Abstract
The growth and organization of the developing brain are known to be influenced by hormones, but little is known about whether disruption of hormones affects cortical regions, such as mPFC. This region is particularly important given its involvement in executive functions and implication in the pathology of many neuropsychiatric disorders. Here, we examine the long-term effects of perinatal exposure to endocrine-disrupting compounds, the phthalates, on the mPFC and associated behavior. This investigation is pertinent as humans are ubiquitously exposed to phthalates through a variety of consumer products and phthalates can readily cross the placenta and be delivered to offspring via lactation. Pregnant dams orally consumed an environmentally relevant mixture of phthalates at 0, 200, or 1000 μg/kg/d through pregnancy and for 10 d while lactating. As adults, offspring were tested in an attentional set-shifting task, which assesses cognitive flexibility. Brains were also examined in adulthood for stereological quantification of the number of neurons, glia, and synapses within the mPFC. We found that, independent of sex, perinatal phthalate exposure at either dose resulted in a reduction in neuron number, synapse number, and size of the mPFC and a deficit in cognitive flexibility. Interestingly, the number of synapses was correlated with cognitive flexibility, such that rats with fewer synapses were less cognitively flexible than those with more synapses. These results demonstrate that perinatal phthalate exposure can have long-term effects on the cortex and behavior of both male and female rats.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Humans globally are exposed on a daily basis to a variety of phthalates, which are endocrine-disrupting chemicals. The effects of phthalate exposure on the developing brain, especially on cognitively relevant regions, such as the mPFC, are not known. Here, we use a rat model of human prenatal exposure to an environmentally relevant mixture of phthalates and find that there is an appreciable reduction in neuron number, synapse number, and size of the mPFC and a deficit in cognitive flexibility. These results may have serious implications for humans given that the mPFC is involved in executive functions and is implicated in the pathology of many neuropsychiatric disorders.
Introduction
The growth and organization of the developing brain are influenced by hormones. Indeed, gonadal hormones play a critical role during perinatal development in regulating apoptosis (Nuñez et al., 2000; Forger, 2009) and synaptic number (Nishizuka and Arai, 1981; Pérez et al., 1990; Simerly, 2002). Although much of this work has focused on subcortical regions where sex hormone receptors are particularly abundant, it is known that these receptors are also present during development in other brains areas, including the cerebral cortex (Pérez et al., 2003; Westberry and Wilson, 2012; Tsai et al., 2015).
Because hormones can impact the developing brain, there is a growing scientific concern regarding the influence of environmental endocrine-disrupting chemicals on neurodevelopment. One such class of chemicals, known as phthalates, is problematic because of their ubiquitous environmental presence. Phthalates, which are often found to be antiandrogenic (Howdeshell et al., 2017), are used as plasticizers in a variety of consumer goods, including plastics, personal care products, fragranced products, pharmaceuticals, clothing, and building materials. Because phthalates are not covalently bound within the matrices of products, they can freely migrate or evaporate out into the environment. Consequently, contact with these products can lead to exposure through transdermal uptake, incidental ingestion, and inhalation. Indeed, diet is presumed to be the main source of exposure to many phthalates due to contamination during production, processing, and packaging of foods (Heudorf et al., 2007).
Considering that phthalates can readily cross the placenta (Mose et al., 2007) and be delivered to offspring via lactation (Lyche et al., 2009), the perinatal period appears to be a particularly vulnerable window to their endocrine-disrupting effects. Indeed, there is indirect evidence that prenatal phthalate exposure, as measured by maternal urinary metabolites, has effects on the developing human brain as exposure has been associated with adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes (Ejaredar et al., 2015). In particular, there are several studies indicating that prenatal phthalate exposure may negatively influence children's behavior, executive function, and incidence of neuropsychiatric disorders (Engel et al., 2010; Swan et al., 2010; Kobrosly et al., 2014; Lien et al., 2015; Jeddi et al., 2016). Although one study found that concurrent phthalate exposure was negatively correlated with cortical thickness in the right middle and superior temporal gyri of children (Park et al., 2015), there is little understanding of potential cellular effects on the developing cortex.
Rodent research on perinatal exposure to a single phthalate has found impairments in water maze performance (e.g., Dai et al., 2015), contextual fear conditioning (DeBartolo et al., 2016), and structural and functional plasticity of the hippocampus (Holahan and Smith, 2015), but no study to date has examined the effect of perinatal phthalate exposure on executive function or the cortex in adulthood. Furthermore, much of the rodent research focuses on exposure to a single phthalate, whereas humans are exposed to many. Thus, further investigation using an environmentally relevant mixture of phthalates is necessary to explore the outcomes of perinatal exposure on executive function and the cortex.
In this study, we use a rat model of human prenatal exposure to investigate the long-lasting effects of an environmentally relevant mixture of phthalates on cognitive flexibility and mPFC neuroanatomy. In particular, cognitive flexibility, which relies on the mPFC (Gruber et al., 2010), was assessed in adulthood using an attentional set-shifting task. Our neuroanatomical investigation focused on the total number of neurons, glia, and synapses within the adult mPFC. The mPFC is also of special interest given its implication in the pathology of many neuropsychiatric disorders, some of which have been positively associated with prenatal phthalate exposure (Ejaredar et al., 2015).
Materials and Methods
Subjects
The animals used in this study and the rearing of litters have been previously described (Kougias et al., 2018). Male (n = 32) and female (n = 32) Long–Evans hooded rats at ∼3 months of age were obtained from Harlan Laboratories (now Envigo) and housed for a minimum of 2 weeks before being paired for breeding in five cohorts, each of which contained all of the experimental groups. Rats were housed in same-sex pairs on a 12 h light/dark cycle with food and water available ad libitum. To reduce exposure to endocrine-disrupting chemicals, all rats were housed in bisphenol A-free polysulfone cages, fed a low phytoestrogen food (Harlan 2020X; Teklad Diets), and hydrated with reverse osmosis-filtered water in glass bottles. All procedures were approved by the University of Illinois Institutional Care and Use Committee and adhere to the National Institute of Health guidelines on the ethical use of animals.
For breeding, opposite sex pairs were placed in suspended wire-bottom cages and inspected daily for sperm plugs, which marked gestational day (GD) 0. The dams were then housed individually, fed a control diet (D10012G) obtained from Research Diets, and assigned to one of three exposure groups (n = 11, 9, 11, respectively): 0, 200, or 1000 μg phthalates/kg body weight. On the day of birth, designated as postnatal day (P) 0, litters were not disturbed; however, litters were culled to 10 pups each on P1 to control for litter size and sex ratio. The control diet was replaced with the 2020× feed on P10. Only one pup from each sex within a litter was used for each endpoint (i.e., one male and female within a litter was used for cognitive behavior testing and another male and female from the same litter for neuroanatomical measurements).
On GD0 and GD1, the dams were given half a cookie (Newman's Own, organic alphabet cookie, vanilla flavor) with tocopherol-stripped corn oil pipetted onto it for acclimatization. Starting on GD2 through P10, dams immediately and completely consumed half a cookie overlaid with the daily dose of phthalate mixture at their corresponding concentration, as phthalate exposure can occur through both placental transfer and lactation in rats (Dostal et al., 1987). To avoid direct exposure to pups, the cookie was given to the dams while away from the nest. This method of oral administration is presumably not stressful and is similar to the major route of human exposure to phthalates. Pups were weaned on P25 and pair-housed with similarly aged animals of the same sex and exposure.
Phthalate mixture
The phthalate mixture was identical to that used by Kougias et al. (2018) The phthalate mixture was derived by back-calculating exposures based on the urinary metabolites of pregnant women in the Champaign-Urbana community (unpublished data), which approximates the U.S. population levels (Corbasson et al., 2016). The phthalate mixture was comprised of 35% diethyl (DEP), 21% bis(2-ethylhexyl) (DEHP), 15% dibutyl (DBP), 15% di-isononyl (DiNP), 8% di-isobutyl (DiBP), and 5% benzyl butyl (BBP) phthalate. The mixture was prepared by suspending 0, 0.6, or 3 mg phthalates/ml tocopherol-stripped corn oil to ensure equivalent volume (1 μl/3 g body weight) in administering the respective 0 (control), 200, or 1000 μg phthalates/kg doses. These doses are relatively low within the rodent literature; and based on the body surface area normalization method (Reagan-Shaw et al., 2008), which is prescribed by the U.S. Food and Drug Administration, a 200 and 1000 μg/kg dose in rats is equivalent to a human dose of 32.43 and 162.16 μg/kg, respectively. These doses align within the range of the estimated daily intakes of humans (Heudorf et al., 2007) and, interestingly, are even below some of the tolerable daily intakes of some governing organizations (Koch et al., 2003). However, there are limitations to translating these doses of phthalates from rats to the human population due to the lack of information on interspecies differences in the toxicokinetics and their associated dose–response relationships.
Behavioral testing: attentional set-shift
Apparatus.
The attentional set-shift apparatus was a plus maze in a relatively small room with dim, indirect lighting. The center of the maze was 10 × 10 cm, with four equiangular arms of 10 cm width and 45 cm length. The entire maze was enclosed with 15-cm-high walls and placed upon a spinning table to allow easy adjustment during testing. The inside walls and floor of two adjacent arms were white, whereas the other two were black; and the floor of one black arm and adjacent white arm were smooth, whereas the other two were textured (i.e., “rough”). At the end of each arm, there were food troughs. For training and testing, a wall-piece could be slid into one of four positions to convert the plus-shaped maze into a T-shaped maze, blocking off entry into a specific arm.
Protocol.
The attentional set shift protocol began in adulthood at ∼P92, at which time rats were food restricted in preparation for testing (similar to Willing and Wagner, 2016). Throughout the 13 day protocol, rats were weighed daily and food restricted to 85%–90% of free-feeding body weight by testing days (i.e., days 12 and 13). On days 1–5, rats were handled for 1 min, then given 3 or 4 reward pellets (TestDiet) in their home cages. On days 6–8, rats underwent a daily habituation trial consisting of being placed in the center of the plus-shaped maze with all four arms baited with a reward pellet. A habituation trial ended upon obtaining all four reward pellets or if 5 min had elapsed. On days 9–11, rats underwent 8 daily trials of pretraining. For each trial, the rat was placed at the end of one arm (start arm) with the opposing arm blocked off and learned to leave the start arm to enter only one of two open arms of the T-shaped maze to potentially obtain a food reward. Trials were rewarded 50% of the time, and the starting arm was quasi-randomly changed on each trial to ensure no learned arm preference. On days 13 and 14, testing on Sets 1 and 2 began, respectively. The trials in both sets were similarly arranged as in pretraining trials, such that the rats were placed in alternating start arms with the option to enter only one of two target arms. Upon food restriction, estrous cycles from a subset of females were determined daily via vaginal lavage and cytology. Most females were in diestrus at the time of testing in Set 1 and Set 2, such that the stage of the estrous cycle did not have any influence on performance within the attentional set-shifting task.
Set 1: training.
Rats were trained to enter a target arm using a specific cue based on color (i.e., white or black; Rule 1) to obtain a food reward (Fig. 1A), and Set 1 concluded when a performance criterion of 8 consecutively correct trials was met. Rats unable to reach this criterion within 120 trials or due to lack of motivation for a food reward were excluded from the study. Consequently, there were 9, 8, and 10 females in the respective 0, 200, and 1000 μg/kg groups, and there were 9, 8, and 11 males in the respective exposure groups. The total number of trials performed to reach criterion were analyzed.
Attentional set-shift task. In this example, (A) a rat is trained to go to black arms in Set 1. Once 8 consecutive trials of correct responses is reached, the rat is removed and is tested in Set 2 the following day. B, In Set 2, the rat is tested with an extradimensional shift in the rewarding cue, such that rough arms are now baited with a reward pellet. Green arrows indicate correct responses. Red arrows indicate incorrect responses.
Set 2: test on an extradimensional shift.
On the next day after Set 1, the contingency for the reward was changed to a different sensory modality, such that rats were now trained to enter a target arm using a cue based on texture (i.e., smooth or rough; Rule 2) to obtain a food reward. This extradimensional, or cross-modal, shift is one form of behavioral flexibility that is mediated by cognitive functions of the PFC (Ragozzino et al., 1999). Rats completed 80 trials in Set 2 regardless of performance. On any given trial in Set 2, there is only one correct choice and one incorrect choice, with incorrect choices being perseverative errors or omission errors. A perseverative error is entering an incorrect arm according to the rule from Set 1, whereas an omission error is an incorrect choice that does not follow the rewarded stimuli from either set (Fig. 1B). For example, a rat is trained to receive a reward pellet on trials of Set 1 if a white arm is chosen and on trials of Set 2 if a rough arm is chosen, then a perseveration error is going to a white smooth arm during Set 2. Whether a rat performed a correct choice or an error across the 80 trials was analyzed, as well as separate analyses of perseverative and omission errors across their respective 40 trials.
Neuroanatomy
Histological preparation.
Brains were collected from littermates of the animals tested in the attentional set shift task. These rats were between P103 and P134, which was at least 2 weeks after they completed behavioral tasks that did not involve food restriction: prepulse inhibition, open field, and object recognition (not reported). Rats were deeply anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital before a transcardial perfusion with 0.1 m PBS followed by 4% PFA fixative solution. The brains were removed and stored in the PFA fixative solution for 24 h followed by being stored in a 30% sucrose (cryoprotectant) solution for 3 d. At this time, brains were coded to keep the experimenter blind to the rat's group. After 3 d in the sucrose solution, the brain was sliced with a freezing microtome into 40 μm coronal slices. One brain from a vehicle-exposed (0 μg/kg) female did not fix properly. Thus, there were 10, 9, and 11 females in the respective 0, 200, and 1000 μg/kg groups and 11, 9, and 11 males in the respective exposure groups.
Methylene Blue/Azure II.
To precisely visualize the rostral and caudal borders of the mPFC, every section at the rostral start of the frontal white matter and the caudal end in which the genu corpus callosum appears were mounted on gelatin-coated slides. For the rest of the mPFC, every fifth section was mounted with the remaining sections placed in a storage solution (30% glycerol, 30% ethylene glycol, 30% distilled water, 10% 0.1 m PBS) and stored in a −20°C freezer for later use. Mounted sections were allowed to dry before being stained with Methylene Blue/Azure II, a cell body stain used for cellular quantification, laminar parcellation, and volume determination. Sections on a slide were covered with Permount and a coverslip.
Immunohistochemistry for synaptophysin.
Three mPFC sections representative of each brain were selected from the storage solution and stained for synaptophysin, a presynaptic vesicle protein used as a marker of synapses as in Drzewiecki et al. (2016). Sections were rinsed 3 times for 5 min in TBS, pH 7.6, placed for 30 min in a blocking solution (20% normal goat serum, 1% BSA, 1% hydrogen peroxide), and then incubated for 48 h at 4°C in the primary antibody (antisynaptophysin, 1:5000, Sigma-Aldrich; mouse monoclonal), which was diluted in Tris-Triton goat (TTG) solution (2% normal goat serum, 0.3% Triton X-100 in TBS). Following primary incubation, sections were rinsed 3 times for 5 min each in TTG followed by incubation with the biotinylated secondary antibody diluted in TTG (anti-mouse IgG antibody, 5 μg/ml, Vector Laboratories) for 90 min at room temperature (∼23°C). Sections were then rinsed for 5 min twice in TTG and twice in TBS. Following rinsing, sections were placed in avidin-biotin complex (Vectastain ABC Kit, Vector Laboratories) for 1 h at room temperature (∼23°C) and then stained with DAB (Sigma-Aldrich Fast 3–3′Diaminobenzidine Tablets) for 2 min. Last, sections were rinsed thoroughly with TBS, mounted on gelatin-coated slides, and dried for at least 24 h before being coverslipped using Permount.
Volume of the mPFC.
Sections stained with Methylene Blue/Azure II were used in determining the volume of the mPFC, as previously detailed by Markham et al. (2007). The area and thickness of layers I, II/III, and V/VI were separately used to calculate the volume of the mPFC. The subcortical white matter within the mPFC sections was also parcellated for volume estimations.
The mPFC, which contains the prelimbic (PL) and infralimbic (IL) regions, were parcellated in reference to the underlying white matter and in accordance to cytoarchitectonic criteria (Van Eden and Uylings, 1985). Parcellation of the rostral mPFC began with the appearance of frontal white matter, and the caudal end of the mPFC coincided with the appearance of the genu of the corpus callosum. The dorsal border of the mPFC was identified by a decrease in layer I size, a less distinct layer I/II border, an increase in the cellular density of layer III, and a broadening of layer V/VI. The ventral border of the mPFC was identified by a loss in laminar distinction, a decrease in layer I size, and a tapering of layer V/VI.
Neuron and glia counts.
The number of neurons and glia were stereologically counted with the optical disector in the sections stained with Methylene Blue/Azure II using StereoInvestigator software (Microbrightfield). After parcellating the mPFC into layers I, II/III, and V/VI (Fig. 2A), the software randomly sampled sites to count within each region. A 35 × 35 × 10 μm (width × height × depth) counting frame was used with 1 μm guard zones at the top and bottom of each section. The experimenter counted cells only if the bottom of the cell was within the volume of the counting frame. By using Methylene Blue/Azure II, neurons and glia are distinguished based upon differences in size, color, and shape. In particular, neurons are larger and are stained dark blue with a distinct nucleus and nucleolus, whereas glia are smaller, turquoise-stained, amorphous cells (Fig. 2B). Given the sparse distribution of cells in layer I, cells were not quantified in this region. However, in each layers II/III and V/VI, a minimum of 150 glia and 200 neurons were counted across sampled sites of at least two sections of the mPFC from a given brain. Separate neuronal and glial densities were calculated for each lamina. These densities were multiplied by their respective laminar volumes to estimate the total number of neurons and glia.
A, Parcellation of layers I, II/III, and V/VI of the mPFC, as well as the underlying white matter, in a Methylene Blue/Azure II section. B, Higher magnification of a Methylene Blue/Azure II section in which neurons can easily be distinguished from glia. C, High magnification of an immunohistochemically stained section of synaptophysin, a marker of synapses. The counting frame has green inclusion and red exclusion lines for stereological counting.
Synapse quantification.
Like the quantification previously described by Drzewiecki et al. (2016) and similar to how neurons and glia were counted above, a minimum of 200 synaptophysin-stained boutons were stereologically counted in each laminar region I, II/III, and V/VI of a given brain (Fig. 2C). In contrast to the cell counts, a 4 × 4 × 6 μm counting frame was used with 0.1 μm guard zones at the top and bottom of each section. Synaptophysin-stained bouton densities for each laminar group (i.e., I, II/III, and V/VI) were calculated. These densities were then multiplied by their respective laminar volumes to estimate the total number of synaptophysin-stained boutons.
Experimental design and statistical analyses
Using SPSS (IBM), the analysis of all neuroanatomical measures was performed using a two-way ANOVA (phthalate exposure × sex) with cohort as a cofactor. For the behavioral analysis of Set 1 of attentional set-shift, Rule 1 (i.e., the rewarding cue: black or white) was used as another cofactor in the two-way ANOVA. For Set 2, given that the dependent variable is repeatedly measured with a binary outcome (i.e., correct or incorrect), a logistic regression was used (Parzen et al., 2011), particularly within a generalized linear mixed model with trial as the repeated measure within each rat subject and phthalate exposure, sex, cohort, and Rule 1 as (co)factors. Post hoc tests were performed using Fisher's least square difference method with each phthalate exposure dose (200 and 1000 μg/kg) only compared with the vehicle-exposed (0 μg/kg) group. Two-tailed correlations were run between neuroanatomical measures and indices of attentional set shift performance for rats within the same litter. Data are mean ± SEM.
Results
Attentional set-shift
Set 1: training
There was no effect of phthalate exposure, but stimulus (F(1,44) = 18.545, p < 0.001) and sex (F(1,44) = 4.569, p = 0.038) were significant for trials to criterion, such that rats with black arms reached criterion sooner than rats with white arms as the rewarding cue and male rats reached criterion sooner than female rats (Fig. 3).
Performance on the black/white discrimination in Set 1 of attentional set-shift. Males performed significantly better than females (*p = 0.038), but there were no effects of phthalate exposure. From left to right on the x-axis: 0, 200, and 1000 μg/kg males followed by the respective female groups; n = 9, n = 8, n = 11, n = 9, n = 8, and n = 10, respectively.
Set 2: extradimensional shift
For correct responses, there was a significant effect of phthalate exposure (F(2,4389) = 6.195, p = 0.002), such that vehicle-exposed rats performed significantly better than both the relatively low (p = 0.001) and high phthalate-exposed rats (p = 0.019). Additionally, there was a significant effect of sex (F(1,4389) = 10.665, p = 0.001), such that males performed significantly better than females (Fig. 4A).
Performance on the extradimensional shift (Set 2) as assessed by (A) percentage correct, (B) perseverative errors, and (C) omission errors across 80 trials in Set 2 of attentional set-shift. Overall, there were phthalate exposure effects (p ≤ 0.030), such that both the low and high phthalate-exposed rats performed significantly worse on this task compared with vehicle-exposed rats. Additionally, males performed significantly better than females, as indicated by more correct responses (*p = 0.001) and fewer perseverative errors (*p = 0.002). From left to right on the x-axis of each measurement: 0, 200, and 1000 μg/kg males followed by the respective female groups; n = 9, n = 8, n = 11, n = 9, n = 8, and n = 10, respectively. a and b indicate groups that are significantly different.
For perseverative errors, there was a significant effect of phthalate exposure (F(2,2189) = 3.526, p = 0.030), such that vehicle-exposed rats had significantly fewer perseverative errors than the relatively low phthalate-exposed rats (p = 0.009). There was also a significant effect of sex (F(1,2189) = 10.665, p = 0.002), such that males performed significantly fewer perseverative errors than females (Fig. 4B).
For omission errors, there was only a significant effect of phthalate exposure (F(2,2189) = 5.159, p = 0.006), such that vehicle-treated rats had significantly fewer omission errors than both the relatively low (p = 0.004) and high phthalate-exposed rats (p = 0.010) (Fig. 4C).
Neuroanatomy
Neurons and glia
The number of neurons in the mPFC was decreased by perinatal phthalate exposure (F(2,51) = 23.834, p < 0.001) at both doses (p < 0.001) (Fig. 5A). The phthalate exposure effect on neuron number was evident in both layers II/III (F(2,51) = 21.891, p < 0.001) and V/VI (F(2,51) = 18.879, p < 0.001). Additionally, the number of neurons in the mPFC was greater in males than in females (F(1,51) = 9.167, p = 0.004), and this occurred in both layers II/III (F(1,51) = 6.931, p = 0.011) and V/VI (F(1,51) = 8.703, p = 0.005), similar to previous work in our laboratory (Markham et al., 2007).
Total number of neurons (A) and glia (B) in the mPFC. Vehicle-exposed rats across both sexes had more neurons in the mPFC (a > b) than the relatively low (p < 0.001) and high phthalate-exposed rats (p < 0.001). Additionally, males had more neurons (*p = 0.004) and glia (*p = 0.011) in the mPFC than females. From left to right on the x-axis for total number of neurons and glia in the mPFC: 0, 200, and 1000 μg/kg males followed by the respective female groups; n = 11, n = 9, n = 11, n = 10, n = 9, and n = 11, respectively.
There were no effects of phthalate exposure on the number of glia in total or in each lamina (Fig. 5B). There was a significant sex difference (F(1,51) = 7.012, p = 0.011), indicating that males had more glia in the mPFC than females, as in Markham et al. (2007). Although males had numerically more glia than females in both laminar groups, it was only significant in layers II/III (F(1,51) = 11.728, p = 0.001) and not in layers V/VI (F(1,51) = 3.351, p = 0.073).
Synaptophysin: marker of synapses
Analysis of the total number of synaptophysin boutons in the mPFC revealed a significant effect of phthalate exposure (F(2,51) = 6.041, p = 0.004), such that vehicle-exposed rats had significantly more synapses in the mPFC than both the relatively low (p = 0.011) and high phthalate-exposed rats (p = 0.002) (Fig. 6A). This phthalate exposure effect was evident in layers II/III (F(2,51) = 3.936, p = 0.026) and V/VI (F(2,51) = 11.053, p < 0.001), but not in layer I (F(2,51) = 1.771, p = 0.180). Additionally, there was a significant effect of sex (F(1,51) = 5.873, p = 0.019), indicating that males had significantly more synapses in the mPFC than females. Although there was a numerical difference between the sexes in each of the laminar groups, it was significant only in layers V/VI (F(1,51) = 7.754, p = 0.008) and not in layer I (F(1,51) = 3.498, p = 0.067) or layers II/III (F(1,51) = 3.674, p = 0.061). Last, there were no significant effects for the number of synaptophysin-labeled synapses per neuron (Fig. 6B).
Total number of synaptophysin boutons (A), a marker of synapses, and (B) synapses per neuron in the mPFC. Vehicle-exposed rats across both sexes had more synaptophysin boutons in the mPFC than the relatively low (p = 0.011) and high phthalate-exposed rats (p = 0.002) (a > b). Males had more synaptophysin boutons in the mPFC than females (*p = 0.019). From left to right on the x-axis for total number of synapses and synapses per neuron in the mPFC: 0, 200, and 1000 μg/kg males followed by the respective female groups; n = 11, n = 9, n = 11, n = 10, n = 9, and n = 11, respectively.
mPFC and underlying white matter volume
Overall, there was a significant effect of phthalate exposure (F(2,51) = 5.207, p = 0.009) on the total volume of the mPFC (Fig. 7A), such that the vehicle-exposed rats had larger volumes than both the relatively low (p = 0.027) and high phthalate-exposed rats (p = 0.003). This effect was significant in layers I (F(2,51) = 3.312, p = 0.044) and V/VI (F(2,51) = 8.219, p = 0.001), but not in layers II/III (F(2,51) = 2.943, p = 0.062). Additionally, there was a significant effect of sex (F(1,51) = 11.993, p = 0.001), with males having larger volumes compared with females, as in previous work (Markham et al., 2007). This sex difference was significant in each laminar group: layer I (F(1,51) = 20.477, p < 0.001), layers II/III (F(1,51) = 8.680, p = 0.005), and layers V/VI (F(1,51) = 8.293, p = 0.006).
Volume of the mPFC (A) and underlying white matter (B). Vehicle-exposed rats across both sexes had larger mPFC volumes than the relatively low (p = 0.027) and high phthalate-exposed rats (p = 0.003) (a > b). Additionally, males had larger mPFC (*p = 0.001) and white matter volumes (*p = 0.002) than females. From left to right on the x-axis for the volume of the mPFC and the underlying white matter: 0, 200, 1000 μg/kg males followed by the respective female groups; n = 11, n = 9, n = 11, n = 10, n = 9, and n = 11, respectively.
There was no effect of phthalate exposure on the volume of the white matter under the mPFC (Fig. 7B), but there was, as expected (Markham et al., 2007), a significant sex difference (F(1,51) = 10.665, p = 0.002), indicating that males had larger white matter volumes than females.
Neuroanatomy-behavior correlations
The number of synaptophysin boutons was significantly, and positively, correlated with the number of correct choices in Set 2 of the attentional set shift task (r = 0.291, p = 0.033), such that rats of littermates with more boutons in the mPFC performed better than those of littermates with fewer. Although not significant, total synaptophysin boutons showed a trend toward a negative correlation with the total perseverative errors in Set 2 of the attentional set shift task (r = −0.243, p = 0.076). These findings were most prominent in the last 40 trials of Set 2 (i.e., the second half) when performance was generally better, such that rats with more synaptophysin boutons performed better on the task than those with fewer, as assessed by total correct choices (r = 0.327, p = 0.016) and perseverative errors (r = −0.302, p = 0.027; Table 1). There were no correlations between the behavioral measures and either neurons or glia.
Neuroanatomy-behavior correlationsa
Discussion
The present study found that perinatal exposure to phthalates resulted in decreased cognitive flexibility and a lower number of neurons and synapses in the mPFC in adulthood across both sexes. Interestingly, the number of synapses within the mPFC also correlated with measures of cognitive flexibility, such that littermates of rats with fewer synapses demonstrated less cognitive flexibility than those of littermates with more synapses. The lack of phthalate effects on the number of synapses per neuron indicates that the primary influence of phthalates may be on neuron number itself. However, it is also possible that the loss of neurons and loss of synapses are separate, interdependent effects. In contrast to the decreased number of neurons, there were no effects of phthalate exposure on the number of glia in the mPFC, so that not all cells decreased despite the reduced volume of the mPFC. It is possible that the lack of a phthalate effect on glia and the underlying white matter is due to the high capacity of glia, and thus myelin, to regenerate (Hattori et al., 1989; Askew et al., 2017).
The present study indicates that perinatal exposure to an environmentally relevant mixture of phthalates resulted in a long-term deficit in cognitive flexibility rather than general learning ability, such that phthalate-exposed male and female rats performed worse on the extradimensional shift, but not on the initial discrimination training. Although cognitive flexibility, to our knowledge, has never been directly examined following perinatal phthalate exposure in either rodents or humans, perinatal phthalate exposure in humans has been associated with symptoms related to autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, disorders with reduced cognitive flexibility (Etchepareborda and Mulas, 2004; Lai et al., 2017).
The phthalate-induced deficit in cognitive flexibility was not mediated by alterations in maternal care, as we have previously reported no effects of this phthalate mixture on maternal behavior (Kougias et al., 2018). However, based on our litter-specific correlations, it appears that this phthalate-induced deficit in cognitive flexibility may be mediated by phthalate-induced neuroanatomical changes. It is not clear why the total number of synapses, but not the number of neurons, correlated with behavior. However, considering that lesions outside of the mPFC are known to alter dendritic complexity and spine density within the mPFC (for review, see Kolb and Gibb, 2015), perhaps there are changes in other brain regions that are driving this deficit in behavior, such that synapses more appropriately reflect the perturbed circuitry. In support of this, a stress-induced decrease in dendritic arborization in the mPFC has been shown to be predictive of impaired performance in an attentional set-shifting task (Liston et al., 2006).
Interestingly, congruent with the behavioral effects of perinatal phthalate exposure, neonatal lesions of the rat mPFC have been shown to decrease cognitive flexibility in adulthood without affecting working memory (Schwabe et al., 2004; Schneider and Koch, 2005a). Moreover, neonatal lesions of the rat mPFC have been shown to disrupt periadolescent social play (Schneider and Koch, 2005b), which is consistent with what we previously reported following perinatal exposure to an identical phthalate mixture (Kougias et al., 2018). Together, this suggests that phthalates have neurotoxic effects on the developing mPFC, especially in light of the decreased number of neurons and synapses found in the current study in response to phthalate exposure.
Additionally, males learned the initial cue (i.e., training) and the extradimensional shift (i.e., cognitive flexibility) more quickly than females. This is broadly consistent with the literature, as sex differences in several learning and memory tasks have been reported in rats (Jonasson, 2005). Furthermore, it is well established that exposure to an acute stressful event can produce, or magnify, sex differences in learning and memory (for review, see Shors, 2004), as well as in behavioral flexibility (for review, see Hurtubise and Howland, 2017), but whether stressful events can generalize to mild food restriction is not well studied. However, Rajab et al. (2014) has demonstrated that 2 h of food restriction has a negative impact on water maze performance in females, but a positive influence in males. Thus, it is possible that the sex differences in cognitive performance found in this study were accentuated with food restriction.
As a class of endocrine-disrupting chemicals, phthalates have a promiscuous set of mechanistic actions that are specific to each phthalate and to the various metabolites (e.g., Chauvigné et al., 2009). Collectively, though, phthalates are known to have antiandrogenic, estrogenic, and antiestrogenic activity (Takeuchi et al., 2005); but given that both sexes were similarly affected by perinatal exposure, phthalates may not be disrupting gonadal steroid effects on sexual differentiation. Although there are indications that neonatal androgens can affect apoptosis in the visual cortex (Nuñez et al., 2000), whether there is a role for sex hormones in the perinatal development of the mPFC is not known. Rather, the ovarian hormones secreted at puberty are the major influence on the sex difference in neuron number in the mPFC (Koss et al., 2015). Furthermore, there are mixed data regarding this mixture's antiandrogenic potential reported by Kougias et al. (2018) where the mixture at either dose did not significantly affect anogenital distance, a reliable marker for prenatal androgen exposure, in either males or females. However, periadolescent social play, which is known to be positively influenced by neonatal androgens, was shown to be decreased at the lower dose, especially in males. Additionally, a similar mixture of five phthalates showed no antiandrogenic activity in vitro (Christen et al., 2012). Therefore, it is difficult to ascribe our results to disrupted gonadal hormones or to speculate on a collective toxicodynamic effect given the erratic nature of phthalate mixtures.
Moreover, phthalates can have such wide-ranging effects as repressing the synthesis of steroidogenic enzymes (Kim et al., 2004; David, 2006), weakly antagonizing cannabinoid receptors (Bisset et al., 2011), suppressing calcium signaling in nicotinic receptors (Liu et al., 2009), and interfering with thyroid (Boas et al., 2012), IGF-1 (Boas et al., 2010), and insulin signaling (Huang et al., 2014; Smerieri et al., 2015). In vitro phthalate exposure can also induce apoptosis in murine neurons (Lin et al., 2011), potentially in a hormone-independent manner (Wójtowicz et al., 2017), and can perhaps suppress cell proliferation (Chen et al., 2011). Thus, there are several potential mechanisms for how phthalates could have affected neuron number, but more detailed conclusions cannot be specified at this time.
Regardless of the mechanistic action of this phthalate mixture, the present neuroanatomical results complement the current literature on exposure to a single phthalate; however, our study, for the first time, shows a long-lasting neuroanatomical change. Previous studies on perinatal exposure to a single phthalate have demonstrated that phthalates can impair functional plasticity of hippocampal circuits and related behavior (for review, see Holahan and Smith, 2015). In particular, perinatal exposure to DBP at a very high dose (500 mg/kg) in rats has been shown to decrease synaptophysin within the hippocampus of both males and females immediately after exposure at P21, but this effect was not significant at P60 (Li et al., 2013). Additionally, prenatal exposure to DEHP has also been shown to result in abnormal neuronal distribution and reduced neuronal number at P7 in the cortex of mice (Komada et al., 2016); however, it is unclear which part of the cortex was investigated. Further research is needed to determine the mechanism underlying these neurotoxic effects of a mixture of phthalates.
Overall, this study is unique in showing that perinatal exposure to an environmentally relevant mixture of phthalates had long-term effects on the mPFC neuroanatomy and behavior of adult rats. These effects were independent of sex, suggesting a common neurotoxic effect of phthalates in the developing cortex of males and females. Furthermore, these effects were seen at both doses of the phthalate mixture, which were relatively low compared with the existing rodent literature and are presumably within the range of the estimated daily intakes of humans. Thus, these results may have serious implications for humans given that mPFC is involved in executive functions and is implicated in the pathology of many neuropsychiatric disorders.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences Grant P01 ES002848-Project 3 to J.M.J., U.S. Environmental Protection Agency Grant 83543401 Project 3 to J.M.J., and National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences Grant T32 ES007326 to D.G.K. This work was conducted in part at the Microscopy Suite of the Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. We thank Laura Cortes, Steven Rhoads, Anthony Franceschelli, and the Animal Care staff in Psychology for assistance.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Janice M. Juraska, Department of Psychology, University of Illinois, 603 E. Daniel Street, Champaign, IL 61820. jjuraska{at}illinois.edu