Abstract
Sensory systems do not develop and function independently of one another, yet they are typically studied in isolation. Effects of multisensory interactions on the developing neocortex can be revealed by altering the ratios of incoming sensory inputs associated with different modalities. We investigated neural responses in primary somatosensory cortex (S1) of short-tailed opossums (Monodelphis domestica; either sex) after the elimination of visual input through bilateral enucleation very early in development. To assess the influence of tactile experience after vision loss, we also examined naturally occurring patterns of exploratory behavior. In early blind (EB) animals, overall levels of tactile experience were similar to those of sighted controls (SC); locomotor activity was unimpaired and accompanied by whisking. Using extracellular single-unit recording techniques under anesthesia, we found that EB animals exhibited a reduction in the magnitude of neural responses to whisker stimuli in S1, coupled with spatial sharpening of receptive fields, in comparison to SC animals. These alterations manifested as two different effects on sensory processing in S1 of EB animals: the ability of neurons to detect single whisker stimulation was decreased, whereas their ability to discriminate between stimulation of neighboring whiskers was enhanced. The increased selectivity of S1 neurons in EB animals was reflected in improved population decoding performance for whisker stimulus position, particularly along the rostrocaudal axis of the snout, which aligns with the primary axis of natural whisker motion. These findings suggest that a functionally distinct form of somatosensory plasticity occurs when vision is lost early in development.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT After sensory loss, compensatory behavior mediated through the spared senses could be generated entirely through the recruitment of brain areas associated with the deprived sense. Alternatively, functional compensation in spared modalities may be achieved through a combination of plasticity in brain areas corresponding to both spared and deprived sensory modalities. Although activation of neurons in cortex associated with a deprived sense has been described frequently, it is unclear whether this is the only substrate available for compensation or if plasticity within cortical fields corresponding to spared modalities, particularly primary sensory cortices, may also contribute. Here, we demonstrate empirically that early loss of vision alters coding of sensory inputs in primary somatosensory cortex in a manner that supports enhanced tactile discrimination.
Introduction
The developing nervous system is highly sensitive to the relative levels and patterns of incoming sensory inputs, not only within, but also across sensory modalities. Numerous studies in humans (for review, see Bavelier and Neville, 2002; Renier et al., 2014; Ricciardi et al., 2014) and other mammals (Karlen et al., 2009; for review, see Butler and Lomber, 2013) have reported the phenomenon of crossmodal plasticity in which cortical areas associated with a deprived sensory modality become responsive to stimuli associated with spared modalities. These effects are observed, to varying degrees, after different forms of sensory loss including congenital abnormalities (Hunt et al., 2006; Chabot et al., 2007), peripheral lesions (Izraeli et al., 2002; Kahn and Krubitzer, 2002; Meredith et al., 2011), decreased sensory-driven activity (Rauschecker and Korte, 1993; Rauschecker, 1996), or naturally occurring loss of vision during evolution (e.g., blind mole rat; Bronchti et al., 2002; Sadka and Wollberg, 2004). Further, behavioral evidence from congenitally blind or deaf mammals, including humans, indicates that, after early sensory loss, animals perform better on tasks mediated by spared sensory systems (Toldi et al., 1994; Rauschecker and Kniepert, 1994; Lomber et al., 2010; for review, see Bavelier and Neville, 2002; Ricciardi et al., 2014).
The majority of these studies have focused on the deprived cortex as the main substrate for functional and behavioral compensation that accompanies early sensory loss. However, an increase in the cortical territory available for processing information in the spared senses via recruitment of the deprived cortex is not necessarily the only mechanism for sensory compensation. Instead, compensation may involve some combination of a takeover of the deprived cortex by spared sensory systems as well as a reorganization of cortical and subcortical structures associated with the spared sensory modalities. However, studies examining the effects of early loss on cortical areas associated with the spared senses are limited, particularly with regard to primary sensory cortices. Rauschecker et al. (1992) reported an expansion of the barrel fields in S1 in neonatally enucleated mice, as determined by cytochrome oxidase staining. A previous study from our laboratory (Kahn and Krubitzer, 2002) found that neurons at some recording sites in primary somatosensory cortex (S1) and primary auditory cortex (A1) were responsive to both somatosensory and auditory stimuli in animals that were bilaterally enucleated very early in development. Beyond this, the effects of early and chronic unimodal sensory loss on neural processing in the spared senses are not well understood.
The goal of the current study was to test whether there were basic differences in tactile processing in S1 of animals that experienced early vision loss (early blind, EB) compared with sighted control (SC) animals. We did this by examining extracellular single-unit activity in response to a simple tactile stimulus, the deflection of a single whisker. Short-tailed opossums are ideal for studies of the consequences of early sensory loss on cortical function because their nervous systems are highly immature at birth, allowing for targeted extrauterine manipulations at much earlier developmental stages than most commonly used mammalian models. Both the visual and somatosensory systems are well developed in adult short-tailed opossums; primary visual cortex (V1) is the primary sensory area that occupies the largest fraction of the neocortex, followed by S1 (Huffman et al., 1999; Kahn et al., 2000). Further, short-tailed opossums exhibit specialized tactile behavior in the form of whisking, a rhythmic and stereotypic back-and-forth motion of the facial whiskers during spatial exploration and navigation (Mitchinson et al., 2011; Grant et al., 2013). Thus, in the absence of one dominant sense (vision), one might expect neural and behavioral shifts toward the other dominant sense (touch). Recently, a study from our laboratory demonstrated reorganization of both thalamic and cortical projections to S1 in bilaterally enucleated animals (J. C. Dooley and L.A. Krubitzer, unpublished observations), in addition to the reorganization of projections to visual cortical areas (Karlen et al., 2006). In light of these observations, we were particularly interested in investigating whether plasticity after early blindness has implications for neural coding in S1.
Materials and Methods
Animals.
A total of 44 short-tailed opossums (Monodelphis domestica) of either sex (SC: 11 females, 11 males; EB: 10 females, 12 males) derived from 13 litters (from 11 mothers) were used for data collection across all behavioral and electrophysiological testing conducted in the current study (see Tables 1 and 2 for details regarding subject information). Extracellular electrophysiological recording techniques were used in 19 adult (4–16 months) animals to examine the response properties and receptive field characteristics for neurons in S1. Nine animals (4 females, 5 males) were bilaterally enucleated on postnatal day 4 (P4) and are referred to as 'early blind' (EB), and 10 animals (5 females, 5 males) served as 'sighted controls' (SC). Portions of electrophysiological data acquired from three SC animals were reported in a previous publication (Ramamurthy and Krubitzer, 2016). All animals were housed in standard laboratory cages and maintained on a 14/10 h light/dark cycle. Food and water were available ad libitum in the home cage. To determine whether enucleation affected opossum behavior, we examined locomotor activity and whisking behavior in SC and EB animals (see below). Protocols were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of California–Davis and experiments were performed according to the criteria outlined in the National Institutes of Health's Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.
Bilateral enucleation.
Bilateral enucleations were performed on short-tailed opossum pups at P4. Because the pups are fused to the mother's nipples until ~P14, the procedure was performed after lightly anesthetizing the mother with Alfaxan (20 mg/kg, IM). Maintenance doses (10–50%) were administered as needed to ensure that the mother remained immobile. Pups were anesthetized by hypothermia through contact with a rubber pouch filled with ice and each eye was removed manually under microscopic guidance. After enucleation, the skin around the eye was repositioned to cover the eye socket and sealed with surgical glue. This entire procedure for both eyes lasted for 3–4 min per animal. Approximately half of each litter was bilaterally enucleated to generate EB group animals and the remaining half served as the SC group. Respiration rate and body temperature of the mother and heartbeat, coloration, and mobility of the pups were monitored throughout the enucleation procedure. Following enucleations, the pups were allowed to rewarm and once the pups and mother were fully recovered, they were returned to the home cage.
Behavioral data collection and analysis.
Animals from three age ranges were used for behavioral data collection: preweaning juvenile (P36–P56), postweaning juvenile (P56–P76), and adult (4–16 months) animals. Qualitative assessments of whisking behavior were made for all animals included in the study. In some cases, high-speed video of whisking was captured using a Casio EX-F1 digital video camera (512 × 384 pixels at 300 frames/s) or an Apple iPhone 6s camera (1280 × 720 pixels at 240 frames/s). Movies of whisking were uniformly adjusted for brightness, contrast, and color balance using VLC media player version 2.2.6 (VideoLAN). Presence or absence of whisking was scored by two independent observers on a binary scale as absent or present using a modification of previously used criteria (Landers and Philip Zeigler, 2006) where “absent” refers to no whisking or small, aperiodic whisker movements and “present” refers to low frequency/low amplitude whisking or normal whisking. Whiskers were also tracked in high-speed video clips (mean frames per clip: SC = 248 ± 34.3 frames; EB = 330 ± 66.4 frames) using the Manual Whisker Annotator (Hewitt et al., 2016) to obtain measurements of whisking frequency (number of whisk cycles per second). To assess the overlap between whisking behavior and locomotor activity, individual animals were monitored for 10 epochs of 30 s each in their home cage, followed by 10 epochs of 30 s each after transfer to a novel cage containing unfamiliar toys (Bio-Serv). Overlap between whisking (W+ = 1, W− = 0) and locomotion (L+ = 1, L− = 0) bouts was quantified as the L/W overlap index as follows: An L/W overlap index value of 1.0 indicates complete overlap between locomotion bouts and whisking bouts and a value of 0.0 indicates no overlap between locomotion bouts and whisking bouts.
To assess locomotor activity in EB and SC animals, video recordings were obtained of juvenile and adult animals in their home cages over 24 h periods using an infrared-enabled camcorder (SEREE HDV-501, 30 frames/s). All home cage behavior data were analyzed offline by three independent observers such that, for multiple video segments, the same time windows were analyzed by at least two observers. Interrater reliability for scored videos was quantified using Cohen's κ coefficient (see “Experimental design and statistical analysis” section below). Video coding of home cage behavior was performed offline using BORIS (Behavioral Observation Research Interactive) software. Locomotion was scored using a binary scale of active states (locomotion bouts) and inactive states (resting bouts). A switch between an active and inactive state was only considered to have occurred if a bout lasted for at least 60 s. For juvenile animals, mean activity levels for the EB pups versus SC pups in each litter was obtained because each litter was housed together during the preweaning and postweaning stages and individual pup identity could not be tracked.
Surgical procedures.
Once enucleated and control animals reached adulthood, we used extracellular recording techniques to examine neural response properties in S1. Animals were anesthetized with urethane (1.25 g/kg, 30% in saline, IP) at the beginning of the experiment, with supplemental doses (0.125–0.313 g/kg, 30% in saline, IP) provided as needed. Respiration and body temperature were monitored throughout the experiment. Dexamethasone (0.4–2.0 mg/kg, IM) was given at the beginning of the surgery to prevent intracranial swelling. Lidocaine (2% solution) was injected subcutaneously at the midline of the scalp and around the ears and the animal was placed in a stereotaxic frame. An incision was made at the midline of the scalp, the temporal muscle was retracted, and a craniotomy was made over parietal cortex. The dura was retracted and the surface of the brain was covered with silicone fluid to prevent desiccation. A digital image was obtained of the exposed neocortex so that electrode penetration sites could be related to vascular patterns. A small screw was inserted into the skull over the opposite hemisphere and the head of the animal was stabilized by cementing the screw to a head post with dental acrylic.
Electrophysiological recordings.
Single-unit extracellular recordings were made from layer IV (400–500 μm depth below the pial surface) using insulated tungsten microelectrodes (FHC; 1–5 MΩ at 1 kHz). Electrodes were lowered using a hydraulic microdrive (David Kopf Instruments). The location of each recording site was marked relative to the vascular pattern on a digital image of the cortical surface. Receptive fields were initially rapidly determined using a handheld probe. If receptive fields were found to be located on the mystacial whisker pad (centered within the grid of 16 large whiskers), computer-controlled whisker deflections were used to measure tuning of single neurons quantitatively (see below). Single and multiunit activity was recorded in response to whisker deflections and all recorded data were amplified (gain: 10,000×; A-M Systems Model 1800 Microelectrode AC Amplifier), streamed as continuous voltage traces sampled at 28 kHz (Power1401; Cambridge Electronic Design), and saved for analysis. Neural activity was monitored using a digital oscilloscope and an audio amplifier during the experiment. Raw traces were band-pass filtered (300–3000 Hz) and spike sorting was performed offline to verify isolation of single units. In addition, local field potentials (LFPs) were recorded in over half of the animals (6 SC, 6 EB) in each experimental group that were used for recording single-unit data. In each case, LFP data were acquired intermittently throughout the recording session (total measurements: n = 27 in SC animals, n = 30 in EB animals). LFP recordings were amplified at 1000× gain and band-pass filtered (0.1–100 Hz).
At the end of the recording session, electrolytic microlesions (10 μA for 10 s) were made and fluorescent probes were inserted at specific locations relative to blood vessel landmarks so that electrophysiological recording sites could be related directly to histologically identified cortical field boundaries and specific laminae of the neocortex.
Whisker stimulation.
Receptive fields and response properties were quantified using methods similar to those described previously by us (Ramamurthy and Krubitzer, 2016). Whiskers were deflected using calibrated computer-controlled piezoelectric actuators. Each individual whisker was trimmed to a length of ~15 mm and inserted into a short, lightweight plastic capillary tube glued to a piezoelectric bimorph element (Q220-AY-203YB; Piezo Systems) fixed on a moveable, jointed arm. During stimulus presentation, the whisker pad was observed under a surgical microscope to ensure that the skin or whiskers adjacent to the deflected whisker were not being extraneously stimulated. Stimuli consisted of 2° ramp-hold-return deflections (4 ms ramp, 100 ms hold, 4 ms return; applied 5 mm from the base of the whisker, causing a 1.4 mm forward excursion of the whisker) with an interstimulus interval of 1–2 s. At each recording site, 50–100 trials per stimulus per whisker were collected. To determine receptive fields of neurons, 16 neighboring whiskers on the mystacial pad were individually deflected in a pseudorandom order (in blocks of 3–50 trials), at every recording site.
Histology.
After electrophysiological recording experiments, animals were injected with a lethal overdose of sodium pentobarbital (Beuthanasia; 250 mg/kg IP) and transcardially perfused with 0.9% saline, followed by 2–4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffer, and then 2–4% paraformaldehyde in 10% phosphate-buffered sucrose. After perfusion, the brain was extracted. In some cases, the brain was left intact and sectioned coronally (3 EB, 3 SC). In other cases, the cortex was peeled from the brainstem and thalamus, flattened between glass sides and sectioned tangential to the cortical surface (6 EB, 7 SC).
For tangentially sectioned tissue, the cortical hemispheres were postfixed (1–2 h) in 4% paraformaldehyde in 10% phosphate-buffered sucrose and then left to soak overnight in 30% phosphate-buffered sucrose. The flattened cortical hemispheres were sectioned at 30 μm on a freezing microtome. Tangential sections of the neocortex were processed for myelin (Gallyas, 1979; Dooley et al., 2013). Electrode sites, blood vessels, and fiduciary probes were transposed from high-resolution images obtained during surgery onto a reconstruction of the cortical surface with cortical field boundaries drawn in Adobe Illustrator CS5. Architectonic boundaries of cortical fields were determined using an entire series of sections stained for myelin. Cortical field boundaries were determined for individual sections and then collapsed onto a single reconstruction and matched to electrode penetration sites by aligning blood vessels and probes.
For coronally sectioned tissue, the whole brain was cut at 40 μm, processed for Nissl substance, and used for reconstructing the position of recording sites relative to laminar boundaries. Digital images of cortical sections were obtained using either a Nikon Multiphot system or an Optronics MicroFire digital microscope camera. When necessary, whole images were adjusted for contrast and brightness using Adobe Photoshop CS5.
Neural data analysis.
All raw data files of electrophysiological recordings were processed blind to the experimental group. To compare brain state and anesthetic depth (Friedberg et al., 1999; Castro-Alamancos and Oldford, 2002; Hirata and Castro-Alamancos, 2011; Pagliardini et al., 2012; Self et al., 2014) between the two experimental groups, LFP recordings of baseline activity were analyzed; the power spectrum of the signal for each LFP recording was computed using fast Fourier transform (FFT) analysis. Single units were isolated during the experiment using template-matching procedures in Spike2 (Cambridge Electronic Design; RRID:SCR_000903) and unit isolation was verified offline in Spike2 using principal component analysis. Only neuronal clusters with a stable waveform and firing rate over the course of a session were included in the analysis. Included units were required to have <0.5% refractory period violations (interspike interval <1.5 ms).
Spike times of well-isolated single units were exported to MATLAB (The MathWorks; RRID:SCR_001622) for analysis using custom scripts. Spike trains were aligned to stimulus onset, and peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs) were constructed using a 1 ms bin width. Neural responses were integrated over a 100 ms time window following stimulus onset for all spike count analyses. Analyses were performed only for onset responses. Spontaneous firing rates were measured during an equivalent time window during the prestimulus recording period. A neuron was considered to exhibit a significant evoked response if the response magnitude exceeded the prestimulus spike count by >2 SDs. Only neurons that displayed a significant onset response to at least 1 whisker were included in all subsequent analyses. The best whisker (BW) for a neuron was defined as the whisker that evoked the highest response magnitude. The whiskers at the 8 positions immediately adjacent to the BW are referred to as first-order (1°) surround whiskers (SW) and the whiskers immediately beyond those are referred to as second-order (2°) SWs.
A tuning curve was generated for each neuron after ranking whisker stimuli in order of the magnitude of the spike count following stimulus onset (rank 1–16; BW = rank 1). Tuning curves were then averaged across neurons to generate a mean tuning curve, or normalized to the peak (the BW-evoked response) and then averaged across neurons to generate the mean normalized tuning curve for each group. Relative magnitudes of neural responses to SWs were obtained by dividing absolute SW-evoked response magnitudes by the BW-evoked response. Sparseness (S) was used to quantify neuronal selectivity, as per Vinje and Gallant (2000) as follows: where ri is the spike count in response to the ith whisker stimulus (averaged across trials) and n is the number of whiskers tested. Values of S near 0% indicate a “dense” code and values near 100% indicate a “sparse” code.
Mean 2D somatotopic receptive fields were examined by aligning the response magnitude matrix (in which each element corresponds to the magnitude of the neural response to the deflection of a single whisker) associated with individual neurons such that the BW was at the center of the receptive field in each case. SW-evoked response magnitudes were normalized by dividing by the BW-evoked response magnitude. Matrices corresponding to individual neurons were then averaged across the population to generate the mean 2D receptive field. Contour plots were generated by plotting 50%, 70%, and 90% isolines (response levels relative to the BW) after smoothing the response magnitude matrix by linear interpolation. Smoothed contour lines were used only for 2D visualization of mean receptive fields. All calculations and statistical comparisons related to mean tuning curves were made using raw, unsmoothed values.
The tuning width index for each neuron was calculated as the mean response to stimulation of the first-order SWs divided by the response to stimulation of the BW, with values closer to 1 indicating broader receptive fields. The shape index was calculated as the difference between the row tuning width index (mean neural response to SWs in BW row ÷ BW-evoked response) and the arc tuning width index (mean response to SWs in BW arc ÷ BW-evoked response) divided by their sum. In cases where the sum of the row and arc tuning width indices was zero, the shape index was assigned to be zero. Shape index values range from −1 to 1, with positive values indicating broader tuning along the row compared with the arc and negative values indicating broader tuning along the arc compared with the row.
We examined the ability of single neurons in S1 to detect whisker stimulation and to discriminate between stimulation of different whiskers using signal detection theory (Green and Swets, 1966; Britten et al., 1992). For each neuron, the total number of spikes that occurred in a 100 ms time window after a single whisker deflection was measured (response magnitude, Rmag). Using these single trial spike counts measured on 50 trials (per whisker) a spike count distribution was generated for each whisker that was deflected. The separability of the spike count distributions associated with different whiskers was then measured using receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis. An ROC curve was generated for each pair of spike count distributions being compared, and the area under the ROC curve (AUROC) was used as a measure of detectability or discriminability performance in each case. An AUROC value of 0.5 indicates performance at chance and an AUROC value of 1.0 indicates perfect performance. For discriminability analysis, the spike count distribution associated with BW deflection for a neuron was compared with spike count distributions associated with deflection of SWs. On average, three to four whiskers typically elicit a significant response in any given neuron in S1 of short-tailed opossums (Ramamurthy and Krubitzer, 2016). Therefore, we examined the ability of the neuron to discriminate between the BW and the three most dominant SWs (SW1, SW2, SW3) that elicited the strongest responses in any given neuron. For each neuron, a separate ROC curve was generated for each comparison made (BW vs SW1, BW vs SW2, BW vs SW3), AUROC was measured for each curve, and the mean of the three AUROC values was used as the discrimination index. For detectability analysis, the spike count distribution for a given neuron associated with whisker (BW/SW1/SW2/SW3) deflection was compared with the spontaneous spike count distribution for the same neuron, and the mean AUROC value was used as the detection index for that neuron.
We also examined the performance of neurons in discriminating between multiple whiskers. For each neuron, spike count distributions evoked by each of the four most dominant whiskers were nonparametrically modeled using kernel smoothing density estimation. After modeling spike count distributions by kernel density estimation, a Bayesian classifier with cross-validation was used to determine the performance of an ideal observer in discriminating between the four most dominant whisker stimuli (BW, SW1, SW2, SW3). A uniform prior was assumed (equal probability of different whisker stimuli). Overall discrimination performance was calculated as the percentage of correct classifications.
Decoding accuracy of whisker row (A, B, C, D) or arc identity (0, 1, 2, 3) based on the population of S1 neurons was determined using a minimum distance classifier (Foffani and Moxon, 2004). The decoding model assumed independence in the activity of different neurons. Since neurons were recorded sequentially, a pseudopopulation was generated in each iteration of the decoding analysis by randomly selecting responses of n recorded neurons from trials corresponding to a given whisker identity (n = 50 in each iteration, so population size was matched for SC and EB groups). Thus, each vector consisted of n elements, where each element was the spike count 0–100 ms after stimulus onset for a given neuron in a given iteration. A leave-one-out cross-validation procedure was used. In the training stage, templates were defined by calculating average number of spikes (0–100 ms) over all training-set trials corresponding to a given whisker, resulting in four templates (corresponding to either rows A–D or arcs 0–3). In the testing stage, a single trial of the pseudopopulation response was classified to the row or arc for which the Euclidean distance between the single trial and the template was the least, compared with distances corresponding to the other templates. Euclidean distance corresponding to a given template was calculated as the sum of the squared differences between each element in the single trial and the template. The decoding analysis was repeated for 200 iterations and average performance and distributions of performance across iterations is reported.
Experimental design and statistical analysis.
The current work uses a between-groups study design (i.e., neural response properties of the EB group were compared with those of the SC group) for all comparisons. Statistical analyses were performed in MATLAB version 8.1.0 (The MathWorks; RRID:SCR_001622) and R version 3.2.0 (R Development Core Team, 2015; RRID:SCR_001905). Summary data are given as mean ± SEM unless otherwise indicated, or as median and interquartile range (IQR), reported as median (IQR). The normality of data was assessed by examining q–q plots and by using the Shapiro–Wilk test. If distributions deviated from normal, a nonparametric test such as the Wilcoxon rank-sum test or a permutation test was selected; if no significant deviations were found, two-sample t tests were used. For videos of home cage behavior scored by two independent observers, interrater reliability was quantified using Cohen's kappa coefficient (κ), a statistical measure of the level of agreement in scoring performed by independent observers (Landis and Koch 1977, Hallgren, 2012). We used this measure because it corrects for the level of agreement in scoring that may be expected purely by chance as follows: where Po is the observed percentage agreement and Pe is the percentage agreement expected by chance. κ = 0 indicates no agreement in scoring between observers and κ = 1 indicates perfect agreement. For our data, mean κ across epochs of home cage behavior scored by two independent observers was 0.861 ± 0.0261, indicating a very high level of interrater reliability (Landis and Koch, 1977).
Cohen's U3 was used to assess effect size of between-group differences in population response profiles to whisker stimuli in EB and SC groups and was calculated in MATLAB using the Measures of Effect Size (MES) Toolbox (Hentschke and Stüttgen, 2011; RRID:SCR_014703). Cohen's U3 indicates the proportion of data points in one group that are lower in value than the median value for the other group (Hentschke and Stüttgen, 2011). Therefore, an effect size value >0.5 in any given time bin indicates that the majority of EB neurons have firing rates lower than the median firing rate for SC neurons in that time bin. Cohen's d was reported as the measure of effect size when comparing group means (small: ±0.2, medium: ±0.5, large: ±0.8; MES Toolbox; Hentschke and Stüttgen, 2011).
Fisher's exact test was used to compare proportions for categorical variables. Kolmogorov–Smirnov test was used to compare cumulative distributions. Two-way ANOVAs were used to compare EB and SC tuning curves and EB and SC behavioral data across different age ranges. ANCOVA was used to compare the slopes of regression lines associated with detection and discrimination indices. A nonparametric multivariate ANOVA (MANOVA) was used to check for influences of sex and individual differences on all measures.
For all statistical tests, differences were considered to be significant when p < 0.05. Statistical tests used and exact p-values for each figure are reported in the Results section.
Results
In the current study, we investigated the effects of early loss of vision on the coding of whisker-mediated touch in S1. Major milestones in the development of the visual and whisker systems in short-tailed opossums, and the timing of our experimental manipulations and measurements relative to these events are summarized in Figure 1. P0 in short-tailed opossums is equivalent to embryonic ages in commonly used rodent models–E11 in the mouse and E12 in the rat (Molnár and Blakemore, 1995; Molnár et al., 1998). Bilateral enucleations were performed at P4, before retinal afferents and thalamocortical axons have reached their respective targets (Taylor and Guillery, 1994; Molnár et al., 1998). Thus, both spontaneous activity in the retina and visually evoked activity were completely and unequivocally eliminated. We examined patterns of locomotion and associated whisking behavior across 22 SC and 22 EB animals. Electrophysiological recording experiments were performed in 10 SC animals and 9 EB animals. Across animals, 60/86 recorded units in the SC group and 64/89 recorded units in the EB group exhibited significant responses to whisker stimuli. For all parameters examined (see Materials and Methods), no statistically significant differences were found between sexes or among animals, so all data were combined for analysis. For a breakdown of age range, sex, weight, experimental groups, and parameters measured, see Tables 1 and 2.
Levels of active tactile behavior do not differ between SC and EB opossums
Any differences observed between SC and EB animals in the neural coding of whisker-mediated touch could potentially be linked to differences in tactile behavior, and therefore, differences in the levels of tactile experience between the two groups. It was critical to rule out the possibility that any neural changes that we measured in EB animals were related to a lack of whisker-mediated experience that could conceivably occur if there were deficits in overall locomotor activity in EB animals, suggesting an inability to compensate for the loss of vision. Further, beyond ruling out deficits in tactile experience, understanding whether levels of tactile experience in EB animals are equivalent to or higher than in SC animals could enable us to make inferences about the mechanisms underlying any compensatory changes observed in the neural representation of touch. For example, if levels of tactile experience are higher in EB animals compared with SC animals, then changes in S1 neural responses may be a consequence of increased reliance on the sense of touch. Alternately, if levels of tactile experience are comparable between EB and SC animals, then the absence of vision itself may be driving compensatory changes in the cortical representation of touch (see Discussion).
Both SC and EB animals engaged in whisking behavior (Fig. 2A–E). Thus, active touch sensation mediated by the whiskers in opossums does not require visual guidance. Distributions of whisking frequency were not significantly different between EB and SC animals across bouts of whisking during free exploration (Fig. 2A,B; SC: n = 25 bouts, EB: n = 17 bouts; mean: SC = 7.91 ± 0.328 Hz, EB = 7.74 ± 0.495 Hz; p = 0.762, d = 0.090, permutation test). SC and EB animals were initially observed in their home cage and then subsequently in a novel cage, in order to capture bouts of rest as well as locomotion during the monitoring period. Whisking was observed in the home cage and the novel cage conditions for both groups of animals (Fig. 2C). Transfer to the novel cage reliably led to bouts of active exploration through whisking, locomotion, or both (Fig. 2C). We quantified the relationship between locomotion and whisking as the L/W overlap index for each animal tested (see Materials and Methods). Whisking and locomotion were strongly coupled in both SC and EB animals (Fig. 2D; mean L/W overlap index: SC = 0.717 ± 0.107, EB = 0.762 ± 0.066; p = 0.711, unpaired t test). Whisking occurred in all locomotion bouts recorded during the monitoring period (Fig. 2E) and in a smaller percentage of resting bouts (SC: 34.3%, EB: 27.6%; p = 0.402, Fisher's exact test). SC and EB groups did not significantly differ in the fractions of locomotion (L+) and resting (L−) bouts that contained whisking (W+). The majority of all whisking bouts in both SC and EB occurred during locomotion (SC: 74.5%, EB: 78.4%; p = 0.526, Fisher's exact test). Given these results, locomotor activity could be used as a proxy for levels of tactile experience in SC and EB animals.
We investigated patterns of locomotor activity (Fig. 2F–J) in the home cage over the 24 h light/dark cycle for EB animals compared to SC animals at juvenile ages (Fig. 2F, top) and adult ages (Fig. 2F, middle and bottom). Duration of activity bouts (Fig. 2G; mean: SC = 7.07 ± 1.29 min, EB = 9.84 ± 1.84 min; p = 0.233, permutation test) and interval event intervals (Fig. 2H; mean: SC = 46.3 ± 6.44 min, EB = 45.6 ± 4.55 min; p = 0.934, permutation test) did not differ between SC and EB animals. A conspicuous difference between SC and EB animals was in the timing of activity events (Fig. 2I; mean: SC = 79.2 ± 5.28%, EB = 50.4 ± 8.51%; p = 0.012, unpaired t test). SC animals were primarily active during the dark hours. In contrast, EB animals were, on average, equally active during light and dark hours; they exhibited circadian rhythms that were not entrained to the 24 h light/dark cycle. Though the timing of activity events differed between groups, SC and EB animals displayed similar total amounts of exploratory activity in the home cage (Fig. 2J; p = 0.540, 2-way ANOVA, main effect of experimental group), with an increase in percentages of daily time spent in exploratory activity for both SC and EB groups with increases in age (Fig. 2J; p = 0.032, 2-way ANOVA, main effect of age group). Therefore, total levels of tactile experience, as measured using our criteria, did not significantly differ between SC and EB animals. If only tactile experience in the absence of vision is considered, then EB animals (being totally blind) spend 100% of active bouts (mean: 3.70 ± 0.990 h/d) in nonvisual exploration compared with SC counterparts, which, on average, spend ~80% of active bouts (mean: 2.30 ± 0.777 h/d) in nonvisual exploration (Fig. 2I,J). However, daily hours spent in nonvisual exploratory activity compared across ages were not significantly different between the SC and EB groups (Fig. 2J; p = 0.318, 2-way ANOVA, main effect of experimental group).
Sampling distributions of S1 neurons were similar for EB and SC animals
We examined the effects of early blindness on the neural coding of touch by recording neural activity in S1 in response to a simple tactile stimulus, the deflection of a single whisker. Neurons were sampled across the entire representation of the mystacial whiskers, and all analyses were restricted to neurons located in S1 (Fig. 3A–D) at depths corresponding to layer IV, verified post hoc in histologically processed tissue (400–500 μm below the pial surface; Fig. 3E–H). The sampling distribution for both EB and SC animals was strongly skewed toward neurons tuned to ventral row whiskers (Fig. 3I and J, left), potentially reflective of a cortical magnification of ventral whiskers, as discussed previously (Ramamurthy and Krubitzer, 2016). Whisker arcs were more uniformly represented in the recorded populations (Fig. 3J, right). The fraction of SC and EB populations tuned to whiskers in each row and arc were not significantly different (row: p = 0.962, arc: p = 0.994, Fisher's exact test). The distributions of recording depths for all layer IV neurons sampled in S1 of SC and EB animals are shown in Figure 3K and were not significantly different between the two groups either for all neurons sampled (p = 0.835, d = 0.0306, permutation test) or for only the neurons that exhibited a significant response to whisker stimuli (p = 0.631, d = 0.0923, permutation test). Examples of units tuned to two different whiskers neighboring each other on the mystacial pad, C0 and C1 respectively, are shown for SC (Fig. 4A,B, E,F, I,J, M,N; layer IV, recording depths: 409 and 415 μm) and EB (Fig. 4C,D, G,H, K,L, O,P; layer IV, recording depths: 411 and 449 μm) animals.
Whisker-evoked firing rates, but not spontaneous firing rates, are lower following early blindness
Whisker stimuli evoked lower magnitude responses in S1 neurons of EB animals compared to those of SC animals (Fig. 5). Population average PSTHs for EB neurons in response to whisker stimuli ranked 1–16 (best to worst) exhibited a relative suppression of firing rates in the case of the BW (rank 1) as well as the majority of SWs (ranks 2–10) except for the lower-ranked whisker stimuli (SW; ranks 11–16), which evoked little to no mean response for either population of neurons (Fig. 5A; p-values for 2-way ANOVA (main effect of experimental group) by whisker rank: 1: 2.23 × 10−308, 2: 3.51 × 10−255, 3: 2.78 × 10−191, 4: 2.21 × 10−118, 5: 2.76 × 10−74, 6: 2.47 × 10−77, 7: 4.56 × 10−35, 8: 4.01 × 10−16, 9: 2.26 × 10−12, 10: 1.90 × 10−04, 11: 0.0492, 12: 0.111, 13: 0.0525, 14: 0.444, 15: 0.611, 16: 0.746). The effect size of relative suppression (Fig. 5A; Cohen's U3) across whisker ranks was greater for population responses associated with stimulation of the most dominant SWs compared to that associated with stimulation of the BW. The relative decrease in firing rates for the EB group of neurons was specific to whisker-evoked responses; spontaneous firing rates were unaltered between the two groups (Fig. 5B; significantly responding neurons, median: SC = 0.813 (0.213–1.50); EB = 0.600 (0.313–1.55), p = 0.789, Wilcoxon rank-sum test; all recorded neurons, median: SC = 0.7396 (0.200–1.35); EB = 0.600 (0.213–1.55), p = 0.9628, Wilcoxon rank-sum test).
Because a few outlier cells (defined as >1.5 times the IQR above the third quartile or below the fourth quartile) with higher baseline firing rates were present (Fig. 5B), results of statistical testing for many subsequent analyses are additionally presented after excluding data corresponding to these cells. When considering neurons that exhibited significant whisker-evoked responses (>2 SDs above the spontaneous firing rate), the SC population included 6 outlier cells (6 of 64 cells) and the EB population included 3 outlier cells (3 of 69 cells) that exhibited higher firing rates.
Previous studies have shown similar effects on neural responses caused by differences in brain states (Castro-Alamancos and Oldford, 2002; Hirata and Castro-Alamancos, 2011) and levels of anesthesia (Friedberg et al., 1999; Erchova et al., 2002; Self et al., 2014). To control for this possibility, we performed spectral analysis of LFPs recorded in S1 of SC and EB animals. Both groups showed a clear peak at lower frequencies in the 0–5 Hz range (Fig. 5C; percentage of total FFT power in the 0–5 Hz band: SC = 82.0 ± 2.65%, EB = 84.5 ± 2.03%). This is consistent with Guedel stage III-3/III-4 anesthesia (Friedberg et al., 1999). There was no significant difference between the two groups in the total power contained across different frequency ranges (Fig. 5C; 5 Hz frequency bands, 0–60 Hz; p = 0.934, 2-way ANOVA, main effect of experimental group). Further, in all cases in which electrophysiological recordings were made (10 SC, 9 EB), temperature and respiration rate (measures that correlate with fluctuations in brain state; Pagliardini et al., 2012) were consistently monitored throughout the experiment. Mean respiration rate (SC: 48.6 ± 3.98 breaths/min, EB: 50.4 ± 3.59 breaths/min) and temperature (SC: 91.2 ± 0.158 °F, EB: 91.1 ± 0.167 °F) measured during neural recordings were not significantly different across all animals between the two groups. Therefore, the differences in whisker-evoked responses between the two groups could not be attributed to differences in brain states or anesthetic levels between the two groups.
Neurons in S1 are more selective to whisker stimuli following early blindness
Receptive fields, measured as the number of whiskers that evoked a significant response for each single unit, were smaller in EB animals than in SC animals, as indicated by the clear leftward shift in the corresponding cumulative distribution plots (Fig. 5D; p = 3.47 × 10−05, outliers excluded: p = 3.41 × 10−05, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test; p = 1.04 × 10−05, outliers excluded: p = 1.64 × 10−05, Wilcoxon rank-sum test). Given that neural responses to whisker stimuli are suppressed in EB animals compared to SC animals (Fig. 5A), smaller receptive field sizes in EB animals could be due to an overall reduction in response strength or could reflect an increase in selectivity.
We quantified stimulus selectivity of S1 neurons in SC and EB animals by computing sparseness (see Materials and Methods). In EB animals, coding sparseness was substantially increased (Fig. 5E; p = 8.75 × 10−05, outliers excluded: p = 1.19 × 10−04, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test), indicating greater selectivity of these neurons for whisker stimuli. A tuning curve was generated for each neuron by plotting the raw spike count values corresponding to each whisker stimulus, arranged by rank. Tuning curves were then averaged across neurons to generate a mean tuning curve for each group. As expected from the population average PSTHs (Fig. 5A), the mean tuning curve for the EB group was suppressed overall, relative to the mean tuning curve for the SC group (Fig. 5F, right; p = 2.48 × 10−31, outliers excluded: p = 3.19 × 10−20, 2-way ANOVA, main effect of experimental group). When individual tuning curves were normalized to their respective peaks (BW-evoked response) and then averaged across the population, mean normalized tuning curves for the EB group were considerably more narrowly tuned than for the SC group (Fig. 5F, left; p = 1.07 × 10−25, outliers excluded: p = 6.74 × 10−20, 2-way ANOVA, main effect of experimental group); nonuniform suppression of responses to whisker stimuli across the receptive field added to tuning selectivity beyond a general reduction in response strength. Taken together, these results suggest that spatial resolution for whisker-mediated touch in S1 is enhanced after early blindness.
Stimulus detectability in S1 is degraded following early blindness, but stimulus discriminability is enhanced
We performed ROC analysis to quantify stimulus detectability and discriminability for single units in S1 in EB and SC animals. For each unit, detectability was assessed by comparing whisker-evoked spike count distributions with spontaneous spike count distributions; discriminability was assessed by comparing spike count distributions evoked by the stimulation of different whiskers (Fig. 6A–D). On average, for the four most dominant whiskers in the receptive field (BW, SW1, SW2, and SW3), AUROC values were significantly lower for EB versus SC animals in the case of whisker stimulus detection (Fig. 6E; p = 1.32 × 10−08, outliers excluded: p = 1.44 × 10−07, 2-way ANOVA, main effect of experimental group). In contrast, mean AUROC values were significantly higher for EB versus SC animals in the case of discrimination of the BW from the three most dominant SWs (SW1, SW2, and SW3; Fig. 6E; p = 5.26 × 10−04, outliers excluded: p = 1.21 × 10−04, 2-way ANOVA, main effect of experimental group). We examined the relationship between detectability and discriminability for single units in S1. The slope of the best-fitting line through mean AUROC values for EB animals was steeper than that for SC animals (Fig. 6F; p = 2.05 × 10−05, ANCOVA, difference in slopes). Therefore, for a given level of stimulus detectability, discriminability was relatively enhanced for S1 neurons in EB animals compared with SC animals.
We further investigated whether there were any differences between EB and SC animals in the ability of the neurons to discriminate between multiple whisker stimuli (Wang et al., 2010; Ollerenshaw et al., 2014) using ideal observer analysis (Fig. 6G,H; see Materials and Methods). Spike count histograms corresponding to different whiskers exhibit varying degrees of overlap (Fig. 6A–D). For each neuron, probability density functions associated with each whisker were estimated nonparametrically from the spike count histogram (Fig. 6G). The identity of the most likely single whisker stimulus represented by the spiking activity could then be predicted using a Bayesian decoding strategy. We found that the distribution of decoding accuracies in the EB group was significantly different from the SC group, displaying a shift toward improved performance in discriminating between the four most dominant whiskers in their receptive fields compared to the SC group (BW, SW1, SW2, and SW3; Fig. 6H; p = 0.002, d = 0.603, permutation test). Thus, although neural response distributions are shifted toward lower spike count values across whisker stimuli in the EB group relative to the SC group, the response distributions associated with the deflection of different whiskers are less overlapping, leading to improved discrimination between multiple whiskers in the receptive field.
Somatotopic tuning curves are spatially sharpened following early blindness, especially along the horizontal axis
We compared the 2D spatial configuration of receptive fields in SC and EB animals, which included the response magnitude of the BW (center of the receptive field) and the eight 1° SWs; mean receptive fields are shown in Figure 7A. Sharpness of tuning was quantified as the tuning width index (see Materials and Methods). Distribution of tuning width indices were shifted toward lower values in EB animals than in SC animals, indicating sharper tuning (Fig. 7B; p = 0.008, outliers excluded: p = 0.024, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test). An alternate explanation for the relative shift in the tuning width index distribution could be that whisker-evoked responses were lost at some SW positions but preserved at other positions in EB versus SC animals, which would result in an alteration of receptive field shape. However, we found that distributions of receptive field shape were not significantly different between the two groups—both SC and EB distributions were skewed toward anisotropic receptive field shapes, mostly elongated along the row axis (Fig. 7C; p = 0.911, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test). Further, when all receptive fields were aligned along the major axis, we found that mean responses evoked by SWs (1°, 2°, 3°) were relatively suppressed along both the major and minor axes (major axis: p = 5.0 × 10−04, minor axis: p = 5.5 × 10−03, unpaired t test). Thus, spatial sharpening occurred along both horizontal and vertical axes of receptive fields in EB animals. Mean normalized somatotopic tuning curves for SC and EB groups were computed after collapsing along either the vertical axis (Fig. 7D) or the horizontal axis (Fig. 7E). EB tuning curves were significantly sharper in both cases (vertical: p = 0.032, horizontal: p = 6.62 × 10−08, 2-way ANOVA, main effect of experimental group); however, the median magnitude of relative suppression for EB animals compared to SC animals was 1.42 times greater along the horizontal axis than along the vertical axis across the 1°, 2°, and 3° SW positions.
S1 decoding accuracy for classifying whisker stimulus location is improved following early blindness, especially along the rostrocaudal axis of the snout
We applied decoding analyses to evaluate the information contained in S1 neuronal populations of SC and EB animals regarding the location of a whisker stimulus on the mystacial pad. For each group, recorded neurons were pooled to generate a pseudopopulation, and a minimum distance classifier (Foffani and Moxon, 2004) was trained using a leave-one-out cross-validation method. The classifier was then used to predict the identity of the whisker stimulus that occurred on each trial in terms of location along either the dorsoventral axis (Fig. 8A; one of four possible whisker rows: A, B, C or D) or the rostrocaudal axis (Fig. 8B; one of four possible whisker arcs: 0, 1, 2 or 3) of the snout. Confusion matrices (Fig. 8C) show the percentage of trials classified into each of the four classes (rows or arcs) for SC and EB populations of neurons, across all iterations. For row classification (ROW), the majority of values fell along the diagonal for both SC and EB populations, indicating that most trials were correctly classified by the decoder (left, top and bottom). For the EB population, improvements in row classification performance observed were mainly due to decreased misclassification of trials corresponding to the upper rows of whiskers (rows A and B). For arc classification (ARC), differences in the performance of the classifier for SC and EB populations were readily apparent. For the SC population, trials corresponding to adjacent arcs were frequently confused with each other across all arcs (arcs 0–3). In contrast, for the EB population, a larger proportion of trials were correctly classified, visible as the increased proportion of values on the diagonal and decreased proportion of values off the diagonal in the confusion matrix. As noted previously (Fig. 3J), the distributions of BWs of the recorded neurons were very similar between SC and EB groups, so these differences in population decoding performance could not be attributed to differences in tuning preferences of the neurons sampled. Decoding accuracies were significantly greater than chance performance (computed by shuffling trial labels; SC ROW: p = 9.99 × 10−04, d = 53.9, EB ROW: p = 9.99 × 10−04, d = 84.8, SC ARC: p = 9.99 × 10−04, d = 16.7, EB ARC: p = 9.99 × 10−04, d = 71.1, permutation test) in all cases (Fig. 8D). Within each experimental group, row classification performance was greater than arc classification performance (SC: p = 9.99 × 10−04, d = 16.8, EB: p = 9.99 × 10−04, d = 30.3, permutation test). The distribution of decoding accuracies for the EB population compared to the SC population across all iterations was shifted toward significantly higher values for both row classification performance (p = 9.99 × 10−04, d = 1.82, permutation test) and arc classification performance (p = 9.99 × 10−04, d = 7.80, permutation test), but the separation between SC and EB distributions was greater for arc classification performance (Fig. 8D). Therefore, the improvement in decoding performance in EB versus SC animals was greatest for whisker stimulus locations along the rostrocaudal axis of the snout.
In summary, we found that neural coding of whisker-mediated touch was altered in animals that experienced vision loss early in development compared with SCs. Whisker stimuli evoked lower magnitude responses in EB animals, but the relative suppression of whisker-evoked responses was greater for nonpreferred stimuli (SWs) than the preferred stimulus (BW) such that S1 neurons were more selective to whisker-mediated touch in EB animals. These changes in response properties led to decreased performance on whisker stimulus detection by S1 neurons, but enhanced their performance on whisker stimulus discrimination. For the population of S1 neurons, whisker stimulus location on the mystacial pad could be decoded with greater accuracy in EB animals, particularly along the rostrocaudal axis of the snout. Overall, the changes in sensory coding observed in S1 after early blindness were consistent with enhanced spatial discrimination of tactile stimuli.
Discussion
Previous studies have reported that barrel fields in S1 are expanded in neonatally enucleated mice and rats (Bronchti et al., 1992; Rauschecker et al., 1992; Zheng and Purves, 1995). Likewise, our laboratory has demonstrated an increase in the total cortical territory occupied by S1 in EB versus SC opossums (Karlen and Krubitzer, 2009). There have been a few reports of improved tactile or auditory performance after early vision loss (Rauschecker and Kniepert, 1994; Toldi et al., 1994; Lomber et al., 2010; Meredith et al., 2011) accompanied in some cases by greater response selectivity of neurons in higher order cortical areas, which play a role in processing multisensory inputs (Korte and Rauschecker, 1993). More recently, alterations in synaptic function have been described in primary cortical areas associated with spared modalities, including S1 (Goel et al., 2006; Jitsuki et al., 2011; Zheng et al., 2014). These changes can even occur in adults following relatively short periods of visual deprivation (up to 1 week) (Petrus et al., 2014; Meng et al., 2015, 2017; Nakajima et al., 2016), with different effects on spared and deprived sensory cortices (He et al., 2012; Petrus et al., 2014). However, to date, it is unknown whether and how long-term unimodal sensory loss affects sensory processing at the level of single neurons and populations of neurons in primary cortical areas of the remaining sensory modalities.
We have demonstrated that neural coding of whisker-mediated touch in S1 is altered in animals that experienced loss of vision very early in development. We found that neurons in S1 of EB animals exhibited an overall reduction in the magnitude of responses to single whisker stimuli for both the preferred stimulus (BW) and the nonpreferred stimuli (SW). Suppression of neural responses to whisker stimuli in our study were not attributable to an overall decrease in cortical excitability, differences in anesthetic state, or a lack of active tactile exploration (Guic et al., 2008, Alwis and Rajan, 2013) following early blindness. Further, suppression was specific to whisker-evoked responses, combined with an increased selectivity of neurons in S1 for single whisker stimuli. Greater stimulus selectivity could, in theory, result from either an attenuation of SW-evoked responses or an increase in BW-evoked responses. We found that suppression of whisker-evoked responses in EB animals relative to SC animals was nonuniform across the receptive field, such that SW-evoked responses were suppressed to a greater extent than BW-evoked responses, resulting in sharper tuning curves. Interestingly, our findings resemble some forms of plasticity previously reported in the S1 whisker representation, which are functionally distinct from Hebbian forms of plasticity (for review, see Feldman and Brecht, 2005) typically observed in studies involving manipulations that create competition among sensory inputs within the whisker system (e.g., deprivation of a subset of whisker inputs though trimming, plucking, or follicle lesion). Polley et al. (2004) found that prolonged exposure of rats to complex, naturalistic environments in adulthood resulted in the contraction of the functional representation of individual whiskers, suppression of whisker-evoked responses, and smaller, sharper receptive fields for neurons in S1. Further, when adult rats were allowed brief periods of whisker-guided exploration in a novel environment after deprivation of all but a single whisker, the direction of Hebbian plasticity was reversed, causing a contraction of the functional representation of the spared whisker rather than its expansion (Polley et al., 1999). A common theme between our study and the work by Polley et al. (1999, 2004) is that plasticity is observed in the somatosensory system of animals that have been subjected to a manipulation that can be expected to increase the animals' dependence on the whisker system–through vision loss in our study, and through transfer to a more tactilely complex environment in the studies by Polley et al. (for review, see Frostig, 2006). In both cases, the net effect was a refinement in the functional organization of the S1 whisker representation such that neural coding of single whisker stimuli was sparser and more metabolically efficient, and displayed increased spatial precision. In EB animals, we found that the ability of S1 neurons to discriminate between neighboring whiskers was enhanced at the expense of their ability to detect single whisker stimuli. Similar changes in barrel cortex have been previously linked to enhanced sensory processing on shorter time scales, specifically during arousal (Castro-Alamancos and Oldford, 2002; Hirata and Castro-Alamancos, 2011) and following sensory adaptation (Wang et al., 2010; Ollerenshaw et al., 2014). Our results suggest that plasticity after sensory loss is not necessarily universally adaptive, but may involve a compromise between certain functions, at least within a given brain region associated with a spared sensory modality. The changes in S1 may reflect a redistribution of function in the brain, given that cortical and subcortical areas normally associated with vision are also recruited for somatosensory processing in EB animals (Kahn and Krubitzer, 2002; Karlen et al., 2006). However, these opposite effects on neuronal performance in S1 may also reflect a genuine trade-off in tactile function; there have been reports of both deficits and enhancements in the behavioral performance of blind individuals depending on the nature of the tactile task used (Sterr et al., 1998a,b, 2003; Alary et al., 2009). To what extent these changes in neural coding of whisker-mediated touch in S1 after early blindness impact behavioral detectability and discriminability of stimuli remains to be investigated. Results from the current study allow us to generate testable hypotheses in this regard. Additional research is also necessary to determine the contribution of plasticity occurring at subcortical levels to the effects observed in S1.
It is possible that the effects of suppression and spatial sharpening are correlates of perceptual learning (Gold and Watanabe, 2010) in S1, which confers long-term experience-dependent improvements in the ability to process tactile information after early blindness (Wong et al., 2011). Previous studies have demonstrated that exposure to passive stimulation of a single whisker results in the suppression and contraction of its functional representation (Welker et al., 1992; Gdalyahu et al., 2012), along with an increase in cortical inhibition (Welker et al., 1989; Knott et al., 2002), possibly due to learned habituation to repetitive stimulation or as a result of homeostatic processes (Welker et al., 1992; Polley, 2002; Foeller and Feldman, 2004). EB animals are forced to depend on whisker-mediated touch as the primary means of navigating their environment using spatial cues. Thus, a use-dependent increase in whisker stimulation after early blindness is one potential mechanism that could drive functional plasticity in the S1 whisker representation.
A noteworthy aspect of somatosensory plasticity following early blindness was the anisotropy observed in receptive field changes and population decoding performance for S1 neurons, such that discrimination of whisker stimulus position was enhanced to a greater extent along the horizontal/rostrocaudal axis of the snout, aligned with the primary direction of natural whisker motion. This could reflect a row bias inherent to the structural and functional organization of the S1 whisker representation (Armstrong-James and Fox, 1987; Bernardo et al., 1990; Lustig et al., 2013; Ramamurthy and Krubitzer, 2016). In addition, simulations of whisking during exploratory activity in rats indicate that whiskers within the same row (same horizontal axis) are most likely to come in simultaneous contact with a surface (Hobbs et al., 2015). Given that mystacial pad morphology and whisking behavior are highly conserved across mammals (Brecht et al., 1997; Grant et al., 2013; Ramamurthy and Krubitzer, 2016), it is reasonable to postulate that similar patterns of whisker contact during exploration by short-tailed opossums may drive use-dependent anisotropies of receptive field alterations in EB versus SC animals.
In the current study, we found that overall levels of tactile experience did not differ significantly between EB and SC groups across all developmental stages tested. We cannot rule out the possibility that context-dependent differences between sighted and blind animals in whisking strategies during exploration (Arkley et al., 2014) may contribute to differences in S1 neural response properties and receptive field structure. However, our findings suggest that factors other than increased whisker stimulation could play a role in somatosensory plasticity following early blindness. For example, tactile experience may need to be accompanied by focused attention to whisker stimuli in order for this plasticity to occur, which may be more likely after early blindness, given the animals' increased dependence on the somatosensory system. Alternatively, or in addition, somatosensory plasticity may have been facilitated by the loss of vision itself (Goldreich and Kanics, 2003), independent of tactile experience. For example, there is some evidence that vision loss, rather than whisker-mediated tactile experience, leads to barrel field expansion in neonatally enucleated rats via epigenetic mechanisms that influence the growth rate and arborization patterns of somatosensory thalamocortical axons (Fetter-Pruneda et al., 2013). Future research incorporating additional sensory deprivation (whisker trimming and dark rearing) or enrichment paradigms with sighted and blind animals will help further clarify the relative effects of tactile experience versus vision loss on neural coding in S1 following early blindness.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the National Eye Institute–National Institutes of Health (Grant R01EY022987-03 to L.A.K. and Grant 2T32EY015387-11 to D.L.R.), the National Science Foundation (Graduate Research Fellowship Program Grant DGE-1650042 to D.L.R.), and the James S. McDonnell Foundation (Grant 220020516 to L.A.K.). We thank Daniel Feldman for many helpful discussions; Mary Baldwin, Andrew Halley, and Mackenzie Englund for comments on a draft of this manuscript; Cindy Clayton for animal care; and Cynthia Weller, Tanner Kovacs, Sarah Daley, Heather Dodson, Carly Jones, Mina Sadraei, Linda Oppizzi, and Mackenzie Englund for experimental assistance.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Leah A. Krubitzer, Center for Neuroscience, University of California–Davis, 1544 Newton Court, Davis, CA 95618. lakrubitzer{at}ucdavis.edu