Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Systems/Circuits

Auditory Predictive Coding across Awareness States under Anesthesia: An Intracranial Electrophysiology Study

Kirill V. Nourski, Mitchell Steinschneider, Ariane E. Rhone, Hiroto Kawasaki, Matthew A. Howard III and Matthew I. Banks
Journal of Neuroscience 26 September 2018, 38 (39) 8441-8452; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0967-18.2018
Kirill V. Nourski
1Department of Neurosurgery,
2Iowa Neuroscience Institute, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa 52242,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Kirill V. Nourski
Mitchell Steinschneider
3Departments of Neurology and Neuroscience, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, New York 10461,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ariane E. Rhone
1Department of Neurosurgery,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Hiroto Kawasaki
1Department of Neurosurgery,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Hiroto Kawasaki
Matthew A. Howard III
1Department of Neurosurgery,
2Iowa Neuroscience Institute, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa 52242,
4Pappajohn Biomedical Institute, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa 52242, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Matthew I. Banks
5Departments of Anesthesiology and Neuroscience, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, Wisconsin 53705
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Matthew I. Banks
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The systems-level mechanisms underlying loss of consciousness (LOC) under anesthesia remain unclear. General anesthetics suppress sensory responses within higher-order cortex and feedback connections, both critical elements of predictive coding hypotheses of conscious perception. Responses to auditory novelty may offer promise as biomarkers for consciousness. This study examined anesthesia-induced changes in auditory novelty responses over short (local deviant [LD]) and long (global deviant [GD]) time scales, envisioned to engage preattentive and conscious levels of processing, respectively. Electrocorticographic recordings were obtained in human neurosurgical patients (3 male, 3 female) from four hierarchical processing levels: core auditory cortex, non-core auditory cortex, auditory-related, and PFC. Stimuli were vowel patterns incorporating deviants within and across stimuli (LD and GD). Subjects were presented with stimuli while awake, and during sedation (responsive) and following LOC (unresponsive) under propofol anesthesia. LD and GD effects were assayed as the averaged evoked potential and high gamma (70–150 Hz) activity. In the awake state, LD and GD effects were present in all recorded regions, with averaged evoked potential effects more broadly distributed than high gamma activity. Under sedation, LD effects were preserved in all regions, except PFC. LOC was accompanied by loss of LD effects outside of auditory cortex. By contrast, GD effects were markedly suppressed under sedation in all regions and were absent following LOC. Thus, although the presence of GD effects is indicative of being awake, its absence is not indicative of LOC. Loss of LD effects in higher-order cortical areas may constitute an alternative biomarker of LOC.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Development of a biomarker that indexes changes in the brain upon loss of consciousness (LOC) under general anesthesia has broad implications for elucidating the neural basis of awareness and clinical relevance to mechanisms of sleep, coma, and disorders of consciousness. Using intracranial recordings from neurosurgery patients, we investigated changes in the activation of cortical networks involved in auditory novelty detection over short (local deviance) and long (global deviance) time scales associated with sedation and LOC under propofol anesthesia. Our results indicate that, whereas the presence of global deviance effects can index awareness, their loss cannot serve as a biomarker for LOC. The dramatic reduction of local deviance effects in areas beyond auditory cortex may constitute an alternative biomarker of LOC.

  • averaged evoked potential
  • consciousness
  • electrocorticography
  • high gamma
  • human auditory cortex
  • propofol

Introduction

Although the molecular and cellular effects of general anesthetics are well defined (Rudolph and Antkowiak, 2004; Franks, 2008), a comparable understanding of loss of consciousness (LOC) under anesthesia at a systems level is lacking. The broad implications of this question are reflected in the convergence of research into anesthesia, sleep, coma, and the neural basis of consciousness (Mashour, 2006; Alkire et al., 2008; Shushruth, 2013). Recent studies suggesting that evaluation of patients with disorders of consciousness (DOC) can be improved by incorporating noninvasive metrics of neural activity (Bayne et al., 2017; Bernat, 2017) and the need for objective measures of awareness under general anesthesia (Avidan et al., 2011) further highlights the importance of this problem.

Although general anesthetics act on subcortical sleep and arousal centers (Devor and Zalkind, 2001; Nelson et al., 2002; Långsjö et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2015), changes in the contents of consciousness are likely secondary to effects on thalamocortical networks (Llinás et al., 1998; Dehaene and Naccache, 2001; Crick and Koch, 2003; Koch et al., 2016); however, specific mechanisms and sites of action are unclear (Nelson et al., 2002; Franks, 2008; Koch et al., 2016). Primary sensory areas are relatively resistant to LOC under anesthesia and sleep (Raz et al., 2014; Nir et al., 2015; Durand et al., 2016), whereas activity in higher-order cortex is markedly suppressed (Portas et al., 2000; Liu et al., 2012; Wilf et al., 2016), as is corticocortical, and especially feedback (FB), connectivity (Massimini et al., 2005; Ferrarelli et al., 2010; Boly et al., 2012; Raz et al., 2014). This suggests that neural correlates of LOC under anesthesia will be observed in higher-order cortex and be revealed during sensory tasks that engage cortical FB pathways (Davis and Johnsrude, 2007).

Detection of auditory novelty is postulated to activate hierarchical sensory cortical networks mediating predictive coding (Mumford, 1992; Rao and Ballard, 1999; Friston, 2005; Garrido et al., 2009). Under the predictive coding hypothesis, the brain uses a generative model to best match sensory observations wherein predictions carried by FB connections from higher-order cortex are compared with sensory-driven inputs into lower-order cortex, and prediction errors are projected to higher levels via feedforward connections (Friston, 2005; Bar, 2009; Clark, 2013). This form of bidirectional information exchange is also central to leading theories of brain function and consciousness (Friston, 2005; Dehaene and Changeux, 2011; Mashour, 2013; Tononi et al., 2016), and its disruption upon LOC would render impossible the process of Bayesian inference postulated to underlie conscious interpretation of sensory stimuli (Friston, 2005; Bastos et al., 2012).

The brain likely uses a hierarchical generative model that operates over multiple spatiotemporal scales, matching the environment characterized by nested sensory objects (e.g., phonemes, words, sentences in spoken language). The local-global deviant (LGD) paradigm is specifically designed to probe auditory predictive coding over multiple temporal integration windows by establishing prior expectations over short (local deviant, [LD]) and long (global deviant, [GD]) time scales (Bekinschtein et al., 2009). This study used the LGD paradigm to investigate the effects of propofol anesthesia on auditory predictive coding. LGD stimuli engage both preattentive and conscious perceptual processes and elicit error signals with distinct latencies and generators (Bekinschtein et al., 2009; Strauss et al., 2015). LD error signals (“effects”) align with the scalp-recorded mismatch negativity (MMN) (Näätänen and Alho, 1995), thought to be primarily generated within the temporal lobe (Bekinschtein et al., 2009; Fishman, 2014; Joos et al., 2014). GD effects align with the P3b component of event-related potentials (Kok, 2001) and activate a broader cortical network (Bekinschtein et al., 2009).

GD effects have been postulated to represent a key signature of conscious processing in the global workspace theory (Bekinschtein et al., 2009; Dehaene and Changeux, 2011). These effects, which dissipate upon LOC, have been proposed as biomarkers for awareness (Faugeras et al., 2012; Strauss et al., 2015; Uhrig et al., 2016). However, these effects are suppressed by inattention (Sergent et al., 2005; Bekinschtein et al., 2009), which can occur while subjects are still conscious (Heinke and Koelsch, 2005). By contrast, LD effects, as manifested by the MMN, are typically described as preattentive and relatively resistant to LOC (Näätänen et al., 2011) yet are also modulated by attention (Näätänen et al., 1993; Chennu et al., 2013) and awareness (Simpson et al., 2002; Strauss et al., 2015). Thus, key questions remain regarding the dependence of LD and GD effects on level of consciousness. Here, we tested the hypothesis that LGD effects can track changes in awareness (i.e., sedation and LOC) under propofol anesthesia, using intracranial recordings obtained simultaneously from key processing stages involved in auditory predictive coding: core and non-core auditory, auditory-related, and PFC.

Materials and Methods

Subjects.

Subjects were 6 neurosurgical patients with medically refractory epilepsy (Table 1) who had been implanted with intracranial electrocorticography (ECoG) electrodes to identify resectable seizure foci (Nourski and Howard, 2015; Nourski et al., 2017, 2018). Research protocols were approved by the University of Iowa Institutional Review Board and the National Institutes of Health, and written informed consent was obtained from all subjects. Subjects could rescind their participation without interrupting their clinical monitoring. Experiments were conducted in the operating room before electrode removal and seizure focus resection surgery.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Subject demographics and electrode coverage

Subjects were right-handed, left language-dominant native English speakers. All subjects underwent standard audiometric and neuropsychological evaluation, and none was found to have hearing or cognitive deficits that would impact the findings of this study. Subject R394 had previously undergone a resection of a cavernoma in the anterior medial temporal lobe, which spared all ROIs in this study, except planum polare. This subject had normal hearing, cognitive abilities, and above-average task performance, justifying this subject's inclusion in the study.

Stimuli.

Stimuli were vowels /ɑ/ and /i/, presented in an LGD paradigm (Bekinschtein et al., 2009; Strauss et al., 2015) (Fig. 1). The vowels were excised from the steady-state vowel portions of syllables /hɑd/ and /hid/, spoken by a female talker (Hillenbrand et al., 1995). On each trial, five 100 ms vowels, each gated with 5 ms on/off ramps and separated by 50 ms silent intervals, were presented, with the fifth vowel being either the same as the first four (local standard) or different (LD; Fig. 1A).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

LD and GD experimental paradigm. A, Schematic of the four experimental stimuli. B, Stimulus sequences. LS, Local standard. C, Comparisons between trials to characterize LD and GD effects. D, Stimulus presentation during awake baseline period and induction of general anesthesia with stepwise increases in propofol dose. Adapted with permission from Strauss et al. (2015).

In each experimental block, the stimuli were presented in four sequences, with the order of the sequences randomized across blocks (Fig. 1B). Subjects were asked to perform a GD target detection task by pressing a button with the hand ipsilateral to the hemisphere from which recordings were made. Each sequence began with a recorded instruction that defined the target stimulus to the subject. For example, Sequence 1 was preceded with the following instruction: “This time, press the button when you hear this sound: /ɑɑɑɑi/. Once again, press the button when you hear this sound: /ɑɑɑɑi/.” This was followed by 10 trials that established the global standard (e.g., /ɑɑɑɑɑ/ for Sequence 1), and then by 72 global standard trials and 18 GD trials, presented in a pseudorandom order with an intertrial interval of 1500 ± 10 ms (mean, SD). The difference in presentation frequency thus constituted the global deviance, and the GD stimulus changed across the four sequences within each block (Fig. 1C). The key feature of this paradigm is that the LD stimulus can be either a global standard (as in Sequences 2 and 4) or a GD (as in Sequences 1 and 3) depending on its presentation frequency within a sequence.

Recording.

ECoG recordings were made using subdural and depth electrodes (Ad-Tech Medical). Subdural arrays consisted of platinum-iridium discs (2.3 mm diameter, 5–10 mm interelectrode distance), embedded in a silicon membrane. Depth arrays (8–12 electrodes, 5 mm interelectrode distance) were stereotactically implanted along the anterolateral-to-posteromedial axis of Heschl's gyrus (HG). Additional arrays targeted insular cortex and provided coverage of planum temporale and planum polare. This allowed for bracketing suspected epileptogenic zones from dorsal, ventral, medial, and lateral aspects (Reddy et al., 2010; Nagahama et al., 2018). Depth electrodes that targeted amygdala and hippocampus provided additional coverage of auditory-related cortex within superior temporal sulcus. A subgaleal electrode was used as a reference. All electrodes were placed solely on the basis of clinical requirements, as determined by the team of epileptologists and neurosurgeons (Nourski and Howard, 2015). ECoG data were amplified, filtered (0.7–800 Hz bandpass, 12 dB/octave rolloff), and digitized at a sampling rate of 2034.5 Hz using a TDT RZ2 processor (Tucker-Davis Technologies).

Reconstruction of the anatomical locations of implanted electrodes, their mapping onto a standardized set of coordinates, and assignment to ROIs were performed using FreeSurfer image analysis suite (version 5.3; Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Harvard, MA) and in-house software, as described previously (Nourski et al., 2014, 2018). Contact locations of the depth and subdural electrodes were extracted from postimplantation MRI and CT scans, respectively, and projected onto preimplantation MRI scans using nonlinear 3D thin-plate spline morphing. Transformation of recording site locations from multiple subjects into standard MNI coordinates was done using linear coregistration to the MNI152 T1 average brain, as implemented in FMRIB Software library (version 5.0; FMRIB Analysis Group, Oxford, UK). Contacts were projected onto the right lateral hemispheric surface and superior temporal plane of the FreeSurfer average template brain. Left hemisphere MNI x-axis coordinates (xMNI) were multiplied by −1 to map them onto the right hemisphere.

Seven ROIs were defined, spanning the cortical hierarchy of auditory predictive coding: core auditory cortex (posteromedial portion of HG [HGPM]); four non-core areas (i.e., anterolateral portion of HG, planum temporale, planum polare, and superior temporal gyrus); auditory-related cortex (insular cortex, superior temporal sulcus, middle temporal gyrus [MTG], supramarginal and angular gyri); and PFC (inferior, middle, and superior frontal, orbital, and transverse frontopolar gyrus) (Table 1). Sites outside these seven ROIs were categorized as “Other.” Assignment of recording sites to ROIs was based on anatomical reconstructions of electrode locations in each subject, informed by automated parcellation of cortical gyri (Destrieux et al., 2010, 2017) and MRI sections in axial, sagittal, and coronal planes for subdural and depth electrode arrays, respectively. Recording sites characterized by excessive noise, or localized to white matter or seizure foci, were excluded from analyses.

Experimental design and statistical analysis.

Stimuli were presented by a TDT RZ2 processor (Tucker-Davis Technologies) and delivered at a comfortable level (60–65 dB SPL) diotically via insert earphones (ER4B, Etymotic Research) integrated into custom-fit earmolds. The target detection task was used to provide additional measures of the subjects' attention and awareness during induction of anesthesia. Subjects operated the button with the hand ipsilateral to the recorded hemisphere. Behavioral performance in the target detection task was characterized by accuracy (hit rate; percentage of detected target stimuli), sensitivity (d′; Zhit rate − Zfalse alarm rate, where Z (p) is the inverse of the cumulative distribution function of the normal distribution), and reaction times (RTs). Button presses had to occur before the onset of the next nontarget trial to be counted as hits.

The first stimulus block was presented immediately before administration of propofol for induction of general anesthesia (Fig. 1D). Following the completion of the first block, propofol was infused at a rate of 50 μg/kg/min (Alaris pump, BD Bioscience), and the rate was increased every 10 min by 25 μg/kg/min (Nourski et al., 2017). Infusion lasted 50 min, during which three additional LGD blocks were presented. The infusions were supervised by a faculty anesthesiologist using standard respiratory, cardiac, and hemodynamic monitoring. No infusion had to be terminated for the patients' safety.

Depth of anesthesia was evaluated before and after each block using the Observer's Assessment of Alertness/Sedation (OAA/S) scale, a gold standard in assessing alertness in clinical setting (Chernik et al., 1990). Responsiveness (calling the subject's name), speech (asking the subject to repeat the sentence, “The quick brown fox jumps over the lazy dog”), facial expression (the degree of facial relaxation), and eyes (the subject's ability to focus and ptosis) were assessed by the experimenter (K.V.N.) and scored on a scale from 1 to 5. The composite OAA/S score, ranging from 5 (awake) to 1 (unresponsive), was defined as the lowest level at which any assessment category was checked, as stipulated by Chernik et al. (1990). The transition from OAA/S = 3 (responsive to loud or repeated command) to OAA/S = 2 (unresponsive in the absence of mild prodding or shaking) was operationally used as the threshold between sedation and LOC (Vanluchene et al., 2004). Depth of anesthesia was assessed additionally using EEG measures: response entropy (RE; E-ENTROPY module; Datex-Ohmeda) (Viertiö-Oja et al., 2004) in Subject R369 and bispectral index (BIS) (Gan et al., 1997) (BIS Complete 4-Channel Monitor; Medtronic) in all other subjects. The relationship between OAA/S scores and EEG measures was examined using linear regression analysis. BIS/RE values were obtained for each OAA/S measurement by averaging within ±2 min of that measurement.

ECoG data were downsampled to 1000 Hz and denoised (Kovach and Gander, 2016). Trials containing voltage deflections >5 SD from the across-block mean for each recording site were considered artifacts and excluded from further analysis. Averaged evoked potentials (AEPs) and high gamma event-related band power (ERBP) were extracted using software written in MATLAB (version 7.14; The MathWorks). AEPs and high gamma activity roughly index inputs to and outputs from the cortical recording site, respectively (Steinschneider et al., 2008), thus providing complementary direct measures of neural activity. High gamma ERBP was calculated by bandpass filtering the ECoG signal (300th order finite impulse response filter, 70–150 Hz passband), followed by Hilbert envelope extraction, log transform, and, for each of the four experimental sequences, normalization to the mean power over the entire duration of the sequence. Averaged AEPs and high gamma ERBP waveforms were smoothed using a fourth-order Butterworth lowpass filter (30 Hz cutoff).

Acoustically responsive sites were identified based on their responses to the first four vowels, as described by Nourski et al. (2014, 2018). Each trial was baseline-corrected by subtracting the mean voltage in the 100 ms interval immediately preceding the onset of the first vowel. Sites were considered responsive if either the upper or the lower bound of the AEP 95% CI was <0 or >0 μV, respectively, for at least 30 ms, and the following peak exceeded voltage at the threshold-crossing time point by at least twofold. For high gamma data, the threshold criterion was based on the lower bound of the 95% CI exceeding 0 dB for at least 30 ms, and the following peak exceeding ERBP at the threshold-crossing time point by at least twofold.

LD and GD effects were defined as significant increases in averaged responses to the deviant versus standard stimuli between 0 and 800 ms following the onset of the fifth vowel, as determined by a nonparametric cluster-based permutation test (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007), as described in detail by Nourski et al. (2018). The permutation distribution was constructed using 10,000 random partitions of experimental conditions (deviant vs standard), and the threshold for the cluster statistic was set at the 99.5th percentile for two-tailed tests (AEP data) and the 99th percentile for the one-tailed tests (high gamma). p values were adjusted using false discovery rate (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995) correction with a significance threshold of q = 0.05.

Effects of brain state transitions on responses to the first four vowels, LD effects, and GD effects were analyzed using logistic regression of each response type separately (vowel, LD, GD), with random intercepts for subject, and hierarchical level (core, non-core, auditory-related, PFC) and brain state (awake, sedated, unconscious) as fixed effects with two-way interactions. To facilitate model convergence, normal (SD = 3) priors were used to limit the parameter space (R package blme) (Chung et al., 2013). Simultaneous CIs for selected contrasts were calculated with R package multcomp (Hothorn et al., 2008). Data are presented as odds ratios (ORs; i.e., the ratio of the odds of responsiveness in one brain state to the odds in the other brain state, computed by exponentiating the coefficients in the regression analysis) along with 95% family-wise CIs. All comparisons are across observed brain state transitions (i.e., awake to sedated and sedated to unconscious).

The time course of deviance effects was described by summing significant clusters across recording sites in all subjects for each ROI at each time point between 0 and 800 ms following the fifth vowel onset. Additionally, cumulative latency probability distribution functions over the same time window were computed for all sites. Time course comparisons between awareness states were performed using two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests.

Results

Effects of general anesthesia on behavioral state

Administration of propofol resulted in decreased OAA/S scores and BIS/RE values (Fig. 2A, top row and lower row, respectively) and a decline in button presses (Fig. 2A, top row, tick marks). Consistent with previous studies using propofol anesthesia (Liu et al., 1997; Ibrahim et al., 2001), there was a reliable correspondence between OAA/S scores and recorded BIS values (RE in Subject R369) in all subjects (Fig. 2A, bottom row, r2 values). As expected, subjects exhibited lower accuracy, sensitivity, and longer RTs under sedation (Fig. 2B).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Awareness assessment and GD target detection task performance across experimental blocks. A, Assessment of each subject's level of sedation using the OAA/S scale (top row), and EEG monitoring: RE in Subject R369 and BIS in all other subjects (bottom row) during each experimental block. B, Hit rates (percentage correctly detected target stimuli; top), sensitivity (d′; middle), and RTs (bottom) for each of the 6 subjects (columns) during the first, second, and third experimental block. None of the subjects pressed the response button during the fourth experimental block. Box-and-whiskers RT plots represent median values and 10th, 25th, 75th, and 90th percentiles; median values for each subject are shown inside boxes. For each block, the two OAA/S scores correspond to assessments made before and after the LGD experimental block.

Effects of general anesthesia on local versus global novelty responses

A combination of subdural and depth arrays allowed for comprehensive assessments of responses from multiple ROIs, encompassing key areas in the auditory processing hierarchy. Typical electrode coverage (Subject R369) is presented in Figure 3A. Coverage of the hemispheric convexity by subdural arrays is depicted in the top. A top-down view of the superior temporal plane (Fig. 3A, bottom) illustrates placement of depth arrays targeting insular cortex, HG, and the dorsal surface of the anterior temporal lobe. Examples of AEPs and high gamma responses from core auditory cortex (HGPM) and auditory-related cortex (MTG) elicited by LGD stimuli during induction of general anesthesia are shown in Figure 3B. Auditory novelty, associated with the onset of the fifth vowel within the stimulus, elicited deviance effects in AEP and high gamma ERBP signals. Both AEP and high gamma LD effects were preserved within HGPM throughout induction. In MTG, an AEP LD effect was present under sedation, but not LOC. In contrast, significant GD effects were abolished under sedation at both sites.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Responses to standard and deviant stimuli during induction of general anesthesia in a representative subject (Subject R369). A, MRI side view of the hemispheric surface and top-down view of superior temporal plane showing electrode coverage in Subject R369. Circles represent recording sites. White circles represent recording sites in HGPM and MTG. Colors represent different ROIs used in the study. B, AEP and high gamma responses recorded from two exemplary sites in response to standard and deviant stimuli. Lines indicate mean values. Shading represents 95% CIs. Thick lines underneath response waveforms indicate significant differences between responses to standard and deviant stimuli.

Details of LGD effects associated with induction of general anesthesia are presented in Figure 4, which depicts data from all subjects plotted in the MNI coordinate space for the three levels of awareness. In the awake state, AEP LD effects were present in all ROIs, whereas high gamma LD effects were absent in PFC (Fig. 4A). Both AEP and high gamma LD effects became progressively restricted to core and non-core auditory cortex under propofol anesthesia. Of note, there was little change in extent of LD effects in temporoparietal cortex from awake to sedated state.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Topography of changes in LD (A) and GD (B) effects during induction of general anesthesia. Summary of data from 6 subjects, plotted in MNI coordinate space and projected onto FreeSurfer average template brain. Left hemisphere MNI x-axis coordinates (xMNI) were multiplied by −1 to map them onto the right-hemisphere common space.

AEP and high gamma GD effects were observed in all ROIs in the awake state (Fig. 4B). GD effects were more sensitive to propofol compared with LD effects and were largely suppressed in all areas under sedation. Of note, the majority of remaining sites with significant AEP (19 of 25) and high gamma (20 of 29) GD effects under sedation were contributed by a single subject (Subject R376). This participant was otherwise typical of the subject cohort, including task performance, extent of electrode coverage, and changes in LD effects during induction.

The description of regional changes in LGD effects during induction of propofol anesthesia shown in Figure 4 could be biased by the extent of ROI electrode coverage. To address this, and to compare changes in LGD effects with changes in overall responsiveness to acoustic stimuli, the presence of responses to vowels and LGD effects across cortical regions was also described in terms of the fractions of responsive sites (Fig. 5), and the effects of changes in brain state on responsiveness analyzed using logistic regression (see Materials and Methods). No significant effect of brain state was detected on AEP vowel responses in core (OR = 0.28 [0.0096, 8.0], p = 0.94). In non-core, there was no effect of sedation (OR = 0.79 [0.33, 1.9], p = 0.99), but responses were suppressed upon LOC (OR = 0.38 [0.18, 0.80], p = 0.0034). Responses were suppressed upon both sedation and LOC in auditory-related cortex (sedation: OR = 0.57 [0.35, 0.94], p = 0.017; LOC: OR = 0.26 [0.14, 0.48], p < 0.001) and PFC (sedation: OR = 0.20 [0.092, 0.42], p < 0.001; LOC: OR = 0.16 [0.034, 0.72], p = 0.0074) (Fig. 5A). No effect on high gamma responses to vowels was detected for any hierarchical level and brain state (p > 0.05 for all contrasts).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Percentages of sites across ROIs that exhibited responses to the first four vowels, LD effects, and GD effects (A–C, respectively). Summary of data from 6 subjects. Differently shaded bars represent measurements made in awake, sedated, and unconscious states.

Analysis of LGD effects (Fig. 5B,C) yielded results consistent with those presented in Figure 4. Specifically, there was no significant difference in AEP and high gamma LD effects between awake and sedated states (p > 0.05 for all contrasts), except for AEP LD effects in PFC, which were suppressed (OR = 0.21 [0.010, 0.45], p < 0.001) (Fig. 5B). In comparison, LD effects were suppressed outside of auditory core cortex upon LOC (p < 0.05 for all contrasts). AEP GD effects were suppressed at all levels of hierarchy in sedation (p < 0.05 for all contrasts). Statistical assessment of state-dependent changes of AEP GD effects upon LOC and high gamma GD effects upon sedation and LOC was precluded by the small number of responsive sites (Fig. 5C).

Sensitivity of FB pathways and higher-order cortical areas to general anesthesia would be expected to alter the time course of cortical activation associated with predictive coding. To address this, the overall time course of LGD effects was measured across all ROIs (Fig. 6). Here, at each time point relative to the onset of the fifth vowel, numbers of ROI sites exhibiting significant differences between responses to standard and deviant stimuli were summed across subjects, demonstrating the extent of cortical activation by auditory novelty on a temporal scale. In the awake state, AEP LD and GD effects exhibited multipeaked time courses that spanned the entire 800 ms analysis window, whereas high gamma effects were largely monophasic and occurred earlier in the analysis window (Fig. 6A). The AEP LD effect was most prominent at 150–250 ms after the fifth vowel onset, whereas the high gamma LD effect peaked at ∼125 ms. The time courses of AEP and high gamma LD effects were slower under sedation relative to awake (AEP: D = 0.093, p < 0.001; high gamma: D = 0.22, p < 0.001; two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests) (Fig. 6A, insets). Upon LOC, the AEP LD effect had a more rapid time course compared with te sedated state (D = 0.086, p < 0.001), reflecting the relatively higher contribution of HGPM sites. A similar effect was observed for high gamma, where the timing of LD effects was more rapid upon LOC compared with sedation (D = 0.25, p < 0.001).

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Time course of LD (A) and GD (B) effects across levels of awareness (columns). Data from 6 subjects. Number of sites within each ROI, exhibiting significant differences between responses to standard and deviant stimuli, are plotted as a function of time after the fifth vowel onset for AEP and high gamma in top and bottom rows, respectively. Insets, Cumulative probability distribution plots comparing the overall time course of deviance effects for all recording sites. Solid line indicates awake. Dashed line indicates sedated. Dotted line indicates unconscious. No data are shown for GD effects upon LOC due to the lack of responsive sites.

As expected, onset and peak latencies of GD effects were generally longer than those of LD effects. The number of sites exhibiting significant AEP GD effects was maximal at ∼400 ms, whereas the high gamma GD effect peaked at 250 ms. The time course of GD effects became less complex in the sedated compared with the awake state. For AEP, this was associated with selective loss of the earliest components (D = 0.18, p < 0.001), whereas for high gamma later components were suppressed under sedation (D = 0.28, p < 0.001). Of note, as GD effects were profoundly suppressed under sedation and abolished upon LOC, these results were based on a small number of significant sites, primarily from a single subject, and should be interpreted with caution.

Relationship between LGD responses and task performance

The physiologic data indicate that loss of GD effects does not index LOC, and instead may reflect changes in the subjects' task engagement. To that end, relationships between cortical LGD responses and task performance before and during induction of general anesthesia were examined using linear regression analysis. Behavioral accuracy (hit rate) while subjects were engaged in the task (i.e., blocks with hit rates >0%) was compared with the percentages of sites in superior temporal gyrus (STG), auditory-related cortex, and PFC that exhibited AEP LGD effects in corresponding experimental blocks. This relationship failed to reach significance for LD AEP effect in all three regions (STG: r2 = 0.058; p = 0.48; auditory-related cortex: r2 = 0.0041; p = 0.84; PFC: r2 = 0.0080; p = 0.77). Likewise, the linear correlation between the prevalence of AEP GD effect in STG and task performance failed to reach significance (r2 = 0.23; p = 0.13). In contrast, there was a trend toward significance for correlation between AEP GD effects and accuracy in auditory-related cortex (r2 = 0.32; p = 0.043, uncorrected) and PFC (r2 = 0.42; p = 0.016, uncorrected). This result is consistent with the model wherein GD effects in auditory-related cortex and PFC index the subject's task performance rather than consciousness per se.

Discussion

Sensitivity of novelty responses to anesthesia

Electrophysiological studies have shown that fundamental aspects of auditory responses in core auditory cortex are relatively robust to LOC under anesthesia (Raz et al., 2014; Durand et al., 2016; Nourski et al., 2017), whereas responses in higher-order areas, including association sensory cortex and PFC, are far more sensitive (Heinke et al., 2004; Plourde et al., 2006; Liu et al., 2012; Nourski et al., 2017). In the present study, modulation of LD effects by propofol exhibited a similar spatial distribution. LD effects remain present in core and non-core auditory cortex, even upon LOC. This observation is similar to that seen in animal models, wherein the modulatory effects of preceding stimuli upon a test sound are present even in the anesthetized state (Calford and Semple, 1995). In contrast, LD effects outside auditory cortex are strongly affected by general anesthesia, indicative of a progressive suppression of downstream nodes within the network that subserves predictive coding at the LD time scale.

The LGD stimulation paradigm simultaneously probes conscious and unconscious processing of novelty within the predictive coding framework. Several studies have provided evidence that, unlike LD effects, which were resistant to LOC, GD effects had the potential to serve as biomarkers for conscious sensory processing. This proposition was based on comparisons between controls and patients with DOC (Bekinschtein et al., 2009; Faugeras et al., 2012), during sleep (Strauss et al., 2015), and, in monkeys, under general anesthesia (Uhrig et al., 2016). The differential sensitivity of the LD and GD effects to behavioral state is consistent with the initial assumption upon which the LGD paradigm was based (i.e., that “maintenance of perceptual representations over time requires consciousness”) (Bekinschtein et al., 2009). However, the data presented here are inconsistent with the proposal that loss of GD effects tracks LOC. In the current study, GD effects were almost entirely suppressed at sedating doses of propofol, even while subjects were still responsive. Results are also inconsistent with an earlier fMRI study in monkeys (Uhrig et al., 2016), which reported a complete suppression of the LD effect by propofol, accompanied by only partial suppression of the GD effect, essentially the opposite to the differential sensitivity demonstrated here. It is possible this discrepancy reflects species-or task-specific effects, or differences in methodology or experimental design.

Although the MMN is classically described as preattentive and thus resistant to LOC (Näätänen et al., 2011), the present study shows that LD effects in PFC are blocked by propofol under sedation. This sensitivity to propofol anesthesia is consistent with previous reports showing that the amplitude of the LD effect and the MMN are sensitive to manipulations of attention (Näätänen et al., 1993; Chennu et al., 2013) and to changes in levels of consciousness (Simpson et al., 2002; Strauss et al., 2015; Uhrig et al., 2016). These reports, as well as the present findings, suggest that the generators of the LD effect and the MMN are multifactorial, with preattentive processing localized to auditory cortex and additional contributions localized to higher-order regions. The latter contributions manifest over longer time scales, likely reflecting a greater degree of polysynaptic processing across brain regions, which in turn would lead to greater susceptibility to disruption by general anesthesia (Hentschke et al., 2017). Consistent with this model, the overall time course of LD effects was truncated upon LOC compared with sedation. Finally, the STG was characterized by some of the most dramatic reductions in LD effects upon LOC in the present study. This suggests that the STG may be a critical hub for initiation of conscious processing of deviance effects.

The AEP GD effect reported in the present study had timing and, in part, spatial distribution comparable with that of the scalp-recorded P300 response. Of note, other studies have specifically linked GD effects to the P3b component of that response (e.g., Bekinschtein et al., 2009). The attenuation of the GD effect and P3b in normal sleeping subjects and DOC patients, and under anesthesia in the monkey, has been interpreted as an indication that GD effects require conscious processing of sensory information (Bekinschtein et al., 2009; Faugeras et al., 2012; Strauss et al., 2015). Given the dependence of these relatively long-latency deviance effects on attention and working memory (Sergent et al., 2005; Bekinschtein et al., 2009; Chennu et al., 2013), this sensitivity to level of consciousness is not surprising. Indeed, current models of loss and recovery of consciousness, both under anesthesia and during natural sleep, emphasize the importance of anesthetic-induced changes in cortical connectivity, and especially FB connectivity (Mashour and Hudetz, 2017). Deviance detection, and especially GD, is postulated to depend on FB projections within the cortical hierarchy underlying auditory predictive coding (Bekinschtein et al., 2009). Thus, its sensitivity to anesthesia is consistent with the sensitivity of FB projections to anesthesia. However, does GD detection reflect consciousness per se, or merely some of its attentional or cognitive components? Loss of GD effects at subhypnotic doses of propofol (Fig. 4) suggests the latter, leading to the conclusion that these cortical responses are not a reliable measure of consciousness. The sensitivity of GD effects to manipulations of attention and the overlap of the areas contributing to GD effects and attention (Hopfinger et al., 2000; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002) suggest that GD effects may be more directly related to attention, which is impaired even under subhypnotic doses of anesthesia (Heinke and Koelsch, 2005), and is absent during all stages of natural sleep and in most DOC patients (Bekinschtein et al., 2009; Faugeras et al., 2012; Strauss et al., 2015).

Caveats and limitations

The experimental subjects in this study had a neurologic disorder and thus may not be representative of a healthy population. However, results were consistent across multiple subjects with different neurologic histories, antiepileptic drug regimens, and seizure foci. All subjects had normal cognitive function and were able to perform the experimental task successfully. Further, recordings from epileptic foci in each subject were excluded from the analyses. Importantly, over the course of the monitoring period, all subjects participated in multiple experimental protocols beyond those described in the present report. These multiple paradigms provide for a comprehensive assessment of cortical function, and evaluation for consistency with a corpus of published data on basic electrophysiologic response properties (Nourski and Howard, 2015; Nourski, 2017). None of the subjects exhibited aberrant behavioral or physiologic responses during performance of these other protocols that would serve as grounds for exclusion from the present study.

It is challenging to clinically assess level of consciousness in human subjects (Sanders et al., 2012; Bayne et al., 2017; Bernat, 2017). Here, responsiveness to verbal command, evaluated by the OAA/S score, was used as a surrogate measure. However, loss of responsiveness could reflect lack of motivation to respond despite continued sensory processing, or could reflect disconnection from the environment accompanied by dreaming, which is reported to occur under propofol anesthesia (Leslie et al., 2009). Without a direct assay of mentation available, responsiveness and/or recall are the current gold standards for assessing consciousness in patients and volunteers being administered anesthesia, and are highly correlated with assays of LOC in animal models (Franks, 2008). In the future, converging lines of evidence based on studies in humans, animal studies establishing underlying mechanisms (Funk et al., 2017), and theoretical and computational models of consciousness (Dehaene and Changeux, 2011; Tononi et al., 2016) will provide a firmer foundation for interrogating consciousness in human subjects.

Functional significance and future directions

Research into the neural basis of consciousness has converged with research examining mechanisms of anesthesia, sleep, and DOC (Mashour, 2006; Alkire et al., 2008; Shushruth, 2013). Establishing neural signatures of altered consciousness and LOC will prove invaluable for advancing these fields of study, and will also have practical applications related to noninvasive monitors of awareness and diagnostic and prognostic tools for use in patients with DOC and mental illness. Clinical evaluation alone lacks sensitivity and specificity, and adjunct objective measures that can reliably index the absence of consciousness would enhance the nosology of DOC (Bayne et al., 2017; Bernat, 2017). The data presented here suggest that auditory novelty responses may be useful in developing such metrics, but it is critical to consider both the specific cortical generators that contribute to the novelty response and the role of other factors (e.g., subjects' motivation and attention during task performance) in mediating activation of the brain regions involved. Future studies that more strictly control for possible task-related effects are imperative for resolving the latter issue, and development of stimulus paradigms independent of active participation of the patient is of translational importance.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health Grants R01-DC04290, R01-GM109086, and UL1-RR024979, National Science Foundation Grant CRCNS-IIS-1515678, and the Hoover Fund. We thank Haiming Chen, Phillip Gander, Bradley Hindman, Christopher Kovach, Bryan Krause, Rashmi Mueller, Caitlin Murphy, Melissa Pauling, Yuri Saalmann, Beau Snoad, and Xiayi Wang for help with data collection, analysis, and comments on the manuscript.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Kirill V. Nourski, Department of Neurosurgery, University of Iowa, 200 Hawkins Drive, 1815 JCP, Iowa City, IA 52242. kirill-nourski{at}uiowa.edu

References

  1. ↵
    1. Alkire MT,
    2. Hudetz AG,
    3. Tononi G
    (2008) Consciousness and anesthesia. Science 322:876–880. doi:10.1126/science.1149213 pmid:18988836
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Avidan MS,
    2. Jacobsohn E,
    3. Glick D,
    4. Burnside BA,
    5. Zhang L,
    6. Villafranca A,
    7. Karl L,
    8. Kamal S,
    9. Torres B,
    10. O'Connor M,
    11. Evers AS,
    12. Gradwohl S,
    13. Lin N,
    14. Palanca BJ,
    15. Mashour GA
    (2011) Prevention of intraoperative awareness in a high-risk surgical population. N Engl J Med 365:591–600. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa1100403 pmid:21848460
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Bar M
    (2009) The proactive brain: memory for predictions. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364:1235–1243. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0310 pmid:19528004
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Bastos AM,
    2. Usrey WM,
    3. Adams RA,
    4. Mangun GR,
    5. Fries P,
    6. Friston KJ
    (2012) Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding. Neuron 76:695–711. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.038 pmid:23177956
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Bayne T,
    2. Hohwy J,
    3. Owen AM
    (2017) Reforming the taxonomy in disorders of consciousness. Ann Neurol 82:866–872. doi:10.1002/ana.25088 pmid:29091304
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Bekinschtein TA,
    2. Dehaene S,
    3. Rohaut B,
    4. Tadel F,
    5. Cohen L,
    6. Naccache L
    (2009) Neural signature of the conscious processing of auditory regularities. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:1672–1677. doi:10.1073/pnas.0809667106 pmid:19164526
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    1. Benjamini Y,
    2. Hochberg Y
    (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc B Stat Methodol 57:289–300.
    OpenUrl
  8. ↵
    1. Bernat JL
    (2017) Nosologic considerations in disorders of consciousness. Ann Neurol 82:863–865. doi:10.1002/ana.25089 pmid:29092102
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Boly M,
    2. Moran R,
    3. Murphy M,
    4. Boveroux P,
    5. Bruno MA,
    6. Noirhomme Q,
    7. Ledoux D,
    8. Bonhomme V,
    9. Brichant JF,
    10. Tononi G,
    11. Laureys S,
    12. Friston K
    (2012) Connectivity changes underlying spectral EEG changes during propofol-induced loss of consciousness. J Neurosci 32:7082–7090. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3769-11.2012 pmid:22593076
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Calford MB,
    2. Semple MN
    (1995) Monaural inhibition in cat auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol 73:1876–1891. doi:10.1152/jn.1995.73.5.1876 pmid:7623087
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Chennu S,
    2. Noreika V,
    3. Gueorguiev D,
    4. Blenkmann A,
    5. Kochen S,
    6. Ibáñez A,
    7. Owen AM,
    8. Bekinschtein TA
    (2013) Expectation and attention in hierarchical auditory prediction. J Neurosci 33:11194–11205. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0114-13.2013 pmid:23825422
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. Chernik DA,
    2. Gillings D,
    3. Laine H,
    4. Hendler J,
    5. Silver JM,
    6. Davidson AB,
    7. Schwam EM,
    8. Siegel JL
    (1990) Validity and reliability of the Observer's assessment of Alertness/Sedation scale: study with intravenous midazolam. J Clin Psychopharmacol 10:244–251. pmid:2286697
    OpenUrlPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Chung Y,
    2. Rabe-Hesketh S,
    3. Dorie V,
    4. Gelman A,
    5. Liu J
    (2013) A nondegenerate penalized likelihood estimator for variance parameters in multilevel models. Psychometrika 78:685–709. doi:10.1007/s11336-013-9328-2 pmid:24092484
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Clark A
    (2013) Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behav Brain Sci 36:181–204. doi:10.1017/S0140525X12000477 pmid:23663408
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Corbetta M,
    2. Shulman GL
    (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:201–215. doi:10.1038/nrn755 pmid:11994752
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Crick F,
    2. Koch C
    (2003) A framework for consciousness. Nat Neurosci 6:119–126. doi:10.1038/nn0203-119 pmid:12555104
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Davis MH,
    2. Johnsrude IS
    (2007) Hearing speech sounds: top-down influences on the interface between audition and speech perception. Hear Res 229:132–147. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2007.01.014 pmid:17317056
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Dehaene S,
    2. Changeux JP
    (2011) Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron 70:200–227. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018 pmid:21521609
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Dehaene S,
    2. Naccache L
    (2001) Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 79:1–37. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00123-2 pmid:11164022
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Destrieux C,
    2. Fischl B,
    3. Dale A,
    4. Halgren E
    (2010) Automatic parcellation of human cortical gyri and sulci using standard anatomical nomenclature. Neuroimage 53:1–15. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.010 pmid:20547229
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Destrieux C,
    2. Terrier LM,
    3. Andersson F,
    4. Love SA,
    5. Cottier JP,
    6. Duvernoy H,
    7. Velut S,
    8. Janot K,
    9. Zemmoura I
    (2017) A practical guide for the identification of major sulcogyral structures of the human cortex. Brain Struct Funct 222:2001–2015. doi:10.1007/s00429-016-1320-z pmid:27709299
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Devor M,
    2. Zalkind V
    (2001) Reversible analgesia, atonia, and loss of consciousness on bilateral intracerebral microinjection of pentobarbital. Pain 94:101–112. doi:10.1016/S0304-3959(01)00345-1 pmid:11576749
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Durand S,
    2. Iyer R,
    3. Mizuseki K,
    4. de Vries S,
    5. Mihalas S,
    6. Reid RC
    (2016) A comparison of visual response properties in the lateral geniculate nucleus and primary visual cortex of awake and anesthetized mice. J Neurosci 36:12144–12156. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1741-16.2016 pmid:27903724
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  24. ↵
    1. Faugeras F,
    2. Rohaut B,
    3. Weiss N,
    4. Bekinschtein T,
    5. Galanaud D,
    6. Puybasset L,
    7. Bolgert F,
    8. Sergent C,
    9. Cohen L,
    10. Dehaene S,
    11. Naccache L
    (2012) Event related potentials elicited by violations of auditory regularities in patients with impaired consciousness. Neuropsychologia 50:403–418. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.015 pmid:22230230
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Ferrarelli F,
    2. Massimini M,
    3. Sarasso S,
    4. Casali A,
    5. Riedner BA,
    6. Angelini G,
    7. Tononi G,
    8. Pearce RA
    (2010) Breakdown in cortical effective connectivity during midazolam-induced loss of consciousness. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:2681–2686. doi:10.1073/pnas.0913008107 pmid:20133802
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    1. Fishman YI
    (2014) The mechanisms and meaning of the mismatch negativity. Brain Topogr 27:500–526. doi:10.1007/s10548-013-0337-3 pmid:24276221
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Franks NP
    (2008) General anaesthesia: from molecular targets to neuronal pathways of sleep and arousal. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:370–386. doi:10.1038/nrn2372 pmid:18425091
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Friston K
    (2005) A theory of cortical responses. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 360:815–836. doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1622 pmid:15937014
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Funk CM,
    2. Peelman K,
    3. Bellesi M,
    4. Marshall W,
    5. Cirelli C,
    6. Tononi G
    (2017) Role of somatostatin-positive cortical interneurons in the generation of sleep slow waves. J Neurosci 37:9132–9148. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1303-17.2017 pmid:28821651
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. ↵
    1. Gan TJ,
    2. Glass PS,
    3. Windsor A,
    4. Payne F,
    5. Rosow C,
    6. Sebel P,
    7. Manberg P
    (1997) Bispectral index monitoring allows faster emergence and improved recovery from propofol, alfentanil, and nitrous oxide anesthesia. BIS utility study group. Anesthesiology 87:808–815. doi:10.1097/00000542-199710000-00014 pmid:9357882
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Garrido MI,
    2. Kilner JM,
    3. Stephan KE,
    4. Friston KJ
    (2009) The mismatch negativity: a review of underlying mechanisms. Clin Neurophysiol 120:453–463. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2008.11.029 pmid:19181570
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Heinke W,
    2. Koelsch S
    (2005) The effects of anesthetics on brain activity and cognitive function. Curr Opin Anaesthesiol 18:625–631. doi:10.1097/01.aco.0000189879.67092.12 pmid:16534303
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Heinke W,
    2. Fiebach CJ,
    3. Schwarzbauer C,
    4. Meyer M,
    5. Olthoff D,
    6. Alter K
    (2004) Sequential effects of propofol on functional brain activation induced by auditory language processing: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Br J Anaesth 92:641–650. doi:10.1093/bja/aeh133 pmid:15064248
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Hentschke H,
    2. Raz A,
    3. Krause BM,
    4. Murphy CA,
    5. Banks MI
    (2017) Disruption of cortical network activity by the general anesthetic isoflurane. Br J Anaesth 119:685–696. doi:10.1093/bja/aex199 pmid:29121295
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Hillenbrand J,
    2. Getty LA,
    3. Clark MJ,
    4. Wheeler K
    (1995) Acoustic characteristics of American English vowels. J Acoust Soc Am 97:3099–3111. doi:10.1121/1.411872 pmid:7759650
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Hopfinger JB,
    2. Buonocore MH,
    3. Mangun GR
    (2000) The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nat Neurosci 3:284–291. doi:10.1038/72999 pmid:10700262
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Hothorn T,
    2. Bretz F,
    3. Westfall P
    (2008) Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biom J 50:346–363. doi:10.1002/bimj.200810425 pmid:18481363
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Ibrahim AE,
    2. Taraday JK,
    3. Kharasch ED
    (2001) Bispectral index monitoring during sedation with sevoflurane, midazolam, and propofol. Anesthesiology 95:1151–1159. doi:10.1097/00000542-200111000-00019 pmid:11684984
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Joos K,
    2. Gilles A,
    3. Van de Heyning P,
    4. De Ridder D,
    5. Vanneste S
    (2014) From sensation to percept: the neural signature of auditory event-related potentials. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 42:148–156. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.02.009 pmid:24589492
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Koch C,
    2. Massimini M,
    3. Boly M,
    4. Tononi G
    (2016) Neural correlates of consciousness: progress and problems. Nat Rev Neurosci 17:307–321. doi:10.1038/nrn.2016.22 pmid:27094080
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Kok A
    (2001) On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology 38:557–577. doi:10.1017/S0048577201990559 pmid:11352145
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Kovach CK,
    2. Gander PE
    (2016) The demodulated band transform. J Neurosci Methods 261:135–154. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.12.004 pmid:26711370
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Långsjö JW,
    2. Alkire MT,
    3. Kaskinoro K,
    4. Hayama H,
    5. Maksimow A,
    6. Kaisti KK,
    7. Aalto S,
    8. Aantaa R,
    9. Jääskeläinen SK,
    10. Revonsuo A,
    11. Scheinin H
    (2012) Returning from oblivion: imaging the neural core of consciousness. J Neurosci 32:4935–4943. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4962-11.2012 pmid:22492049
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    1. Leslie K,
    2. Sleigh J,
    3. Paech MJ,
    4. Voss L,
    5. Lim CW,
    6. Sleigh C
    (2009) Dreaming and electroencephalographic changes during anesthesia maintained with propofol or desflurane. Anesthesiology 111:547–555. doi:10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181adf768 pmid:19672164
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Liu J,
    2. Singh H,
    3. White PF
    (1997) Electroencephalographic bispectral index correlates with intraoperative recall and depth of propofol-induced sedation. Anesth Analg 84:185–189. doi:10.1213/00000539-199701000-00033 pmid:8989022
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Liu X,
    2. Lauer KK,
    3. Ward BD,
    4. Rao SM,
    5. Li SJ,
    6. Hudetz AG
    (2012) Propofol disrupts functional interactions between sensory and high-order processing of auditory verbal memory. Hum Brain Mapp 33:2487–2498. doi:10.1002/hbm.21385 pmid:21932265
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Llinás R,
    2. Ribary U,
    3. Contreras D,
    4. Pedroarena C
    (1998) The neuronal basis for consciousness. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 353:1841–1849. doi:10.1098/rstb.1998.0336 pmid:9854256
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. ↵
    1. Maris E,
    2. Oostenveld R
    (2007) Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. J Neurosci Methods 164:177–190. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024 pmid:17517438
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Mashour GA
    (2006) Integrating the science of consciousness and anesthesia. Anesth Analg 103:975–982. doi:10.1213/01.ane.0000232442.69757.4a pmid:17000815
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Mashour GA
    (2013) Cognitive unbinding: a neuroscientific paradigm of general anesthesia and related states of unconsciousness. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 37:2751–2759. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.09.009 pmid:24076246
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Mashour GA,
    2. Hudetz AG
    (2017) Bottom-up and top-down mechanisms of general anesthetics modulate different dimensions of consciousness. Front Neural Circuits 11:44. doi:10.3389/fncir.2017.00044 pmid:28676745
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Massimini M,
    2. Ferrarelli F,
    3. Huber R,
    4. Esser SK,
    5. Singh H,
    6. Tononi G
    (2005) Breakdown of cortical effective connectivity during sleep. Science 309:2228–2232. doi:10.1126/science.1117256 pmid:16195466
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  53. ↵
    1. Mumford D
    (1992) On the computational architecture of the neocortex: II. The role of cortico-cortical loops. Biol Cybern 66:241–251. doi:10.1007/BF00198477 pmid:1540675
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Näätänen R,
    2. Alho K
    (1995) Mismatch negativity: a unique measure of sensory processing in audition. Int J Neurosci 80:317–337. doi:10.3109/00207459508986107 pmid:7775056
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Näätänen R,
    2. Paavilainen P,
    3. Tiitinen H,
    4. Jiang D,
    5. Alho K
    (1993) Attention and mismatch negativity. Psychophysiology 30:436–450. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb02067.x pmid:8416070
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Näätänen R,
    2. Kujala T,
    3. Winkler I
    (2011) Auditory processing that leads to conscious perception: a unique window to central auditory processing opened by the mismatch negativity and related responses. Psychophysiology 48:4–22. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01114.x pmid:20880261
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Nagahama Y,
    2. Kovach CK,
    3. Ciliberto M,
    4. Joshi C,
    5. Rhone AE,
    6. Vesole A,
    7. Gander PE,
    8. Nourski KV,
    9. Oya H,
    10. Howard MA 3rd.,
    11. Kawasaki H,
    12. Dlouhy BJ
    (2018) Localization of musicogenic epilepsy to Heschl's gyrus and superior temporal plane: case report. J Neurosurg 129:157–164. doi:10.3171/2017.3.JNS162559 pmid:28946181
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Nelson LE,
    2. Guo TZ,
    3. Lu J,
    4. Saper CB,
    5. Franks NP,
    6. Maze M
    (2002) The sedative component of anesthesia is mediated by GABA(A) receptors in an endogenous sleep pathway. Nat Neurosci 5:979–984. doi:10.1038/nn913 pmid:12195434
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Nir Y,
    2. Vyazovskiy VV,
    3. Cirelli C,
    4. Banks MI,
    5. Tononi G
    (2015) Auditory responses and stimulus-specific adaptation in rat auditory cortex are preserved across NREM and REM sleep. Cereb Cortex 25:1362–1378. doi:10.1093/cercor/bht328 pmid:24323498
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Nourski KV
    (2017) Auditory processing in the human cortex: an intracranial electrophysiology perspective. Laryngoscope Investig Otolaryngol 2:147–156. doi:10.1002/lio2.73 pmid:28894834
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Nourski KV,
    2. Steinschneider M,
    3. McMurray B,
    4. Kovach CK,
    5. Oya H,
    6. Kawasaki H,
    7. Howard MA 3rd.
    (2014) Functional organization of human auditory cortex: investigation of response latencies through direct recordings. Neuroimage 101:598–609. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.004 pmid:25019680
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Nourski KV,
    2. Howard MA 3rd.
    (2015) Invasive recordings in the human auditory cortex. Handb Clin Neurol 129:225–244. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-62630-1.00013-5 pmid:25726272
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Nourski KV,
    2. Banks MI,
    3. Steinschneider M,
    4. Rhone AE,
    5. Kawasaki H,
    6. Mueller RN,
    7. Todd MM,
    8. Howard MA 3rd.
    (2017) Electrocorticographic delineation of human auditory cortical fields based on effects of propofol anesthesia. Neuroimage 152:78–93. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.061 pmid:28254512
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Nourski KV,
    2. Steinschneider M,
    3. Rhone AE,
    4. Kawasaki H,
    5. Howard MA 3rd.,
    6. Banks MI
    (2018) Processing of auditory novelty across the cortical hierarchy: an intracranial electrophysiology study. Neuroimage. Advance online publication. Retrieved Aug. 13, 2018. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.027. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.027 pmid:30114466
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Plourde G,
    2. Belin P,
    3. Chartrand D,
    4. Fiset P,
    5. Backman SB,
    6. Xie G,
    7. Zatorre RJ
    (2006) Cortical processing of complex auditory stimuli during alterations of consciousness with the general anesthetic propofol. Anesthesiology 104:448–457. doi:10.1097/00000542-200603000-00011 pmid:16508391
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Portas CM,
    2. Krakow K,
    3. Allen P,
    4. Josephs O,
    5. Armony JL,
    6. Frith CD
    (2000) Auditory processing across the sleep–wake cycle: simultaneous EEG and fMRI monitoring in humans. Neuron 28:991–999. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00169-0 pmid:11163282
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Rao RP,
    2. Ballard DH
    (1999) Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nat Neurosci 2:79–87. doi:10.1038/4580 pmid:10195184
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Raz A,
    2. Grady SM,
    3. Krause BM,
    4. Uhlrich DJ,
    5. Manning KA,
    6. Banks MI
    (2014) Preferential effect of isoflurane on top-down versus bottom-up pathways in sensory cortex. Front Syst Neurosci 8:191. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2014.00191 pmid:25339873
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Reddy CG,
    2. Dahdaleh NS,
    3. Albert G,
    4. Chen F,
    5. Hansen D,
    6. Nourski K,
    7. Kawasaki H,
    8. Oya H,
    9. Howard MA 3rd.
    (2010) A method for placing Heschl gyrus depth electrodes. J Neurosurg 112:1301–1307. doi:10.3171/2009.7.JNS09404 pmid:19663547
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Rudolph U,
    2. Antkowiak B
    (2004) Molecular and neuronal substrates for general anaesthetics. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:709–720. doi:10.1038/nrn1496 pmid:15322529
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Sanders RD,
    2. Tononi G,
    3. Laureys S,
    4. Sleigh JW
    (2012) Unresponsiveness not equal unconsciousness. Anesthesiology 116:946–959. doi:10.1097/ALN.0b013e318249d0a7 pmid:22314293
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Sergent C,
    2. Baillet S,
    3. Dehaene S
    (2005) Timing of the brain events underlying access to consciousness during the attentional blink. Nat Neurosci 8:1391–1400. doi:10.1038/nn1549 pmid:16158062
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Shushruth S
    (2013) Exploring the neural basis of consciousness through anesthesia. J Neurosci 33:1757–1758. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5215-12.2013 pmid:23365215
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  74. ↵
    1. Simpson TP,
    2. Manara AR,
    3. Kane NM,
    4. Barton RL,
    5. Rowlands CA,
    6. Butler SR
    (2002) Effect of propofol anaesthesia on the event-related potential mismatch negativity and the auditory-evoked potential N1. Br J Anaesth 89:382–388. doi:10.1093/bja/89.3.382 pmid:12402715
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Steinschneider M,
    2. Fishman YI,
    3. Arezzo JC
    (2008) Spectrotemporal analysis of evoked and induced electroencephalographic responses in primary auditory cortex (A1) of the awake monkey. Cereb Cortex 18:610–625. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm094 pmid:17586604
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Strauss M,
    2. Sitt JD,
    3. King JR,
    4. Elbaz M,
    5. Azizi L,
    6. Buiatti M,
    7. Naccache L,
    8. van Wassenhove V,
    9. Dehaene S
    (2015) Disruption of hierarchical predictive coding during sleep. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112:E1353–E1362. doi:10.1073/pnas.1501026112 pmid:25737555
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  77. ↵
    1. Tononi G,
    2. Boly M,
    3. Massimini M,
    4. Koch C
    (2016) Integrated information theory: from consciousness to its physical substrate. Nat Rev Neurosci 17:450–461. doi:10.1038/nrn.2016.44 pmid:27225071
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. Uhrig L,
    2. Janssen D,
    3. Dehaene S,
    4. Jarraya B
    (2016) Cerebral responses to local and global auditory novelty under general anesthesia. Neuroimage 141:326–340. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.004 pmid:27502046
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Vanluchene AL,
    2. Struys MM,
    3. Heyse BE,
    4. Mortier EP
    (2004) Spectral entropy measurement of patient responsiveness during propofol and remifentanil: a comparison with the bispectral index. Br J Anaesth 93:645–654. doi:10.1093/bja/aeh251 pmid:15321934
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Viertiö-Oja H,
    2. Maja V,
    3. Särkelä M,
    4. Talja P,
    5. Tenkanen N,
    6. Tolvanen-Laakso H,
    7. Paloheimo M,
    8. Vakkuri A,
    9. Yli-Hankala A,
    10. Meriläinen P
    (2004) Description of the Entropy algorithm as applied in the Datex-Ohmeda S/5 Entropy Module. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 48:154–161. doi:10.1111/j.0001-5172.2004.00322.x pmid:14995936
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    1. Wilf M,
    2. Ramot M,
    3. Furman-Haran E,
    4. Arzi A,
    5. Levkovitz Y,
    6. Malach R
    (2016) Diminished auditory responses during NREM sleep correlate with the hierarchy of language processing. PLoS One 11:e0157143. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0157143 pmid:27310812
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    1. Zhang Z,
    2. Ferretti V,
    3. Güntan I,
    4. Moro A,
    5. Steinberg EA,
    6. Ye Z,
    7. Zecharia AY,
    8. Yu X,
    9. Vyssotski AL,
    10. Brickley SG,
    11. Yustos R,
    12. Pillidge ZE,
    13. Harding EC,
    14. Wisden W,
    15. Franks NP
    (2015) Neuronal ensembles sufficient for recovery sleep and the sedative actions of alpha2 adrenergic agonists. Nat Neurosci 18:553–561. doi:10.1038/nn.3957 pmid:25706476
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 38 (39)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 38, Issue 39
26 Sep 2018
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Auditory Predictive Coding across Awareness States under Anesthesia: An Intracranial Electrophysiology Study
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Auditory Predictive Coding across Awareness States under Anesthesia: An Intracranial Electrophysiology Study
Kirill V. Nourski, Mitchell Steinschneider, Ariane E. Rhone, Hiroto Kawasaki, Matthew A. Howard III, Matthew I. Banks
Journal of Neuroscience 26 September 2018, 38 (39) 8441-8452; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0967-18.2018

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Auditory Predictive Coding across Awareness States under Anesthesia: An Intracranial Electrophysiology Study
Kirill V. Nourski, Mitchell Steinschneider, Ariane E. Rhone, Hiroto Kawasaki, Matthew A. Howard III, Matthew I. Banks
Journal of Neuroscience 26 September 2018, 38 (39) 8441-8452; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0967-18.2018
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • averaged evoked potential
  • consciousness
  • electrocorticography
  • high gamma
  • human auditory cortex
  • propofol

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Axonal ER Ca2+ Release Selectively Enhances Activity-Independent Glutamate Release in a Huntington Disease Model
  • Sound improves neuronal encoding of visual stimuli in mouse primary visual cortex
  • Decoding of working memory contents in auditory cortex is not distractor-resistant
Show more Research Articles

Systems/Circuits

  • Sound improves neuronal encoding of visual stimuli in mouse primary visual cortex
  • Model-Based Approach Shows ON Pathway Afferents Elicit a Transient Decrease of V1 Responses
  • The Neural Basis for Biased Behavioral Responses Evoked by Galvanic Vestibular Stimulation in Primates
Show more Systems/Circuits
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.