photo-local field potential” (LFP; Shao et al., 2013). For this measure, a 1.8–2.2 MΩ field potential pipette was placed in the center of the D barrel in L4 at the end of each slice recording, and TTX (1 μm) and kynurenic acid (2 mm) were added to the bath. LFP evoked by L4 photostimulation at 1.6× Eθ was measured, which reflects bulk photocurrent in the L4 neuron population. Photo-LFP magnitude correlated with tdTomato fluorescence (data not shown). To ensure an equivalent magnitude of presynaptic stimulation between sham and deprived (Dep) groups, we restricted analysis to slices with L4 photo-LFP magnitude of >0.6 and <0.25 mV. Overall, 34 of 49 slices from 18 of 24 mice passed brightness and photo-LFP criteria, and were used for recordings.
PV–PYR cell pairs.
To study unitary synaptic connections, we made dual whole-cell recordings from cocolumnar PV and PYR neurons (<60 μm apart). The PV cell was recorded in current clamp using K gluconate internal, and the PYR cell was recorded in voltage clamp using Cs gluconate internal. A five-spike train was evoked in the PV cell using current pulses (2 ms, 1–2 nA, 50 ms interval). Unitary IPSCs (uIPSCs) were recorded in the PYR cell at 0 mV. Approximately 30 sweeps were collected. uIPSC amplitude was calculated in a 1.8 ms window centered on the peak. IPSCs were scored as outward currents >1 SD above spontaneous Vm, initiating within 3.6 ms of a presynaptic spike. Two cells were considered to be connected if the probability of observing an IPSC was significantly greater after the first PV spike than during spontaneous activity before the spike (assessed over 30 sweeps, α = 0.05, binomial test). Paired-pulse ratio was defined as second uIPSC/first uIPSC amplitude. The coefficient of variation (CV) was calculated from the variance of the first uIPSC − the variance of a spontaneous epoch. uIPSC decay time constant (τ) was calculated from a single exponential fit.
PV intrinsic excitability.
PV cells were recorded in whole-cell current-clamp mode. Vrest was measured immediately after break-in. Series resistance artifacts were compensated by bridge balance. Input resistance was calculated as the slope of the linear fit of steady-state ΔVm during −50, 0, and +50 pA current steps. The membrane time constant (τ) was calculated by single exponential fit of the first 80 ms of the −50 pA current step. Rheobase was defined as the minimum current injection (500 ms) that elicited one or more spikes on five consecutive sweeps. The firing–current (F–I) relationship was measured using increasing currents above rheobase. F–I curve slope was measured from linear fit. Spike threshold was defined as the Vm at which the second derivative of Vm was >6 SDs above the prestimulus period. Spike latency was defined as the time to spike threshold. Spike shape was analyzed for the second to sixth spikes during each current injection, excluding the first spike, which has a systematically different threshold in L2/3 PV cells (Goldberg et al., 2008). Cells with a spike threshold of less than −20 mV, indicative of an undercompensated bridge, were excluded from analysis.
Potassium currents in PV neurons.
Voltage-sensitive potassium (Kv) currents were recorded in voltage clamp, using K gluconate internal. To minimize calcium-gated K currents, the internal contained 5 mm BAPTA, and the Ringer's solution contained low CaCl2 (0.5 mm). Sweeps were collected at 2.5 s intervals. To measure delayed rectifier K currents, the bath contained the synaptic blockers 50 μm d-AP5, 10 μm NBQX, 100 μm saclofen, and 3 μm SR 95531 hydrobromide (gabazine); the sodium channel blocker 0.1 μm TTX; the M-current (IM) blocker 10 μm XE 991 dihydrochloride; the hyperpolarization-activated cation current (Ih) blocker 0.1 mm ZD2788; and 0.1% bovine serum albumin. After a prepulse to −70 mV, we applied 300 ms voltage steps every 10 mV (−70 to −10 mV) and every 5 mV (−100 to −80 mV). Leak currents were measured from the second series, extrapolated to the first series, and subtracted. Delayed rectifier currents were identified as noninactivating outward currents that activate at approximately −40 to −50 mV (Kv1 family) or −20 to −30 mV (Kv2 family). Current amplitude was measured in the last 100 ms of the 300 ms pulse. Inactivating (A-type) currents are also present in these recordings but inactivate in <50 ms, and were ignored in analysis (Coetzee et al., 1999). To separate Kv1.1 from other delayed rectifier currents, dendrotoxin (DTX)-K (100 nm) was washed in, and DTX-insensitive currents were subtracted from the baseline current.
To measure inactivating A-type current (IA), we used a combination of pharmacology and inactivation properties. The bath contained the synaptic blockers 50 μm d-AP5, 10 μm NBQX, 100 μm saclofen, 3 μm SR 95531 hydrobromide (gabazine); 0.1 μm TTX; and 20 mm TEA to block most delayed rectifier current and IM. A 200 ms prepulse to −70 mV was applied, and then Vm was stepped to 0 mV for 300 ms. In interleaved sweeps, the prepulse was changed to −40 mV, which inactivates IA. We isolated IA during the 0 mV step by subtracting sweeps with the −40 mV prepulse from sweeps with the −70 mV prepulse. Cells were held at −100 mV for 2.5 s between sweeps to allow recovery from inactivation. Current magnitude was quantified as integrated current from 1 to 26 ms after onset of the 0 mV step. A small amount of noninactivating current was also observed after subtraction, and likely reflects Ih, which was not blocked in these experiments. We only analyzed the IA component. Because IA amplitude correlated with Rseries, cells were only included if residual Rseries was 5.5–10.5 MΩ, which allowed precise matching of Rseries between sham and deprived cell populations (7.6 ± 0.3 and 7.5 ± 0.3 MΩ, respectively; n = 16 cells/group).
Drugs.
Drugs were from Tocris Bioscience, except TTX and dendrotoxin-k, which were both from Alomone Labs.
Parallel conductance model of PYR synaptic potentials.
We used a standard parallel conductance model (Wehr and Zador, 2003) to predict synaptic potentials that would be generated in L2/3 PYR cells from recorded L4-evoked synaptic conductances. The model simulated synaptically evoked changes in Vm (ΔVm) resulting from L4-evoked excitatory conductance (Gex) and inhibitory conductance (Gin) waveforms, recorded at 1.4× Eθ in a single PYR neuron. Gex and Gin waveforms were constrained to be non-negative and were smoothed (Savitzky-Golay, 1 ms window). ΔVm was predicted using the parallel conductance equation:
where C was 240 pF, which was the average membrane capacitance measured in PYR neurons in our study. Grest was defined as 1/Rinput, where Rinput was the average input resistance measured for deprived cells (94 MΩ) or sham cells (100 MΩ). We simulated ΔVm for cells at a resting equibrium potential (Erest) = −55 mV, to estimate the effect of feedforward synaptic input on Vm as a cell approaches spike threshold. Vm was calculated by integrating Equation 1 from a starting value of Vm = −55 mV with 0.1 ms time resolution, using Euler's method. This is the same method used in the study by House et al. (2011).
Experimental design and statistical analysis
D-row deprived and sham mice were littermates, and were recorded interleaved either on the same day or on alternate days. All reported deprivation effects were observed across at least three independent litters. For each measurement type, data were tested for Gaussian distribution before use of parametric statistics. Non-Gaussian data were evaluated using log-transformed data, nonparametric statistics, or by permutation test, as indicated in the text. Reported values are mean ± SEM, except where indicated. All statistical tests are identified in the Results. Statistical analysis was performed in Matlab, Excel, or R.
Results
To test whether brief, 1 d whisker deprivation drives inhibitory circuit plasticity in L2/3 of S1, we plucked the right-side D-row whiskers in mice at P17–P20, 24 h before slice preparation at P18–P21. We cut S1 slices in the “across-row” plane that allows D whisker columns to be visually identified by transillumination (Finnerty et al., 1999; Allen et al., 2003; Fig. 1A, top). We used multiple approaches to compare cell and circuit physiology in L2/3 of the D column between 1 d D-row-deprived mice and age-matched, sham-deprived littermates. Most experiments used PV-Cre;tdTomato mice to enable fluorescence-guided recording from L2/3 PV neurons.
One day of deprivation weakens L4-evoked inhibition and increases the E–I conductance ratio in L2/3 PYR cells. A, Top, S1 slice with stimulating electrode in L4 of D barrel and a schematic recording electrode. Bottom, Circuit for L4–L2/3 feedforward excitation and inhibition. B, L4-evoked IPSCs and EPSCs in an example L2/3 PYR cell. Increasing currents are responses to L4 stimulation at 1.0–1.4× Eθ. C, Mean L4-evoked Gin at 1.4× Eθ before and after NBQX application (n = 6 cells). Shading is the SEM. D, Mean L4-evoked Gin and Gex waveforms at 1.2× Eθ across all sham (n = 14) and deprived (n = 17) cells. Shading is the SEM. E, Input–output curves for integrated Gin and Gex with increasing L4 stimulation intensity. Points are the mean ± SEM. Two deprived cells showed unclamped action currents at 1.4× Eθ at Vhold = 0 mV, and were therefore omitted from Gin analysis at 1.4× Eθ. The p values are for sham vs deprived factor in two-way ANOVA. F, Left, Within-cell comparison of Gex vs Gin at 1.2× Eθ. Each point is a cell. Lines are linear regression for sham (black) and deprived (gray). G, E–I conductance ratio, quantified as Gex/(Gex + Gin), across stimulus intensities. Bars shown are the mean ± SEM, as in all subsequent figures. The p value is sham vs deprived factor in two-way ANOVA. H, Vrest, Rinput, and Eθ for each cell (dots) and population mean (open circle).
L4-evoked feedforward Gex and Gin in L2/3 PYR cells
We first studied feedforward L4-L2/3 excitation and inhibition onto PYR cells by electrically stimulating in L4 and measuring evoked EPSCs and IPSCs in L2/3 PYR cells. EPSCs and IPSCs were separated by recording at −68 and 0 mV (ECl and EAMPA; Xue et al., 2014; Fig. 1B). NMDA receptors were blocked with d-AP5 (50 μm) in all experiments. For each L2/3 PYR cell, we determined the minimal L4 stimulation intensity that evoked a reliable EPSC, termed Eθ. We then measured input–output curves for L4-evoked EPSCs and IPSCs at 1.0–1.4 times Eθ. L4 stimulation in this range did not evoke spikes from Vrest in L2/3 PYR cells (n = 90 cells, 37 mice), indicating that L4 stimulation activated feedforward, rather than recurrent, L2/3 circuits.
Stimulation near Eθ typically elicited an EPSC and a small IPSC. Stronger stimulation recruited larger currents, with inhibition dominating over excitation by 1.2–1.4× Eθ (Fig. 1B), similar to rats (House et al., 2011; Shao et al., 2013). NBQX (10 μm) blocked 85 ± 3.6% of the L4-evoked IPSC at 1.4× Eθ (Fig. 1C; n = 6 cells, 5 mice), and the latency of the NBQX-sensitive IPSC was 1.05 ± 0.26 ms longer than the EPSC in the same cell. These properties indicate that ∼85% of the measured IPSC was feedforward, disynaptic inhibition, and that contamination by monosynaptic inhibition was modest. For analysis, EPSCs and IPSCs were converted to conductances (Gex and Gin) using standard methods (Wehr and Zador, 2003; House et al., 2011).
One day of whisker deprivation reduced input–output curves for both L4-evoked Gex and Gin in L2/3 PYR cells (sham: n = 14 cells, 11 mice; Dep: n = 17 cells, 12 mice; Gex: F(1,4) = 8.44, p = 0.0043; Gin: F(1,4) = 11.2, p = 0.001; two-factor ANOVA; Fig. 1D,E). At 1.2× Eθ, mean integrated excitation was reduced by 36%, and inhibition was reduced by 53%. Gex and Gin were correlated within individual cells (linear fit, R2 = 0.79; Xue et al., 2014), and deprivation reduced the slope of this relationship, suggesting preferential loss of Gin (Fig. 1F). We calculated the excitation-inhibition (E–I) conductance ratio as E/(E + I) for each cell, and found a significant increase in E/(E + I) for deprived cells across stimulus intensities (F(1,4) = 5.88, p = 0.017; Fig. 1G). Vrest, Rinput, and Eθ were not altered by deprivation, indicating that recording quality and stimulation efficacy were equal in sham versus deprived groups (Fig. 1H). Thus, 1 d of deprivation reduced both Gin and Gex, as previously reported for ≥5 d of deprivation in rats (Shepherd et al., 2003; House et al., 2011; Shao et al., 2013), and Gin was preferentially weakened to elevate E–I conductance ratio. This inhibitory plasticity is the most rapid yet reported in S1 cortex.
Optogenetic activation of the feedforward L4–L2/3 circuit
To confirm that deprivation affected the L4–L2/3 projection specifically, we assayed L4-L2/3 circuits optogenetically. We expressed ChR2-tdTomato in L4 excitatory cells in S1 using a Cre-dependent viral vector in Scnn1a-Tg3-Cre mice, which express Cre in L4 excitatory cells (Adesnik and Scanziani, 2010; Madisen et al., 2010; Fig. 2A). D-row whiskers were deprived or sham deprived at P17–P21, and slices were made 24 h later. We activated L4 neurons using a calibrated blue laser spot centered in the D barrel in L4 and recorded postsynaptic currents from a L2/3 PYR cell in the D column in voltage clamp (Fig. 2B). We determined Eθ as the minimal laser intensity that evoked a reliable EPSC in the L2/3 PYR cell, and measured input–output curves for EPSCs and IPSCs at 1.0–2.0× Eθ. Near Eθ, photostimulation evoked small EPSCs and IPSCs. Increasing photostimulus intensity recruited larger EPSCs and IPSCs, with IPSCs increasing preferentially to dominate the response (Fig. 2C). This result is similar to L4 electrical stimulation (Fig. 1) and to optogenetic activation of L2/3 recurrent networks in slices and in vivo (Mateo et al., 2011; Shao et al., 2013). Evoked currents were somewhat smaller and slower than with electrical stimulation. Evoked IPSCs were completely blocked by the glutamate receptor antagonist kynurenic acid (2 mm), with no contamination by monosynaptic inhibition (Fig. 2D).
Optogenetically evoked L4–L2/3 excitatory and inhibitory conductances in L2/3 PYR cells are reduced by deprivation. A, Histological section of S1 from Scnn1a-Tg3-cre mouse injected with AAV2.9-CAGGS-flex-tdTom. tdTomato signal (red) is restricted to L4, except for a few L5 neurons. White outlines are L4 barrels. B, Live fluorescence image during a physiology experiment of slice from a mouse injected with flex-ChR2-tdTom virus. Bright signal is ChR2-tdTomato in L4 soma and axons. Schematic shows photostimulation spot in L4 and whole-cell recording in L2/3 of the D column. C, Photo-evoked IPSCs and EPSCs in an example L2/3 PYR cell. Increasing currents are responses to L4 optogenetic stimulation at 1.0–2.0× Eθ. D, Mean IPSC (n = 6 cells) is completely blocked by bath application of kynurenic acid. E, Left, Mean input–output curves for L4-evoked Gex and Gin with increasing photostimulus intensity. Points are the mean ± SEM. The p values are for sham vs deprived factor in two-factor ANOVA on log-transformed data. Right, Mean conductance waveforms at 1.6× Eθ. F, E–I conductance ratio across stimulation intensities, quantified as Gex/(Gex + Gin). G, Sham and deprived groups did not differ in photo-LFP amplitude (measured in L4 at 1.6× Eθ), EPSC at Eθ, or laser power at Eθ. Each dot is a cell, open circles are the population mean ± SEM.
One day of deprivation reduced input–output curves for both photo-evoked Gex and Gin (n = 21 sham cells, n = 26 deprived cells; Gex: F(1,5) = 13.4, p = 0.0003; Gin: F(1,5) = 12.3, p = 0.00054; two-factor ANOVA on log-transformed data; Fig. 2E). Deprivation reduced Gex and Gin waveforms, but kinetics were unaffected. Deprivation also increased E–I ratio, calculated as E/(E + I) within each cell (F(1,5) = 11.1, p = 0.00096; Fig. 2F). This was particularly apparent at low photostimulus intensities, representing initial recruitment of the feedforward circuit. There was no difference between sham and deprived groups in photo-LFP magnitude in L4, which reflects bulk ChR2 photocurrent (see Materials and Methods), size of the EPSC at Eθ or laser power at Eθ (t test, p = 0.16, p = 0.47, 0.96). These results indicate similar presynaptic L4 activation and initial recruitment of L4-L2/3 excitation in the two conditions (Fig. 2G). Thus, deprivation weakens feedforward L4-L2/3 excitation and inhibition, measured selectively by L4 optogenetic stimulation.
Gex and Gin magnitude, kinetics, and input–output curve shape differed moderately between electrical and ChR2 stimulation experiments (Figs. 1D–G, 2E,F). This likely reflects differences in cellular specificity, spike synchrony, and spatial focus of the two methods, and the use of Rseries compensation in the electrical but not the optogenetics experiment. Deprivation produced nearly identical physiological effects despite these methodological differences.
L4-evoked spiking in L2/3 PV cells
To identify the circuit mechanism underlying weakened L4-L2/3 feedforward inhibition, we studied L2/3 PV interneurons, which classically mediate this inhibition (Helmstaedter et al., 2008; House et al., 2011; Xue et al., 2014). We first tested whether deprivation alters L4-evoked spiking of L2/3 PV cells. We recorded in the D column of PV-Cre;tdTomato mice (Fig. 3A,B). In each column, we determined the mean Eθ in two PYR cells and then recorded L4-evoked spikes in L2/3 PV cells in cell-attached mode in response to L4 electrical stimulation at multiples of Eθ. In sham mice, a small number of PV spikes occurred at <1.4× Eθ, and most PV spiking occurred from 1.8 to 2.2× Eθ (n = 28 PV cells, 7 mice; Fig. 3C). L2/3 PYR cells recorded in cell-attached mode did not spike at all in this stimulus range (n = 24 cells, 6 mice). One day of D-row deprivation strongly reduced spike probability in PV cells (n = 19 cells, 6 mice, Fig. 3C) as well as the fraction of PV cells spiking to each stimulus (Fig. 3D). Deprivation reduced the mean spike probability by 63%, from 0.49 ± 0.07 spikes/stimulus in sham cells (combined across 1.8–2.2× Eθ) to 0.18 ± 0.05 in deprived cells. This decrease was significant (p = 0.034, permutation test on summed spikes across the input–output curve). Deprivation did not alter the latency of evoked spikes (at 2.2×: sham, 5.71 ± 0.52 ms; deprived, 5.23 ± 0.42 ms). The rapid loss of feedforward inhibition in L2/3 PYR cells is therefore mediated, at least in part, by reduced L4-evoked spiking in PV neurons, as occurs with brief deprivation in visual cortex (Kuhlman et al., 2013).
Deprivation reduces L4-evoked spiking of L2/3 PV cells. A, Left, Fluorescent PV cells in an S1 slice from PV-Cre/TdTomato mouse. White, L4 barrels. Right, Schematic of cell-attached recording of L4-evoked spikes in L2/3 PV neurons. B, Example spiking data from one PV neuron. Ten sweeps at three stimulation intensities are shown. Gray, Stimulus artifact. C, Mean spike probability for PV and PYR neurons from sham mice (top), and for PV neurons in sham vs deprived mice (bottom). D, Fraction of PV cells spiking in sham vs deprived mice.
Synaptic input to L2/3 PV cells
Prolonged (6–12 d) whisker deprivation in rat S1 and brief (1 d) visual deprivation in mouse V1 both reduce spiking of L2/3 PV cells by weakening L4 excitatory synaptic drive onto PV cells (House et al., 2011; Kuhlman et al., 2013; Sun et al., 2016). To test whether the same mechanism is engaged during 1 d whisker deprivation in mice, we made whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings from L2/3 PV cells in PV-Cre;tdTomato mice. We measured input–output curves for L4 electrical stimulation-evoked Gex and Gin in PV cells at 1.0–2.2× Eθ, where Eθ was determined as the mean Eθ for two cocolumnar PYR cells. Surprisingly, L4-evoked Gex was identical between sham and deprived cells (sham: n = 16 cells, 10 mice; deprived: n = 12 cells, 8 mice; F(1,6) = 1.5, p = 0.22, two-factor ANOVA), as was Gin (also F(1,6) = 1.5, p = 0.22; Fig. 4A,B). Vrest, Rinput, and Eθ were unchanged between sham and deprived PV cells (Fig. 4C), as were Rseries and whole-cell capacitance (data not shown). We also performed separate recordings from PV cells in current clamp, and measured input–output curves for L4-evoked PSPs. Baseline Vm was held at −68 mV in this experiment, which is equal to mean Vrest for L2/3 PV cells (Fig. 4C). The L4-evoked PSP peak was identical between sham and deprived mice (sham: n = 6 cells, 3 mice; deprived: n = 6 cells, 4 mice; F(1,6) = 1.53, p = 0.22, ANOVA; Fig. 4D). Thus, multiple measures of synaptic input to PV cells revealed apparently normal synaptic input after 1 d of deprivation, suggesting that PV spiking is reduced by a different mechanism than that for prolonged whisker deprivation.
L4-evoked synaptic input to L2/3 PV cells remains normal during deprivation. A, Mean L4-evoked excitatory synaptic conductance in sham and deprived mice. Left, Mean Gex waveform at 1.6× stimulation intensity. Right, Mean integrated Gex across stimulus intensities. Shaded regions and bars are the SEM. B, Mean L4-evoked inhibitory synaptic conductance. Conventions are as in A. Curves are displaced slightly along the x-axis for readability. C, Vrest, Rinput, and Eθ for L2/3 PV cells in the experiments in A and B. D, Left, L4-evoked PSPs in two example PV cells at 1.0, 1.2,…, 2.2× Eθ. Right, Mean PSP peak above −68 mV baseline Vm, for all PV cells in sham vs deprived mice. One cell in each group spiked beginning at 1.8× Eθ.
L2/3 PV inhibition onto L2/3 PYR cells
Weakening of feedforward inhibition in L2/3 PYR cells could also reflect weakening or loss of inhibitory synapses on L2/3 PYR cells, as occurs after long-duration activity manipulations (Xue et al., 2014). To test this, we first examined whether 1 d of deprivation weakens uIPSCs from L2/3 PV to PYR cells. We made dual whole-cell recordings from nearby L2/3 PV and PYR cells in D whisker columns (Fig. 5A). We recorded uIPSCs in the PYR cell (in voltage clamp with Cs internal at 0 mV Vhold) in response to a five-spike train in the PV cell (Fig. 5B). uIPSC amplitude was not decreased in deprived slices. Indeed, deprived pairs showed a nonsignificant trend for a larger first uIPSC (uIPSC1; sham: n = 11 pairs, 9 mice, 183 ± 87 pA; deprived: n = 10 pairs, 8 mice; amplitude, 280 ± 84 pA; t(19) = −0.80, p = 0.43, t test; Fig. 5B). This was not due to variations in intersoma distance (sham, 24.2 ± 5.1 μm; deprived, 35.4 ± 4.6 μm, p = n.s.; t(18) = −1.58, p = 0.13, t test). Deprivation did not alter the paired-pulse ratio (uIPSC2/uIPSC1), the failure rate for uIPSC1, the CV for uIPSC1, or connection probability (Fig. 5C). Deprivation did not alter short-term depression during the five-spike train (Fig. 5D). Deprivation did speed the decay kinetics of uIPSC1 (decay τ: sham, 12.8 ± 1.9 ms; deprived, 7.8 ± 0.5 ms; t(18) = 2.54, p = 0.02, t test), which could suggest changes in GABA receptor subunit composition or other modification of gating properties with deprivation (Fritschy and Panzanelli, 2014; Fig. 5E).
Unitary PV → PYR IPSCs and mIPSCs on L2/3 PYR cells. A, Schematic for PV → PYR paired recordings. Bottom, Example presynaptic PV spike train. Calibration: 20 mV, 50 ms. B, Left, Presynaptic PV spike train and example uIPSCs evoked in one deprived and one sham PYR cell. Iinj, Current injection to evoke presynaptic spikes. Right, Mean uIPSC1 waveform and amplitude for all sham and deprived pairs. Each dot is one pair. C, Paired-pulse ratio (uIPSC2/uIPSC1), uIPSC1 failure rate, uIPSC1 CV, and the probability of connected pairs. Each dot is one pair. D, Left, Population mean uIPSC train for sham and deprived pairs (top), normalized to the first uIPSC peak (bottom). Deprived pairs showed no evidence of uIPSC weakening, and a nonsignificant trend toward uIPSC strengthening, with no change in short-term plasticity during the train (right). E, Mean uIPSC1 across all cells and analysis of decay τ. Each dot is one pair. F, Example mIPCSs recorded at 0 mV in one sham and one deprived L2/3 PYR cell. G, Mean mIPSC waveform (n = 16 sham, n = 14 deprived cells). H, mIPSC amplitude and IEI for sham and deprived cells. Each circle is one cell.
To screen more broadly for the weakening of inhibitory synapses on PYR cells, we analyzed spontaneous mIPSCs in L2/3 PYR cells in the D column (Fig. 5F). Deprivation did not weaken mIPSCs. Instead, we observed a nonsignificant trend toward larger mIPSCs (sham: 21.5 ± 0.75 pA amplitude, n = 16 cells, 3 mice; deprived: 23.7 ± 0.1 pA, n = 14 cells, 3 mice; t(28) = −1.69, p = 0.10, t test), and no change in interevent interval (IEI: sham, 77.0 ± 5.0 ms; deprived, 73.6 ± 7.0 ms; t(28) = 0.40, p = 0.69). Thus, we found no evidence for weakened inhibitory synapses onto L2/3 PYR cells after 1 d of deprivation, either from mIPSCs or PV > PYR uIPSCs. If anything, small trends were apparent toward increased uIPSCs and increased mIPSC amplitude, reminiscent of the strengthening of PV > PYR uIPSCs after ≥5 d of deprivation in rats (House et al., 2011).
Reduced intrinsic excitability of L2/3 PVs cells
Could the reduction in L4-evoked PV spiking reflect the reduced intrinsic excitability of PV cells? PV intrinsic excitability is plastic in response to extensive pharmacological or genetic blockade of network activity (Miller et al., 2011; Dehorter et al., 2015) and to 30 d whisker deprivation (Sun, 2009). To test whether 1 d of deprivation alters intrinsic excitability, we made current-clamp recordings from L2/3 PV cells in the presence of synaptic blockers (in μm: 50 d-AP5, 10 NBQX, and 3 gabazine) and injected 500 ms current steps to evoke spikes and measure F–I curves (Fig. 6A). Deprivation reduced PV spiking, causing a 22% reduction in the slope of F–I curves (n = 24 sham cells, n = 23 deprived cells; F(1,6) = 12.6, p = 0.0004, two-factor ANOVA). This corresponded to a reduction from 31.2 ± 2.1 to 25.0 ± 2.1 spikes at 80 pA above rheobase (Fig. 6B). This was associated with a depolarized spike threshold (sham: −35.3 ± 0.05 mV, n = 7626 spikes; Dep: −32.3 ± 0.05 mV, n = 6123 spikes, p = 1e-5, t test, calculated for all spikes at 0–40 pA above rheobase; Fig. 6C). In contrast, Vrest, membrane time constant, input resistance, and rheobase were unchanged (Fig. 6D). Analysis of spike shape showed that deprivation increased the mean spike threshold by 2.6 mV and decreased spike height (peak threshold, 54.2 ± 1.3 vs 50.0 ± 1.6 mV; t(44) = 2.08, p = 0.04, t test), but did not alter peak Vm, spike width, or afterhyperpolarization (p = 0.34, p = 0.13, p = 0.49; Fig. 6E,F).
Deprivation causes a reduction in the intrinsic excitability of L2/3 PVs. A, Spike trains for example sham (top) and deprived (bottom) L2/3 PV cells to current injection at 40 pA above rheobase. B, Mean F–I curve for spiking in sham and deprived PV cells. Symbols show the mean ± SEM. C, Mean spike threshold, for all spikes recorded at 0–40 pA above rheobase, in 20 ms time bins. Many PV cells exhibit a rapid-onset first spike with systematically lower threshold (first point, see example in G). D, Resting properties and rheobase for each PV cell. Bars are means. E, Mean shape of second spike at 80 pA above rheobase, across all cells. Shaded regions are ±SEM. F, Quantification of spike shape for spikes at 80 pA above rheobase. The p values are from two-tailed t tests. G, Spike trains for an example PV cell showing delayed spike onset at rheobase. H, Spiking of an example sham (bottom) and deprived (top) PV cell at 5 pA above rheobase, showing longer spike latency in the deprived cell. I, Mean first-spike latency in sham and deprived cells. J, Mean spike probability in 10 ms bins after current injection onset (20–80 pA above rheobase). The p values in I and J are for sham vs deprived factor in two-way ANOVA.
Many L2/3 PV cells exhibit a delayed spiking phenotype characterized by a long first-spike latency at rheobase (Gibson et al., 1999; Goldberg et al., 2008), which we also observed (Fig. 6G). Prolonged upmodulation or downmodulation of network activity can alter first-spike latency due to the plasticity of voltage-activated potassium currents (Goldberg et al., 2008; Dehorter et al., 2015). We found that 1 d of deprivation increased first-spike latency by ∼20 ms (F(1,6) = 9.6, p = 0.0021, two-factor ANOVA; Fig. 6H,I), which lowered spike probability in early time windows after current injection onset (F(1,9) = 11.2, p = 0.0009; Fig. 6J). Thus, deprivation substantially decreased intrinsic the excitability of L2/3 PV cells by increasing spike threshold, reducing spike probability, and delaying the time to first spike after current injection.
Deprivation upregulates voltage-activated Kv currents in L2/3 PV cells
To investigate the mechanisms for reduced intrinsic excitability of L2/3 PV cells, we focused on two voltage-activated potassium currents that are known to regulate spike threshold in PV cells–the low threshold, which sustained delayed-rectifier potassium current (mediated by Kv1 channels), and the transient IA current (mediated by Kv1.4 and Kv4 channels; Gutman et al., 2005; Goldberg et al., 2008; Sun, 2009; Dehorter et al., 2015). Kv1 channels modulate spike threshold and first-spike latency in PV cells (Goldberg et al., 2008; Dehorter et al., 2015), and their expression is bidirectionally regulated by large-scale manipulations of neural activity (Dehorter et al., 2015). IA also regulates PV spike threshold and may be altered by sustained whisker deprivation (Sun, 2009). Whether either is altered rapidly by brief sensory manipulations is unknown.
To measure delayed rectifier current, we used a whole-cell voltage-clamp protocol that stepped Vhold from −70 to −10 mV in 10 mV steps (Fig. 7A,B). The bath contained synaptic blockers (d-AP5, NBQX, saclofen, and gabazine), the sodium channel blocker TTX (0.1 μm), the IM blocker XE 991 (10 μm), the Ih blocker ZD2788 (0.1 mm), and low Ca2+ (0.5 mm) to reduce Ca-activated K currents. The K gluconate internal contained BAPTA to further reduce Ca-activated K currents. These conditions isolate delayed rectifier currents mediated by Kv1 and Kv2 families (which can be separated by activation threshold) and A-type currents that can be identified by their rapid inactivation. To further separate Kv1.1 from other currents, midway through each recording we washed in the blocker DTX-K at 0.1 μm, a Kv1.1-selective concentration (Robertson et al., 1996; Wang et al., 1999; Shen et al., 2004), and measured DTX-K-insensitive (non-Kv1.1) and DTX-K-sensitive currents. We obtained complete data from 13 sham PV cells and 13 deprived PV cells. Deprivation did not alter leak current (Fig. 7C), but increased a steady-state K current that activated at −40 to −50 mV (Fig. 7D,E, top row, F(1,5) = 5.16, p = 0.025, two-factor ANOVA). This activation range is characteristic of Kv1 family channels (Coetzee et al., 1999). Steady-state currents in the presence of DTX-K were also significantly larger in deprived cells (Fig. 7D,E, middle row; F(1,5) = 17.4, p = 0.0001), suggesting that non-Kv1.1 channel currents were upregulated. DTX-K-sensitive currents, calculated by subtraction, were unchanged (Fig. 7D,E, bottom row). These data indicate that a Kv1 channel, but not a Kv1.1 channel, is upregulated by 1 d of deprivation to reduce the near-threshold excitability of L2/3 PV cells.
Deprivation upregulates voltage-activated K currents in L2/3 PV cells. A, Voltage-clamp protocol to measure delayed rectifier K currents, and example currents from one PV neuron in a sham mouse. SS, Steady-state current analysis window. B, I–V curve for SS and leak current for cells in A. C, Leak conductance in sham and deprived cells, during baseline and DTX conditions. D, Mean current waveforms across sham and deprived PV cells at −60 to −10 mV Vhold, measured at baseline and under DTX conditions (top and middle), and the calculated DTX-sensitive current (bottom). E, I–V plots for leak-subtracted steady-state current across cells, for each condition in D. Pronounced currents activate at −40 to −50 mV. The p values are for sham vs deprived factor in two-way ANOVA. F, Voltage protocol for isolating IA. IA was measured at 0 mV by subtracting currents with a −40 mV 200 ms prestep (which inactivates IA) from currents with a −70 mV preset (which does not inactivate IA). Traces show an example cell. G, Mean IA waveform from sham and deprived cells. H, IA magnitude (integrated over first 25 ms) for sham and deprived cells. Each circle is one cell. Bars show the mean ± SEM.
We examined IA in separate experiments using a voltage protocol that isolates IA based on its rapid inactivation at −40 mV (Guan et al., 2011; Fig. 7F). We stepped Vhold to 0 mV, which strongly activates IA, either from a −70 mV prepulse, where IA is not inactivated, or from a 200 ms −40 mV prepulse, which inactivates IA. The bath contained synaptic blockers, TTX, 0.5 mm Ca2+ to reduced Ca-activated K currents, and 20 mm TEA to block most delayed rectifier K currents. Under these conditions, subtraction of −40 mV prepulse traces from −70 mV prepulse traces reveals IA, which shows characteristic inactivation over ∼30 ms (Fig. 7F). Data were obtained from 16 sham and 16 deprived cells (6 mice each). IA current magnitude was greater in Deprived PV cells than in sham PV cells (sham, 10.5 ± 1.0 nA * ms; deprived, 14.2 ± 1.1 nA * ms; t(30) = −2.4, p = 0.021, t test; Fig. 7G,H). Thus, both IA and delayed rectifier currents were increased by deprivation in L2/3 PV cells, which are expected to elevate spike threshold and reduce spike probability.
Functional effect of reduced inhibition on L2/3 PYR synaptic potentials
What is the functional effect of reduced PV-mediated inhibition on overall feedforward PSPs in L2/3 PYR cells? This will depend on its coordination with the co-occurring reduction in feedforward excitation in these neurons. We first addressed this question using a standard parallel conductance model (Wehr and Zador, 2003; House et al., 2011) to predict the net PSP produced in each L2/3 PYR cell by the measured L4-evoked Gex and Gin waveforms at 1.4 Eθ (data from Fig. 1). The model calculates the PSP produced by Gex and Gin waveforms at a specific baseline Vm, given excitatory and inhibitory reversal potentials (0 and −68 mV). Input resistance and cell capacitance were set to the average values obtained from current-clamp recordings (sham: 95 MΩ, 240 pF, n = 12 cells; Dep: 101 MΩ, 240 pF, n = 13 cells). The model is passive and has no free parameters. We used a baseline Vm of −55 mV to predict PSPs generated just below spike threshold, which may be most relevant to understanding evoked spiking in vivo.
Figure 8A shows an example cell, with the predicted EPSP and IPSP generated by the measured Gex and Gin waveforms separately, and the total PSP predicted from Gex and Gin acting together. Deprived cells showed a significantly smaller predicted IPSP (from Gin alone) than sham cells (−5.4 ± 0.71 and −7.6 ± 0.95 mV; n = 14 and 15; p = 0.046, Wilcoxon rank-sum test), and a trend toward a smaller predicted EPSP (from Gex alone; 8.8 ± 1.6 and 13.2 ± 3.0 mV, p = 0.16). This represents a similar average reduction in predicted IPSP and EPSP amplitudes (29% and 33%, respectively; Fig. 8B). Predicted EPSP/IPSP ratio, quantified as E/(E + I) within each cell, was also unchanged (sham: 0.60 ± 0.03, n = 14; Dep: 0.61 ± 0.03, n = 15; p = 0.67). This contrasts with the preferential weakening of Gin over Gex at the conductance level (Fig. 1). Modeling Gex and Gin together predicted a total PSP comprising a characteristic EPSP–IPSP sequence (Pouille and Scanziani, 2001; Gabernet et al., 2005; House et al., 2011). Strikingly, deprivation did not alter the predicted PSP peak, although the late hyperpolarization was reduced (Fig. 8C,D). This indicates that deprivation-induced changes in Gin and Gex are quantitatively coordinated to maintain the stability of peak feedforward PSPs in L2/3 PYR cells in the just-subthreshold regime.
Changes in Gex and Gin are coordinated to maintain stable peak PSP amplitude in L2/3 PYR cells. A, Example neuron showing measured L4-evoked Gex and Gin conductance waveforms (left) and predicted PSPs from a baseline Vm of −55 mV (right). PSPs were predicted from Gex alone (EPSP), Gin alone (IPSP), or from both Gex and Gin together (total PSP). B, Peak predicted EPSP (from Gex alone) and IPSP (from Gin alone) at 1.4× Eθ in each sham and deprived cell from Figure 1. Error bars are the mean ± SEM for sham and deprived populations. C, Predicted total PSP for each sham and deprived neuron, and population means (thick). Arrow, L4 stimulus. Dots, PSP peaks. Vertical tick, Analysis time for late inhibition. Right, Population mean PSPs calculated from peak-aligned PSPs in individual cells. D, Quantification of changes in predicted peak PSP and late inhibition following deprivation. E, Real L4-evoked PSPs measured in sham and deprived L2/3 PYR neurons from an estimated baseline synaptic Vm of −55 mV. Thick lines, Population means. One deprived cell was omitted because it spiked. Right, Population mean PSPs calculated from peak-aligned PSPs in individual cells. F, Quantification of deprivation effects on peak PSP and late amplitude during the IPSP.
To test the model predictions, we measured PSPs in L2/3 PYR cells in response to L4 stimulation at 1.4× Eθ, using K gluconate internal (n = 12 sham, n = 13 deprived). Current was injected somatically to achieve an estimated baseline synaptic Vm of −55 mV (see Materials and Methods). While PSP amplitude was heterogeneous across cells, the mean PSP consisted of an EPSP–IPSP sequence that was very similar to the modeled PSPs (Fig. 8E). Confirming the model, deprivation did not alter PSP peak (p = 0.68, Wilcoxon rank-sum test), but tended to reduce the late hyperpolarization phase of the PSP (nonsignificant trend, p = 0.18; Fig. 8F). PSP duration was not significantly increased (width at half-height: sham: 6.4 ± 2.4 ms, n = 12; Dep: 9.6 ± 2.3 ms, n = 13; p = 0.19). Thus, deprivation-induced changes in Gin and Gex were functionally balanced for L2/3 PYR cells just below spike threshold, so that L4-evoked synaptic responses remained stable after deprivation, at least for low-frequency inputs.
Discussion
Whisker deprivation weakens PV-mediated feedforward inhibition in L2/3 of S1 in just 24 h, as rapidly as visual deprivation in V1 or hearing loss in A1 (Kuhlman et al., 2013; Sun et al., 2016; Resnik and Polley, 2017). Thus, rapid inhibitory circuit plasticity is common across sensory cortex. Optogenetic L4 stimulation showed that plasticity involves the L4-L2/3 feedforward microcircuit (Fig. 2). However, because L2/3 PV neurons are shared among feedforward, recurrent, and long-range projections, inhibition within all these circuits is likely to be reduced in L2/3 by whisker deprivation. Prior studies demonstrated plasticity on this 1 d timescale for inhibitory neuron axon/dendrite structure, inhibitory synapse number, and functional strength of PV input and output synapses (Knott et al., 2002; Maffei et al., 2006, 2010; Marik et al., 2010; Keck et al., 2011; Chen et al., 2012; van Versendaal et al., 2012; Chen and Nedivi, 2013; Kuhlman et al., 2013). Here we discovered equally rapid plasticity of PV intrinsic excitability. Thus, virtually every level of PV circuits exhibits rapid plasticity in response to sensory experience.
The cellular implementation of PV circuit plasticity appears to differ between cortical areas and with the duration of deprivation. In rat S1, sustained (≥5 d) whisker deprivation weakens L4-L2/3 feedforward inhibition by weakening L4 excitatory input to fast-spiked (FS; presumed PV) neurons, with no change in FS intrinsic excitability (House et al., 2011). In mouse V1, 1 d of monocular deprivation similarly weakens L4-evoked excitatory synaptic input to PV neurons (Kuhlman et al., 2013). However, 1 d of whisker deprivation in mice changes neither net excitatory nor inhibitory synaptic input to PV neurons in S1, but instead reduces PV intrinsic excitability by increasing spike threshold (Figs. 4, 5, 6). Thus, PV circuits appear to use different plasticity mechanisms to achieve a similar overall reduction in circuit output. Our findings suggest that within S1, the most rapid mechanism for disinhibition in L2/3 is reduction in PV intrinsic excitability, followed more slowly by the weakening of L4 excitatory synapses onto PV neurons.
PV intrinsic excitability is plastic in response to strong or prolonged genetic or pharmacological suppression or enhancement of cortical activity (Miller et al., 2011; Dehorter et al., 2015) and epilepsy (Li et al., 2011). Our results show that this plasticity also occurs to natural variations in sensory use, and thus is a normally occurring, rapid plasticity mechanism in cortex.
Mechanism for altered PV intrinsic excitability
Deprivation altered the intrinsic excitability of PV cells near spike threshold, but not at rest. Near-threshold excitability is strongly regulated in L2/3 PV neurons by low-threshold Kv1 delayed rectifier channels, which help set spike threshold and spike latency (Goldberg et al., 2008). Recent studies identified molecular pathways for slower activity-dependent regulation of Kv1 currents via the transcription factor Er81 (Dehorter et al., 2015) or by neuregulin (NRG1) signaling through its receptor ErbB4 (Li et al., 2011). Modulation of PV cell activity by direct genetic or pharmacological interventions (Dehorter et al., 2015), epilepsy (Li et al., 2011), or synaptic stimulation (Campanac et al., 2013) regulates Kv1 channels to alter near-threshold PV intrinsic excitability. Our results are consistent with this same mechanism occurring rapidly during sensory deprivation, because deprivation increased a voltage-activated sustained current that activated near spike threshold (−40 mV), which is characteristic of Kv1 family channels (Fig. 7). The molecular identity of the channels that mediate this current remains unknown, but it likely involves a Kv1 family member that is not Kv1.1. Thus, we propose that activity-dependent modulation of Kv1 function, which was previously observed during artificial activity manipulation or epilepsy, is recruited with brief alterations of sensory experience to regulate PV circuit function.
Deprivation also increased IA potassium values. Upregulation of IA was suggested previously to occur in L4 fast-spiking neurons after prolonged (30 d) whisker deprivation, and to cause elevated spike threshold and spike latency (Sun, 2009). We find that this occurs within 1 d of deprivation. Thus, multiple Kv channels are rapidly regulated in PV cells by whisker experience to change near-threshold but not resting excitability.
What signaling pathways mediate experience-dependent regulation of potassium channel function in PV neurons? This is unknown, but one candidate is NRG1/ErbB4 signaling, which controls many aspects of PV circuit development and plasticity (Mei and Nave, 2014). NRG1/ErbB4 is required for visual deprivation-induced weakening of PV circuits (Sun et al., 2016) and for epilepsy-dependent regulation of PV intrinsic excitability (Li et al., 2011).
PV circuit weakening and sensory response homeostasis in L2/3 of S1
Rapid weakening of PV circuits contributes to homeostatic stabilization of sensory-evoked spiking in L2/3 PYR cells, which was observed following brief reductions in whisker input. In classical whisker map plasticity, ≥5 d of D-row whisker deprivation weakens spiking to deprived whiskers in L2/3 PYR cells, mediated in part by long-term depression (LTD) at excitatory L4-L2/3 PYR synapses (Fox, 2002; Feldman, 2009). With briefer 1–3 d whisker deprivation, whisker-evoked spiking remains normal or is slightly increased, even though whisker-evoked synaptic excitation is already weakened in L2/3 PYR cells. This is due to a substantial drop in whisker-evoked inhibition in L2/3 PYR cells, measured with whole-cell recording in vivo (Li et al., 2014). Our results identify the circuit loci for this plasticity within L4-L2/3 feedforward excitatory and inhibitory circuits. Deprivation weakens L4-L2/3 feedforward excitation onto PYR cells in <1 d (Figs. 1, 2), as expected for a rapid LTD process, and similar to visual deprivation-induced LTD in L2/3 of V1 (Heynen et al., 2003). At the same time, L4-L2/3 feedforward inhibition is weakened more strongly, by rapid reduction in PV intrinsic excitability, and this precisely counteracts the loss of feedforward excitation onto L2/3 PYR cells, thus preserving L4-evoked PSPs (Fig. 8). The LTD and PV intrinsic excitability mechanisms operate within different neurons, and therefore must be coordinated to accurately stabilize L2/3 PYR synaptic and spiking responses, but how this is achieved is unknown.
We found that 1 d deprivation reduces feedforward Gin preferentially over Gex in L2/3 PYR cells, elevating E–I conductance ratio. Remarkably, this change in E–I conductance ratio is appropriate to maintain stable feedforward PSP magnitude, for cells just below spike threshold (Fig. 8). This is because in this Vm regime, the driving force on inhibition is lower than the excitation, so a larger change is required in Gin than Gex to produce an equivalent change (ΔVm) in IPSP versus EPSP amplitude, which approximately sum to yield a stable PSP peak. Thus, rapid plasticity of L2/3 PV intrinsic excitability appears calibrated to homeostatically maintain stable feedforward synaptic responses in the L2/3 PYR network. This explains why whisker-evoked spiking responses in L2/3 remain largely stable following brief whisker deprivation in vivo (Li et al., 2014). Interestingly, while brief deprivation preserves short-latency whisker-evoked spiking in L2/3 in vivo, it modestly increases long-latency spikes and spike jitter (Li et al., 2014), which may reflect the modest broadening of the PSP peak or the loss of the late IPSP (Fig. 8). With longer (≥5 d) deprivation, feedforward Gex and Gin are reduced equally in L2/3 PYR cells, which predicts a smaller feedforward net PSP (House et al., 2011). This may explain the loss of whisker-evoked spikes in L2/3 during classical map plasticity (Li et al., 2014).
Longer deprivation drives other mechanisms for activity-dependent weakening of PV circuits, including the weakening of excitatory synaptic input to PV cells (House et al., 2011; Kuhlman et al., 2013; Sun et al., 2016) and the weakening of unitary PV → PYR output synapses (Xue et al., 2014). How different plasticity mechanisms are coordinated within PV circuits remains unclear.
Rapid disinhibition has several advantages as a mechanism for network homeostasis. It is faster than classical homeostatic synaptic scaling or homeostatic plasticity of PYR intrinsic excitability, which take 2–3 d to occur in vivo (Breton and Stuart, 2009; Lambo and Turrigiano, 2013; Gainey and Feldman, 2017). It is highly efficient, because plasticity in a single PV cell will regulate the firing rate in hundreds of local PYR cells (Packer and Yuste, 2011). It also does not require broad adjustment of synaptic strength at hundreds or thousands of excitatory input synapses onto PV cells (Sun et al., 2016). We speculate that global adjustment of PV circuit gain by the regulation of PV intrinsic excitability may provide coarse regulation of excitation–inhibition balance, which is followed by slower, more precise adjustment of unitary PV → PYR synapses for target cell-specific regulation (Xue et al., 2014). Vasoactive intestinal peptide (VIP) interneuron circuits provide even faster, dynamic disinhibition (timescale of seconds), but whether these circuits are plastic in response to experience is unknown (Fu et al., 2015).
While reduced PV intrinsic excitability preserves the net feedforward PSP peak, the late IPSP component shows a strong tendency to be reduced (Fig. 8). This suggests that while PV circuit plasticity promotes stable synaptic responses to sparse, low-frequency input (which is the dominant activity regime in L2/3 of S1; Barth and Poulet, 2012), it may also result in increased depolarization in L2/3 PYR cells during high-frequency input trains due to enhanced temporal summation. This may facilitate long-term potentiation and Hebbian map reorganization (Kuhlman et al., 2013; Gambino et al., 2014), similar to VIP-mediated disinhibition on short timescales (Williams and Holtmaat, 2018).
Note Added in Proof: The labels and colors for Sham and Deprived groups were inadvertently switched in Panel J of Figure 6 in the Early Release version published April 20, 2018. The figure has now been corrected.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants 1R01-NS-073912 and F32-NS-087893 (to M.A.G.), and a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship (to J.W.A.). We thank Brooke Angel and Dhruv Malik for histology and solution making.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Daniel E. Feldman, 142 Life Sciences Addition, Mail Code 3200, Departement of Molecular & Cell Biology, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 92093-3200. dfeldman{at}berkeley.edu