Skip to main content

Umbrella menu

  • SfN.org
  • eNeuro
  • The Journal of Neuroscience
  • Neuronline
  • BrainFacts.org

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Preparing a Manuscript
    • Submission Guidelines
    • Fees
    • Journal Club
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SfN.org
  • eNeuro
  • The Journal of Neuroscience
  • Neuronline
  • BrainFacts.org

User menu

  • Log in
  • Subscribe
  • My alerts

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • Subscribe
  • My alerts
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Preparing a Manuscript
    • Submission Guidelines
    • Fees
    • Journal Club
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Subscriptions
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Development/Plasticity/Repair

Metaplasticity in the Visual Cortex: Crosstalk Between Visual Experience and Reactive Oxygen Species

José Francis-Oliveira, Guilherme S. Vilar Higa, Lívia Mendonça Munhoz Dati, Ianê Carvalho Shieh and Roberto De Pasquale
Journal of Neuroscience 20 June 2018, 38 (25) 5649-5665; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2617-17.2018
José Francis-Oliveira
Universidade de São Paulo, Departamento de Fisiologia e Biofísica, ACidade Universitária, Butantã, São Paulo, SP 05508-000, Brazil
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for José Francis-Oliveira
Guilherme S. Vilar Higa
Universidade de São Paulo, Departamento de Fisiologia e Biofísica, ACidade Universitária, Butantã, São Paulo, SP 05508-000, Brazil
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Lívia Mendonça Munhoz Dati
Universidade de São Paulo, Departamento de Fisiologia e Biofísica, ACidade Universitária, Butantã, São Paulo, SP 05508-000, Brazil
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ianê Carvalho Shieh
Universidade de São Paulo, Departamento de Fisiologia e Biofísica, ACidade Universitária, Butantã, São Paulo, SP 05508-000, Brazil
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Roberto De Pasquale
Universidade de São Paulo, Departamento de Fisiologia e Biofísica, ACidade Universitária, Butantã, São Paulo, SP 05508-000, Brazil
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Metaplasticity is the regulation of synaptic plasticity based on the history of previous synaptic activation. This concept was formulated after observing that synaptic changes in the visual cortex are not fixed, but dynamic and dependent on the history of visual information flux. In visual cortical neurons, sustained synaptic stimulation activate the enzymatic complex NOX2, resulting in the generation of reactive oxygen species (ROS). NOX2 is the main molecular structure responsible for translating neural activity into redox modulation of intracellular signaling pathways involved in plastic changes. Here, we studied the interaction between NOX2 and visual experience as metaplastic factors regulating synaptic plasticity at the supergranular layers of the mouse visual cortex. We found that genetic inhibition of NOX2 reverses the polarizing effects of dark rearing from LTP to LTD. In addition, we demonstrate that this process relies on changes in the NMDA receptor functioning. Altogether, this work indicates a role of ROS in the activity-dependent regulation of cortical synaptic plasticity.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Synaptic plasticity in the visual cortex is modulated by the history of sensory experience and this modulation has been defined as metaplasticity. Dark rearing facilitates synaptic potentiation as a mechanism optimizing the range of synaptic modification. This process requires the production of reactive oxygen species mediated by the enzymatic complex NOX2. If the activity of NOX2 is inhibited, then visual deprivation results in synaptic depression. These findings increase our knowledge about metaplasticity and help in our understanding of how neural activity modulates cellular mechanisms of synaptic change.

  • dark rearing
  • metaplasticity
  • NMDA receptor
  • NOX2
  • ROS
  • visual cortex

Introduction

Metaplasticity is the modification in the properties of synaptic plasticity as a function of previous history of neural and synaptic activity (Abraham and Bear, 1996). This concept comprehends physiological factors that are capable of modulating the induction of synaptic changes. One of the most studied phenomena of metaplasticity is the effect of visual experience on LTP and LTD in the visual cortex (Kirkwood et al., 1996; Philpot et al., 2003). Reduction of synaptic activity due to dark rearing facilitates LTP and reduces LTD, by increasing NMDA receptor (NMDAR) functioning (Czepita et al., 1994). These findings have been interpreted by means of a model theorizing the existence of a synaptic modification threshold (Bienenstock et al., 1982), which is modulated as a function of the average activity in the neuronal network. This process maintains synaptic changes within a proper dynamic range, allowing the stimulus-driven strengthening of neural connections (Bear, 2003).

Because metaplasticity adjusts the threshold according to the recent level of global activity, any cellular factor resulting from prolonged synaptic stimulation is a potential metaplastic mediator (Girouard et al., 2009). In the brain, sustained neuronal activity leads to considerable production of reactive oxygen species (ROS) (Halliwell, 1992), suggesting the intriguing possibility that these molecules have a metaplastic function in synaptic transmission.

The idea that ROS function purely in a harmful manner has given way to a different view that these molecules can modulate specifically various pathways that are relevant for synaptic plasticity (Atkins and Sweatt, 1999; Yermolaieva et al., 2000; Massaad and Klann, 2011; Beckhauser et al., 2016). Similarly to phosphorylation and dephosphorylation, the oxidation of cysteine and methionine residues provides a mechanism to alter the function of several proteins rapidly and reversibly (Chiarugi, 2005). Proteins with activity that responds to redox changes include tyrosine phosphatases, membrane receptors, transcription factors, kinases, and ion channels (Choi and Lipton, 2000; Hidalgo and Donoso, 2008; Bodhinathan et al., 2010; Yang et al., 2010; Calero et al., 2011; Massaad and Klann, 2011; Rodríguez-Muñoz and Garzón, 2013; Beckhauser et al., 2016).

Among all of the cellular structures involved in redox signaling, the enzymatic complexes of the NOX family are those that generate ROS more directly after intense synaptic stimulation (Massaad and Klann, 2011). In the hippocampus and cerebral cortex, NOX enzymes are highly expressed in dendritic synaptic sites and participate in postsynaptic intracellular pathways important for plasticity (Serrano et al., 2003; Kim et al., 2005; Tejada-Simon et al., 2005; Massaad and Klann, 2011). The NOX2 isoform is stimulated by NMDAR activation and this event produces large and controlled amounts of superoxide in a rapid process that links extracellular stimuli to subsequent signaling processes (Bindokas et al., 1996; Kishida et al., 2005; Brennan et al., 2009; Girouard et al., 2009; Guemez-Gamboa et al., 2011; Reyes et al., 2012; De Pasquale et al., 2014). Although the physiological roles of NOX2 in the brain are only partially understood, it has been proposed that one of its functions could be the redox modulation of neurotransmission and plasticity at glutamatergic synapses (Sorce and Krause, 2009; Sorce et al., 2010). Considering ROS as highly reactive and diffusible molecules, NOX2 activation could be involved in the metaplastic regulation of synaptic changes. In agreement with this idea, previous studies demonstrated that nonfunctional NOX2 impairs synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus and visual cortex (Kishida et al., 2006; De Pasquale et al., 2014).

In the present study, we introduce the possibility that NOX2 does not have a mere permissive role in synaptic modification, but rather could have a metaplastic function in the regulation of experience-dependent synaptic plasticity. To test this hypothesis, we performed LTP and LTD experiments using the dark-rearing protocol and knock-out mice for the catalytic subunit of NOX2 (gp91phox−/−). Our results indicate that the loss of functional NOX2 overturns the effects of dark rearing by changing the polarization of synaptic plasticity from potentiation to depression.

Materials and Methods

Animals.

Wild-type (C57BL/6) and gp91phox−/− mice of either sex (The Jackson Laboratory) were used throughout this work. All animals used in our experiments were at the age of postnatal day 20 (P20) to P24 and were kept in an animal facility at 23 ± 2°C with ad libitum access to food and water at all times. Mice were divided into four experimental groups: normally reared wild-type (NW), dark-reared wild-type (DW), normally reared gp91phox−/− (NK), and dark-reared gp91phox−/− (DK). Normally reared animals were maintained on a 12/12 h light/dark cycle and dark-reared animals were deprived from light exposure from birth to the beginning of experimental procedures. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care Committee of the Institute of Biomedical Sciences, University of São Paulo, Brazil (CEUA ICB/USP #090, fls. 1°).

Preparation and maintenance of brain slices.

Animals were deeply anesthetized thought isoflurane inhalation (AErrane; Baxter Pharmaceuticals) and then decapitated. The brain was quickly removed and submerged in cooled (0°C) oxygenated (5% CO2–95% O2) dissection buffer containing the following (in mm): 206 sucrose, 25 NaHCO3, 2.5 KCl, 10 MgSO4, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 0.5 CaCl2, and 11 d-glucose). Brains were glued to an iron platform and sectioned by vibratome. Visual coronal slices (∼300 μm thick) were prepared and rapidly transferred to a holding chamber containing artificial CSF (ACSF) containing the following (in mm): 125 NaCl, 25 NaHCO3, 3 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 1 MgCl2, 2 CaCl2, and 25 d-glucose. Slices were kept oxygenated at room temperature (20–25°) for at least 1 h before proceeding with electrophysiological recordings.

Pharmacology.

In some electrophysiological experiments, the NMDAR blockers MK-801 (50 or 100 μm; Tocris Bioscience) and CP-101,606 (20 μm; Sigma-Aldrich) were used. In experiments to isolate NMDAR responses, we prepared a magnesium-free ACSF containing the AMPA receptor (AMPAR) blocker DNQX (50 μm; Tocris Bioscience) and the GABA receptor (GABAR) blocker picrotoxin (50 μm; Tocris Bioscience). MK-801 stock solutions were prepared in warm distilled water. DNQX, picrotoxin, and CP-101,606 stock solutions were prepared in DMSO (0.1%). All drugs were diluted in ACSF to their final concentration before use.

Electrophysiological recordings.

Cortical slices containing the visual cortex were placed in a submersion-type recording chamber upon a modified microscope stage and maintained at 30°C with constant perfusion of oxygenated ACSF (5% CO2–95% O2). Whole-cell recordings were made from layer 2/3 of primary visual cortex (V1). Layer 2/3 was identified by differential interference contrast microscopy: layer 1 was typically thin and aneuronal, whereas layer 4 showed a dense packing of cell bodies. Layers 2/3 can be recognized as being located between layers 1 and 4. All recordings were obtained from regular spiking pyramidal-shaped cells. Recording pipettes were fabricated from borosilicate glass (Garner Glass) with input resistances of ∼4–6 MΩ and were filled with intracellular solution containing the following (in mm): 135 K-gluconate, 7 NaCl, 10 HEPES, 2 Na2ATP, 0.3 Na3GTP, 2 MgCl2, and 0.1–0.5% biocytin, pH 7.3 with KOH, and osmolality 290 mOsm. All experiments were performed using a visualized slice setup under a differential interference contrast-equipped Nikon Eclipse E600FN microscope. Recordings were made using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier and pClamp software (Molecular Devices). Only recordings from cells with membrane potentials <−65 mV, access resistance <20 MΩ, and input resistance >100 MΩ and <1000 MΩ were included in our data. Cells were discarded if any of these values changed >20% during the experiment. Postsynaptic responses were recorded in current-clamp mode.

Electrical stimulation.

Synaptic responses were evoked by electrically stimulating inputs to neurons of layer 2/3 (0.1 Hz; 0.2 ms). Stimulation was delivered through a concentric bipolar electrode (125 μm diameter; FHC) placed in the middle of the cortical thickness of layer 4. The role of ROS on experience-dependent synaptic plasticity was investigated using knock-out animals for the catalytic subunit of NOX2 (gp91phox−/−). Only responses that we determined to be monosynaptic were included in the data based on the following criteria: latencies <3 ms, latency jitter <1 ms, and no failures for stimulation levels above threshold. For LTP/LTD experiments, the intensity of stimulation was increased gradually from a subthreshold level by steps of 5 μA until a response was evoked. The intensity was then adjusted to evoke 2–5 mV responses.

To induce synaptic plasticity, a pairing protocol in which presynaptic stimulation was coupled with postsynaptic depolarization was used. Three different values of voltage membrane potential were used (0, −20, or −40 mV) to allow differential NMDAR recruitment during the induction, thus leading to different synaptic plasticity expression (LTP, no plasticity, LTD). To this end, the recording mode was switched from current-clamp to voltage-clamp mode. The induction consisted in a pairing protocol of 200 epochs (1 Hz) during which the voltage membrane was kept at 0, −20, or −40 mV. Changes in synaptic strength were quantified as changes in the initial amplitude of the postsynaptic potential normalized by the mean baseline response obtained during the first 10 min of stable recordings. In all graphs showing the LTP or LTD time course, the EPSP amplitude was normalized with respect to the averaged EPSP value of the last 5 min of baseline. The LTP and LTD magnitudes were analyzed by calculating the average EPSP value of the last 5 min (35–40 min) of the postsynaptic period. This value was normalized and the baseline was subtracted to obtain the relative increase or decrease of EPSP.

In some experiments, we aimed to determine the relationship between stimulation intensity and the evoked synaptic responses. In these experiments, the intensity stimulation values were normalized with respect to the “minimal stimulation” intensity, which is defined as follows: stimulation level was increased from subthreshold in 5 μA steps until a reliable response was triggered; intensity was then further increased by 5 μA from this point and this was the minimum level of intensity stimulation.

Immunohistochemistry.

Animals were anesthetized with ketamine (10 mg/kg) and xylazine (0.5 mg/kg) and then submitted to transcardiac perfusion with PBS solution (0.9% NaCl on 0.2 m PB, pH 7.4), followed by PB paraformaldehyde at 2% (2% PFA on 0.1 m PB, pH 7.4) for fixation. Brains were removed and postfixed for 4 h in the same solution and subsequently transferred to a 30% sucrose PB solution (30% sucrose on 0.1 m PB, pH 7.4) for cryoprotection and stored at 4°C until further processing.

Coronal sections (30 μm) were obtained on a freezing microtome. The slices were washed for 10 min 3 times in PB 0.1 m and incubated in a solution containing the primary antibody anti-NR2A (NMDAε1 antibody; C-17, sc-1468; Santa Cruz Biotechnology) or anti-NR2B (NMDAε2 antibody; C-20, sc-1469; Santa Cruz Biotechnology) for 24 h at 1:250 dilution, containing 50 μl of normal donkey serum diluted in 0.3% Triton X-100. After incubation with primary antibodies, sections were washed for 10 min 3 times in PB 0.1 m and then incubated for 2 h with 1:200 dilution of biotinylated secondary antibody (donkey anti-goat IgG; Jackson ImmunoResearch). Next, slices were washed as described previously here and then incubated with avidin–biotin complex (1:100; ABC Elite Kit; Vector Laboratories) for 2 h. After washing, visualization was done using a mixture of 0.05% diaminobenzidine and 0.01% hydrogen peroxide. The slices were washed in PB and mounted on slides with glycerol-based mounting medium, dried at room temperature, dehydrated with graded solutions of ethanol followed by xylol, and then covered with Perm (Fisher Scientific) and coverslips for permanent storage.

Neurons that were NR2A-positive or NR2B-positive were counted in the layer 2/3 of the V1. Each slice was photographed with a 20× magnification using a photomicroscope coupled to a digital camera and photographs were stored in a computer for later digital analysis. Data analysis was made by manual counting of positive neurons performed by an investigator blinded to conditions. A contrast threshold was set to ensure the inclusion of only neurons that could be easily distinguished from the background, excluding false positives. A size threshold was also set to eliminate points too large or too small to be considered as neurons. For each group, slices were counted, generating an average of positive neurons for each group, for further statistical analysis.

Western blots.

Tissue samples were obtained from acute cortical slices, which were dissected under an optic microscope to isolate and select the layer 2/3 of the visual cortex. For this purpose, brain slices were prepared as described above for electrophysiology experiments. Immunoblots were performed as described previously (Hernandes et al., 2013). The membranes were incubated for 24 h with the same antibodies used in immunohistochemistry experiments: goat polyclonal anti-NR2A (NMDAε1 antibody; C-17, sc-1468; Santa Cruz Biotechnology) or anti-NR2B (NMDAε2 antibody; C-20, sc-1469; Santa Cruz Biotechnology) at 1:500 dilution and a mouse monoclonal anti-β-actin (1:1000; Sigma-Aldrich) for 2 h. The probed proteins were developed using a chemiluminescent kit (ECL; GE Healthcare). Bands were captured with the scanner C-digit (LI-COR) and images were quantified with ImageJ. Data were always plotted in relation to controls (NW animals).

Statistical analysis.

LTP and LTD were assessed by means of a paired t test. We compared the synaptic response of the baseline with that of the postinduction period. For both periods, synaptic response was calculated by averaging the last 5 min EPSP values. Differences of synaptic change between 2 experimental groups were assessed by comparing the average EPSP recorded during the last 5 min (35–40 min) with an unpaired t test.

A χ2 was applied to study differences among groups in the distribution probability of inducing plasticity. In such analysis, cells were classified as LTP or LTD when the induction protocol causes significant change in the EPSP according to the Wilcoxon test (p < 0.05).

Some analysis that summarized the combined action of two or three factors on plasticity were performed by using a two-ways or a three-ways ANOVA respectively. Intensity/response curves of our four experimental groups were analyzed by means of a three-way ANOVA. Finally, a two-way ANOVA analyzed the effects of genotype and rearing condition on NR2A and NR2B subunit expression.

The effects of CP-101,606 (20 μm) or vehicle were assessed by means of a paired t test. The average NMDAR-mediated EPSP of the 5 min baseline was compared with the averaged synaptic response of the last 5 min of drug application (15–20 min). Differences of CP-101,606 effect between experimental groups were analyzed by comparing the average NMDAR-mediated EPSP recorded during the last 5 min (15–20 min) by means of a two-way ANOVA.

Results

The goal of the present work was to study the interaction between two metaplastic factors: visual experience and of ROS. The first step was to evaluate how dark rearing and genetic suppression of NOX2 modulate the plasticity curve, in which synaptic change is expressed as a function of the postsynaptic depolarization applied during the induction protocol. In our previous study, we found that, in knock-out animals for the NOX2 enzyme (gp91phox−/−), the induction of LTP and LTD was impaired through inhibition of NMDAR activity (De Pasquale et al., 2014). Because deprivation of experience potentiates the NMDAR function (Yoshimura et al., 2003; Guo et al., 2012), we hypothesized that dark exposure could rescue the deficit in plasticity caused by the lack of functional NOX2.

NOX2 and dark rearing modulate synaptic plasticity

−40 mV pairing protocol

We first examined how visual experience together with ROS production would affect the LTD of layer 2/3 cells. As described in the Materials and Methods, we evaluated LTP by analyzing the EPSPs obtained from the last 5 min of the postsynaptic recording. The results shown in Figure 1 indicate that, in NW animals, presynaptic stimulation paired with −40 mV voltage clamp induces significant reduction of synaptic strength (Fig. 1A; 10 cells, 10 slices, 7 animals, p < 0.01 on a paired t test), but the same induction protocol does not cause significant variation of EPSPs in DW mice (Fig. 1A; 10 cells, 10 slices, 7 animals; p > 0.1 on a paired t test). In the visual cortex, deprivation of visual experience shifts the plasticity induction threshold by increasing the probability of inducing LTP (Kirkwood et al., 1995; Guo et al., 2012). Consistently, our results showed that pairing at −40 mV fails to trigger LTD in DW animals and this depends on the increased probability of obtaining LTP after the induction, as shown in Figure 1C (p < 0.05, χ2 test).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

In wild-type animals, visual deprivation suppresses LTD induced through pairing at −40 mV. A, Magnitude of EPSPs before and after the induction of plasticity through the −40 mV conditioning protocol in wild-type animals. EPSP values are normalized with respect to the baseline. Slices obtained from NW mice are compared with slices obtained from DW mice. LTD was successfully induced in normally reared (10 cells, 10 slices, 7 animals, p < 0.01, paired t test), but not in dark-reared animals (10 cells, 10 slices, 7 animals, p > 0.1, paired t test). Examples of EPSP traces show the amplitude before (black trace) and after (gray trace) the induction. B, Magnitude of EPSPs before and after the induction of plasticity through the −40 mV conditioning protocol in gp91phox−/− animals. EPSP values are normalized with respect to the baseline. Slices obtained from NK mice are compared with slices obtained from DK mice. The protocol failed to induce synaptic change both in normally reared (10 cells, 10 slices, 7 animals, p > 0.1, paired t test) and dark-reared animals (10 cells, 10 slices, 7 animals, p > 0.1, paired t test). Examples of EPSP traces show the amplitude before (black trace) and after (gray trace) induction. C, Percentage of cells exhibiting LTP, LTD, or no variation in wild-type animals after induction with the −40 mV pairing protocol. The normal rearing condition is compared with dark rearing. Dark rearing increases significantly the probability of inducing LTP (p < 0.05, χ2 test). D, Percentage of cells exhibiting LTP, LTD, or no variation in gp91phox−/− animals after induction with the −40 mV pairing protocol. The normal rearing condition is compared with dark rearing. Dark rearing significantly increases the probability of inducing LTP (p < 0.05, χ2 test), but does not significantly modify the probability distribution (p > 0.1, χ2 test). In C and D, cells are classified as LTP or LTD when the induction causes significant change in the postsynaptic response according to the Wilcoxon test (p < 0.05).

Conversely, in knock-out mice, the LTD induced by pairing at −40 mV is abolished, regardless of rearing condition (Fig. 1B; NK: 10 cells, 10 slices, 7 animals, p > 0.1 on a paired t test; DK: 10 cells, 10 slices, 7 animals; p > 0.1 on a paired t test). Figure 1D shows that NK animals respond to pairing at −40 mV by expressing similar probability values for LTP, LTD, or no effect and dark exposure does not alter this profile significantly (Fig. 1D; p > 0.1, χ2 test).

Therefore, genetic ablation of NOX2 impairs the LTD induced through −40 mV paired conditioning and visual deprivation is not able to rescue the knock-out phenotype. However, wild-type animals did not exhibit the typical depression induced by this protocol because dark rearing is capable of increasing the probability of inducing LTP.

−20 mV pairing protocol

Pairing presynaptic stimulation with postsynaptic depolarization at −20 mV is not effective at inducing either LTP or LTD (Fig. 2A; NW: 10 cells, 10 slices, 6 animals, p > 0.1, paired t test; DW: 9 cells, 9 slices, 6 animals, p > 0.1, paired t test). In fact, as shown in Figure 2C, pairing at −20 mV results in the same probability of inducing LTP or LTD, so the resulting average does not differ from baseline. In wild-type animals, dark rearing fails to promote significant change in the relative distribution of probability (p > 0.1 on a χ2 test), so pairing at −20 mV does not produce relevant effects in the average of postsynaptic responses.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

In gp91phox−/− animals, visual deprivation promotes LTD through pairing at −20 mV. A, Magnitude of EPSPs before and after the induction of plasticity through the −20 mV conditioning protocol in wild-type animals. EPSP values are normalized with respect to the baseline Slices obtained from NW mice are compared with slices obtained from DW mice. No synaptic change was observed in normally reared animals (10 cells, 10 slices, 6 animals, p > 0.1, paired t test) and dark-reared animals (9 cells, 9 slices, 6 animals, p > 0.1, paired t test). Examples of EPSP traces show the amplitude before (black trace) and after (gray trace) induction. B, Magnitude of EPSPs before and after induction of plasticity through the −20 mV conditioning protocol in gp91phox−/− animals. EPSP values are normalized with respect to the baseline Slices obtained from normally reared mice are compared with slices obtained from dark-reared mice. LTD was successfully induced in DK (9 cells, 9 slices, 6 animals, p < 0.01, paired t test), but not in NK animals (10 cells, 10 slices, 7 animals, p > 0.1, paired t test). Examples of EPSP traces show the amplitude before (black trace) and after (gray trace) induction. C, Percentage of cells exhibiting LTP, LTD, or no variation in wild-type animals after induction with the −20 mV pairing protocol. The normal rearing condition is compared with dark rearing. Dark rearing does not significantly modify the probability distribution (p > 0.1, χ2 test). D, Percentage of cells exhibiting LTP, LTD, or no variation in gp91phox−/− animals after induction with the −20 mV pairing protocol. The normal rearing condition is compared with dark rearing. Dark rearing significantly increases the probability of inducing LTD (p < 0.05, χ2 test). In C and D, cells are classified as LTP or LTD when induction causes significant change in the postsynaptic response according to the Wilcoxon test (p < 0.05).

However, in cells from knock-out mice, the situation is different: in NK mice, the amplitude of EPSP does not change after pairing at −20 mV (Fig. 2B; NK: 10 cells, 10 slices 7 animals, p > 0.1, paired t test); nevertheless, in DK animals, the average magnitude of synaptic responses is significantly decreased under the same induction protocol (Fig. 2B; DK: 9 cells, 9 slices 6 animals, p < 0.01, paired t test), showing that dark exposure increases the chance of inducing LTD in cells derived from gp91phox−/− mice (Fig. 2D: p < 0.05, χ2 test).

Together, these results indicate that the −20 mV protocol promotes synaptic depression in knock-out animals reared in dark conditions.

0 mV pairing protocol

In layer 2/3 neurons derived from wild-type animals, the 0 mV pairing paradigm induced robust, homosynaptic LTP (Fig. 3A; NW: 11 cells, 11 slices, 8 animals, p < 0.05, paired t test; DW: 10 cells, 10 slices, 8 animals; p < 0.01, paired t test). No statistical difference was observed comparing the potentiation of NW and DW mice. Figure 3C shows that pairing at 0 mV correlated with a high probability of inducing LTP both under normal conditions and dark rearing. No significant difference was found when comparing the distribution of probability of the two wild-type groups (p > 0.1, χ2 test).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

In gp91phox−/− animals, visual deprivation promotes LTD through pairing at 0 mV. A, Magnitude of EPSPs before and after induction of plasticity through the 0 mV conditioning protocol in wild-type animals. EPSP values are normalized with respect to the baseline Slices obtained from normally reared mice are compared with slices obtained from dark-reared mice. LTP was successfully induced in NW animals (11 cells, 11 slices, 8 animals, p < 0.05, paired t test) and DW animals (10 cells, 10 slices, 8 animals; p < 0.01, paired t test). Examples of EPSP traces show the amplitude before (black trace) and after (gray trace) induction. B, Magnitude of EPSPs before and after induction of plasticity through the 0 mV conditioning protocol in gp91phox−/− animals. EPSP values are normalized with respect to the baseline Slices obtained from normally reared mice are compared with slices obtained from dark-reared mice. LTD was observed in DR-gp91phox−/− mice (9 cells, 9 slices, 6 animals, p < 0.01, paired t test), whereas no significant effect on synaptic response was induced in NK animals (9 cells, 9 slices, 6 animals, p > 0.1, paired t test). Examples of EPSP traces show the amplitude before (black trace) and after (gray trace) induction. C, Percentage of cells exhibiting LTP, LTD, or no variation in wild-type animals after induction with the 0 mV pairing protocol. The normal rearing condition is compared with dark rearing. Dark rearing does not significantly modify the probability distribution (p > 0.1, χ2 test). D, Percentage of cells exhibiting LTP, LTD, or no variation in gp91phox−/− animals after induction with 0 mV pairing protocol. The normal rearing condition is compared with dark rearing. Dark rearing significantly increases the probability of inducing LTD (p < 0.05, χ2 test). In C and D, cells are classified as LTP or LTD when induction causes significant change in the postsynaptic response according to the Wilcoxon test (p < 0.05).

As described previously (De Pasquale et al., 2014), pairing at 0 mV induces no net change in EPSP amplitude of slices obtained from NK animals (Fig. 3B; 9 cells, 9 slices, 6 animals, p > 0.1, paired t test). However, cells recorded from DK mice respond to the 0 mV induction protocol undergoing remarkable postsynaptic depression (Fig. 3B; 9 cells, 9 slices, 6 animals, p < 0.01, paired t test). Dark rearing greatly increases the probability of inducing LTD in this protocol when ROS production is low (Fig. 3D, p < 0.05, χ2 test). These results demonstrated that dark exposure restores synaptic plasticity in gp91phox−/− animals, yet such recovery is not bidirectional because it only allows the expression of LTD.

The results of this experiment suggest that ROS production could have metaplastic effects allowing the expression of LTD in dark-reared knock-out animals. Figure 6A summarizes the results shown so far. In conclusion, the effects of the visual deprivation depend on NOX2 activity: under normal conditions, dark exposure favors the induction of LTP; nevertheless, when dark rearing is combined with genetic ablation of NOX2, neurons undergo LTD. The analysis performed with a three-way ANOVA (genotype, rearing, and conditioning as factors) indicates that there is a significant difference between wild-type and gp91phox−/− animals (p < 0.001) and this difference depends on rearing condition (p < 0.01) and voltage-clamp value (p < 0.05).

Synaptic plasticity requires the activation of NMDARs

The LTP and LTD of visual cortical supragranular layers require the activation of NMDARs (Kirkwood and Bear, 1995; Philpot et al., 2003, 2007; Yoshimura et al., 2003; Jiang et al., 2007; Smith et al., 2009; Li and Wang, 2013; De Pasquale et al., 2014). Therefore, NOX2 inhibition and visual deprivation may interact to alter the profile of NMDAR-dependent plasticity to favor LTD over LTP according to our hypothesis. Alternatively, another form of LTD based on different mechanisms might be recruited under these conditions. To address this issue, the NMDAR function was suppressed by treating the slices with the NMDAR blocker MK-801 and the results are presented in this section.

Synaptic plasticity and NMDA blocking in wild-type animals

In normally reared wild-type mice, LTP can be induced by pairing presynaptic stimulation with 0 mV postsynaptic voltage (Fig. 4A; 11 cells, 11 slices, 8 animals; p < 0.05, paired t test) and potentiation is prevented when the slices are incubated with MK-801 at 50 μm (Fig. 4A; 9 cells, 9 slices, 7 animals; p > 0.1, paired t test). Pairing presynaptic stimulation with −40 mV postsynaptic voltage induces LTD in the same animals (Fig. 4B,C; 10 cells, 10 slices, 7 animals; p < 0,01, paired t test) and blockage of NMDARs with the same concentration of MK-801 does not abolish the LTD (Fig. 4B; 12 cells, 12 slices, 8 animals; p < 0.05, paired t test). However, in the presence of MK-801, the LTD was significantly reduced compared with untreated slices (p < 0.05, t test).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

LTP and LTD of wild-type animals require the activation of the NMDAR. A, Magnitude of EPSPs before and after induction of plasticity through the 0 mV conditioning protocol in NW animals. EPSP values are normalized with respect to the baseline. Slices treated with MK-801 (50 μm) are compared with untreated slices. LTP was successfully induced in untreated slices (11 cells, 11 slices, 8 animals, p < 0.05, paired t test), but not in slices treated with MK-801 (9 cells, 9 slices, 7 animals; p > 0. 1, paired t test). Examples of EPSP traces show the amplitude before (black trace) and after (gray trace) induction. B, Magnitude of EPSPs before and after induction of plasticity through the −40 mV conditioning protocol in NW animals. EPSP values are normalized with respect to the baseline. Slices treated with MK-801 (50 μm) are compared with untreated slices. LTD was successfully induced in untreated (10 cells, 10 slices, 7 animals, p < 0.01, paired t test) and treated slices (12 cells, 12 slices, 8 animals; p < 0.01, paired t test). The LTD of treated slices is significantly lower compared with untreated (p < 0,05, t test). Examples of EPSP traces show the amplitude before (black trace) and after (gray trace) induction. C, Magnitude of EPSPs before and after induction of plasticity through the −40 mV conditioning protocol in NW animals. EPSP values are normalized with respect to the baseline. Slices treated with MK-801 (100 μm) are compared with untreated slices. LTD was successfully induced in untreated (10 cells, 10 slices, 7 animals, p < 0.01, paired t test), but not in treated slices (10 cells, 10 slices, 6 animals; p > 0. 1, paired t test). Examples of EPSP traces show the amplitude before (black trace) and after (gray trace) induction.

During the induction of NMDAR-dependent synaptic plasticity, the correlation of presynaptic and postsynaptic activity is converted by the voltage-dependent NMDAR into postsynaptic Ca2+ influx (Kirkwood and Bear, 1995). In this way, mild increases of postsynaptic Ca2+ can trigger LTD, whereas larger elevations of intracellular Ca2+ would elicit LTP. This model indicates that LTD can be induced by relying on important but limited NMDAR contribution, compared with LTP, which might be more sensitive to pharmacological receptor blockage.

To test this hypothesis, we applied MK-801 at a higher concentration (100 μm) when inducing synaptic depression in slices from our NW animals. Neuronal EPSPs recorded in these conditions do exhibit complete blockage of LTD (Fig. 4C; 10 cells, 10 slices, 6 animals; p > 0.1, paired t test). Therefore, our results confirmed our hypothesis, suggesting that synaptic depression mechanisms are based on lower levels of NMDAR activity compared with mechanisms of synaptic potentiation.

Synaptic plasticity and NMDA blocking in gp91phox−/− animals

We have demonstrated previously that it is possible to induce LTD in gp91phox−/− animals when they are reared in the dark. It remains to be seen whether this form of LTD requires the involvement of NMDAR. To study this possibility, we used the pairing at 0 mV in slices obtained from DK animals while blocking NMDAR with MK-801.

In untreated slices from DK animals, pairing presynaptic stimulation with 0 mV postsynaptic voltage results in depression of EPSPs (Fig. 5A,B; 9 cells, 9 slices, 6 animals; p < 0.05, paired t test). NMDAR blockage with MK-801 at 50 μm does not change this result, as shown in Figure 5A (p > 0.1, t test). However, depression of EPSPs with this protocol can be abolished when the slices are treated with MK-801 at 100 μm (Fig. 5B; 9 cells, 9 slices, 6 animals; p > 0.1, paired t test), confirming our previous assumption that LTD would be more resilient to NMDAR blockage. Therefore, our findings pointed out that dark-reared gp91phox−/− animals were capable of expressing a NMDAR-dependent form of LTD, suggesting that low ROS might affect plasticity through modulation of NMDAR activity.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

LTD of gp91phox−/− animals requires the activation of the NMDAR. A, Magnitude of EPSPs before and after induction of plasticity through the 0 mV conditioning protocol in DK animals. EPSP values are normalized with respect to the baseline. Slices treated with MK-801 (50 μm) are compared with untreated slices. LTD was successfully induced in untreated slices (9 cells, 9 slices, 6 animals, p < 0.05, paired t test) and treated slices (9 cells, 9 slices, 6 animals; p < 0.05, paired t test). The LTD of treated slices is not significantly different from that of untreated slices (p > 0.05, t test). Examples of EPSP traces show the amplitude before (black trace) and after (gray trace) induction. B, Magnitude of EPSPs before and after induction of plasticity through the 0 mV conditioning protocol in DK animals. EPSP values are normalized with respect to the baseline. Slices treated with MK-801 (100 μm) are compared with untreated slices. LTD was successfully induced in untreated slices (9 cells, 9 slices, 6 animals, p < 0.05, paired t test), but not in treated slices (9 cells, 9 slices, 6 animals; p > 0.1, paired t test). Examples of EPSP traces show the amplitude before (black trace) and after (gray trace) induction. C, Graph summarizing the effects of MK-801 on LTD in wild-type and gp91phox−/− animals. Synaptic changes are plotted against the MK-801concentration values. MK-801 significantly impairs induction of synaptic depression and this effect is similar in NW and DK (p < 0.001 and p > 0.1, respectively, two-way ANOVA).

Figure 5C summarizes all results obtained from the LTD experiments that adopted the NMDAR blocker. LTD sensitivity to different MK-801 concentration values is shown for NW and DK mice. NMDAR involvement is similar in both cases: a 100 μm concentration of MK-801 was needed to induce blockage of synaptic depression. We analyzed the two types of LTD by performing an analysis with a two-way ANOVA. The application of MK-801 affects the possibility of inducing synaptic depression significantly (p < 0.001), with no significant difference between the NW and DK groups (p > 0.1).

NOX2 and dark rearing modulate the activity of NMDARs

In a previous work, it was shown that synaptic plasticity is impaired in gp91phox−/− mice and that this is due to a deficiency in NMDAR functioning (De Pasquale et al., 2014). However, here, we demonstrated that plasticity “reopens” in the direction of LTD when gp91phox−/− animals are reared in the dark. It is known that dark rearing increases the efficacy of the NMDAR in layer 2/3 neurons (Yoshimura et al., 2003; Guo et al., 2012). This evidence suggests that dark exposure might allow partial recovery of NMDAR activity in gp91phox−/− animals. Such recovery would be sufficient to induce LTD. To test this hypothesis, a magnesium-free ACSF solution with an AMPAR blocker (DNQX, 50 μm) and a GABAR blocker (picrotoxin, 50 μm) were used to isolate the evoked EPSPs mediated by NMDAR activation (NMDAR-EPSPs). Under these conditions, NMDAR-EPSPs were recorded in NW (21 cells, 7 animals), DW (13 cells, 6 animals), NK (24 cells, 7 animals), and DK (21 cells, 7 animals) mice.

In all groups, postsynaptic responses disappeared after the application of MK-801 (100 μm). Figure 6B shows the results: in wild-type animals, dark rearing increases the NMDAR-EPSPs. NK mice instead appeared as the group exhibiting the lowest amplitudes of NMDAR-EPSPs. Dark exposure increases the responses of gp91phox−/− animals, but such an increase does not reach the same level of NW animals. A three-way ANOVA was realized (genotype, rearing condition, and intensity of stimulation as factors) to evaluate statistical significance and we found a significant difference among groups in terms of genotype, rearing conditions, and stimulation intensity (p < 0.001 for all comparisons). Genotype and rearing conditions modify NMDAR-EPSPs without significant interaction (p > 0.1); that is, genetic suppression of NOX2 does not prevent dark rearing from enhancing NMDAR-EPSPs. Increasing in the stimulation intensity is followed by increasing in the evoked response, but this relationship depends significantly on the genotype (p < 0.001), indicating that gp91phox−/− animals exhibit lower enhancements of NMDAR-EPSP compared with wild-type.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Dark rearing and genetic ablation of NOX2 modify synaptic plasticity and the NMDAR-mediated postsynaptic responses. A, Graph summarizing the combined effects of genetic suppression of NOX2 and dark rearing on plasticity. Synaptic changes are plotted as a function of the postsynaptic voltage clamp applied during the pairing protocol. Dark exposure facilitates induction of LTP in wild-type mice while promoting LTD in gp91phox−/− mice. Synaptic changes of wild-type animals are different from those of gp91phox−/− and this difference depends on rearing condition and induction protocols (p < 0.001, p < 0.01 and p < 0.05, respectively, three-way ANOVA). B, Magnitude of NMDAR-EPSPs (mV) recorded from slices obtained from NW (21 cells, 7 animals), DW (13 cells, 6 animals), NK (24 cells, 7 animals), and DK (21 cells, 7 animals) animals. The stimulation intensities are normalized with respect to the intensity value evocating the minimal response. For each group, example traces of recorded NMDAR-EPSP for 1.5, 2, and 2.5 normalized intensities are shown. Groups are statistically different in terms of genotype, rearing conditions, and stimulation intensity (p < 0.001 for all comparisons, three-way ANOVA).

These results demonstrate that dark rearing partially restores the reduction of NMDAR functioning caused by genetic deletion of NOX2. This finding may explain the unidirectional and partial recovery of plasticity that uniquely expresses as LTD.

NOX2 and dark rearing modulate the NR2A/NR2B ratio

NMDAR activity is regulated through the variable composition of two subtypes of NR2 subunits, NR2A and NR2B (Yashiro and Philpot, 2008). NMDAR subunits of the NR2B subtype are more efficient and the presence of this subunit is necessary for the induction of LTP (Yoshimura et al., 2003; Guo et al., 2012). In the visual cortex, the relationship between NR2A and NR2B subunits is one of the crucial factors that regulates synaptic plasticity in response to visual experience. Typically, animals reared in the dark show higher expression of NMDAR containing the NR2B subunits (Quinlan et al., 1999; Chen and Bear, 2007).

After observing that dark rearing and genetic suppression of NOX2 change the functionality of the NMDAR, we tested the hypothesis that visual experience and NOX2 may converge in modulating synaptic plasticity through modification of the NR2B/NR2A ratio in layer 2/3 neurons of V1. To evaluate this hypothesis, we performed immunohistochemical experiments that aimed to quantify the expression of NR2A and NR2B subunits in our experimental groups. Figure 7 shows the results obtained from the labeling of NR2A- and NR2B-positive neurons.

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Genetic deletion of NOX2 and dark rearing change the NR2A/NR2B ratio. A, Examples of NR2A labeling in layer 2/3 of the visual cortex of NW animals (1), DW (2), NK (3), and DK (4). B, Graph showing the number of positive cells observed in a 10,000 μm2 area of layer 2/3 in NW (25 slices, 9 animals), DW (21 slices, 8 animals), NK (24 slices, 8 animals), and DK animals (26 slices, 9 animals). gp91phox−/− animals showed higher labeling of NR2A than the wild-type and dark rearing does not change this profile (p < 0.01, two-way ANOVA, for the genotype factor, p > 0.1 for the rearing condition factor). The difference between genotypes does not depend on the rearing condition (p > 0.1, two-way ANOVA for the interaction between genotype and rearing condition). C, Examples of NR2B labeling in layer 2/3 of the visual cortex of NW animals (1), DW (2), NK (3), and DK (4). D, Graph showing the number of positive cells observed in a 10,000 μm2 area of layer 2/3 in NW (28 slices, 10 animals), NK (26 slices, 8 animals), DW (28 slices, 9 animals), and DK (26 slices, 8 animals) animals. Wild-type mice exhibited higher NR2B labeling than gp91phox−/− animals (p < 0.001, two-way ANOVA, for the genotype factor). Dark rearing increases the expression of NR2B and this increase is greater for wild-type animals (p < 0.001, two-way ANOVA, for the rearing condition factor and p < 0.01 for the interaction between genotype and rearing condition). E, Graph showing the results of NR2A subunit expression of layer 2/3 obtained from Western blot experiments performed in NW (20 animals), DW (20 animals), NK (20 animals), and DK animals (20 animals). Western blotting samples for each group are shown. The NR2A densities of DW, NK, and DK were normalized to the protein density value obtained from the NW. No statistic difference was found in terms of genotype and rearing condition (p > 0.1, two-way ANOVA). F, Graph showing the results of NR2B subunit expression of layer 2/3 obtained from Western blot experiments performed in NW (19 animals), DW (18 animals), NK (18 animals), and DK (18 animals) animals. Western blotting samples for each group are shown. The relative NR2B densities of DW, NK, and DK were normalized to the protein density value obtained from the NW. Dark rearing increases the relative expression of the NR2B subunit (p < 0.05, two-way ANOVA). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

We analyzed these results by using a two-way ANOVA considering genotype and rearing condition as factors. For NR2A, NK animals (Fig. 7A,B; NK: 24 slices, 8 animals; NW: 25 slices, 9 animals) exhibit higher concentration of NMDAR containing NR2A, compared with NW mice. Dark exposure does not change this profile (Fig. 7A,B; DW: 21 slices, 8 animals; DK: 26 slices, 9 animals). The groups are statistically different in terms of genotype (p < 0.01), but do not differ by rearing condition (p > 0.1) and the difference determined by genotype does not depend on the rearing condition (p > 0.1). For NR2B, NW have a higher concentration of NR2B-positive cells compared with NK (Fig. 7C,D; NW: 28 slices, 10 animals; NK 26 slices, 8 animals). When wild-type and gp91phox−/− animals are reared in the dark (Fig. 7C,D; DW: 28 slices, 9 animals; DK: 26 slices, 8 animals), the expression of NR2B subunit increases and such an increase is higher for the wild-type animals. In fact, the groups are statistically different in terms of genotype (p < 0.001) and rearing condition (p < 0.001). The effect of dark rearing is stronger for wild-type mice compared with gp91phox−/− mice (p < 0.01).

We next aimed to implement the results obtained from immunohistochemistry with Western blot experiments. Therefore, we quantified the relative expression of NR2A and NR2B subunits of tissue samples obtained from the visual cortical layer II/III from the four experimental groups (NW, DW, NK, and DK). For NR2A expression (Fig. 7E; NW: 20 animals, DW: 20 animals, NK: 20 animals and DK animals: 20 animals), the groups do not differ statistically by genotype and rearing condition (p > 0.1, two-way ANOVA). Nevertheless, regarding the NR2B subunit (NW: 19 animals; DW: 18 animals, NK: 18 animals and DK: 18 animals), we found that that dark rearing significantly increases the protein concentration, independently of genotype (p < 0.05, two-way ANOVA).

Therefore, results obtained from the immunohistochemistry experiments indicated a NR2A/NR2B ratio increase in the cell bodies of NK animals compared with other groups. This effect, however, was not confirmed by Western blot analysis, which accounts for the protein concentration in the cell. Conversely, both immunohistochemistry and Western blot data pointed out a dark rearing-induced increase in the concentration of the NR2B subunit.

Dark rearing modulates the NR2B component response of NMDAR-EPSPs

After assessing the alteration in the protein levels of NR2B subunit imposed by dark rearing and genetic suppression of NOX2, we decided to test whether these changes on NR2B protein levels indeed modify the NMDAR functioning. To address this issue, we applied the highly NR2B-selective antagonist CP-101,606 (20 μm) and recorded the NMDAR-mediated EPSP for 20 min, thus evaluating the contribution of the NR2B subunit to the response magnitude. To isolate NMDAR responses, we used a magnesium-free ACSF solution with an AMPAR blocker (DNQX, 50 μm) and a GABAR blocker (picrotoxin, 50 μm). We first compared the effects of CP-101,606 and its vehicle (DMSO) in the NW animals. Reduction of NMDAR responses was evaluated by averaging the EPSPs recorded during the last 5 min. Although DMSO did not alter the EPSP amplitude (Fig. 8A: 7 cells, 7 slices, 4 animals p > 0.1, paired t test), CP-101,606 reduced NMDAR functioning significantly (7 cells, 7 slices, 4 animals p < 0.001, paired t test).

Figure 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 8.

Dark rearing modifies the contribution of NR2B subunit in the NMDAR-mediated postsynaptic responses. A, Magnitude of NMDAR-EPSPs recorded from slices of NW animals before and after exposure to CP-101,606 (20 μm; NR2B component blocker) and vehicle (DMSO). Percentage values are reported as referred to the NMDAR-EPSPs average recorded before the drug application. Representative NMDAR-EPSP traces before (black trace) and after (dark gray trace) CP 101,606 or vehicle are shown. Complete extinguishing of NMDAR-EPSP through MK-801 application (100 μm; light gray trace) is also shown. CP-101,606 application significantly reduces the magnitude of NMDAR-EPSPs (7 cells, 4 animals; p < 0.001, paired t test), whereas vehicle did not have any significant effect (7 cells, 4 animals; p > 0.05, paired t test). B, Magnitude of NMDAR-EPSPs recorded from slices obtained from NW (7 cells, 4 animals), DW (8 cells, 5 animals), NK (9 cells, 5 animals), and DK (8 cells, 5 animals) animals before and after CP-101,606 application. Examples of NMDAR-EPSP traces before (black trace) and after (gray trace) CP-101,606 application are shown for each group. Dark reading of wild-type and gp91phox−/− animals intensifies significantly the NMDA-EPSP magnitude reduction imposed by CP-101,606, demonstrating an increase of NR2B contribution in both groups (p < 0.01, two-way ANOVA).

We then decided to quantify the NR2B component of NMDAR-mediated EPSPs in the other experimental groups: DW (8 cells, 8 slices, 5 animals), NK (9 cells, 9slices, 5 animals), and DK (8 cells, 8 slices, 5 animals). Our results demonstrated that NR2B blockage reduces the NMDAR-EPSP amplitude in all analyzed conditions (Fig. 8B). We performed a two-way ANOVA test (genotype and rearing condition as factors), which demonstrated a significant difference among groups in terms of rearing condition (p < 0.01), showing that dark rearing increases the NR2B component of the NMDAR response in both wild-type and gp91phox−/− animals.

These results indicate a dark-rearing-induced increase of NR2B function, which could be due to a higher protein concentration in the plasma membrane that modifies the functionality of the NMDAR directly.

Metaplastic effect of NOX2 and dark rearing on basal synaptic transmission

In previous sections, we showed that visual deprivation may promote opposite signs of synaptic change depending on NOX2 functioning. We also showed that these depend on dark rearing and NOX2 inhibition having opposite effects on NMDAR activity. Based on these observations, we hypothesized that basal synaptic transmission could be altered as a result of these two metaplastic factors interacting with spontaneous neuronal activity. According to our hypothesis, dark rearing should increase synaptic strength in wild-type animals while leading to synaptic depression in gp91phox−/− animals.

Therefore, we performed a current step stimulation (1.0, 1.5, 2.0, and 2.5×) in layer 4 of V1 using the minimum current (1×) to evocate synaptic responses in neurons of layer 2/3 in NW (20 cells, 5 animals), DW (25 cells, 6 animals), NK (19 cells, 5 animals), and DK (14 cells, 5 animals) slices. Our results demonstrates DW animals have a higher synaptic response magnitude provided by 1.5, 2.0, and 2.5 of stimulation intensity (Fig. 9A). The analysis performed with a three-way ANOVA (genotype, rearing, and intensity as factors) demonstrates there are significant differences among groups in terms of genotype (p < 0.05), rearing (p < 0.01), and stimulation intensity (p < 0.0001), with a significant interaction between genotype and rearing (p < 0.05).

Figure 9.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 9.

Dark rearing and genetic ablation of NOX2 modify postsynaptic response. A, Magnitude of the synaptic response (mV) recorded from slices obtained from NW (20 cells, 5 animals), DW (25 cells, 6 animals), NK (19 cells, 5 animals), and DK (14 cells, 5 animals) animals. The stimulation intensities were normalized with respect to the intensity value evocating the minimal synaptic response. Representative traces of recorded postsynaptic response for 2.5 normalized intensity are shown for each group. Groups are statistically different in terms of genotype and rearing conditions, with the rearing effect being dependent on genotype (p < 0.05 and p < 0.01, respectively; p < 0.05 for the rearing condition–genotype interaction, three-way ANOVA). B, Graph showing the minimum current intensity (normalized to the NW average value) applied to evoke a synaptic response for the NW (20 cells, 5 animals), DW (25 cells, 6 animals), NK (19 cells, 5 animals), and DK (13 cells, 5 animals) experimental groups. The minimal current intensity used to evoke an EPSP in DW slices was lower compared with DK (p < 0.001 for the genotype factor and p < 0.01 for the rearing condition–genotype interaction, two-way ANOVA), showing that dark exposure increases excitability in wild-type animals, whereas it reduces it in gp91phox−/− animals. ***p < 0.001.

Finally, we aimed to verify whether NOX2 and visual experience effects on synaptic plasticity also reflect on the minimal stimulation required to evoke synaptic responses in layer 2/3 neurons (Fig. 9B). Two-way ANOVA analysis (genotype and rearing condition as factors) demonstrated a significant difference among groups in terms of genotype (p < 0.01) and a significant interaction between genotype and rearing condition (p < 0.05).

Therefore, as predicted by our hypothesis, dark rearing showed opposite effects on basal synaptic transmission depending on NOX2 functioning. In wild-type animals, visual deprivation increases synaptic strength, reflecting higher voltage values of EPSP saturation and lower current of minimal stimulations. In the gp91phox−/− animals instead, dark rearing leads to synaptic efficacy reduction, which in turns increases the amount of current required to evoke excitatory responses.

Discussion

In this work, we aimed to study the crosstalk between visual experience and redox signaling and how it impinges on synaptic changes of the visual cortex. The metaplastic effects of dark exposure on visual cortical plasticity have been widely documented by previous studies (Kirkwood and Bear, 1995; Guo et al., 2012). Our study introduces the first evidence that such effects can be modulated by ROS. We showed that inhibition of NOX2 changes the pairing protocol plasticity curve, overturning the effects of dark rearing from synaptic potentiation to synaptic depression.

Comparison of gp91phox−/− and wild-type animals confirmed that the activation of NOX2 is essential to trigger LTP and LTD in normal rearing conditions, as seen in a previous study (De Pasquale et al., 2014). However, we found that gp91phox−/− animals can undergo LTD when reared in the dark. Therefore, our first important result indicated that impairing the activity of NOX2, leading to less ROS production in neurons, changes the polarity of experience-dependent synaptic plasticity.

Factors that modify NMDAR function also change the relationship between postsynaptic depolarization and synaptic change (Abraham and Bear, 1996; Bear, 2003; Philpot et al., 2003). NMDAR efficiency is enhanced by visual deprivation and diminished by NOX2 inactivity. Based on this, we hypothesized that metaplastic effects of visual deprivation and/or NOX2 involve changes in the NMDAR functioning. As a first step in this direction, we demonstrated that the onset of LTD in dark-reared gp91phox−/− animals depends on the activation of the NMDAR. Interestingly, the blockade of LTD in this case requires the same concentration of the NMDA blocker that we applied to suppress depression in wild-type animals, suggesting that the two induction processes share similar conditions in terms of NMDAR contribution.

Consistently with previous studies (Kirkwood and Bear, 1995; Kirkwood et al., 1995; Quinlan et al., 1999; Chen and Bear, 2007; Guo et al., 2012), we found that dark exposure increased the NMDAR-mediated postsynaptic responses in wild-type animals. This finding explains the upward shift of the plasticity curve toward LTP. Nevertheless, genetic deletion of NOX2 downregulates NMDAR function, preventing synaptic change in both directions. When gp91phox−/− animals were reared in the dark, the NMDAR response increased significantly despite such an increase being moderated. Most likely, the level of NMDAR activity in dark reared gp91phox−/− animals was insufficient for the expression of LTP, but enough to induce LTD. These conclusions are summarized in Figure 10.

Figure 10.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 10.

Model summarizing the metaplastic effects of dark rearing and genetic suppression of NOX2. For each experimental group, synaptic plasticity and NMDAR activity are represented. Top, Synaptic change is plotted against the depolarization value (mV) applied during the pairing induction. Bottom, Corresponding activation of the NMDAR receptor, highlighting the thresholds required to induce LTP and LTD.

Finally, we analyzed the expression of the NR2A and NR2B subunits to provide further evidence that the metaplastic effects that we observed implied changes in NMDAR functioning. NMDARs containing the NR2B subunit are able to carry more Ca2+ inside the postsynaptic neuron (Erreger et al., 2005; Sobczyk et al., 2005), making this subunit essential for the induction of LTP (Quinlan et al., 1999; Yoshimura et al., 2003; Chen and Bear, 2007; Guo et al., 2012). In normal rearing conditions, wild-type mice show more expression of NR2B subunits, whereas the gp91phox−/− group exhibits higher expression of the NR2A subtype. These results could partially explain the deficit of synaptic plasticity associated with genetic suppression of NOX2. Dark rearing promoted a twofold increment of the NR2B subunit expression in wild-type mice and this result was expected based on the electrophysiological results that we described here for the same group: the suppression of LTD, the higher probability of LTP induction, and the higher NMDAR-mediated response.

Sensory information entering the visual cortex modifies the NR2A/NR2B ratio through metaplastic processes that depend upon experience-related neural activity (Yashiro and Philpot, 2008). NMDAR activation during synaptic stimulation appears to facilitate transcription of NR2A and suppress translation of NR2B (Hoffmann et al., 2000; Yashiro and Philpot, 2008), so dark-reared animals present higher expression of NR2B. Cellular mechanisms underlying such changes are still unclear, although it has been proposed the PKA/CREB pathway could mediate the activity-dependent increases of NR2A subunit expression (Yashiro and Philpot, 2008). In our data, visual deprivation combined with genetic ablation of NOX2 produces a significant increase in NR2B labeling. This is consistent with our electrophysiological data because the NMDAR-mediated postsynaptic responses of the gp91phox−/− animals were stronger after visual deprivation. In the same direction, the contribution of NR2B component in the NMDA-mediated EPSPs of gp91phox−/− and wild-type animals was increased after dark rearing.

Our results suggest that ROS production is important to promote synaptic strengthening. Accordingly, emerging evidence indicates a key role of ROS in enhancing the activity of several protein kinases involved in the induction and maintenance of LTP. Genetic or pharmacological manipulations that reduce the generation of ROS affect LTP negatively in multiple structures of the nervous system, suggesting that appropriated redox states are crucial for synaptic potentiation (Gahtan et al., 1998; Klann et al., 1998; Klann, 1998; Kishida et al., 2006; Gao et al., 2007; De Pasquale et al., 2014; Ji et al., 2015). ROS generation via NOX2 is required for NMDAR-dependent stimulation of several proteins required for LTP in several brain structures, including the visual cortex (Di Cristo et al., 2001; Frankland et al., 2001; Knapp and Klann, 2002; Kishida et al., 2005; Giovannini, 2006; Massaad and Klann, 2011). Furthermore, generation of ROS inhibits the function of K+ channels and enhances the levels of intracellular Ca2+, facilitating the induction of LTP (Marengo et al., 1998; Hidalgo, 2005; Nazıroğlu, 2011; Lee et al., 2013; Sahoo et al., 2014). Specific situations that might lead to differential levels of neuronal ROS for metaplastic purposes are still unclear because investigations about redox signaling in physiological mechanisms are relatively recent. However, there is some evidence suggesting a possible role of ROS in the maturation of the visual cortical circuitry.

In the visual cortex, early age is a period of high excitatory activity, intense synaptic strengthening, and the formation of new neural connections (Kirkwood and Bear, 1995; Morales et al., 2002; Yoshimura et al., 2003; Tau and Peterson, 2010; Lehmann et al., 2012). The probability of inducing LTP is particularly high within the first 3 weeks and progressively decreases with aging (Yoshimura et al., 2003; De Pasquale et al., 2014). Glutamatergic stimulation of excitatory synapses increases intracellular and extracellular levels of ROS mediated by NOX2 (Brennan et al., 2009; Guemez-Gamboa et al., 2011; Reyes et al., 2012), suggesting that redox signaling could be a mechanism supporting LTP in neurons sharing a recent history of intense neural activity. Consistent with this view, cortical neurons are known to produce high levels of ROS in young brains and the NOX2 complex is an important source involved in this process (Tsatmali et al., 2005; Dickinson et al., 2011; Wilhelm et al., 2016). Superoxide produced via NOX2 undergoes fast transmutation to hydrogen peroxide, which can act as an autocrine and paracrine signal (Covarrubias et al., 2008). Hydrogen peroxide diffusion through membranes may determine the propagation of redox signals to a population of neurons, coordinating developmental events involved in the formation of neuronal circuitries (Coyoy et al., 2013). The fact that gp91phox−/− animals exhibit an anticipated and more severe age-dependent decline of visual cortical LTP supports this hypothesis (De Pasquale et al., 2014).

Synaptic stimulation in layer 2/3 of the visual cortex may result in two contrasting metaplastic processes: neuronal activity may favor LTP through increased production of ROS, but at the same time, experience-derived synaptic activation may promote the expression of the NR2A subunit, limiting the occurrence of synaptic potentiation. This means that the effects of dark rearing on plasticity may depend on the general state of neuronal activity, which may not be a simple reflection of visual stimulation. When feedforward inputs from layer 4 are silenced by visual deprivation, neurons of layer 2/3 are still considerably stimulated by intracortical information coming from nonvisual brain areas (Goel et al., 2006; Petrus et al., 2015; Burkhalter, 2016; Pafundo et al., 2016). In this context, the effects of dark rearing would depend on the general state of neuronal activity. When the overall excitatory activity produces considerable amounts of ROS, visual deprivation upregulates the LTP mechanisms to optimize the dynamic range for synaptic modification. Despite this, if reduction of synaptic activity is more general and not restricted to visual information flow, then LTD becomes the predominant form of plasticity. Therefore, the reduced visual stimulation associated with ROS deficiency would favor synaptic elimination rather than reinforcement.

In conclusion, we found that NOX2 activation and visual experience can interact to regulate plasticity and this effect is mediated via changes in the NMDAR functioning. Dark rearing facilitates synaptic potentiation, but ROS production is required for this outcome. If ROS cellular levels are low, then synaptic depression becomes the main form of synaptic modification. Together, our findings suggest that NOX2-derived ROS could be important modulators of cortical neuroplasticity, possibly acting as an activity-dependent factor of positive synaptic selection. This process could be a potential mechanism regulating the maintenance/elimination of neural connections based on the history of neural activity.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by grant 2012/07522-0, grant 2011/23874-0 and grant 2016/2584-0, São Paulo Research Foundation (FAPESP).

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Roberto De Pasquale, Universidade de São Paulo, Sala 239, Departamento de Fisiologia e Biofísica, Av. Prof. Lineu Prestes, 1524, ICB, Cidade Universitária, Butantã, São Paulo, SP 05508-000. robertode{at}usp.br

References

  1. ↵
    1. Abraham WC,
    2. Bear MF
    (1996) Metaplasticity: the plasticity of synaptic plasticity. Trends Neurosci 19:126–130. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(96)80018-X pmid:8658594
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Atkins CM,
    2. Sweatt JD
    (1999) Reactive oxygen species mediate activity-dependent neuron-glia signaling in output fibers of the hippocampus. J Neurosci 19:7241–7248. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-17-07241.1999 pmid:10460230
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    1. Bear MF
    (2003) Bidirectional synaptic plasticity: from theory to reality. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 358:649–655. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1255 pmid:12740110
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Beckhauser TF,
    2. Francis-Oliveira J,
    3. De Pasquale R
    (2016) Reactive oxygen species: physiological and physiopathological effects on synaptic plasticity. J Exp Neurosci 10:23–48. doi:10.4137/JEN.S39887 pmid:27625575
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Bienenstock EL,
    2. Cooper LN,
    3. Munro PW
    (1982) Theory for the development of neuron selectivity: orientation specificity and binocular interaction in visual cortex. J Neurosci 2:32–48. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.02-01-00032.1982 pmid:7054394
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Bindokas VP,
    2. Jordán J,
    3. Lee CC,
    4. Miller RJ
    (1996) Superoxide production in rat hippocampal neurons: selective imaging with hydroethidine. J Neurosci 16:1324–1336. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-04-01324.1996 pmid:8778284
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    1. Bodhinathan K,
    2. Kumar A,
    3. Foster TC
    (2010) Intracellular redox state alters NMDA receptor response during aging through Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II. J Neurosci 30:1914–1924. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5485-09.2010 pmid:20130200
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Brennan AM,
    2. Suh SW,
    3. Won SJ,
    4. Narasimhan P,
    5. Kauppinen TM,
    6. Lee H,
    7. Edling Y,
    8. Chan PH,
    9. Swanson RA
    (2009) NADPH oxidase is the primary source of superoxide induced by NMDA receptor activation. Nat Neurosci 12:857–863. doi:10.1038/nn.2334 pmid:19503084
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Burkhalter A
    (2016) The network for intracortical communication in mouse visual cortex. In: Micro-, meso- and macro-connectomics of the brain (Kennedy H, Van Essen DC, Christen Y, eds), pp 31–43. Cham, Switzerland:Springer.
  10. ↵
    1. Calero CI,
    2. Vickers E,
    3. Moraga Cid G,
    4. Aguayo LG,
    5. von Gersdorff H,
    6. Calvo DJ
    (2011) Allosteric modulation of retinal GABA receptors by ascorbic acid. J Neurosci 31:9672–9682. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5157-10.2011 pmid:21715633
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Chen WS,
    2. Bear MF
    (2007) Activity-dependent regulation of NR2B translation contributes to metaplasticity in mouse visual cortex. Neuropharmacology 52:200–214. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2006.07.003 pmid:16895734
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Chiarugi P
    (2005) PTPs versus PTKs: the redox side of the coin. Free Radic Res 39:353–364. doi:10.1080/10715760400027987 pmid:16028361
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Choi YB,
    2. Lipton SA
    (2000) Redox modulation of the NMDA receptor. Cell Mol Life Sci 57:1535–1541. doi:10.1007/PL00000638 pmid:11092448
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Covarrubias L,
    2. Hernández-García D,
    3. Schnabel D,
    4. Salas-Vidal E,
    5. Castro-Obregón S
    (2008) Function of reactive oxygen species during animal development: passive or active? Dev Biol 320:1–11. doi:10.1016/j.ydbio.2008.04.041 pmid:18555213
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Coyoy A,
    2. Olguín-Albuerne M,
    3. Martínez-Briseño P,
    4. Morán J
    (2013) Role of reactive oxygen species and NADPH-oxidase in the development of rat cerebellum. Neurochem Int 62:998–1011. doi:10.1016/j.neuint.2013.03.009 pmid:23535068
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Czepita D,
    2. Reid SN,
    3. Daw NW
    (1994) Effect of longer periods of dark rearing on NMDA receptors in cat visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 72:1220–1226. doi:10.1152/jn.1994.72.3.1220 pmid:7807206
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. De Pasquale R,
    2. Beckhauser TF,
    3. Hernandes MS,
    4. Giorgetti Britto LR
    (2014) LTP and LTD in the visual cortex require the activation of NOX2. J Neurosci 34:12778–12787. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1414-14.2014 pmid:25232114
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Di Cristo G,
    2. Berardi N,
    3. Cancedda L,
    4. Pizzorusso T,
    5. Putignano E,
    6. Ratto GM,
    7. Maffei L
    (2001) Requirement of ERK activation for visual cortical plasticity. Science 292:2337–2340. doi:10.1126/science.1059075 pmid:11423664
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Dickinson BC,
    2. Peltier J,
    3. Stone D,
    4. Schaffer DV,
    5. Chang CJ
    (2011) Nox2 redox signaling maintains essential cell populations in the brain. Nat Chem Biol 7:106–112. doi:10.1038/nchembio.497 pmid:21186346
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Erreger K,
    2. Dravid SM,
    3. Banke TG,
    4. Wyllie DJ,
    5. Traynelis SF
    (2005) Subunit-specific gating controls rat NR1/NR2A and NR1/NR2B NMDA channel kinetics and synaptic signalling profiles. J Physiol 563:345–358. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2004.080028 pmid:15649985
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Frankland PW,
    2. O'Brien C,
    3. Ohno M,
    4. Kirkwood A,
    5. Silva AJ
    (2001) Alpha-CaMKII-dependent plasticity in the cortex is required for permanent memory. Nature 411:309–313. doi:10.1038/35077089 pmid:11357133
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Gahtan E,
    2. Auerbach JM,
    3. Groner Y,
    4. Segal M
    (1998) Reversible impairment of long-term potentiation in transgenic Cu/Zn-SOD mice. Eur J Neurosci 10:538–544. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00058.x pmid:9749716
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Gao X,
    2. Kim HK,
    3. Chung JM,
    4. Chung K
    (2007) Reactive oxygen species (ROS) are involved in enhancement of NMDA-receptor phosphorylation in animal models of pain. Pain 131:262–271. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2007.01.011 pmid:17317010
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Giovannini MG
    (2006) The role of the extracellular signal-regulated kinase pathway in memory encoding. Rev Neurosci 17:619–634. pmid:17283607
    OpenUrlPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Girouard H,
    2. Wang G,
    3. Gallo EF,
    4. Anrather J,
    5. Zhou P,
    6. Pickel VM,
    7. Iadecola C
    (2009) NMDA receptor activation increases free radical production through nitric oxide and NOX2. J Neurosci 29:2545–2552. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0133-09.2009 pmid:19244529
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    1. Goel A,
    2. Jiang B,
    3. Xu LW,
    4. Song L,
    5. Kirkwood A,
    6. Lee HK
    (2006) Cross-modal regulation of synaptic AMPA receptors in primary sensory cortices by visual experience. Nat Neurosci 9:1001–1003. doi:10.1038/nn1725 pmid:16819524
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Guemez-Gamboa A,
    2. Estrada-Sánchez AM,
    3. Montiel T,
    4. Páramo B,
    5. Massieu L,
    6. Morán J
    (2011) Activation of NOX2 by the stimulation of ionotropic and metabotropic glutamate receptors contributes to glutamate neurotoxicity in vivo through the production of reactive oxygen species and calpain activation. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 70:1020–1035. doi:10.1097/NEN.0b013e3182358e4e pmid:22002428
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Guo Y,
    2. Huang S,
    3. de Pasquale R,
    4. McGehrin K,
    5. Lee HK,
    6. Zhao K,
    7. Kirkwood A
    (2012) Dark exposure extends the integration window for spike-timing-dependent plasticity. J Neurosci 32:15027–15035. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2545-12.2012 pmid:23100424
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Halliwell B
    (1992) Reactive oxygen species and the central nervous system. J Neurochem 59:1609–1623. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.1992.tb10990.x pmid:1402908
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Hernandes MS,
    2. Santos GD,
    3. Café-Mendes CC,
    4. Lima LS,
    5. Scavone C,
    6. Munhoz CD,
    7. Britto LR
    (2013) Microglial cells are involved in the susceptibility of NADPHoxidase knockout mice to 6-hydroxy-dopamine-induced neurodegeneration. PLoS One 8:e75532. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0075532 pmid:24086556
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Hidalgo C
    (2005) Cross talk between Ca2+ and redox signalling cascades in muscle and neurons through the combined activation of ryanodine receptors/Ca2+ release channels. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 360:2237–2246. doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1759 pmid:16321793
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Hidalgo C,
    2. Donoso P
    (2008) Crosstalk between calcium and redox signaling: from molecular mechanisms to health implications. Antioxid Redox Signal 10:1275–1312. doi:10.1089/ars.2007.1886 pmid:18377233
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Hoffmann H,
    2. Gremme T,
    3. Hatt H,
    4. Gottmann K
    (2000) Synaptic activity-dependent developmental regulation of NMDA receptor subunit expression in cultured neocortical neurons. J Neurochem 75:1590–1599. pmid:10987840
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Ji G,
    2. Li Z,
    3. Neugebauer V
    (2015) Reactive oxygen species mediate visceral pain-related amygdala plasticity and behaviors. Pain 156:825–836. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000120 pmid:25734993
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Jiang B,
    2. Treviño M,
    3. Kirkwood A
    (2007) Sequential development of long-term potentiation and depression in different layers of the mouse visual cortex. J Neurosci 27:9648–9652. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2655-07.2007 pmid:17804625
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    1. Kim MJ,
    2. Shin KS,
    3. Chung YB,
    4. Jung KW,
    5. Cha CI,
    6. Shin DH
    (2005) Immunohistochemical study of p47Phox and gp91Phox distributions in rat brain. Brain Res 1040:178–186. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2005.01.066 pmid:15804439
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Kirkwood A,
    2. Bear MF
    (1995) Elementary forms of synaptic plasticity in the visual cortex. Biol Res 28:73–80. pmid:8728822
    OpenUrlPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Kirkwood A,
    2. Lee HK,
    3. Bear MF
    (1995) Co-regulation of long-term potentiation and experience-dependent synaptic plasticity in visual cortex by age and experience. Nature 375:328–331. doi:10.1038/375328a0 pmid:7753198
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Kirkwood A,
    2. Rioult MC,
    3. Bear MF
    (1996) Experience-dependent modification of synaptic plasticity in visual cortex. Nature 381:526–528. doi:10.1038/381526a0 pmid:8632826
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Kishida KT,
    2. Pao M,
    3. Holland SM,
    4. Klann E
    (2005) NADPH oxidase is required for NMDA receptor-dependent activation of ERK in hippocampal area CA1. J Neurochem 94:299–306. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2005.03189.x pmid:15998281
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Kishida KT,
    2. Hoeffer CA,
    3. Hu D,
    4. Pao M,
    5. Holland SM,
    6. Klann E
    (2006) Synaptic plasticity deficits and mild memory impairments in mouse models of chronic granulomatous disease. Mol Cell Biol 26:5908–5920. doi:10.1128/MCB.00269-06 pmid:16847341
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  42. ↵
    1. Klann E
    (1998) Cell-permeable scavengers of superoxide prevent long-term potentiation in hippocampal area CA1. J Neurophysiol 80:452–457. doi:10.1152/jn.1998.80.1.452 pmid:9658063
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Klann E,
    2. Roberson ED,
    3. Knapp LT,
    4. Sweatt JD
    (1998) A role for superoxide in protein kinase C activation and induction of long-term potentiation. J Biol Chem 273:4516–4522. doi:10.1074/jbc.273.8.4516 pmid:9468506
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    1. Knapp LT,
    2. Klann E
    (2002) Potentiation of hippocampal synaptic transmission by superoxide requires the oxidative activation of protein kinase C. J Neurosci 22:674–683. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-03-00674.2002 pmid:11826097
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    1. Lee CR,
    2. Machold RP,
    3. Witkovsky P,
    4. Rice ME
    (2013) TRPM2 channels are required for NMDA-induced burst firing and contribute to H(2)O(2)-dependent modulation in substantia nigra pars reticulata GABAergic neurons. J Neurosci 33:1157–1168. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2832-12.2013 pmid:23325252
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  46. ↵
    1. Lehmann K,
    2. Schmidt KF,
    3. Löwel S
    (2012) Vision and visual plasticity in ageing mice. Restor Neurol Neurosci 30:161–178. doi:10.3233/RNN-2012-110192 pmid:22348872
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Li YH,
    2. Wang J
    (2013) Membrane insertion of new AMPA receptors and LTP induced by glycine is prevented by blocking NR2A-containing NMDA receptors in the rat visual cortex in vitro. Curr Neurovasc Res 10:70–75. doi:10.2174/156720213804805927 pmid:23151080
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Marengo JJ,
    2. Hidalgo C,
    3. Bull R
    (1998) Sulfhydryl oxidation modifies the calcium dependence of ryanodine-sensitive calcium channels of excitable cells. Biophys J 74:1263–1277. doi:10.1016/S0006-3495(98)77840-3 pmid:9512024
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Massaad CA,
    2. Klann E
    (2011) Reactive oxygen species in the regulation of synaptic plasticity and memory. Antioxid Redox Signal 14:2013–2054. doi:10.1089/ars.2010.3208 pmid:20649473
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Morales B,
    2. Choi SY,
    3. Kirkwood A
    (2002) Dark rearing alters the development of GABAergic transmission in visual cortex. J Neurosci 22:8084–8090. pmid:12223562
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  51. ↵
    1. Nazıroğlu M
    (2011) TRPM2 cation channels, oxidative stress and neurological diseases: where are we now? Neurochem Res 36:355–366. doi:10.1007/s11064-010-0347-4 pmid:21140288
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Pafundo DE,
    2. Nicholas MA,
    3. Zhang R,
    4. Kuhlman SJ
    (2016) Top-down-mediated facilitation in the visual cortex is gated by subcortical neuromodulation. J Neurosci 36:2904–2914. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2909-15.2016 pmid:26961946
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  53. ↵
    1. Petrus E,
    2. Rodriguez G,
    3. Patterson R,
    4. Connor B,
    5. Kanold PO,
    6. Lee HK
    (2015) Vision loss shifts the balance of feedforward and intracortical circuits in opposite directions in mouse primary auditory and visual cortices. J Neurosci 35:8790–8801. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4975-14.2015 pmid:26063913
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  54. ↵
    1. Philpot BD,
    2. Espinosa JS,
    3. Bear MF
    (2003) Evidence for altered NMDA receptor function as a basis for metaplasticity in visual cortex. J Neurosci 23:5583–5588. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-13-05583.2003 pmid:12843259
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  55. ↵
    1. Philpot BD,
    2. Cho KK,
    3. Bear MF
    (2007) Obligatory role of NR2A for metaplasticity in visual cortex. Neuron 53:495–502. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.01.027 pmid:17296552
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Quinlan EM,
    2. Olstein DH,
    3. Bear MF
    (1999) Bidirectional, experience-dependent regulation of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor subunit composition in the rat visual cortex during postnatal development. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96:12876–12880. doi:10.1073/pnas.96.22.12876 pmid:10536016
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    1. Reyes RC,
    2. Brennan AM,
    3. Shen Y,
    4. Baldwin Y,
    5. Swanson RA
    (2012) Activation of neuronal NMDA receptors induces superoxide-mediated oxidative stress in neighboring neurons and astrocytes. J Neurosci 32:12973–12978. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1597-12.2012 pmid:22973021
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. ↵
    1. Rodríguez-Muñoz M,
    2. Garzón J
    (2013) Nitric oxide and zinc-mediated protein assemblies involved in mu opioid receptor signaling. Mol Neurobiol 48:769–782. doi:10.1007/s12035-013-8465-z pmid:23666425
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Sahoo N,
    2. Hoshi T,
    3. Heinemann SH
    (2014) Oxidative modulation of voltage-gated potassium channels. Antioxid Redox Signal 21:933–952. doi:10.1089/ars.2013.5614 pmid:24040918
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Serrano F,
    2. Kolluri NS,
    3. Wientjes FB,
    4. Card JP,
    5. Klann E
    (2003) NADPH oxidase immunoreactivity in the mouse brain. Brain Res 988:193–198. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(03)03364-X pmid:14519542
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Smith GB,
    2. Heynen AJ,
    3. Bear MF
    (2009) Bidirectional synaptic mechanisms of ocular dominance plasticity in visual cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364:357–367. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0198 pmid:18977732
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  62. ↵
    1. Sobczyk A,
    2. Scheuss V,
    3. Svoboda K
    (2005) NMDA receptor subunit-dependent [Ca2+] signaling in individual hippocampal dendritic spines. J Neurosci 25:6037–6046. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1221-05.2005 pmid:15987933
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  63. ↵
    1. Sorce S,
    2. Krause KH
    (2009) NOX enzymes in the central nervous system: from signaling to disease. Antioxid Redox Signal 11:2481–2504. doi:10.1089/ars.2009.2578 pmid:19309263
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Sorce S,
    2. Schiavone S,
    3. Tucci P,
    4. Colaianna M,
    5. Jaquet V,
    6. Cuomo V,
    7. Dubois-Dauphin M,
    8. Trabace L,
    9. Krause KH
    (2010) The NADPH oxidase NOX2 controls glutamate release: a novel mechanism involved in psychosis-like ketamine responses. J Neurosci 30:11317–11325. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1491-10.2010 pmid:20739552
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  65. ↵
    1. Tau GZ,
    2. Peterson BS
    (2010) Normal development of brain circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology 35:147–168. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.115 pmid:3055433
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Tejada-Simon MV,
    2. Serrano F,
    3. Villasana LE,
    4. Kanterewicz BI,
    5. Wu GY,
    6. Quinn MT,
    7. Klann E
    (2005) Synaptic localization of a functional NADPH oxidase in the mouse hippocampus. Mol Cell Neurosci 29:97–106. doi:10.1016/j.mcn.2005.01.007 pmid:15866050
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Tsatmali M1,
    2. Walcott EC,
    3. Crossin KL
    (2005) Newborn neurons acquire high levels of reactive oxygen species and increased mitochondrial proteins upon differentiation from progenitors. Brain Res 1040(1–2):137–150. pmid:15804435
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Wilhelm J,
    2. Vytášek R,
    3. Uhlík J,
    4. Vajner L
    (2016) Oxidative Stress in the Developing Rat Brain due to Production of Reactive Oxygen and Nitrogen Species. Oxid Med Cell Longev 2016:1–12. doi:10.1155/2016/5057610
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  69. ↵
    1. Yang YJ,
    2. Wu PF,
    3. Long LH,
    4. Yu DF,
    5. Wu WN,
    6. Hu ZL,
    7. Fu H,
    8. Xie N,
    9. Jin Y,
    10. Ni L,
    11. Wang JZ,
    12. Wang F,
    13. Chen JG
    (2010) Reversal of aging-associated hippocampal synaptic plasticity deficits by reductants via regulation of thiol redox and NMDA receptor function. Aging Cell 9:709–721. doi:10.1111/j.1474-9726.2010.00595.x pmid:20569240
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Yashiro K,
    2. Philpot BD
    (2008) Regulation of NMDA receptor subunit expression and its implications for LTD, LTP, and metaplasticity. Neuropharmacology 55:1081–1094. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.07.046 pmid:18755202
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Yermolaieva O,
    2. Brot N,
    3. Weissbach H,
    4. Heinemann SH,
    5. Hoshi T
    (2000) Reactive oxygen species and nitric oxide mediate plasticity of neuronal calcium signaling. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:448–453. doi:10.1073/pnas.97.1.448 pmid:10618438
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  72. ↵
    1. Yoshimura Y,
    2. Ohmura T,
    3. Komatsu Y
    (2003) Two forms of synaptic plasticity with distinct dependence on age, experience, and NMDA receptor subtype in rat visual cortex. J Neurosci 23:6557–6566. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-16-06557.2003 pmid:12878697
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
View Abstract
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 38 (25)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 38, Issue 25
20 Jun 2018
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Metaplasticity in the Visual Cortex: Crosstalk Between Visual Experience and Reactive Oxygen Species
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Article Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Citation Tools
Metaplasticity in the Visual Cortex: Crosstalk Between Visual Experience and Reactive Oxygen Species
José Francis-Oliveira, Guilherme S. Vilar Higa, Lívia Mendonça Munhoz Dati, Ianê Carvalho Shieh, Roberto De Pasquale
Journal of Neuroscience 20 June 2018, 38 (25) 5649-5665; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2617-17.2018

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Metaplasticity in the Visual Cortex: Crosstalk Between Visual Experience and Reactive Oxygen Species
José Francis-Oliveira, Guilherme S. Vilar Higa, Lívia Mendonça Munhoz Dati, Ianê Carvalho Shieh, Roberto De Pasquale
Journal of Neuroscience 20 June 2018, 38 (25) 5649-5665; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2617-17.2018
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • dark rearing
  • metaplasticity
  • NMDA receptor
  • NOX2
  • ROS
  • visual cortex

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Top-Down Feedback Controls the Cortical Representation of Illusory Contours in Mouse Primary Visual Cortex
  • Serotonergic plasticity in the dorsal raphe nucleus characterizes susceptibility and resilience to anhedonia
  • Stimulation augments spike sequence replay and memory consolidation during slow-wave sleep
Show more Research Articles

Development/Plasticity/Repair

  • Serotonergic plasticity in the dorsal raphe nucleus characterizes susceptibility and resilience to anhedonia
  • p75NTR and DR6 Regulate Distinct Phases of Axon Degeneration Demarcated by Spheroid Rupture
  • Isolation of LMX1a Ventral Midbrain Progenitors Improves the Safety and Predictability of Human Pluripotent Stem Cell-Derived Neural Transplants in Parkinsonian Disease
Show more Development/Plasticity/Repair
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2019 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci   Print ISSN: 0270-6474   Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.