Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Dynamic Interactions between Top–Down Expectations and Conscious Awareness

Erik L. Meijs, Heleen A. Slagter, Floris P. de Lange and Simon van Gaal
Journal of Neuroscience 28 February 2018, 38 (9) 2318-2327; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1952-17.2017
Erik L. Meijs
1Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University and Radboud University Medical Center, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Erik L. Meijs
Heleen A. Slagter
1Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University and Radboud University Medical Center, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands,
2Department of Psychology, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Heleen A. Slagter
Floris P. de Lange
1Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University and Radboud University Medical Center, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Floris P. de Lange
Simon van Gaal
1Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University and Radboud University Medical Center, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands,
2Department of Psychology, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Simon van Gaal
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Task design and behavioral results of Experiments 1 and 2. A, The trial structure of the AB task used in Experiments 1 and 2. Each trial consisted of a stream of rapidly presented letters in which predefined target letters had to be detected and then reported at the end of the stream. The first target (T1: a green G or H) always appeared at the fifth position. The second target (T2: D or K) was presented at varying SOAs (lags) after the first one and was marked by placeholders. The identity of T1 predicted which of the T2 targets was most likely to appear, thereby introducing validly and invalidly predicted T2 targets. On 20% of the trials no second target was presented and a random distractor letter was presented instead. B, Percentage correct T2 target detection at each of the T1–T2 lags for valid expectations, invalid expectations, and T2-absent trials in Experiment 1. Validly predicted T2s were significantly more often perceived than invalidly predicted T2s. C, Percentage of T2 target detection at each of the T1–T2 lags after a valid or invalid expectation or on a T2-absent trial for Experiment 2. Again, validly predicted T2s were more often perceived, in particular at short lags. Error bars represent SEM.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    ERP effects related to T2 prediction validity. A, Topographic maps of the difference between validly and invalidly predicted T2s over time (0 corresponds to T2 onset). Cluster-based permutation tests were used to isolate the significant events, while correcting for multiple comparisons across time and (electrode) space. On each head map, channels with a significant effect for ≥50% of its time window are highlighted. B, The average ERP time course of the 10 channels shown on the head map on the left, shown separately for each validity condition. The significant time-window is marked by a black line above the x-axis. Invalidly predicted T2s were associated with greater frontocentral negativity than validly predicted T2s. C, Bar graphs showing the average amplitude of the four conditions (visibility × prediction) for the significant neural event shown in B. In all plots error bars represent SEM.

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    ERP effects related to T2 visibility analyses. A, Topographic maps showing the difference between seen and missed T2s over time (0 corresponds to T2 onset). Cluster-based permutation tests were used to isolate the significant events while correcting for multiple comparisons across time and (electrode) space. On each head map, channels showing a significant difference for ≥50% of its time window are highlighted. Three events were isolated based on the permutation tests. B, D, F, For each of the events individually, the average ERP time course of the 10 channels shown on the head map on the left, separately for T2-seen and T2-missed conditions is shown. The significant time-window is marked by a black line above the x-axis. C, E, G, Bar graphs showing the average amplitude of the four conditions (visibility × prediction) for the significant neural events shown in B, D, F. In all plots error bars represent SEM.

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Task design and behavioral results of Experiment 3. A, Trial structure of the task used in Experiment 3. T1 visibility was staircased at ∼75% correct by manipulating its duration (on 20% of trials no T1 was presented). B, Percentage of correct T2 target detection at each of the T1–T2 lags after a valid or invalid expectation and on a T2-absent trials for trials where T1 was correctly reported (T1 seen). As in Experiments 1 and 2, when T1 was seen, validly predicted T2s were more often detected than invalidly predicted T2s. C, Solid lines show percentage of T2 target detection at each of the T1–T2 lags after a valid or invalid expectation and on T2-absent trials for trials where T1 was presented but missed. In contrast to T1-seen trials (B), when T1 was not seen, validity did not enhance T2 detection. However, a missed T1 still triggered a significant AB, compared with trials on which no T1 was presented (dotted line). Error bars represent SEM.

Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 38 (9)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 38, Issue 9
28 Feb 2018
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Dynamic Interactions between Top–Down Expectations and Conscious Awareness
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Dynamic Interactions between Top–Down Expectations and Conscious Awareness
Erik L. Meijs, Heleen A. Slagter, Floris P. de Lange, Simon van Gaal
Journal of Neuroscience 28 February 2018, 38 (9) 2318-2327; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1952-17.2017

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Dynamic Interactions between Top–Down Expectations and Conscious Awareness
Erik L. Meijs, Heleen A. Slagter, Floris P. de Lange, Simon van Gaal
Journal of Neuroscience 28 February 2018, 38 (9) 2318-2327; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1952-17.2017
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • attentional blink
  • consciousness
  • electroencephalography
  • expectation
  • visual perception

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Multi-omics analysis reveals miR-7220-5p alleviates N2O addictive behaviors via NR2B/ERK/CREB signaling
  • Disrupted Neurogenesis from Basal Intermediate Precursor Cells Alters the Postnatal Neocortex in the TcMAC21 Mouse model of Down Syndrome
  • HDAC3 Serine 424 Phospho-mimic and Phospho-null Mutants Bidirectionally Modulate Long-Term Memory Formation and Synaptic Plasticity in the Adult and Aging Mouse Brain
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • HDAC3 Serine 424 Phospho-mimic and Phospho-null Mutants Bidirectionally Modulate Long-Term Memory Formation and Synaptic Plasticity in the Adult and Aging Mouse Brain
  • Phospho-CREB Regulation on NMDA Glutamate Receptor 2B and Mitochondrial Calcium Uniporter in the Ventrolateral Periaqueductal Gray Controls Chronic Morphine Withdrawal in Male Rats
  • Is It Me or the Train Moving? Humans Resolve Sensory Conflicts with a Nonlinear Feedback Mechanism in Balance Control
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.