Abstract
Motor and premotor cortices are crucial for the control of movements. However, we still know little about how these areas contribute to higher-order motor control, such as deciding which movements to make and when to make them. Here we focus on rodent studies and review recent findings, which suggest that—in addition to motor control—neurons in motor cortices play a role in sensory integration, behavioral strategizing, working memory, and decision-making. We suggest that these seemingly disparate functions may subserve an evolutionarily conserved role in sensorimotor cognition and that further study of rodent motor cortices could make a major contribution to our understanding of the evolution and function of the mammalian frontal cortex.
Introduction
Primate motor and premotor cortices are some of the most intensely studied structures in all of neuroscience. Despite our sizeable knowledge, several major conceptual questions remain open. For example, the classic controversy over whether motor cortex acts mainly as a musclelotopic map of the body, organizing low-level features of movements (e.g., force; Evarts, 1968; Asanuma, 1975) or mainly represents high-level movement kinematics (Fetz, 1992; Omrani et al., 2017) has recently been further complicated by the observation that motor cortex appears to be organized both somatotopically and according to behavioral categories (Graziano et al., 2002; Graziano, 2016). Second, it is still an open question whether population activity sums to generate motor output (Georgopoulos et al., 1982, 1986), or whether preparatory activity, for example (Tanji and Evarts, 1976) is better understood as acting to configure the state of a dynamical system (Shenoy et al., 2013). Third, in a sense, motor control is decision-making (Wolpert and Landy, 2012), but we still know little about how motor cortices contribute to actually deciding how and when to act (or not to act; Ebbesen and Brecht, 2017), beyond simply managing the execution of the selected motor plans (Gold and Shadlen, 2007; Thura and Cisek, 2014; Remington et al., 2018). Finally, the discovery of mirror neuron responses in premotor (di Pellegrino et al., 1992), but also in proper M1 (Tkach et al., 2007; Dushanova and Donoghue, 2010) and corticospinal M1 neurons (Vigneswaran et al., 2013; Kraskov et al., 2014) raises intriguing questions about how motor cortices contribute to motor imagery, action understanding, social meta-cognition and cognition more generally (Kilner and Lemon, 2013).
A comparative study of forebrain motor control in rodents in addition to primates (and other species; Ocaña et al., 2015), could be a powerful way to advance our understanding of the evolution and function of the mammalian frontal cortex. In recent years, there has been massive advances in tools for monitoring and manipulating neural activity of awake, behaving rodents with cellular and subcellular resolution, beyond what is currently practical in primates. For example, there are currently abundantly available transgenic lines and viral tools (Heldt and Ressler, 2009; Witten et al., 2011; Harris et al., 2014), optogenetics (Deisseroth, 2015; Kim et al., 2017), DREADDs (Whissell et al., 2016), in vivo multiphoton imaging of various sensors (Broussard et al., 2014; Yang and Yuste, 2017), high-density electrophysiology (Buzsáki et al., 2015; Jun et al., 2017) and genome editing tools (Heidenreich and Zhang, 2016). Further, as we will outline in this review, it is possible to train rats to solve complex and demanding motor-cognitive tasks and precisely quantify (for example by high-speed videography, Rigosa et al., 2017; Nashaat et al., 2017) the kinematics of limb and whisker movements to investigate the temporal dynamics and outcome of decision making processes, spanning from known sensory input, across internal deliberation to final motor output (Ölveczky, 2011; Peters et al., 2017b; Svoboda and Li, 2018).
One obstacle to applying knowledge learned in rodents to understand primate cortex is that the correspondence between rodent and primate motor cortices is mostly unknown and that current naming schemes are inconsistent and confusing (Brecht, 2011). For example, tracing studies (Zingg et al., 2014) and classic delineation of rat frontal cortex by perithreshold intracortical microstimulation (Hall and Lindholm, 1974; Gioanni and Lamarche, 1985; Neafsey et al., 1986) suggests a large, somatotopically organized primary motor representation (“ratunculus”), that encompasses most of frontal cortex (Fig. 1A). However, the real picture is more complex and stimulation and anatomical tracing suggests, that forelimb movements, for example, are controlled by two, spatially segregated regions (caudal and rostral forelimb areas; Neafsey and Sievert, 1982; Rouiller et al., 1993; Fig. 1B). Nomenclature, that relies on comparative anatomy to name motor structures in the rat brain after their putative corresponding primate homologues, often suggest conflicting naming schemes. Thus, the same region of rat frontal cortex is referred to in the literature as primary vibrissa motor cortex (VMC; whisker M1; Brecht et al., 2004a,b; Berg and Kleinfeld, 2003; Hill et al., 2011; Ebbesen et al., 2017), secondary motor cortex (M2; a putative homolog of primate supplementary motor areas; Paxinos and Watson, 1982; Murakami et al., 2014, 2017; Mimica et al., 2018), the frontal orientation field (FOF; a putative homolog of the primate frontal eye field; Erlich et al., 2011; Hanks et al., 2015), frontal area 2 (FR2; Insanally et al., 2018), ventral frontal motor cortex (vFMCx; Lee et al., 2008) and medial agranular cortex (AGm; Smith and Alloway, 2013; Fig. 1C–E). In the mouse, the terminology is comparably varied and the same region also goes under several names, such as vibrissa/whisker motor cortex (vM1: Huber et al., 2012; wM1: Matyas et al., 2010; Sreenivasan et al., 2015; 2016), secondary motor cortex (M2; Schneider et al., 2014; Nelson and Mooney, 2016; Siniscalchi et al., 2016), medial agranular motor cortex (also M2; Nelson et al., 2013), frontal motor cortex (fMR; Goard et al., 2016), and secondary motor area (MOs; Allen Mouse Brain Atlas, Lein et al., 2007; Zingg et al., 2014). This variety of terminology is confusing and can hamper discovery and exchange both between primate and rodent researchers and within the rodent community.
Fortunately, the inconsistency in nomenclature has been beneficial in some ways. Because it is unclear which primate motor structures the various regions of rodent frontal cortex correspond to, this neuronal population has been investigated from very divergent vantage points, something that is actually rare in neuroscience, and implicated in a surprising variety of functions. Here, we review recent studies, which have investigated the role of rodent frontal cortex, in classic motor control of whisker and limb movements, in processing sensory stimuli, and in higher-order functions, such as motor decision-making, both in self-initiated action and in tasks, that require integration of sensory information over time. We conclude by highlighting major open questions and future directions.
Frontal control of whisker movements
A relatively large portion of rodent motor cortex is involved in whisker control. Active vibrissal touch is a major sensing strategy of rats and mice, small nocturnal mammals who live in dark tunnels. These animals have evolved highly specialized neural circuitry for expert control of whisker movements. Rats move their whiskers individually during active touch sensing (Welker, 1964; Sachdev et al., 2002; O'Connor et al., 2010; Zuo et al., 2011; Voigts et al., 2015), in anticipation of head turning (Towal and Hartmann, 2006) and during social interactions (Wolfe et al., 2011). The fine motor control for active vibrissal touch mirrors the fine motor control of primate and human fingertips (Diamond, 2010; Prescott et al., 2011). Analogous to the enlarged representation of digital movements in the primate and human motor homunculus (Leyton and Sherrington, 1917; Penfield and Boldrey, 1937), the vibrissa motor representation in frontal cortex (as assessed by intracortical microstimulation) is huge, taking up ∼6.5% of the whole cortical sheet (Hall and Lindholm, 1974; Gioanni and Lamarche, 1985; Neafsey et al., 1986; Zilles and Wree, 1995; Brecht et al., 2004a).
Similarly to circuits controlling locomotion (Franz and Lashley, 1917; Kiehn, 2006), the whisking rhythm is generated subcortically by a central pattern generator in the brainstem reticular formation (Gao et al., 2001; Moore et al., 2013; Deschênes et al., 2016), but the way by which the VMC modulates brainstem circuits to select behaviorally appropriate movement output is still an open question. Just like in the primate distal limb system (Cheney and Fetz, 1980), there is a monosynaptic corticomotoneuronal pathway from layer 5 of VMC onto motoneurons in the facial nucleus but, in contrast to the primate system, these direct connections are extremely sparse (Grinevich et al., 2005; Sreenivasan et al., 2015). The vast majority of descending axons from the VMC affect motoneurons oligosynaptically and terminate onto brainstem interneurons (Hattox et al., 2002; Grinevich et al., 2005; Sreenivasan et al., 2015; Jeong et al., 2016). In general, the relationship between VMC activity and whisking kinematics is surprisingly weak; much weaker, for example, than the tight correlations between M1 activity and distal limb movements in primates (Lemon, 2008) or rodents (Isomura et al., 2009; Miri et al., 2017). Early studies identified single VMC neurons, whose activity significantly correlated with whisking kinematics, but- in contrast to the primate (Georgopoulos et al., 1982, 1986)- there was no relationship between population activity and whisking (Carvell et al., 1996; Hill et al., 2011; Friedman et al., 2012; Gerdjikov et al., 2013).
A population pattern has only begun to emerge in recent studies. One recent study (Sreenivasan et al., 2016) looked at the laminar distribution of activity during whisking in head fixed mice, and found an overall decrease in the activity of layer 2/3 neurons during whisking. Neurons in layer 5 (corticofugal neurons) had a mixed response. At the population level, there was a tiny increase in the median, but a large increase in the mean layer 5 activity around the onset of whisking. This skewed firing rate distribution could indicate a role for a subpopulation of high-firing-rate VMC neurons in whisking initiation, perhaps analogous to how fast-spiking parvalbumin-expressing GABAergic neurons in forelimb motor cortex show strong increases around the onset of reaching movements (Estebanez et al., 2017). Another recent study investigated activity in layer 5 of VMC in freely moving rats during several types of self-initiated vibrissal behaviors: Exploratory whisking in the air, whisking to palpate objects and social whisking during facial interactions with conspecifics (Fig. 2A; Ebbesen et al., 2017). All whisking behaviors were associated with an ∼21% overall decrease in spike rates in layer 5 of VMC. Recordings from layer 5 VMC neurons in socially interacting rats revealed that social whisking was associated with reduced cellular excitability and membrane hyperpolarization, suggesting increased inhibition during whisking (Ebbesen et al., 2017). These observations suggest that VMC gates the subcortical whisking pattern generator, such that a decrease in descending cortical input to downstream brainstem targets ultimately leads to whisker protraction and disinhibits whisking (Deschênes et al., 2016; Ebbesen et al., 2017; Guest et al., 2018).
Several other observations align with this “suppressive motor control” interpretation, chiefly among them the curious fact that while the whisker musculature and vibrissal motoneurons are laid out for forward movement of whiskers (Dörfl, 1982; Klein and Rhoades, 1985; Herfst and Brecht, 2008; Haidarliu et al., 2010; 2014), intracortical microstimulation of layer 5 (Hall and Lindholm, 1974; Gioanni and Lamarche, 1985; Neafsey et al., 1986; Berg and Kleinfeld, 2003; Brecht et al., 2004a; Haiss and Schwarz, 2005; Ferezou et al., 2007; Tandon et al., 2008; Matyas et al., 2010; Ebbesen et al., 2017) and even stimulation of single pyramidal neurons (Fig. 2B; Brecht et al., 2004b) in VMC almost exclusively evokes whisker retraction, not, as perhaps expected (Graziano et al., 2002; Graziano, 2016), the behaviorally relevant forward whisker movements. Observations after optogenetic manipulation (Sreenivasan et al., 2015, 2016; Auffret et al., 2018) are more mixed (Wolff and Ölveczky, 2018). Similarly, disinhibition of VMC by GABA antagonists induces myoclonic whisker retractions, which are time-locked to bouts of disinhibition-induced increases in multiunit activity (Fig. 2C; Castro-Alamancos, 2006). Mirroring the effects of VMC activation, unilateral lesioning (Gao et al., 2003) and unilateral inactivation (Ebbesen et al., 2017) of VMC moves the contralateral whiskers forward and increases whisking contralaterally (Fig. 2D).
Frontal control of limb movements
Stimulation and tracing studies implicate two frontal regions in the control of forelimb movements: caudal forelimb area (CFA) and rostral forelimb area (RFA; Fig. 1B; Neafsey and Sievert, 1982; Rouiller et al., 1993; Tennant et al., 2011). Both areas are activated during skilled forelimb movement and are essential for the execution of trained limb movement (Hira et al., 2013; Brown and Teskey, 2014; Guo et al., 2015; Schiemann et al., 2015; Miri et al., 2017; Morandell and Huber, 2017; Galiñanes et al., 2018), but the relationship between movement and spiking activity is dynamic (Peters et al., 2014, 2017a) and is different between RFA and CFA. For example, activity in RFA, but not in CFA, is sensitive to behavioral context (Saiki et al., 2014), contralateral movement bias of activity is weaker in RFA than CFA (Soma et al., 2017), and activity in RFA, but not CFA, is larger for externally triggered movements than internally triggered movements (Kimura et al., 2017).
Some observations indicate that RFA is a premotor structure, that represents higher-order information about movements, whereas CFA encodes concrete motor information, such as motor commands to the musculoskeletal system. However, the similarity of activity patterns and the effects of stimulation in RFA and CFA suggest that they are part of a highly integrated computational unit (Hyland, 1998; Harrison et al., 2012; Saiki et al., 2014; Hira et al., 2015; Morandell and Huber, 2017; Wang et al., 2017). In both RFA and CFA, two major types of deep layer pyramidal neurons send their axons to other areas: intratelencephalic (IT-type) neurons project bilaterally to the cerebral cortex and striatum, whereas extratelencephalic (ET-type) neurons, project to the thalamus, spinal cord and other areas ipsilaterally (Harris and Shepherd, 2015; Baker et al., 2018). In a study, in which spike patterns in CFA and RFA were separated by projection targets (determined by optogenetic stimulation), ET-type neurons showed postspike suppression in spike autocorrelograms, which was independent of behavioral conditions (Saiki et al., 2018). The CFA ET-type neurons exhibited larger bias toward contralateral movement compared with RFA, and IT-type neurons had a large fraction of bilateral movement activity especially in RFA (Soma et al., 2017).
Sensory representations in frontal cortex
Motor planning requires the integration of sensory input to generate appropriate motor output. Accordingly, the frontal motor cortex is widely connected to sensory cortices, including auditory, visual, and somatosensory cortex (Reep et al., 1987; Hoover and Vertes, 2007; Barthas and Kwan, 2017). Somatosensory signals in frontal motor cortex have been widely explored. Several motor cortical neurons have low-latency, “S1”-like responses to whisker deflection (Kleinfeld et al., 2002; Ferezou et al., 2007; Huber et al., 2012; Smith and Alloway, 2013; Zagha et al., 2015) and tactile stimulation of forelimbs (Estebanez et al., 2017). In addition, there are strong recurrent corticocortical connections between VMC and barrel cortex, the primary whisker representation in S1 (Mao et al., 2011; Kinnischtzke et al., 2014, 2016). Motor cortical feedback modulates sensory processing in S1 (Zagha et al., 2013; Manita et al., 2015) and motor cortical activity facilitates sensory responses in barrel cortex and the thalamus (Lee et al., 2008). Imaging of axonal projections from M1 to layer 1 barrel cortex while mice performed an object localization task has shown that M1 neurons carry information about task-related variables, including touch (Petreanu et al., 2012). Moreover, several studies have shown that layer 2/3 M1 neurons respond to both touch stimuli (whisker-dependent object detection) and motor behaviors such as whisker movements or licking during sensory go/no-go tasks (Huber et al., 2012; Zagha et al., 2015).
In addition to touch responsive neurons, some motor cortical neurons respond to auditory stimuli. In two regions of ferret frontal motor cortex, the dorsal orbital gyrus (a putative homolog of primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; Duque and McCormick, 2010) and anterior sigmoid gyrus (a putative homolog of primate premotor cortex; Fritz et al., 2010), trial-averaged spiking activity is modulated by auditory stimuli (tones or clicks) during a conditioned avoidance task, but only weakly modulated in a passive context where auditory stimuli were presented before behavior (Fritz et al., 2010). Frontal cortical neurons that were auditory responsive showed both suppression and enhancement of spiking activity to the target tone. Auditory-evoked responses have also been reported in mouse motor cortex during reaching (Estebanez et al., 2017) and licking (Siniscalchi et al., 2016) tasks. In the anterior part of mouse motor cortex, auditory-evoked responses are modulated by behavioral context (Siniscalchi et al., 2016). Auditory representations are present in PV+ forelimb M1 neurons in the absence of sound-triggered movements, suggesting that sensory input alone can drive M1 neurons (Estebanez et al., 2017). Interestingly, PV+ neurons in M1 are not indiscriminately driven by sensory input as the proportion of neurons that can be modulated by somatosensory input is greater than the proportion of neurons that are auditory-responsive suggesting a dissociation in sensory signals that can modulate PV+ cells in M1 (Estebanez et al., 2017).
There are corticocortical connections between auditory cortex and mouse secondary motor cortex (Nelson et al., 2013; Nelson and Mooney, 2016) that contribute to suppressing sound-evoked responses in auditory cortex during locomotion (Schneider et al., 2014). These connections might be important for disambiguating self-generated sounds from other sounds in the environment (Schneider and Mooney, 2015), but motor cortex might also participate more directly in auditory processing. A recent study found that neurons in rat FR2 (Fig. 1D) were more informative about task-relevant auditory stimuli than auditory cortical neurons (Insanally et al., 2018). Although auditory cortex reliably responds to pure tones in untrained animals, when tones take on behavioral significance (i.e., when the animal is trained to respond to tones for food reward) stimulus information is more prevalent and pervasive in frontal cortex. An interspike-interval-based decoder revealed that cells that appeared to be choice-selective when viewed at the level of the trial-averaged firing rate (i.e., cells that displayed “ramping activity”) were found to be highly stimulus selective on single trials (Insanally et al., 2018).
Although it is well established that primate frontal cortices play a major role in visual sensorimotor transformations (Hatsopoulos and Suminski, 2011), we still know little about how rodent frontal motor cortices contribute to the processing of visual stimuli. Corticocortical projections from rodent frontal cortex relay motor-related signals to primary visual cortex (Zhang et al., 2014; Leinweber et al., 2017) and there are reciprocal connections between frontal motor cortex and higher visual areas (Itokazu et al., 2018). Frontal motor cortical neurons modulate their activity during the presentation of visual stimuli (Goard et al., 2016; Itokazu et al., 2018). However, population decoding did not provide significant information about the type of visual stimulus (horizontal vs vertical drifting gratings) in a go/no-go task (Goard et al., 2016). In a study where rats had to integrate sensory evidence in LED flashes, neurons in rat frontal motor cortex exhibited transient responses to visual stimulation and also encoded the amount of sensory evidence provided (Scott et al., 2017). Whether information about visual stimuli is embedded in spike timing (as was the case for auditory signals in rat FR2) remains an open question.
Together, these results suggest that motor cortex plays an important role in the processing of sensory input across several sensory modalities. Sensory signals in motor cortex might provide important feedback about ongoing motor programs for tuning motor output in the short term. In the long term, sensory feedback may be important for motor learning and behavioral strategizing, more generally. Lesion studies point to an important role of rodent frontal cortex in the learning of motor tasks (Huber et al., 2012; Kawai et al., 2015; Zagha et al., 2015) and motor cortical activity is reorganized across motor leaning (Laubach et al., 2000; Komiyama et al., 2010; Huber et al., 2012; Peters et al., 2014, 2017a; Makino et al., 2017; Miri et al., 2017; for review, see Peters et al., 2017b).
Ruling out covert muscular responses during sensory stimulation is difficult and seemingly “sensory” responses in motor cortices may actually be “motor” responses (e.g., preparatory activity, a gated movement or subtle, unnoticed movements). On the other hand, both somatosensory and motor cortex receive direct thalamic input (Deschênes et al., 1998; Ohno et al., 2012; Hooks et al., 2015), both areas project to the spinal cord and brainstem (Catsman-Berrevoets and Kuypers, 1976; Groos et al., 1978; Sreenivasan et al., 2015) and stimulation of either area elicits movements with low latency (Leyton and Sherrington, 1917; Penfield and Boldrey, 1937; Gioanni and Lamarche, 1985; Neafsey et al., 1986; Matyas et al., 2010; Sreenivasan et al., 2015), pointing to a role for both areas in motor control and sensory integration.
Frontal control of action timing
Motor control and decision-making involves not only choosing among multiple alternative options but also deciding when to act. Murakami et al. (2014) studied a role of rat secondary motor cortex in the action timing decision (M2; Fig. 1C). They used a waiting time decision task, where a rat had to decide how long to keep waiting for a delayed reward and when to give up and act for an immediately available, but less valuable option. Neural recording from M2 during this behavioral task revealed that firing rates in a subpopulation of M2 neurons encode the animal's waiting time. Such a firing-rate-based representation can feed into an integrator circuit, and be transformed into ramping activity and a mechanism to detect threshold crossing of the ramping activity could serve to generate a movement signal, at the end of a waiting period. Consistent with a role in an integration-to-threshold process, the authors found ramping activity during the waiting period, which reached a threshold level at the time of “give-up” behavior (Ratcliff, 1978; Gold and Shadlen, 2007). A similar neural signal of action timing is observed in primate premotor cortices in various contexts, for example ramp-to-threshold like responses in sensory decision tasks (Ding and Gold, 2012) and timing tasks (Lebedev et al., 2008; Mita et al., 2009). This suggests a conserved premotor mechanism, both in rodents and primates, in deciding and planning timing of future actions.
In contrast to M2, where a significant fraction of neurons encode waiting time in single trials, neurons in medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC; a major input structure of M2) contained very few neurons encoding the trial-to-trial waiting times (Murakami et al., 2017). Instead, mPFC neurons only encoded a slowly fluctuating component of waiting-time variability, a decision bias to wait shorter or longer, which fluctuated over >10 trials, and was modulated by previous experience. This finding is in line with the idea that higher-order areas, such as the PFC, can integrate information over time as well as space more extensively than lower sensory cortical areas (Hasson et al., 2008; Murray et al., 2014), and extends this idea to a motor domain: The prefrontal cortex contains information about decision biases or strategies that are adjusted slowly over time according to past integrated experiences and/or internal states, but it cares less about planning imminent actions. Instead, more dedicated motor areas, such as M2, are critical in planning what to do next and when to execute it, based on top-down influences from prefrontal areas.
Frontal role in planning orienting movements
The existence of a homolog of the primate frontal eye field (FEF) in rat frontal cortex was first suggested by Leonard (1969), based on similarities in connectivity with subcortical areas. Lesion (Reep et al., 2004) and microstimulation (Sinnamon and Galer, 1984) data supported that hypothesis. This may seem surprising, given that rats are not known for their visual abilities (Yilmaz and Meister, 2013; but see Seabrook et al., 2017). However, the primate FEF is not an “eye” field, but rather a “gaze” field or an orienting field. Microstimulation of FEF in animals who can freely move their heads generates coordinated head-eye movements (Knight and Fuchs, 2007) and the human FEF is activated when remembering spatial locations behind the head (Tark and Curtis, 2009). All bilaterally symmetric animals need to plan and generate orienting movements to actively sense the world (Ocaña et al., 2015). With that in mind, the idea of a homolog, a rat FOF, to the (poorly named) FEF seems less absurd.
To further investigate this homology Erlich et al. (2011) trained rats on a memory-guided orienting task (MGO) inspired by the classically FEF-dependent memory-guided saccade task (Funahashi et al., 1991; Chafee and Goldman-Rakic, 1998; Dias and Segraves, 1999). In the MGO task, rats must fixate during the presentation of a brief auditory cue (e.g., low-frequency = go left, high-frequency = go right) and during a subsequent memory delay period (Fig. 3A). After a go cue, rats are rewarded if they orient to the correct side. Given that FOF is also VMC, Erlich et al. (2011) performed several control experiments to determine whether the rats were using a whisker strategy to perform the task. Neither shaving off the mystacial whiskers nor injecting lidocaine into the whisker pad on one side had a significant effect on behavior in well trained animals. Moreover, using video analysis, they showed that during the memory period the whiskers were retracted and rhythmic whisking was suppressed even as activity was ramping up (consistent with the findings described in “Frontal control of whisker movements”; Ebbesen et al., 2017).
Unilateral pharmacological inactivation of FOF resulted in an ipsilateral bias, similar to findings in primate FEF (Sommer et al., 1997; Dias and Segraves, 1999). Furthermore, neural activity during the memory-period predicted the upcoming choice of the animal, with neural encoding of the upcoming motor response that was low during the sensory cue, and gradually increased during the memory delay period (Fig. 3C). Temporally precise unilateral optogenetic inactivation (halorhodopsin, eNpHR3.0) of the FOF revealed that inactivation during the first half of the cue period, the time of minimal information encoding, resulted in the largest ipsilateral choice bias, whereas inactivation during the second half of the memory period, the time of maximal information encoding, resulted in the smallest choice bias (Fig. 3B). Inactivation during the motor action resulted in no bias or change in movement kinetics (Kopec et al., 2015).
To further investigate the role of FOF in orienting decisions, rats were also trained on a gradual accumulation of evidence task, the Poisson Clicks task (Brunton et al., 2013), similar to the primate random dot motion accumulation task (Shadlen and Newsome, 1996). In the Clicks task the cue consisted of two independent streams of Poisson timed auditory clicks with different underlying rates, presented from speakers to the animal's left and right, after which the rat must respond to the side that played more clicks (Fig. 3D). Only inactivation that overlapped the end of the auditory cue and fixation period resulted in a choice bias (Fig. 3E; Hanks et al., 2015).
The different inactivation effect timings between the MGO and Poisson Clicks tasks can be reconciled as due to the different demand for flexibility in the movement plan. In the Clicks task the randomly varying Poisson click times require a movement plan which can be updated as new information is accumulated. In the MGO task, on the other hand, the click train is periodic and there is no new information during the delay, so a rigid and robust movement plan is more appropriate.
Results from both tasks could be replicated by modeling the FOF as a bistable attractor network (Machens et al., 2005; Wong and Wang, 2006). In the MGO task the network rapidly falls into one of two strong basins of attraction required to maintain the information during the memory delay period (Fig. 3G). As the network settles, the information encoding increases (Erlich et al., 2011), and the network becomes more resistant to perturbation (Kopec et al., 2015). A similar model but with weaker attractors (Fig. 3H) is capable of replicating findings during the Poisson Clicks task (Erlich et al., 2015; Piet et al., 2017). Incoming information could more easily move the network between two categorical states, consistent with recordings of neurons in the FOF during the Clicks task (Fig. 3F; Hanks et al., 2015). This allowed the network to use information from the entire click train, implementing a flexible plan, which allows it to recover from perturbations early in the stimulus.
Despite the apparent parsimony of this description, many questions remain. According to these models, the FOF is just one element in a distributed network for planning orienting movements (Kopec et al., 2015). The superior colliculus has been identified as another key element, but possibly other prefrontal cortical areas (for planning directional licks, see Chen et al., 2017; Svoboda and Li, 2018) or subcortical regions such as thalamus and basal ganglia (Yartsev et al., 2018) play an important role as well. Moreover, the models do not explain the heterogeneity observed in the firing patterns of FOF neurons (Erlich et al., 2011; Hanks et al., 2015). Cell-type-specific recording and laminar recordings would create important constraints on future models.
Conclusion and future directions
In this review, we summarized some recent investigations into the mechanisms by which rodent frontal cortex contributes to: active-sensing with whisking, preparation and execution of limb movements, sensory-coding, and preparation and execution of orienting movements. While the cortical regions subserving limb movements (CFA and RFA) are fairly well demarcated (Fig. 1A), the other functions reviewed are all found in a highly overlapping region of rat motor cortex (Fig. 1D–E). Is the diversity of function revealed here a specialty of rat frontal cortex? It is tempting to speculate that the smaller brain of the rodent (compared with human or macaque) creates less pressure for wiring efficiency (Chklovskii et al., 2002) and this results in neurons with distinct functional roles intermixed in the cortical surface. Thus, although the enlarged whisker representation in rodent motor cortex (Fig. 1A) mirrors the enlarged distal limb representation in primate M1, the same area also shares similarities with other frontal premotor structures in the primate brain.
A possible role of rodent frontal cortex in tuning (Hill et al., 2011; Friedman et al., 2012; Gerdjikov et al., 2013) and initiating (Sreenivasan et al., 2016) whisker movements is similar to motor control by primate M1, but a prominent role in whisker movement suppression (Ebbesen et al., 2017) is not. In humans and primates, other prefrontal and frontal structures than M1 are paramount for behavioral inhibition and movement suppression (Miller, 2000; Kim and Lee, 2011). For example, neurons in the supplementary eye field play an important role in saccade suppression and countermanding (Schall et al., 2002; Carpenter, 2004; Stuphorn et al., 2010; Shadmehr, 2017) and the presupplementary and supplementary motor areas (SMA) are crucial for voluntary movement inhibition (Nachev et al., 2008; Chen et al., 2010; Wardak, 2011; Filevich et al., 2012a,b). Similarly to how VMC lesions (Gao et al., 2003) or inactivation (Ebbesen et al., 2017) increases whisking, lesions to the SMA evokes involuntary, “alien” movements in humans (Brainin et al., 2008). Further investigations of movement suppression by rat motor cortex could be a way to explore the evolutionary origins of behavioral inhibition by frontal cortex more generally (Laubach et al., 2015; Barthas and Kwan, 2017; Ebbesen and Brecht, 2017).
Similar to the control of vibrissal touch by rat motor cortices, the primate FEF is involved in active-sensing (i.e., visual search; Bichot et al., 2001). Like rat motor cortices, the FEF contains neurons with short-latency sensory responses (Joiner et al., 2017). During action timing and perceptual decision-making, some FEF neurons ramp to threshold before an orienting response (Ding and Gold, 2012) and encode cognitive aspects of decision-making (Teichert et al., 2014). Similar to rat motor cortices during orienting movements, the FEF can be modeled with attractor dynamics (Wimmer et al., 2014). Despite these similarities there are also important differences. The primate FEF plays a direct role in eye movements and visuospatial attention (Moore and Fallah, 2001; Noudoost and Moore, 2011; Gregoriou et al., 2014) and provides an attention-like modulation of visual cortical activity (Moore and Armstrong, 2003; Ekstrom et al., 2008). The tight integration between primate FEF and the oculomotor system is mirrored by similar observations on the human FEF (Grosbras and Paus, 2002) and by studies on the forebrain gaze-control area of the barn owl (a putative avian homolog of primate FEF; Winkowski and Knudsen, 2006; Knudsen, 2007). Like the primate FEF (Merrikhi et al., 2017), rodent frontal motor cortex modulates the activity of the superior colliculus and both primary and higher-order visual areas (Zhang et al., 2014; Leinweber et al., 2017; Itokazu et al., 2018), but, as outlined in this review, unlike primate FEF, rodent frontal motor cortex is much more integrated with the whisker somatomotor system than with the visual system.
The differences between the results in primate FEF and rodent frontal motor cortex may be related to sensorimotor specializations. The FEF has mostly been studied in humans and monkeys, species with foveal vision. For animals with fovea, the target of fixation is almost always the focus of attention and cognition. The peripheral visual system determines how to shift that focus. This generates strong pressure for a tight connection between visual input and the orienting system. As mentioned, rats and mice are adapted for navigating in burrows. Their mystacial whiskers are analogous to the peripheral visual system and their micro-vibrissa (the small whiskers around the mouth and nose) are analogous to foveal vision (Grant et al., 2012). A dangerous object (e.g., a snake) detected via the mystacial whiskers should lead to rapid orienting away from the object. Conversely, a delicious object (e.g., a cricket) should lead to rapid orienting toward that object (Anjum et al., 2006). Thus, as the primate FEF directs spatial attention through its close integration with the visual and oculomotor systems, this region of rodent motor cortex can be considered an orientation field, directing spatial attention through its close integration with the whisker and orienting systems. However, it is well documented that primate FEF plays a role in spatial working memory and spatial attention even when subjects are not planning a movement (Tark and Curtis, 2009; Squire et al., 2013). It remains to be seen whether this is also true of rodent FOF.
This idea fits well with other observations. For example, while the primate superior colliculus is a primarily visuomotor structure (Basso and May, 2017), the rodent superior colliculus plays a major role in integrating vibrissal touch information and controlling vibrissa movement (Miyashita and Mori, 1995; Hemelt and Keller, 2007, 2008; Bezdudnaya and Castro-Alamancos, 2014). In line with a role in head and whisker orienting, head and neck posture modulates activity in rodent frontal motor cortex (Mimica et al., 2018). Moreover, because orienting the whisker field toward a stimulus left or right of the snout requires retracting the whiskers on one side and bringing the whiskers on the opposite side forward, this might help explain why rodent frontal motor cortex field appears so heavily involved in whisker retraction (Ebbesen et al., 2017).
Rodents have great potential as comparative model organisms in motor research. To unlock this potential, it is vital to maintain a broad ethological perspective (Krakauer et al., 2017) and remember two key points: (1) Rodents are not small primates, but have distinct sensorimotor specializations. Neural circuits for control of foveal vision in primates may be mirrored by circuits for sensing by active vibrissal touch in mice and rats. (2) In contrast to sharply delineated motor structures in primates, rodent cortical boundaries are blurry and cortical areas overlap. The same region of rat frontal cortex (Fig. 1C–E) shares similarities with multiple motor and premotor structures in the primate brain (e.g., M1, SMA, and FEF). With these points in mind, investigations of rodent motor cortices, such as the ones reviewed here, can help advance our understanding of the evolution and specialization of neural circuits for sensorimotor cognition across phylogeny.
Footnotes
This work was supported by The Novo Nordisk Foundation (C.L.E.); NYU Provost's Postdoctoral Fellowship and NIDCD K99/R00 Pathway to Independence Award DC015543-01A1 (M.N.I.); F32 MH098572/MH/NIMH NIH HHS/USA, and Howard Hughes Medical Institute/USA (C.D.K.); Uehara Memorial Foundation, Fundação Bial 127/08 and Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia SFRH/BPD/46314/2008 (M.M.); JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number JP18J01678 and JP17K12703 (A.S.); and Program of Shanghai Academic/Technology Research Leader 15XD1503000 and Science and Technology Commission of Shanghai Municipality 15JC1400104 (J.C.E.).
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Christian Ebbesen, New York University, 540 First Avenue, New York, NY 10016. christian.ebbesen{at}nyumc.org