Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Neurobiology of Disease

Altered Gamma Oscillations during Motor Control in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Kyung-min An, Takashi Ikeda, Yuko Yoshimura, Chiaki Hasegawa, Daisuke N. Saito, Hirokazu Kumazaki, Tetsu Hirosawa, Yoshio Minabe and Mitsuru Kikuchi
Journal of Neuroscience 5 September 2018, 38 (36) 7878-7886; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1229-18.2018
Kyung-min An
1Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan,
2Division of Socio-Cognitive-Neuroscience, Department of Child Development United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Kyung-min An
Takashi Ikeda
1Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan,
2Division of Socio-Cognitive-Neuroscience, Department of Child Development United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Takashi Ikeda
Yuko Yoshimura
1Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan,
2Division of Socio-Cognitive-Neuroscience, Department of Child Development United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan,
3Institute of Human and Social Sciences, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa 920-1192, Japan, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Yuko Yoshimura
Chiaki Hasegawa
1Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Daisuke N. Saito
1Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan,
2Division of Socio-Cognitive-Neuroscience, Department of Child Development United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Daisuke N. Saito
Hirokazu Kumazaki
1Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan,
2Division of Socio-Cognitive-Neuroscience, Department of Child Development United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Hirokazu Kumazaki
Tetsu Hirosawa
4Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Yoshio Minabe
1Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan,
2Division of Socio-Cognitive-Neuroscience, Department of Child Development United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan,
4Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Mitsuru Kikuchi
1Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan,
2Division of Socio-Cognitive-Neuroscience, Department of Child Development United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Mitsuru Kikuchi
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Autism is hypothesized to result in a cortical excitatory and inhibitory imbalance driven by inhibitory interneuron dysfunction, which is associated with the generation of gamma oscillations. On the other hand, impaired motor control has been widely reported in autism. However, no study has focused on the gamma oscillations during motor control in autism. In the present study, we investigated the motor-related gamma oscillations in autism using magnetoencephalography. Magnetoencephalographic signals were recorded from 14 right-handed human children with autism (5 female), aged 5–7 years, and age- and IQ-matched 15 typically developing children during a motor task using their right index finger. Consistent with previous studies, the autism group showed a significantly longer button response time and reduced amplitude of motor-evoked magnetic fields. We observed that the autism group exhibited a low peak frequency of motor-related gamma oscillations from the contralateral primary motor cortex, and these were associated with the severity of autism symptoms. The autism group showed a reduced power of motor-related gamma oscillations in the bilateral primary motor cortex. A linear discriminant analysis using the button response time and gamma oscillations showed a high classification performance (86.2% accuracy). The alterations of the gamma oscillations in autism might reflect the cortical excitatory and inhibitory imbalance. Our findings provide an important clue into the behavioral and neurophysiological alterations in autism and a potential biomarker for autism.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Currently, the diagnosis of autism has been based on behavioral assessments, and a crucial issue in the diagnosis of autism is to identify objective and quantifiable clinical biomarkers. A key hypothesis of the neurophysiology of autism is an excitatory and inhibitory imbalance in the brain, which is associated with the generation of gamma oscillations. On the other hand, motor deficits have also been widely reported in autism. This is the first study to demonstrate low motor performance and altered motor-related gamma oscillations in autism, reflecting a brain excitatory and inhibitory imbalance. Using these behavioral and neurophysiological parameters, we classified autism and control group with good accuracy. This work provides important information on behavioral and neurophysiological alterations in patients with autism.

  • autism
  • E/I balance
  • gamma
  • magnetoencephalography
  • movement
  • young children

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by impaired social interactions, disordered communication, restricted interests, and repetitive behaviors (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Currently, the diagnosis of ASD is mainly based on behavioral observations. One of the crucial issues in the diagnosis of ASD is to identify an objective and quantifiable biomarker of ASD.

A key hypothesis of the neurophysiology of ASD is that the cortical excitatory and inhibitory (E/I) balance is altered by decreased neuronal inhibition in patients with ASD (Rubenstein and Merzenich, 2003; Rubenstein, 2010). The cortical E/I balance is highly associated with inhibitory GABAergic neurotransmission, which is reflected in gamma band oscillations (Traub et al., 2003; Whittington and Traub, 2003; Bartos et al., 2007; Cardin et al., 2009; Buzsáki and Wang, 2012). In previous studies using magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS), individuals with ASD exhibited significantly decreased levels of the inhibitory neurotransmitter GABA in the frontal lobe (Harada et al., 2011), auditory cortex (Gaetz et al., 2014; Rojas et al., 2014; Port et al., 2017), and motor cortex (Gaetz et al., 2014). GABA concentrations measured in vivo positively correlated with the frequency of gamma oscillations in the visual (Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2009) and motor cortices (Gaetz et al., 2011); that is, a low GABA concentration is associated with a low frequency of gamma oscillations. Because GABAergic dysfunction is one of the key hypotheses of the neurophysiology of ASD, a lower frequency of gamma oscillations would be expected to be observed in patients with ASD.

In addition, individuals with ASD have shown either a lack of or reduced gamma band activities during visual (Milne et al., 2009; Sun et al., 2012; Snijders et al., 2013), auditory (Wilson et al., 2007; Gandal et al., 2010), and tactile stimulations (Khan et al., 2015). We speculated that the reduced power of gamma oscillations would be observed in some other brain areas in subjects with ASD.

Notably, abnormalities in motor control have been widely reported in patients with ASD (Teitelbaum et al., 1998; Noterdaeme et al., 2002; Jansiewicz et al., 2006; Bryson et al., 2007; Fournier et al., 2010; London, 2014). A meta-analysis of 51 studies confirmed the prevalent and significant motor deficits in patients with ASD (Fournier et al., 2010). These motor abnormalities have been suggested to constitute a core symptom of ASD (Fournier et al., 2010; London, 2014). Additionally, these movement disturbances have been detected even in infants with ASD, and they potentially represent the earliest identifiable clinical dysfunction in subjects with ASD (Teitelbaum et al., 1998; Bryson et al., 2007). Regarding evoked cortical responses, some EEG studies have reported a reduced amplitude of motor-evoked potentials in patients with ASD (Rinehart et al., 2006; Enticott et al., 2009). However, no previous study has focused on the motor-induced gamma oscillations that reflect the cortical E/I balance in patients with ASD. A large number of previous studies on normal human subjects have reported an obvious increase in the spectral power of gamma band oscillations during motor control (Cheyne et al., 2008; Muthukumaraswamy, 2010; Cheyne and Ferrari, 2013; Cheyne, 2013). Gamma oscillations provide important information related to actual motor control and the initiation of movement (Muthukumaraswamy, 2010; Cheyne and Ferrari, 2013). These motor-induced gamma oscillations, which reflect the E/I balance, might be altered in subjects with ASD.

Based on the key neurophysiological hypothesis (reduced neuronal inhibition in ASD), we hypothesized that the ASD group in the present study would show altered motor-induced gamma oscillations with a low peak frequency and reduced power. In addition, as reported in the previous studies, we also hypothesized that the ASD group would show reduced motor-evoked fields and low behavioral performance during a motor task. Last, we examined whether these indices using the motor-induced gamma oscillations and behavioral performance represent a potentially sufficient biomarker of ASD.

To test our hypotheses, we recorded the motor-induced cortical oscillations during finger movement using child-customized magnetoencephalography (MEG) that provides a high temporal and good spatial resolution.

Materials and Methods

Participants.

Fourteen young children with ASD (mean ± SD age, 6.09 ± 0.64 years; 5 females) and 15 age- and IQ-matched typically developing (TD) children (5.78 ± 0.48 years; no female) participated in this study. All participants were right-handed based on the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971). Participants were recruited from Kanazawa University Hospital. Parents of all children provided full written informed consent to participate in the study, and the procedures were approved by the Ethics Committee of Kanazawa University Hospital.

The ASD diagnoses were based on DSM-V criteria for autism or Asperger syndrome (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), the Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders (Wing et al., 2002), and/or the Autism Diagnostic Observational Schedule, Generic (ADOS) (Lord et al., 2000). All diagnoses were confirmed by local psychiatrists and clinical speech therapists.

We assessed the intelligence of all participants using the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC), and a significant difference in achievement scores was not observed between the two groups (t(27) = 0.830, p = 0.414). The autistic traits of all the participants were evaluated by their parents based on the Social Responsiveness Scale-2 (SRS-2) (Constantino, 2012). A significant difference in SRS-2 scores was observed between the TD and ASD groups (t(27) = −5.724, p = 0.000021). The Vineland-II (Sparrow et al., 2005) “Movement” subtest was used to determine the general motor function of all the participants. The ASD group showed a significantly lower score for the “Movement” subscale (t(27) = 3.497, p = 0.002). Their low Vineland motor standard score was consistent with a previous study (Ozonoff et al., 2008). We provide additional details about the participants in Table 1.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Participant characteristicsa

Experimental design.

For child participants, we developed a video game-like motor task using Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems). Participants performed a video game-like motor task involving a button-press using their right index finger during MEG recordings. The video game-like motor task consisted of 10 blocks of 10 trials per block to collect 100 button-press responses. Button-press responses were measured using a nonmagnetic fiber optic response pad (Current Designs). Before starting the motor task, the participants were asked to hold a button response pad and rest their right index finger on a response button.

Figure 1A shows the experimental paradigm of the video game-like motor task during one trial. The character in the video game was a cute puppy. At the beginning of each trial, a mission image indicated which fruit would be a target for the puppy (see Fig. 1Aa). After 1200 ms, the puppy ran in the left side of the screen, and the fixation point was presented in the middle part of the screen (see Fig. 1Ab). The participants were asked to gaze at the fixation point to reduce artifacts due to eye movement. The target fruit image randomly appeared on the fixation point 1.5–2.5 s after the fixation point was presented (see Fig. 1Ac). If a visual target appeared, participants were instructed to press a button as soon as possible, but only once (see Fig. 1Ad). When the participant pressed a button, the puppy jumped and caught the fruit for 800 ms (see Fig. 1Ae). Visual target stimuli were presented randomly every 3.5–4.5 s after the button-press response. If the participant pressed a button without detecting the visual target, this failure caused the puppy to fall down, and the trial was repeated again. The failed trials were not used for data analysis. If the puppy collected 10 fruits, one block was completed. A fanfare was heard, and a bone with a red ribbon was given to the puppy as a prize after each block to encourage participants.

The MEG signals were recorded for 9 min during the motor task to collect 100 successful trials. The visual stimuli were projected on a screen using an LCD projector (IPSiO PJWX6170N, Ricoh). The degree of the visual angle was 21% in the vertical axis and 26% in the horizontal axis.

MEG recording.

Before the experiment, participants received a detailed explanation of the motor task and performed one block of the motor task as a practice trial to become familiar with the experimental paradigm and surroundings.

MEG recording conditions were similar to those reported in previous studies (Kikuchi et al., 2013; Yoshimura et al., 2014; Hasegawa et al., 2016). The cortical responses to finger movement were measured using a whole-head 151 channel MEG system for children (PQ 1151 R, Yokogawa/KIT), located in the MEG Center of Ricoh in a magnetically shielded room. Participants were placed in a comfortable supine position on a bed while they performed the motor task.

Four head-positioning coils were attached to the head surface (i.e., Cz, 5 cm anterior part from Cz, and 5 cm superior side of the left and right preauricular regions) to determine the location of the participant's head in the MEG helmet. We measured the locations of the positioning coils and >100 head surface points using a 3D digitizer (Fastrak, Polhemus). The locations of the positioning coils were recorded before the MEG recordings commenced. During the MEG recording, two experimenters were seated next to the participants in the shielded room to encourage them. In addition, the participants were carefully monitored using a video monitoring system to assess their compliance with the instructions and to record any notable artifacts, such as head motion, inappropriate head position, and consistent attention to the screen.

MEG data were digitized at a sampling rate of 2000 Hz and filtered with a 200 Hz low-pass filter. After MEG recording, the positioning coils were replaced with MRI-visible markers. Images of the brain structure were obtained from all participants using a 1.5 T MRI scanner (SIGNA Explorer, GE Healthcare) to compute the individual head models for the source analysis. The T1-weighted gradient echo and Silenz pulse sequence images (TR = 435.68 ms, TE = 0.024 ms, flip angle = 7°, FOV = 220 mm, matrix size = 256 × 256 pixels, slice thickness = 1.7 mm, and 130 transaxial images) were used as an anatomical reference.

Data analysis.

We analyzed the MEG data using the Brainstorm toolbox (Tadel et al., 2011) and MATLAB (The MathWorks). Raw data were bandpass filtered from 0.3 to 200 Hz and notch filtered at 60, 120, and 180 Hz. We rejected the artifacts caused by eye blinks, eye movements, and heartbeats using an independent component analysis method (“RunICA” implemented in Brainstorm, www.sccn.ucsd.edu/eeglab/). We identified the independent components representing the cardiac and ocular signals by visual inspection based on their time course and topography. After removing these artifacts, the remaining independent components were back-projected into the signal space. Thereafter, the data were segmented from −3 to 3 s following each button-press. We rejected the failed trials and trials containing muscle artifacts.

For the source analysis, we computed the weighted minimum norm estimates (wMNE) (Hämäläinen and Ilmoniemi, 1994; Hauk, 2004; Lin et al., 2006) implemented in the Brainstorm toolbox. Individual MRIs were used to build an overlapping sphere conductor model. We estimated the noise-covariance matrix for each subject using the premovement baseline period (−2 to −1.5 s). We performed the wMNE source analysis using an overlapping-sphere head model with a Tikhonov regularization factor (λ = 0.1).

All preprocessed trials were bandpass filtered between 0.3 to 30 Hz and averaged for each participant to obtain movement-related fields. The baseline was selected from −2 to −1.5 s before movement onset. We computed the cortical sources of individual motor fields (MFs) using wMNE, and these individual cortical sources were projected on the ICBM152 template anatomy in MNI coordinates (Table 2). Grand-averaged cortical sources for all participants in the TD and ASD groups were calculated (see Fig. 2A), and we confirmed that the maximum cortical source of MFs was located in the primary motor cortex (M1). For further analysis, we selected M1 from the Desikan-Killiany atlas (Desikan et al., 2006) defined using FreeSurfer version 6.0 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). We obtained the source waveforms by calculating the mean signals for every voxel in the contralateral M1.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Individual button response times and source locations and magnitudes of the MFs at 20–40 ms

For the time-frequency analysis, we calculated time-frequency representations (TFRs) in the bilateral M1 at 1–100 Hz using a 7 cycle Morlet-wavelet for each single trial source data. The TFRs were converted to percentage changes in power relative to the premovement baseline (−2 to −1.5 s). TFRs were averaged for each subject and then grand-averaged for all participants in the TD and ASD groups. In the TFRs from M1 (see Fig. 3), we visually observed group difference in the movement-induced gamma oscillations.

First, we determined the specific frequency, which had a maximum power within the −100 to 200 ms time window for the 60 to 100 Hz frequency range in the individual TFRs from the M1. Second, as shown in Figure 3, grand-averaged TFRs revealed that finger movement elicited a robust increase in the gamma band (70–90 Hz) in the bilateral M1 during the time windows of 0–100 ms. We averaged the power values in these time and frequency windows to calculate the power values for the gamma oscillations. We used these peak frequencies and power values in the subsequent statistical analyses.

Statistical analyses.

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS version 24.0 (IBM). We used two-sample t tests (two-tailed) to compare differences in the characteristics of participants in the TD and ASD groups in terms of age, K-ABC score, SRS-2 score, and score on the Vineland-II “Movement” subtest. To test our hypothesis, we applied two-sample t tests (one-tailed) to compare the button response time and amplitude of MFs. For comparison of the frequency and power of the movement-induced gamma oscillations, as we obtained these values from both hemispheres, we used two-way ANCOVA in which “diagnosis, 2 levels (1, TD and 2, ASD)” was the between-group factor, “hemisphere, 2 levels (1, contralateral and 2, ipsilateral)” was the within-group factor and sex served as the covariance (male = 0; female = 1). For variables displaying significant differences between two groups, we tested the correlation between these variables and ADOS scores (i.e., severity of symptoms) using Spearman's ρ correlation analysis. For all statistical tests, we used an alpha level of 0.05.

We applied Fisher's linear discriminant analysis with cross-validation to test its predictive accuracy in classifying the participants into two categories: TD and ASD. For this analysis, we used behavioral and cortical oscillatory parameters displaying robust significant differences between the two groups. In the cross-validation test, each case was classified by the functions derived from all other cases, and this process was repeated for all cases. Receiver operator characteristic (ROC) curves were plotted for sensitivity (on the y-axis) versus 1 − the specificity (on the x-axis). The area under the ROC curve (AUC) was used as an index of the participant's discriminative capacity.

As an additional analysis of male TD (n = 15) and male ASD (n = 9) groups, we compared variables displaying significant differences between the TD and ASD (including both genders) groups to exclude any gender effect.

Results

Button response time

To calculate the button response time (the latency between visual-target onset and button-press onset), we only analyzed successful trials, in which the participants pressed the response button within the allowed time window (200–2000 ms according to the visual trigger). Individual button response times are presented in Table 2. A significantly longer mean response time was observed for the ASD group (mean ± SD, 601.7 ± 183.1 ms) than for the TD group (438.7 ± 91.7 ms) (t(27) = −2.999, p = 0.004; Fig. 1B). In the additional analysis only for male subjects, this significant difference still remained (t(22) = −3.100, p = 0.005). The button response time of the ASD group (including both genders) was not significantly correlated with the ADOS score (ρ = 0.341, p = 0.233).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Experimental paradigm and button response times for the TD and ASD groups. A, The video game-like motor task was developed for child participants. The goal of this motor task is to collect fruits. While the puppy is running, fruits appear as a visual target. After the mission image is presented (Aa), the fixation point is randomly presented in the middle part of the screen for 1.5–2 s (Ab). When the target appears at the fixation point (Ac), participants press the button as soon as possible (Ad). The puppy jumps to collect the fruits after the participant presses the button (Ae). In one trial, the visual target randomly appears every 3.5–4.5 s after the button press, and this process is repeated 10 times in each of 10 blocks. B, The ASD group showed a significantly prolonged button response time than the TD group (t(27) = −2.999, p = 0.004). **p < 0.01.

Motor-evoked magnetic fields

Figure 2A shows the grand-averaged cortical sources of MF components (t = 20–40 ms) in the 15 TD children and 14 children with ASD. The cortical sources of MFs were observed in the sensorimotor and premotor cortices in both groups. We observed lower cortical activation of MFs in the ASD group than in the TD group. Individual peak source locations and magnitudes for the MFs are presented in Table 2. In the contralateral M1, the grand-averaged source waveforms showed MF peaks at ∼30 ms following movement onset in both groups (Fig. 2B). The ASD group showed a significantly reduced peak amplitude of MFs compared with the TD group in the 20–40 ms time window (t(27) = 2.251, p = 0.017). In the additional analysis only for male subjects, this significant difference still remained (t(22) = 1.995, p = 0.030). The amplitude of MFs was not correlated with the ADOS total score in the ASD group (including both genders) (ρ = −0.310, p = 0.281).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Cortical sources and source waveforms of MFs in the TD and ASD groups. A, Grand-averaged cortical sources of the MFs at 20–40 ms in the TD (top) and ASD groups (bottom). Both groups showed motor-evoked cortical activity in the sensorimotor cortex and premotor cortex. B, Grand-averaged source waveforms (filtered 0.5–30 Hz) from the contralateral M1 in the TD (blue trace) and ASD groups (red trace). A significantly greater amplitude of the MF component (asterisk) was observed in the ASD group than in the TD group (t(27) = 2.251, p = 0.017). L, Left hemisphere (i.e., contralateral); R, right hemisphere (i.e., ipsilateral). *p < 0.05.

Motor-related gamma oscillations

Group-averaged TRFs from the bilateral M1 during finger movement were separately plotted for the TD and ASD groups (Fig. 3). We observed movement-induced gamma oscillations from the bilateral M1 in the 70 to 90 Hz range.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Group-averaged time-frequency plots for the TD and ASD groups. Movement-related oscillatory changes are shown for the bilateral M1 in the TD (top) and ASD groups (bottom). Yellow and red represent relative increases in power. Blue represents relative decreases in power compared with the power of the premovement baseline (−2 to −1.5 s).

The motor-related gamma oscillations appeared at movement onset and lasted for ∼100 ms. The mean power and peak frequency of the gamma oscillations in each group are shown in Table 3. Regarding the gamma frequency, the two-way ANCOVA revealed a significant interaction (i.e., group vs hemisphere; F(1,26) = 4.453, p = 0.045). As a result of the post hoc test between two groups for contralateral and ipsilateral M1, the ASD group exhibited a lower peak frequency of motor-related gamma oscillations from the contralateral M1, as shown in Figure 4A (t(27) = 2.825, p = 0.005), but not from the ipsilateral M1 (t(27) = 0.365, p = 0.359). In the additional analysis only for male subjects, this significant difference observed in the contralateral M1 still remained (t(22) = 2.732, p = 0.006). In the ASD group (including both genders), the peak frequency of gamma oscillations from the contralateral M1 correlated inversely with the ADOS score, reflecting the severity of social interaction and communication symptoms (ρ = −0.618, p = 0.019) (Fig. 4B). In the additional analysis only for male subjects, this significant correlation still remained (ρ = −0.774, p = 0.014).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Motor-related gamma oscillations in the bilateral primary motor cortexa

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Frequencies of the contralateral gamma oscillations in the TD and ASD groups and their correlation with the ADOS score in subjects with ASD. A, The ASD group showed a lower frequency of motor-related gamma oscillations from the contralateral M1 (t(27) = 2.825, p = 0.005). B, Scatterplot showing the correlation between the frequency of the contralateral motor-related gamma oscillations and the ADOS total score. The negative correlation between the frequency of the gamma oscillations and ADOS total score is shown (Spearman's ρ = −0.618, p = 0.019). **p < 0.01.

Figure 5A shows the cortical sources of motor-related gamma oscillations in both participant groups. Regarding the gamma power, the two-way ANCOVA revealed no significant interaction (i.e., group vs hemisphere; F(1,26) = 0.946, p = 0.340); however, there was a significant main group effect (i.e., TD vs ASD; F(1,26) = 7.618, p = 0.010) and a significant main hemisphere effect (i.e., contralateral vs ipsilateral; F(1,26) = 11.682, p = 0.002). As a result of the post hoc test between two groups for contralateral and ipsilateral M1 (Fig. 5B), the ASD group showed a reduced gamma power in the contralateral (t(27) = 2.165, p = 0.020) and ipsilateral M1 (t(27) = 3.158, p = 0.002) compared with the TD group. In the additional analysis only for male subjects, this significant differences were still remained in the contralateral (t(22) = 2.338, p = 0.015) and ipsilateral M1 (t(22) = 2.792, p = 0.005). In the ASD group (including both genders), the power of gamma oscillations from the bilateral M1 was not significantly correlated with the ADOS score (contralateral: ρ = −0.300, p = 0.298; ipsilateral: ρ = 0.371, p = 0.192).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Cortical sources of the motor-related gamma oscillations in the TD and ASD groups and power comparisons between the two groups. A, Finger movement increased the power of gamma oscillations in the sensorimotor cortex. The peak location is noted in MNI coordinates. The ASD group (bottom) showed a reduced gamma power compared with the TD group (top). B, Comparison of the bilateral gamma power between the TD and ASD groups. The ASD group showed a reduced gamma power in the contralateral (t(27) = 2.165, p = 0.020) and ipsilateral M1 (t(27) = 3.158, p = 0.002). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

Classification using linear discriminant analysis

We observed robust significant differences in the button response time, the frequency of contralateral M1 gamma, and the power of ipsilateral M1 gamma between the two groups. Therefore, we initially used these three variables to classify participants into the TD and ASD groups. A linear discriminant analysis classifier identified participants in the two groups with 86.2% accuracy (85.7% sensitivity and 86.7% specificity). Even when we used two of the three parameters (i.e., button response time and power of the ipsilateral M1 gamma oscillations), the linear discriminant analysis classifier correctly identified the group assignments of the participants with 86.2% accuracy (85.7% sensitivity and 86.7% specificity) (Fig. 6A). The ROC curve showed the predictive ability, as the AUC was 91% (Fig. 6B).

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Discriminant classifier results using behavioral and neurophysiological parameters. A, Based on the parameters of response time and ipsilateral gamma power, the linear discriminant analysis accurately classified 86.2% of subjects in the TD and ASD groups (sensitivity = 85.7%; specificity = 86.7%). B, The ROC curve shows a good discriminative capacity for participants with an area under the ROC curve (AUC) value of 0.91.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this neurophysiological study is the first to explore gamma oscillations during motor control in patients with ASD. The ASD group showed a prolonged response time during the motor task compared with the TD group. We observed a low peak frequency and reduced power of motor-related gamma oscillations in the ASD group. As expected, we identified a sufficient index to classify the TD and ASD groups using behavioral performance and neurophysiological gamma oscillations.

Button response time

The ASD group showed a button response time that was ∼160 ms longer than that in the TD group. Previous behavioral studies have reported low motor performance on tasks involving gait and balance, fine and gross movement, and movement planning in individuals with ASD (Teitelbaum et al., 1998; Noterdaeme et al., 2002; Jansiewicz et al., 2006; Bryson et al., 2007; Mostofsky et al., 2009; Fournier et al., 2010). In addition, individuals with ASD have shown a delay in the latency to movement during a precued motor task (Glazebrook et al., 2008; Nazarali et al., 2009). Consistent with the results from these previous studies, we observed lower motor performance in the ASD group in the present study.

Motor-evoked magnetic fields

We observed the expected cortical sources of MF components in the sensorimotor cortex and premotor cortex. In the contralateral M1, the latencies of the MFs were ∼30 ms after movement onset. Although MFs from adult participants have been observed at ∼50 ms before a mechanical button press (Cheyne and Weinberg, 1989; Kristeva et al., 1991), children showed prolonged latencies of MFs at ∼20 ms after the button press (Cheyne et al., 2014), similar to the values reported in the present study.

In the present study, the amplitude of the MF components was decreased in the ASD group, similar to previous EEG studies reporting that individuals with ASD exhibited abnormalities in movement-related potentials (Rinehart et al., 2006; Enticott et al., 2009). The amplitude of MFs in subjects with ASD was not correlated with the ADOS total score. The severity of ASD symptoms might be not reflected in the movement-evoked cortical activity (i.e., MFs).

Motor-related gamma oscillations

Both groups of children displayed robust movement-related gamma oscillations from the M1 in the 70–90 Hz range at the ∼0–100 ms time window. Previous MEG studies have reported that transient finger movements induced gamma oscillations from the M1 in children (Gaetz et al., 2010; Cheyne et al., 2014), similar to the gamma oscillations described in adults (Cheyne et al., 2008; Muthukumaraswamy, 2010).

Transient and narrow-band gamma oscillations are highly localized in the M1 in the 70–90 Hz range, as determined using electrocorticograms (Pfurtscheller et al., 2003; Ball et al., 2008), scalp EEG (Ball et al., 2008; Darvas et al., 2010), and MEG recordings (Cheyne et al., 2008; Muthukumaraswamy, 2010). Movement-related gamma oscillations have been observed for both cued and voluntary movements and were observed during active but not passive movement (Muthukumaraswamy, 2010). Movement-related gamma oscillations might reflect a disinhibition of movement through corticobasal ganglia motor circuits and have a facilitatory effect on movement initiation (Cheyne et al., 2008). In the present study, we identified two aspects of motor-related gamma oscillations that were altered in the ASD compared with the TD group.

First, we observed a significantly lower peak frequency of gamma oscillations in the ASD than the TD group. Gamma band oscillations are generated by GABAergic interneurons, which are attributed to the cortical E/I balance (Traub et al., 2003; Whittington and Traub, 2003; Bartos et al., 2007; Cardin et al., 2009; Buzsáki and Wang, 2012). The E/I imbalance has been reported as a key neurophysiological hypothesis of ASD (Rubenstein and Merzenich, 2003; Rubenstein, 2010). Using MRS, a low concentration of the inhibitory neurotransmitter GABA in M1 has been reported in individuals with ASD (Gaetz et al., 2014), supporting the E/I imbalance (toward excitatory) model of autism. Regarding the peak frequency of gamma oscillations and the GABAergic system, pharmacological human studies have produced controversial results. The frequency of gamma oscillations induced by visual stimuli was decreased following the administration of GABA enhancer (Campbell et al., 2014; Lozano-Soldevilla et al., 2014; Magazzini et al., 2016), whereas gamma oscillations induced by the movement task were not affected after GABA enhancer administration (Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2013; Campbell et al., 2014; Lozano-Soldevilla et al., 2014). Intriguingly, nonpharmacological human studies using MRS and MEG have demonstrated positive relationships between the GABA concentration and the gamma frequency in visual (Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2009) and motor (Gaetz et al., 2011) cortices. In the present study, the frequency of motor-related gamma oscillations in the ASD group was lower than those in the TD group. Therefore, we speculate that the lower frequency of motor-related gamma oscillations observed in the ASD group is related to their lower GABA concentration in the M1. In addition, a significant negative correlation between the peak frequency of gamma oscillations and the ADOS total score was observed, reflecting the ASD symptom severity. This correlation implied that the subjects with severe autism symptoms tended to display a low peak frequency of motor-related gamma oscillations, reflecting a low GABA concentration.

Second, the ASD group showed a significant reduction in motor-related gamma power in the bilateral M1. Reduced gamma band activities during sensory processing have been reported in individuals with ASD (Simon and Wallace, 2016). Gamma activity has been found to be either absent or reduced in individuals with ASD in response to visual (Milne et al., 2009; Sun et al., 2012; Snijders et al., 2013), auditory (Wilson et al., 2007; Gandal et al., 2010), and tactile stimulations (Khan et al., 2015). Although motor-related gamma responses differ from other sensory-related gamma responses in many respects, the motor-related gamma oscillations were also disrupted in the ASD group in the present study, similar to other sensory-related gamma oscillations in the ASD group.

The observation of altered motor-related gamma oscillations in children with ASD may be the result of a regional downregulation in neurotransmitter (i.e., GABA) levels in the motor cortex, which might account for the cortical E/I imbalance of individuals with ASD. Additionally, there is a possibility that altered motor-related gamma oscillations could reflect the immature or delayed development of motor control in young children with ASD. A previous study using MEG demonstrated that some younger children (e.g., 3–4 years old) showed motor-related gamma oscillations predominantly in the lower gamma frequency (i.e., 35–45 Hz) (Cheyne et al., 2014). Therefore, the results from the present study may be explained by the cortical E/I imbalance and/or immature motor system in young children with ASD.

In conclusion, although the cortical E/I imbalance and motor deficits have been widely reported in individuals with ASD, this is the first study to focus on gamma oscillations (a candidate indicator of the E/I balance) during motor control in subjects with ASD. In the present MEG study, we investigated gamma oscillations during a video game-like motor task in young children with ASD and age- and IQ-matched TD children. We observed behavioral and neurophysiological alterations in the ASD group. A prolonged button response time in the ASD group might reflect disruptions in basic motor control. The low peak frequency and reduced power of motor gamma oscillations in subjects with ASD suggested that they had lower GABA concentrations and a neural E/I imbalance. The low peak frequency of motor-related gamma oscillations correlated with the lower social ability among the ASD symptoms. Using these behavioral performance and cortical gamma oscillation findings, we could classify participants into the TD and ASD groups with good accuracy.

Further studies with a longitudinal design, larger sample size, and wider age range are necessary to draw a definitive conclusion regarding the neurodevelopmental alterations in individuals with ASD and to assess a more reliable discriminant classifier between TD and ASD.

During the MEG recordings, we recorded the head movement of the children subjects using video monitors. MEG signals, where head of the subject obviously moved, were eliminated from the analysis by visual inspection. Further investigations with a quantification algorithm for head movement will provide more reliable data.

In the present study, we focused on young children with ASD and TD children because an early diagnosis of ASD is helpful in supporting developmental follow-up in children with ASD. Our study provides important information that will improve our understanding of the neurophysiological mechanism underlying the earlier development of social abilities and motor control in children with ASD. As a highly noninvasive method, MEG could provide a potential biomarker for ASD by applying the observed behavioral and neurophysiological alterations in patients with ASD.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by Center of Innovation Program from the Japan Science and Technology Agency (https://www.coistream.osaka-u.ac.jp/en). The funder had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. We thank Sachiko Kitagawa and Yukiko Saotome for conducting the behavioral and MEG experiments; Mutsumi Ozawa and Yoko Morita for preparing the experiments; and the children and their parents who participated in this study.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Mitsuru Kikuchi, Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, 13-1 Takara-machi, Kanazawa 920-8640, Japan. mitsuruk{at}med.kanazawa-u.ac.jpm

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, Ed 5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
  2. ↵
    1. Ball T,
    2. Demandt E,
    3. Mutschler I,
    4. Neitzel E,
    5. Mehring C,
    6. Vogt K,
    7. Aertsen A,
    8. Schulze-Bonhage A
    (2008) Movement related activity in the high gamma range of the human EEG. Neuroimage 41:302–310. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.032 pmid:18424182
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Bartos M,
    2. Vida I,
    3. Jonas P
    (2007) Synaptic mechanisms of synchronized gamma oscillations in inhibitory interneuron networks. Nat Rev Neurosci 8:45–56. doi:10.1038/nrn2044 pmid:17180162
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Bryson SE,
    2. Zwaigenbaum L,
    3. Brian J,
    4. Roberts W,
    5. Szatmari P,
    6. Rombough V,
    7. McDermott C
    (2007) A prospective case series of high-risk infants who developed autism. J Autism Dev Disord 37:12–24. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0328-2 pmid:17211728
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Buzsáki G,
    2. Wang XJ
    (2012) Mechanisms of gamma oscillations. Annu Rev Neurosci 35:203–225. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150444 pmid:22443509
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Campbell AE,
    2. Sumner P,
    3. Singh KD,
    4. Muthukumaraswamy SD
    (2014) Acute effects of alcohol on stimulus-induced gamma oscillations in human primary visual and motor cortices. Neuropsychopharmacology 39:2104–2113. doi:10.1038/npp.2014.58 pmid:24622470
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Cardin JA,
    2. Carlén M,
    3. Meletis K,
    4. Knoblich U,
    5. Zhang F,
    6. Deisseroth K,
    7. Tsai LH,
    8. Moore CI
    (2009) Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature 459:663–667. doi:10.1038/nature08002 pmid:19396156
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Cheyne D
    (2013) MEG studies of sensorimotor rhythms: a review. Exp Neurol 245:27–39. doi:10.1016/j.expneurol.2012.08.030 pmid:22981841
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Cheyne D,
    2. Weinberg H
    (1989) Neuromagnetic fields accompanying unilateral finger movements: pre-movement and movement-evoked fields. Exp Brain Res 78:604–612. pmid:2612603
    OpenUrlPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Cheyne D,
    2. Ferrari P
    (2013) MEG studies of motor cortex gamma oscillations: evidence for a gamma ‘fingerprint’ in the brain? Front Hum Neurosci 7:575. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00575 pmid:24062675
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Cheyne D,
    2. Bells S,
    3. Ferrari P,
    4. Gaetz W,
    5. Bostan AC
    (2008) Self-paced movements induce high-frequency gamma oscillations in primary motor cortex. Neuroimage 42:332–342. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.178 pmid:18511304
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Cheyne D,
    2. Jobst C,
    3. Tesan G,
    4. Crain S,
    5. Johnson B
    (2014) Movement-related neuromagnetic fields in preschool age children. Hum Brain Mapp 35:4858–4875. doi:10.1002/hbm.22518 pmid:24700413
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Constantino JN
    (2012) Social Responsiveness Scale, Ed 2 (SRS-2). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
  14. ↵
    1. Darvas F,
    2. Scherer R,
    3. Ojemann JG,
    4. Rao RP,
    5. Miller KJ,
    6. Sorensen LB
    (2010) High gamma mapping using EEG. Neuroimage 49:930–938. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.041 pmid:19715762
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Desikan RS,
    2. Ségonne F,
    3. Fischl B,
    4. Quinn BT,
    5. Dickerson BC,
    6. Blacker D,
    7. Buckner RL,
    8. Dale AM,
    9. Maguire RP,
    10. Hyman BT,
    11. Albert MS,
    12. Killiany RJ
    (2006) An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31:968–980. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021 pmid:16530430
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Enticott PG,
    2. Bradshaw JL,
    3. Iansek R,
    4. Tonge BJ,
    5. Rinehart NJ
    (2009) Electrophysiological signs of supplementary-motor-area deficits in high-functioning autism but not Asperger syndrome: an examination of internally cued movement-related potentials. Dev Med Child Neurol 51:787–791. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03270.x pmid:19416338
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Fournier KA,
    2. Hass CJ,
    3. Naik SK,
    4. Lodha N,
    5. Cauraugh JH
    (2010) Motor coordination in autism spectrum disorders: a synthesis and meta-analysis. J Autism Dev Disord 40:1227–1240. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-0981-3 pmid:20195737
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Gaetz W,
    2. MacDonald M,
    3. Cheyne D,
    4. Snead OC
    (2010) Neuromagnetic imaging of movement-related cortical oscillations in children and adults: age predicts post-movement beta rebound. Neuroimage 51:792–807. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.077 pmid:20116434
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Gaetz W,
    2. Edgar JC,
    3. Wang DJ,
    4. Roberts TP
    (2011) Relating MEG measured motor cortical oscillations to resting γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) concentration. Neuroimage 55:616–621. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.077 pmid:21215806
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Gaetz W,
    2. Bloy L,
    3. Wang DJ,
    4. Port RG,
    5. Blaskey L,
    6. Levy SE,
    7. Roberts TP
    (2014) GABA estimation in the brains of children on the autism spectrum: measurement precision and regional cortical variation. Neuroimage 86:1–9. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.068 pmid:23707581
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Gandal MJ,
    2. Edgar JC,
    3. Ehrlichman RS,
    4. Mehta M,
    5. Roberts TP,
    6. Siegel SJ
    (2010) Validating γ oscillations and delayed auditory responses as translational biomarkers of autism. Biol Psychiatry 68:1100–1106. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.031 pmid:21130222
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Glazebrook CM,
    2. Elliott D,
    3. Szatmari P
    (2008) How do individuals with autism plan their movements? J Autism Dev Disord 38:114–126. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0369-1 pmid:17436068
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Hämäläinen MS,
    2. Ilmoniemi RJ
    (1994) Interpreting magnetic fields of the brain: minimum norm estimates. Med Biol Eng Comput 32:35–42. doi:10.1007/BF02512476 pmid:8182960
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Harada M,
    2. Taki MM,
    3. Nose A,
    4. Kubo H,
    5. Mori K,
    6. Nishitani H,
    7. Matsuda T
    (2011) Non-invasive evaluation of the GABAergic/glutamatergic system in autistic patients observed by MEGA-editing proton MR spectroscopy using a clinical 3 tesla instrument. J Autism Dev Disord 41:447–454. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1065-0 pmid:20652388
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Hasegawa C,
    2. Ikeda T,
    3. Yoshimura Y,
    4. Hiraishi H,
    5. Takahashi T,
    6. Furutani N,
    7. Hayashi N,
    8. Minabe Y,
    9. Hirata M,
    10. Asada M,
    11. Kikuchi M
    (2016) Mu rhythm suppression reflects mother-child face-to-face interactions: a pilot study with simultaneous MEG recording. Sci Rep 6:34977. doi:10.1038/srep34977 pmid:27721481
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Hauk O
    (2004) Keep it simple: a case for using classical minimum norm estimation in the analysis of EEG and MEG data. Neuroimage 21:1612–1621. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.018 pmid:15050585
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Jansiewicz EM,
    2. Goldberg MC,
    3. Newschaffer CJ,
    4. Denckla MB,
    5. Landa R,
    6. Mostofsky SH
    (2006) Motor signs distinguish children with high functioning autism and Asperger's syndrome from controls. J Autism Dev Disord 36:613–621. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0109-y pmid:16609826
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Khan S,
    2. Michmizos K,
    3. Tommerdahl M,
    4. Ganesan S,
    5. Kitzbichler MG,
    6. Zetino M,
    7. Garel KL,
    8. Herbert MR,
    9. Hämäläinen MS,
    10. Kenet T
    (2015) Somatosensory cortex functional connectivity abnormalities in autism show opposite trends, depending on direction and spatial scale. Brain 138:1394–1409. doi:10.1093/brain/awv043 pmid:25765326
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Kikuchi M,
    2. Shitamichi K,
    3. Yoshimura Y,
    4. Ueno S,
    5. Hiraishi H,
    6. Hirosawa T,
    7. Munesue T,
    8. Nakatani H,
    9. Tsubokawa T,
    10. Haruta Y,
    11. Oi M,
    12. Niida Y,
    13. Remijn GB,
    14. Takahashi T,
    15. Suzuki M,
    16. Higashida H,
    17. Minabe Y
    (2013) Altered brain connectivity in 3-to 7-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder. Neuroimage Clin 2:394–401. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2013.03.003 pmid:24179793
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Kristeva R,
    2. Cheyne D,
    3. Deecke L
    (1991) Neuromagnetic fields accompanying unilateral and bilateral voluntary movements: topography and analysis of cortical sources. Clin Neurophysiol 81:284–298. pmid:1714823
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Lin FH,
    2. Witzel T,
    3. Ahlfors SP,
    4. Stufflebeam SM,
    5. Belliveau JW,
    6. Hämäläinen MS
    (2006) Assessing and improving the spatial accuracy in MEG source localization by depth-weighted minimum-norm estimates. Neuroimage 31:160–171. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.054 pmid:16520063
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. London EB
    (2014) Categorical diagnosis: a fatal flaw for autism research? Trends Neurosci 37:683–686. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2014.10.003 pmid:25465942
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Lord C,
    2. Risi S,
    3. Lambrecht L,
    4. Cook EH Jr.,
    5. Leventhal BL,
    6. DiLavore PC,
    7. Pickles A,
    8. Rutter M
    (2000) The autism diagnostic schedule–generic: a standard measures of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord 30:205–223. doi:10.1023/A:1005592401947 pmid:11055457
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Lozano-Soldevilla D,
    2. ter Huurne N,
    3. Cools R,
    4. Jensen O
    (2014) GABAergic modulation of visual gamma and alpha oscillations and its consequences for working memory performance. Curr Biol 24:2878–2887. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.10.017 pmid:25454585
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Magazzini L,
    2. Muthukumaraswamy SD,
    3. Campbell AE,
    4. Hamandi K,
    5. Lingford-Hughes A,
    6. Myers JF,
    7. Nutt DJ,
    8. Sumner P,
    9. Wilson SJ,
    10. Singh KD
    (2016) Significant reductions in human visual gamma frequency by the GABA reuptake inhibitor tiagabine revealed by robust peak frequency estimation. Hum Brain Mapp 37:3882–3896. doi:10.1002/hbm.23283 pmid:27273695
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Milne E,
    2. Scope A,
    3. Pascalis O,
    4. Buckley D,
    5. Makeig S
    (2009) Independent component analysis reveals atypical electroencephalographic activity during visual perception in individuals with autism. Biol Psychiatry 65:22–30. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.07.017 pmid:18774554
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Mostofsky SH,
    2. Powell SK,
    3. Simmonds DJ,
    4. Goldberg MC,
    5. Caffo B,
    6. Pekar JJ
    (2009) Decreased connectivity and cerebellar activity in autism during motor task performance. Brain 132:2413–2425. doi:10.1093/brain/awp088 pmid:19389870
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Muthukumaraswamy SD
    (2010) Functional properties of human primary motor cortex gamma oscillations. J Neurophysiol 104:2873–2885. doi:10.1152/jn.00607.2010 pmid:20884762
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Muthukumaraswamy SD,
    2. Edden RA,
    3. Jones DK,
    4. Swettenham JB,
    5. Singh KD
    (2009) Resting GABA concentration predicts peak gamma frequency and fMRI amplitude in response to visual stimulation in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:8356–8361. doi:10.1073/pnas.0900728106 pmid:19416820
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    1. Muthukumaraswamy SD,
    2. Myers JF,
    3. Wilson SJ,
    4. Nutt DJ,
    5. Lingford-Hughes A,
    6. Singh KD,
    7. Hamandi K
    (2013) The effects of elevated endogenous GABA levels on movement-related network oscillations. Neuroimage 66:36–41. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.054 pmid:23110884
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Nazarali N,
    2. Glazebrook CM,
    3. Elliott D
    (2009) Movement planning and reprogramming in individuals with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 39:1401–1411. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0756-x pmid:19466535
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Noterdaeme M,
    2. Mildenberger K,
    3. Minow F,
    4. Amorosa H
    (2002) Evaluation of neuromotor deficits in children with autism and children with a specific speech and language disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 11:219–225. doi:10.1007/s00787-002-0285-z pmid:12469239
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Oldfield RC
    (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4 pmid:5146491
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Ozonoff S,
    2. Young GS,
    3. Goldring S,
    4. Greiss-Hess L,
    5. Herrera AM,
    6. Steele J,
    7. Macari S,
    8. Hepburn S,
    9. Rogers SJ
    (2008) Gross motor development, movement abnormalities, and early identification of autism. J Autism Dev Disord 38:644–656. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0430-0 pmid:17805956
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Pfurtscheller G,
    2. Graimann B,
    3. Huggins JE,
    4. Levine SP,
    5. Schuh LA
    (2003) Spatiotemporal patterns of beta desynchronization and gamma synchronization in corticographic data during self-paced movement. Clin Neurophysiol 114:1226–1236. doi:10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00067-1 pmid:12842719
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Port RG,
    2. Gaetz W,
    3. Bloy L,
    4. Wang DJ,
    5. Blaskey L,
    6. Kuschner ES,
    7. Levy SE,
    8. Brodkin ES,
    9. Roberts TP
    (2017) Exploring the relationship between cortical GABA concentrations, auditory gamma-band responses and development in ASD: evidence for an altered maturational trajectory in ASD. Autism Res 10:593–607. doi:10.1002/aur.1686 pmid:27696740
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Rinehart NJ,
    2. Tonge BJ,
    3. Bradshaw JL,
    4. Iansek R,
    5. Enticott PG,
    6. Johnson KA
    (2006) Movement-related potentials in high-functioning autism and Asperger's disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol 48:272–277. doi:10.1017/S0012162206000594 pmid:16542514
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Rojas DC,
    2. Singel D,
    3. Steinmetz S,
    4. Hepburn S,
    5. Brown MS
    (2014) Decreased left perisylvian GABA concentration in children with autism and unaffected siblings. Neuroimage 86:28–34. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.045 pmid:23370056
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Rubenstein JL
    (2010) Three hypotheses for developmental defects that may underlie some forms of autism spectrum disorder. Curr Opin Neurol 23:118–123. doi:10.1097/WCO.0b013e328336eb13 pmid:20087182
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Rubenstein JL,
    2. Merzenich MM
    (2003) Model of autism: increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes Brain Behav 2:255–267. doi:10.1034/j.1601-183X.2003.00037.x pmid:14606691
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Simon DM,
    2. Wallace MT
    (2016) Dysfunction of sensory oscillations in autism spectrum disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 68:848–861. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.016 pmid:27451342
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Snijders TM,
    2. Milivojevic B,
    3. Kemner C
    (2013) Atypical excitation-inhibition balance in autism captured by the gamma response to contextual modulation. Neuroimage Clin 3:65–72. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2013.06.015 pmid:24179850
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Sparrow SS,
    2. Cicchetti DV,
    3. Balla DA
    (2005) Vineland adaptive behavior scales, Ed 2. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
  54. ↵
    1. Sun L,
    2. Grützner C,
    3. Bölte S,
    4. Wibral M,
    5. Tozman T,
    6. Schlitt S,
    7. Poustka F,
    8. Singer W,
    9. Freitag CM,
    10. Uhlhaas PJ
    (2012) Impaired gamma-band activity during perceptual organization in adults with autism spectrum disorders: evidence for dysfunctional network activity in frontal-posterior cortices. J Neurosci 32:9563–9573. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1073-12.2012 pmid:22787042
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  55. ↵
    1. Tadel F,
    2. Baillet S,
    3. Mosher JC,
    4. Pantazis D,
    5. Leahy RM
    (2011) Brainstorm: a user-friendly application for MEG/EEG analysis. Comput Intell Neurosci 2011:879716. doi:10.1155/2011/879716 pmid:21584256
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Teitelbaum P,
    2. Teitelbaum O,
    3. Nye J,
    4. Fryman J,
    5. Maurer RG
    (1998) Movement analysis in infancy may be useful for early diagnosis of autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95:13982–13987. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.23.13982 pmid:9811912
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    1. Traub RD,
    2. Cunningham MO,
    3. Gloveli T,
    4. LeBeau FE,
    5. Bibbig A,
    6. Buhl EH,
    7. Whittington MA
    (2003) GABA-enhanced collective behavior in neuronal axons underlies persistent gamma-frequency oscillations. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100:11047–11052. doi:10.1073/pnas.1934854100 pmid:12960382
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. ↵
    1. Whittington MA,
    2. Traub RD
    (2003) Interneuron diversity series: inhibitory interneurons and network oscillations in vitro. Trends Neurosci 26:676–682. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2003.09.016 pmid:14624852
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Wilson TW,
    2. Rojas DC,
    3. Reite ML,
    4. Teale PD,
    5. Rogers SJ
    (2007) Children and adolescents with autism exhibit reduced MEG steady-state gamma responses. Biol Psychiatry 62:192–197. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.002 pmid:16950225
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Wing L,
    2. Leekam SR,
    3. Libby SJ,
    4. Gould J,
    5. Larcombe M
    (2002) The diagnostic interview for social and communication disorders: background, inter-rater reliability and clinical use. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 43:307–325. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00023 pmid:11944874
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Yoshimura Y,
    2. Kikuchi M,
    3. Ueno S,
    4. Shitamichi K,
    5. Remijn GB,
    6. Hiraishi H,
    7. Hasegawa C,
    8. Furutani N,
    9. Oi M,
    10. Munesue T,
    11. Tsubokawa T,
    12. Higashida H,
    13. Minabe Y
    (2014) A longitudinal study of auditory evoked field and language development in young children. Neuroimage 101:440–447. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.034 pmid:25067819
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 38 (36)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 38, Issue 36
5 Sep 2018
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Altered Gamma Oscillations during Motor Control in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Altered Gamma Oscillations during Motor Control in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Kyung-min An, Takashi Ikeda, Yuko Yoshimura, Chiaki Hasegawa, Daisuke N. Saito, Hirokazu Kumazaki, Tetsu Hirosawa, Yoshio Minabe, Mitsuru Kikuchi
Journal of Neuroscience 5 September 2018, 38 (36) 7878-7886; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1229-18.2018

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Altered Gamma Oscillations during Motor Control in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Kyung-min An, Takashi Ikeda, Yuko Yoshimura, Chiaki Hasegawa, Daisuke N. Saito, Hirokazu Kumazaki, Tetsu Hirosawa, Yoshio Minabe, Mitsuru Kikuchi
Journal of Neuroscience 5 September 2018, 38 (36) 7878-7886; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1229-18.2018
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • autism
  • E/I balance
  • gamma
  • magnetoencephalography
  • movement
  • young children

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Vigilance and behavioral state-dependent modulation of cortical neuronal activity throughout the sleep/wake cycle
  • Brain functional connectivity mapping of behavioral flexibility in rhesus monkeys
  • Accumulation System: Distributed Neural Substrates of Perceptual Decision Making Revealed by fMRI Deconvolution
Show more Research Articles

Neurobiology of Disease

  • Vigilance and behavioral state-dependent modulation of cortical neuronal activity throughout the sleep/wake cycle
  • Brain functional connectivity mapping of behavioral flexibility in rhesus monkeys
  • Accumulation System: Distributed Neural Substrates of Perceptual Decision Making Revealed by fMRI Deconvolution
Show more Neurobiology of Disease
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2022 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.