Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Systems/Circuits

Cognition-Enhancing Vagus Nerve Stimulation Alters the Epigenetic Landscape

Teresa H. Sanders, Joseph Weiss, Luke Hogewood, Lan Chen, Casey Paton, Rebekah L. McMahan and J. David Sweatt
Journal of Neuroscience 1 May 2019, 39 (18) 3454-3469; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2407-18.2019
Teresa H. Sanders
1Department of Pharmacology,
2Department of Biomedical Engineering, and
3Neuroscience Program, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee 37232
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Teresa H. Sanders
Joseph Weiss
1Department of Pharmacology,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Luke Hogewood
2Department of Biomedical Engineering, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Lan Chen
3Neuroscience Program, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee 37232
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Casey Paton
1Department of Pharmacology,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Casey Paton
Rebekah L. McMahan
1Department of Pharmacology,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
J. David Sweatt
1Department of Pharmacology,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for J. David Sweatt
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) has been shown to enhance learning and memory, yet the mechanisms behind these enhancements are unknown. Here, we present evidence that epigenetic modulation underlies VNS-induced improvements in cognition. We show that VNS enhances novelty preference (NP); alters the hippocampal, cortical, and blood epigenetic transcriptomes; and epigenetically modulates neuronal plasticity and stress–response signaling genes in male Sprague Dawley rats. Brain–behavior analysis revealed structure-specific relationships between NP test performance (NPTP) and epigenetic alterations. In the hippocampus, NPTP correlated with decreased histone deacetylase 11 (HDAC11), a transcriptional repressor enriched in CA1 cells important for memory consolidation. In the cortex, the immediate early gene (IEG) ARC was increased in VNS rats and correlated with transcription of plasticity genes and epigenetic regulators, including HDAC3. For rats engaged in NPTP, ARC correlated with performance. Interestingly, blood ARC transcripts decreased in VNS rats performing NPTP, but increased in VNS-only rats. Because DNA double-strand breaks (DSBs) facilitate transcription of IEGs, we investigated phosphorylated H2A.X (γH2A.X), a histone modification known to colocalize with DSBs. In agreement with reduced cortical stress–response transcription factor NF-κB1, chromatin immunoprecipitation revealed reduced γH2A.X in the ARC promoter. Surprisingly, VNS did not significantly reduce transcription of cortical or hippocampal proinflammatory cytokines. However, TNFRSF11B (osteoprotegerin) correlated with NPTP as well as plasticity, stress–response signaling, and epigenetic regulation transcripts in both hippocampus and cortex. Together, our findings provide the first evidence that VNS induces widespread changes in the cognitive epigenetic landscape and specifically affects epigenetic modulators associated with NPTP, stress–response signaling, memory consolidation, and cortical neural remodeling.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Recent studies have implicated vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) in enhanced learning and memory. However, whereas epigenetic modifications are known to play an important role in memory, the particular mechanisms involved in VNS-enhanced cognition are unknown. In this study, we examined brain and behavior changes in VNS and sham rats performing a multiday novelty preference (NP) task. We found that VNS activated specific histone modifications and DNA methylation changes at important stress–response signaling and plasticity genes. Both cortical and hippocampal plasticity changes were predictive of NP test performance. Our results reveal important epigenetic alterations associated with VNS cognitive improvements, as well as new potential pharmacological targets for enhancing cortical and hippocampal plasticity.

  • chromatin
  • DNA double-strand breaks
  • epigenetic
  • learning and memory
  • plasticity
  • vagus nerve stimulation

Introduction

Vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) has been shown to facilitate plasticity and memory in animal models and humans (Clark et al., 1999; Hays et al., 2014; Alvarez-Dieppa et al., 2016; Borland et al., 2016; Cao et al., 2016; Sun et al., 2017). However, although the acetylcholine- and norepinephrine-driven immunomodulatory effects of VNS have been thoroughly examined (Borovikova et al., 2000; Berridge and Waterhouse, 2003; Roosevelt et al., 2006; Rosas-Ballina et al., 2011; Andersson and Tracey, 2012; Hulsey et al., 2017; Patel et al., 2017), the epigenetic mechanisms underlying VNS-enhanced learning and memory have not yet been studied. Here, we investigated how VNS facilitates memory formation and consolidation through epigenetic mechanisms of histone modifications (Levenson et al., 2004; Itzhak et al., 2013; Zentner and Henikoff, 2013), DNA methylation (Levenson et al., 2006; Miller et al., 2008; Feng et al., 2010, Meadows et al., 2015; Jones, 2012), and RNA changes in hippocampus, cortex, and blood.

Learning activates waves of altered gene expression in the hippocampus (Levenson et al., 2004; Lubin et al., 2008; Bekinschtein et al., 2007). Early changes in gene expression initiate memory formation and then further changes occur 12–24 h after training to enable memory persistence through the process of systems consolidation (Frankland et al., 2006; Bekinschtein et al., 2007). During systems consolidation, memories are uploaded from the hippocampus to the cortex for persistent storage (Levenson et al., 2004; Miller et al., 2010; Itzhak et al., 2013; Zentner and Henikoff, 2013). Epigenetic mechanisms such as chromatin remodeling, DNA methylation, and RNA–DNA–protein interactions are critical for effective learning and memory (Levenson et al., 2006; Miller et al., 2008, 2010; Day and Sweatt, 2010; Feng et al., 2010; Jones, 2012; Kaas et al., 2013; Meadows et al., 2015; Kennedy and Sweatt, 2016; Kennedy et al., 2016; Savell et al., 2016). Chromatin remodeling affects early memory formation by modulating gene transcription and also controls persistent memory storage by increasing or decreasing access to cortical chromatin (Levenson et al., 2004; Lubin et al., 2008; Day and Sweatt, 2011; Itzhak et al., 2013; Sweatt, 2013; Zentner and Henikoff, 2013; Zovkic et al., 2013). Chromatin remodeling typically involves modifications to histone tails such as phosphorylation or acetylation (both increase access to DNA), methylation, or ubiquitination. DNA methyltransferases (DNMTs) regulate cytosine methylation at cytosine–guanine dinucleotide rich-sequences within DNA (CpG islands), thereby altering local chromatin structure and the transcription of local plasticity genes (Levenson et al., 2006; Miller et al., 2008, 2010; Feng et al., 2010; Jones, 2012; Meadows et al., 2015).

The intertwining of memory and epigenetic mechanisms has inspired efforts to identify an “epigenetic code” or “histone code” that enumerates the relationships between epigenetic modulators and synaptic plasticity, learning, and memory (Turner, 2000; Rosenfeld et al., 2006; Zovkic et al., 2013). Indeed, many relationships between epigenetic effects and the underlying modulation of transcription and translation have been identified. For example, the histone modification H3K4me3 (trimethylation of histone H3 on lysine 4) is known to be associated with increased transcriptional activation (Dai et al., 2018) and the exchange of canonical histone subunit 2A (H2A) with histone variant H2A.X is known to play a role in signaling DNA damage events (Pinto and Flaus, 2010; Sharma et al., 2012) such as double-strand breaks (DSBs), as well as providing a foundation for the assembly of repair machinery such as ligases and cofactors.

In this study, we show that VNS enhances novelty preference (NP) during a multiday object interaction task and investigate whether epigenetic mechanisms underlie the enhancement. Through joint behavioral and transcriptome analysis with specific epigenetic assays for DNA methylation (bisulfite conversion/pyrosequencing) and histone modifications (chromatin immunoprecipitation, ChIP), we identify VNS-induced epigenetic and transcriptome modifications associated with enhanced cognition, reduced stress–response signaling, cortical synaptic plasticity, calcium signaling changes, and increased hippocampal output.

Materials and Methods

Subjects.

All experiments were approved by the Vanderbilt University Animal Research Committee. Thirty-five adult male Sprague Dawley rats (4–5 months old; 350 ± 50 g) were used for this study.

VNS device construction.

VNS cuff electrodes were built based on the designs used in previous studies (Engineer et al., 2011). Briefly, the cuff of the bipolar stimulating electrode is constructed from 4 mm of Micro-Renethane tubing (1.8 mm inner diameter). The electrodes are made of coated multistranded platinum iridium wire /″(0.006”). The region in contact with the nerve is stripped of insulation for 8 mm and attached to the inner surface of the cuff separated by 2 mm. Silk thread is used to enable closing the cuff edges during surgery. A head cap consisting of a four-channel Omnetics plug soldered to gold pins is connected to the VNS cuff wire leads during surgery.

VNS surgical protocol.

Before surgery, rats were given buprenorphine (0.03 mg/kg, i.p.) as preemptive analgesia. Animals were then anesthetized with ketamine/xylazine and placed in a stereotaxic apparatus (Kopf Instruments). The head cap with the four-channel Omnetics plug was attached to the skull with screws and dental cement. VNS rats were then implanted with a bipolar cuff electrode around the left cervical vagus nerve. As in humans, only the left vagus nerve was stimulated because the right vagus nerve innervates the sinoatrial node and can cause cardiac complications. Leads from the electrode were tunneled subcutaneously to the top of the head and affixed to the head cap. Sham rats received an identical surgery including installation of a head cap with plug, exposure of the left cervical vagus nerve (Alvarez-Dieppa et al., 2016), and creation of a subcutaneous tunnel from the nerve site to the head cap as in VNS rats. However, for sham rats, no cuff was implanted.

Calibrating and confirming effects of VNS on heart rate, oxygen saturation, and behavior.

To ensure that cuffs were operating as expected based on previous studies (Alvarez-Dieppa et al., 2016; Borland et al., 2016), we verified that 30 s of VNS at 60 Hz and 0.8 mA transiently decreased the blood oxygen saturation and/or reduced the breathing rate in all VNS implanted rats. However, the standard intermittent 30 Hz VNS protocol used in this study had no measurable effect on either heart rate or oxygen saturation. These results are consistent with our visual observations that intermittent VNS bursts as specified in our protocol cause no noticeable behavioral response or change in heart rate or breathing.

VNS stimulation protocol.

A four-channel connector from the stimulus generator was plugged into each rat's head cap before all VNS and sham stimulation sessions. Cuff impedances were measured before and after implantation to ensure that all cuffs fell within the appropriate range. Stimulation parameters used in the current study were based on previous plasticity studies (Clark et al., 1999; Engineer et al., 2011). Stimulation parameters were identical for all VNS rats in the study. Specifically, charge-balanced biphasic pulses with amplitude 0.8 mA were delivered as a train of 15 100 μs pulses at 30 Hz (500 ms train duration). Stimulation trains were delivered every 17.5 s over a 30 min period. Sham rats were connected to the stimulus generator in the same way as VNS rats except they received no stimulation.

Behavioral testing.

To measure the effects of VNS on cognition stimulated and sham rats (N = 12, n = 7 VNS and n = 5 sham, cohort 3) were tested using NP tasks 1 d after receiving VNS (or sham VNS) during object familiarization. Previous studies in rats have found the NP task, also referred to as the novel object recognition task, to be very useful for studying short-term memory, immediate-term memory, and long-term memory (Broadbent et al., 2010; Gaskin et al., 2010; Haettig et al., 2011; Antunes and Biala, 2012). In our study, we used four objects: two different rope/bead rat toys purchased at a pet store, a jar lid, and a Lego toy car. After an initial habituation session in the open field, rats were allowed to become familiar with the first object on day 1 during administration of 30 min of intermittent VNS bursts (see Fig. 1A). On day 2, the first object was placed in its original location and a second object was placed in the opposite corner of the open field. Rats were tested for 10 min to determine whether they preferred the novel (second) object over the familiar (first) object. As in previous studies, cognitive enhancement was assessed by calculating the difference in the amount of time spent interacting with the novel object (to2) compared with the familiar object (to1), expressed as a fraction of the total time spent interacting with either object during the test as follows: (Haettig et al., 2011) Embedded Image Immediately following the 10 min NP test, rats received 30 min of VNS in the arena, enabling familiarization with the objects from that day's NP task. On days 3 and 4, the “oldest” object was removed, the object introduced on the previous day became the familiar object (o1), and an unfamiliar novel object (o2) was added. As on day 2, rats performed the 10 min NP test and then immediately received 30 min of VNS while in the arena. Note that both the sham and VNS rats had stimulation cables continuously attached to their head caps for the entirety of the 10 min test and 30 min stimulation phases. The use of a commutator ensured that the rats could move freely about the arena.

Behavioral data extraction.

Rats were videotaped while performing the NP task. Start and ending times of stimulation were recorded in a log book. Videotapes were then reviewed by blinded analysts who recorded time stamps and durations when rats interacted with the learned object (o1) and the novel object (o2). Interactions were categorized as proximity, whisking, nudging, pawing, biting, and combinations of interactions. Each rat's novel object preference measures were calculated based on the summed total times of interaction for each of the two objects during the 10 min testing period for each day.

Nucleic acid isolation.

For all experiments, animals were killed by rapid decapitation after completion of day 1 (cohort 1) or day 4 (cohorts 2 and 3) of stimulation. Brains were immediately removed and dissected. Brain regions were sectioned, flash frozen on dry ice, and stored at −80°C. Cortical and hippocampal sections were obtained from 3 mm (A/P) coronal brain sections centered on −3.5 mm (bregma). Trunk blood was collected in PAXgene blood RNA tubes, kept at room temperature for 24 h, and then frozen. Tissue samples were processed for nucleic acid isolation using the AllPrep DNA/RNA/miRNA Universal Kit (Qiagen). Blood was processed with the Invitrogen Ribopure blood kit. Nucleic acid concentrations were determined using a Nanodrop spectrophotometer (Thermo Scientific).

qRT-PCR.

Total RNA was split into aliquots for qRT-PCR and RNA sequencing (RNA-seq). qRT-PCR aliquots were reverse transcribed using the iScript RT-PCR kit (Bio-Rad). qRT-PCR amplifications were performed in triplicate using an iQ5 real-time PCR system (Bio-Rad) at 95°C for 5 min, followed by 40 cycles of 95°C for 15 s and 60°C for 60 s, and then incubation at 72°C for 10 min followed by real-time melt analysis to verify product specificity. Hprt1 was used as an internal control for normalization using the Ct method (Livak and Schmittgen, 2001). Oligonucleotide primer information is available on request.

RNA-seq.

RNA libraries were produced and adaptor ligated; paired-end reads were sequenced on an Illumina HiSeq-2500 following library quality control. Quality control on raw reads was performed with FastQC (version 0.10.1) and adaptor trimming and removal of trimmed reads shorter than 20 bp was performed with Trimmomatic (Bolger et al., 2014). Trimmed reads were mapped to the UCSC Rattus norvegicus rn6 genome (version 0.13.0). Samtools (version 0.1.19–96b5f2294a) were used to sort SAM files and de-duplicate reads. Bowtie2 was used to align reads (Langmead and Salzberg, 2012). The subread featureCounts function was used to obtain counts. EdgeR was used for normalization and calculation of fold changes (Robinson et al., 2010). Gene ontology (GO) analyses were performed using Cytoscape (Shannon et al., 2003) with the BinGO plugin (Maere et al., 2005) and Gene set enrichment analysis (GSEA) (Subramanian et al., 2005).

Pyrosequencing of bisulfite-converted DNA.

Bisulfite conversion of DNA was performed using the Qiagen EpiTect Bisulfite Kit. The NF-κB1 promoter primers were custom designed and then ordered through Integrated DNA Technologies. Amplification was performed using the Pyromark PCR kit. Pyrosequencing was performed using the Qiagen Pyromark Q24 system.

ChIP.

ChIP analysis was performed as follows. First, 200 mg of frozen tissue was homogenized, resuspended in PBS, and cross-linked in 1% formaldehyde for 10 min at room temperature. The tissue pellet was resuspended in nuclei swelling buffer containing protease inhibitor. The separated nuclei were lysed in SDS lysis buffer containing protease inhibitors. The resulting chromatin was sonicated (Diagenode) at 4°C with 10 bursts for 30 s, with a 30 s cooling interval between each burst. The average length of sonicated chromatin was determined by resolving it on a 1.2% agarose gel and was found to be ∼300–800 bp. The sample was then centrifuged at 13,000 rpm for 10 min at 4°C to remove cell debris from the chromatin lysate. One milliliter of sheared chromatin was diluted in 10 ml of ChIP dilution buffer. Ten percent of the diluted lysate was subsequently incubated overnight with 2 μg of primary antibody/magnetic bead solution at 4°C. Normal rabbit IgG antibody was used as the negative control to demonstrate nonspecific binding. Antibodies were considered negative for binding if the resulting value was equal to or less than the IgG value (ratio to input). The supernatant of normal rabbit IgG was saved as the input control for PCR after clean-up. Antibody/protein/DNA complexes were eluted from beads and then supernatants were incubated at 65°C for 4–5 h after the addition of 20 μl of 5 mol/L NaCl and 1 μg of RNase A to reverse the formaldehyde cross-linking and to release the DNA fragments. Samples were treated with proteinase K at 37°C for 1 h to remove any protein and DNA was purified with a QiaPrep miniprep kit (Qiagen). Each real-time PCR used 5% of immunoprecipitated DNA. Histone binding was expressed as the ratio to the input. Standards and the samples were simultaneously amplified in a 20 μl reaction volume with primers designed to amplify genomic sequences at the promoter region of ARC.

Transcript/behavior correlation analysis.

Correlation analysis was performed in MATLAB using Pearson correlation (corrcoef). For rats that performed the behavioral task (cohort 3), all transcripts changed by VNS with p < 0.1 were included in the analysis. For hippocampal tissues, the RNA-seq dataset was used. Overall RNA-seq correlation significance was assessed by calculating the number of transcripts that correlated with a set of randomly drawn behavioral scores (uniform distribution with the same min and max as the true behavioral scores, N = 1000 iterations). Morpheus was used to generate the correlation heat map (https://software.broadinstitute.org/morpheus). For cortex, qRT-PCR data were used to examine behavioral correlation because the cortical RNA-seq was performed on rats not subjected to the behavioral task. RNA-seq data were used to examine genes that correlated with ARC in the cortex.

Experimental design and statistical analysis.

Details of experimental design are presented in the first two Results sections. Animal numbers were calculated based on parameters required to adequately power previous rodent studies. Data analysis was performed in R or MATLAB (version R2017; The MathWorks) and graphed using edgeR, Prism for Windows (version 6.00; GraphPad Software), or MATLAB. Normality was formally tested and verified where appropriate. Statistical significance was designated at p < 0.05 for all analyses. Statistical significance was measured using two-sided unpaired t tests. Adjusted p-values were calculated using ANOVA multiple comparisons. FDRs were calculated using Benjamini and Hochberg false discovery rate correction. For correlations, data were analyzed using Pearson correlation coefficients in MATLAB.

Results

Our study investigated whether epigenetic alterations underlie VNS enhancement of plasticity and cognition. The first set of experiments confirmed that 30 min of VNS during object familiarization can be used to enhance next-day novel object preference in rats. The second set of experiments demonstrated that 4 consecutive days of 30 min VNS induces widespread epigenetic changes in the cortex, hippocampus, and blood, including altered epigenetic modulation of stress–response signaling and cortical plasticity genes.

VNS enhances preference for novel objects

We applied electrical stimulation 30 min per day through implanted cuff electrodes or sham cuffs on the left cervical vagus nerve in Sprague Dawley rats (see Materials and Methods). Stimulation parameters (30 Hz, 0.8 mA, 100 μs biphasic pulses) were similar to the parameters used in previous rat and human VNS studies (Clark et al., 1999; Engineer et al., 2011). Rats were exposed to the first object on day 1 during VNS (Fig. 1A). On days 2–4, the familiar object from the previous day (o1) was placed in its original location and a second object (o2) was placed in the opposite corner of the open field. Rats were tested for 10 min to determine whether they distinguished the novel object (o2) from the familiar object (o1). Cognitive enhancement was assessed by calculating the difference in the amount of time spent interacting with the novel object (to2) compared with the familiar object (to1), expressed as a fraction of the total time spent interacting with either object Embedded Image (Haettig et al., 2011). Following testing, rats again received 30 min of VNS. During the test periods, rats that had received VNS demonstrated significantly increased novel object preference (Δnovel_object_preference = 0.7, p = 0.02, n = 12; Fig. 1B). VNS rats also spent less time interacting with the familiar object before interacting with the novel object, observed in previous studies to be an indicator of enhanced learning (to1stim/to1sham = 0.2, p = 0.0003, n = 12; Fig. 1C) (Broadbent et al., 2010; Gaskin et al., 2010). Compared with VNS rats, sham rats spent more time overall interacting with the objects. This prolonged interaction time has been interpreted as an indication of slower learning in previous studies that allowed multiple familiarization days before novel object introduction (Gaskin et al., 2010; Antunes and Biala, 2012). Additionally, because the sham rats interacted with the novel object for a similar amount of time as the VNS rats (Fig. 1C), it is unlikely that anxiety played a role in the sham rats' lack of preference for the novel object. Finally, it is important to note that, despite an average negative novel object preference measure in sham rats, the overall difference in the amount of time the sham rats spent interacting with the novel objects (to2) versus the familiar objects (to1) during the 10 min test phase was not statistically significant (to2sham/to1sham = 0.7, p = 0.45, n = 5).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

VNS enhances preference for novel objects. A, On day 1, rats were introduced to the first object while receiving 30 min of stimulation. On day 2 and following, the object introduced on the previous day (familiar object) was placed in the same location and a novel object was introduced. Rats were observed for a 10 min test period while interacting with the two objects and then stimulated for 30 min. B, Rats receiving VNS displayed increased novel object preference compared with sham stimulated rats (Δnovel_object_preference = 0.7). C, VNS rats spent more time interacting with the novel objects than the familiar objects (ratio = 3.2). Sham rats spent more total time interacting with the objects than VNS rats. The average time that shams spent interacting with the novel object was not significantly different from the average time spent with the familiar object. D, VNS rats, but not shams, explored the novel object more than the familiar object during all three 10 min test periods (exploration ratio > 0.5). Novel object preference typically subsided during the 30 min stimulation period following the test period. N = 12 rats, n = 5 sham and n = 7 stimulated, *p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.01. Error bars indicate SEM.

As observed in previous studies (Mumby et al., 2002), for each trial (day), novel object preference was generally diminished for both sham and stimulated rats after several minutes of exploration (Fig. 1D). Interestingly, although the cumulative novel object preference was significant over all 3 d (Fig. 1B), the daily exploration ratio, determined by Embedded Image (Mumby et al., 2002) for VNS rats compared with shams, was significantly increased on days 2 and 3 (p = 0.01 and p = 0.05, respectively), but not on day 4 (p = 0.62), perhaps due to increased novelty preference with days of participation for the sham rats (Fig. 1D and see Discussion).

Four 30 min sessions of VNS changed expression of plasticity genes in the cortex, hippocampus, and blood

For our second set of experiments, we extracted brain and blood samples from three cohorts of rats: cohort 1 received a single 30 min session of VNS (N = 15, n = 8 VNS and n = 7 sham), cohort 2 received 4 consecutive days of VNS with no task (N = 8 rats, n = 4 VNS and n = 4 sham), and cohort 3 received 4 consecutive days of VNS during the 30 min object familiarization period (N = 12 rats, n = 7 VNS and n = 5 sham; Fig. 1). Samples were extracted within 30 min of the last session of VNS for each rat. We did not find evidence of changes in plasticity gene expression in cohort 1 (single session of VNS with no consolidation; Fig. 2A). However, in cohorts 2 and 3, qRT-PCR (Fig. 2B–D) and Western blot (data not shown) analysis revealed multiple changes in expression of plasticity-related genes in cortex, hippocampus, and blood. Subsequent RNA-seq revealed widespread transcriptional changes in epigenetic modulators, stress–response signaling, and plasticity genes in both the cortex and hippocampus (see Figs. 3, 5). Specific epigenetic investigation revealed DNA methylation changes and learning-related histone modifications.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Thirty minutes of VNS delivered on 4 consecutive days modulates transcription of a panel of learning- and memory-associated genes. A, Transcript quantities in the hippocampus, cortex, and blood did not change significantly after a single 30 min VNS treatment. B, C, Plasticity-related transcripts were significantly changed by VNS in the cortex and hippocampus after 4 d of stimulation (stim). Transcripts from rats participating in the NP behavioral task (downward pointing triangle) changed in the same direction as transcripts from rats that received stimulation alone (upward pointing triangle). D, In the blood, IEG transcripts (ARC and FOS) from rats that received stimulation alone were increased, whereas transcripts from rats that participated in the NP task were reduced. A, Results from cohort 1 (single day of stim). For B–D, results from cohort 2 are indicated by the dashed line (sham) and the upward pointing triangle (stim), whereas results from cohort 3 are indicated by the black squares (sham) and the downward pointing triangle (stim). Asterisks without pair-bars reflect significance relative to the dashed line (sham without NP test). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, unpaired t test. Error bars indicate SEM.

Initially, we examined a panel of genes known to be involved in learning and memory, including the IEGs ARC and FOS, a neurotrophic factor (BDNF), and epigenetic regulators [histone deacetylase 2 (HDAC2) and H2AFX, the gene that encodes histone 2A variant X (H2A.X)]. Results from this initial panel were used to guide further assays and analysis.

We found no significant changes in any of the panel genes after a single 30 min session of VNS (cohort 1, N = 15, n = 8 VNS and n = 7 sham; Fig. 2A). However, after 4 consecutive days of 30 min stimulation, we found significantly changed transcription (Fig. 2B–D). The differential transcription profiles varied between the three locations sampled. In the hippocampus, we found that transcripts for each gene changed in the same direction in VNS rats (increased or decreased) regardless of whether the NP test was performed (Fig. 2B, cohort 2 without the NP test, N = 8 rats, n = 4 VNS and n = 4 sham, orange upward-pointing arrow; cohort 3 with the NP test, N = 12 rats, n = 7 VNS and n = 5 sham, red downward-pointing arrow). This was also true in the cortex, where the largest transcriptional changes occurred in BDNF (fold change = 2.7, p = 0.05), ARC (fold change = 1.5, p = 0.04), and FOS (fold change = 1.2, p = 0.05) (Fig. 2C).

HDAC2 transcripts were decreased in both hippocampus and blood (Fig. 2B,D). H2AFX (associated with DNA double-strand breaks) transcripts were decreased in the hippocampus and cortex (Fig. 2B,C).

IEGs ARC and FOS transcript quantities in the blood depend on whether rats performed the NP task

Interestingly, in the blood, the direction of the transcriptional changes for the IEGs depended on whether the NP task was performed rather than on whether the rat received stimulation (Fig. 2D and see Discussion). Rats that received VNS without the NP task had increased ARC and FOS, whereas VNS rats that performed the task had reduced ARC (fold change = −0.8, p = 0.0004) and FOS (fold change = −0.8, p = 0.018). Note that this NP task-associated reduction in blood transcripts was also significant for ARC in the sham rats (black square, fold change = −0.6, p = 0.013).

Based on this initial evidence that VNS changes transcription of IEGs and epigenetic regulators, we chose to further probe the VNS-induced transcriptional changes. We analyzed RNA-seq results from cortical and hippocampal tissues (see Figs. 3, 5) extracted from VNS and sham rats that received 30 min of stimulation on 4 consecutive days. Although further analysis of the trunk blood would have been beneficial, we did not pursue blood RNA-seq because initial sample quality checks did not meet the stringent thresholds recommended by our RNA-seq process.

We verified the cell populations used for RNA-seq were balanced between the sham and stimulated rat samples by quantifying cell identity transcripts. The RNA-seq mRNA counts for a standard panel of cell identity transcripts showed no significant differences between the samples from sham and stimulated rats for either the cortex (Fig. 3B) or the hippocampus (Fig. 5C).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

VNS alters cortical transcription. A, RNA-seq, DNA methylation, and ChIP data analysis pipeline. B, Cortical cell identity transcripts revealed evidence of both glial (OLIG1, OLIG2, GFAP) and neuronal (MAP2, SYP) cells. However, no significant differences were found between the sham and stimulated cell identity transcripts. Error bars indicate SEM. C, Differentially transcribed cortical genes in VNS versus sham-stimulated rats (p < 0.01). Genes annotated with text are significantly changed either individually (FDR < 0.25, filled blue circles) or as part of a GO group (adjusted p < 0.05, filled black circles). D, Synaptic plasticity GO categories upregulated by VNS (GSEA biological process categories, p < 0.01).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Transcript classes involved in cortical neuronal plasticity form a network of correlations. A, Transcripts from synaptic receptors and other plasticity genes correlate with epigenetic modifiers, NF-κB1, and TNFRSF11B. B, Epigenetic modifiers increased by VNS correlate with ARC, plasticity, stress signaling, and TNFRSF11B. Decreased SIN3B (a transcriptional corepressor) correlates with increased SETD7, and NMDA and nicotinic receptor subunits. C, Stress signaling is reduced by VNS and correlates with reduced GABAA and AMPA receptor subunits and immune receptors. D, Note the central role of TNFRSF11B in regard to the other three transcript classes. Cortical RNA-seq, n = 8, cohort 2, abs(ρ) > 0.8, p < 0.01.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

VNS alters hippocampal transcription. A, Location of hippocampal sample shown on a coronal rat brain section. B, H3K4me3, an epigenetic marker found in active promoters, was decreased in the hippocampus, suggesting reduced transcription. C, As in the cortex, cell identity transcripts revealed the presence of both glial and neuronal cells. Although the profile differed from that of cortical transcripts (increased GFAP and reduced SYP), no significant differences were found between the sham and stimulated cell identity transcripts. Error bars indicate SEM. D, Differentially transcribed genes in VNS versus sham rats (p < 0.01). Genes annotated with text are significantly changed (FDR < 0.25, filled blue circles) or as part of a GO set (adjusted p < 0.05, filled black circle). E, Differentially transcribed epigenetic modulation genes in VNS versus sham rats (p < 0.01). Genes annotated with text are significantly changed (p < 0.01, FDR < 0.25). F, Representative significantly increased GO categories from GSEA Biological Processes (p < 0.01). G, Principal component analysis of hippocampal epigenetic transcript data enables separation of the sham (black) and VNS (red) rats.

VNS without a task induces widespread changes in cortical transcription

Because VNS caused significant cortical plasticity even in rats that did not perform a behavioral task (Fig. 2C), we examined the overall “preparatory” cortical plasticity changes induced by VNS alone (Fig. 3, cohort 2). In all, 427 cortical genes were differentially transcribed (Fig. 3C, ||log2(fold change)|| (logFC) > 1, p < 0.05, N = 8 rats, n = 4 VNS and n = 4 sham), with a majority indicating increased transcription (55%, 233 of 427). VNS strongly increased transcription of plasticity genes such as CAMK2D (logFC = 2.0 p = 0.04), translation elongation factor modifier DPH1 (logFC = 3.7, p = 1.5 × 10−6), and ovarian tumor suppressor candidate 2 OVCA2 (logFC = 3.0, p = 7.0 × 10−11). Nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subunit β-2 (CHRNB2) was also increased (logFC = 2.8, p = 0.0007), likely due to VNS upregulation of cholinergic signaling. MAP3K6, a MAP kinase important for vascular endothelial growth signaling, was also increased (logFC = 2.8, p = 0.0008). The most decreased cortical transcripts were stress–response signaling transcription factor subunit NF-κB1 (logFC = −5.9, p = 9.6 × 10−6), EVPL (logFC = −4.0, p = 3.5E-13), and cytokine receptor TNFRSF11B, also known as osteoprotegerin (OPG, logFC = −5.0, p = 0.01).

VNS also induced transcriptional changes in many epigenetic modulators important for learning and memory, such as HDAC3 (logFC = 2.2, p = 0.0035) and de novo DNA methyltransferase DNMT3A (logFC = 2.6, p = 0.015). Transcription of KAT8 (MYST1), a histone modifier that acetylates lysine 16 on H4 (H4K16ac, associated with open chromatin), decreased (logFC = −2.5, p = 0.006) and correlated with reduced transcription of RASGRP4 [Pearson correlation coefficient (ρ) = 0.98, p = 2.8E-5] and cytokine receptor TNFRSF11B (ρ = 0.85, p = 0.007). HIST3H2B, typically increased in the presence of DNA DSBs, decreased as well (logFC = −2.6, p = 0.009).

GO analysis revealed increased transcription in several important plasticity categories (Fig. 3D, GSEA Biological Process gene set Version c5.bp.v6.1.symbols.gmt; Subramanian et al., 2005). These GO categories included genes known to be critical for learning and memory such as ARC, MECP2 (methyl CpG binding protein 2), CAMK2D, and CHRNB2. The GO category “regulation of myelination” was also upregulated in part due to increased transcription of HDAC3 and MTOR (logFC = 1.6, p = 0.03), a serine/threonine protein kinase that regulates myelination in conjunction with cell growth, cell survival, protein synthesis, autophagy, and transcription. GO categories specifically related to epigenetic mechanisms were also increased, including “Histone H3 deacetylation.”

Other studies have addressed the cholinergic effects of VNS. Therefore, we chose to focus on genes newly linked to VNS by our transcriptome analysis.

Stress–response signaling, neuronal plasticity, and epigenetic regulation transcripts correlate with one another

To further probe the signaling related to the reduced stress–response transcripts, we examined correlations between NF-κB1 and other genes in the cortical RNA-seq data. Ninety-five genes correlated significantly with NF-κB1 (Pearson correlation, abs(ρ) > 0.8, p < 0.01, see methods). Positively correlated genes included NRAS, IL7R, and AMPA and GABAA receptor subunits (GRIA4, GABRR1).

Because neural plasticity gene ARC was significantly increased in the cortex of rats from both cohort 2 and cohort 3, we also investigated the 124 RNA-seq transcripts that correlated with ARC (abs(ρ) > 0.8, p < 0.01). Positively correlated genes included receptor subunits (GABRG3, GRIN2D) and multiple epigenetic regulators. ARC transcription negatively correlated with transcription of serotonin receptor 5A (HTR5A), and matrix metalloproteinase-17 (MMP17). 5-HT5A negatively influences cAMP (Gi protein-coupled) and has been implicated in psychiatric conditions such as schizophrenia. Increased MMPs are found to associate with disease processes such as arthritis and metastasis due to their degradation of the extracellular matrix, so the decrease provides supporting evidence for a VNS therapeutic effect on arthritis (Bassi et al., 2017).

Transcription of many epigenetic regulators correlated with cortical ARC transcription, including SETD7 (H3K4 histone methyltransferase involved in control of cell proliferation, differentiation, and ER stress), SETDB1 (histone methyltransferase involved in H3K9 methylation and H3K14 acetylation), ANKRA2 (involved in HDAC Class II signaling), DNMT3A, and HDAC3 (class I HDAC). To identify potential interactions between transcripts that correlated with NF-κB1, ARC, and the ARC-correlating epigenetic regulators, we mapped the network of correlations between four transcript classes: plasticity genes, epigenetic regulators, stress–response signaling, and immune receptors (Fig. 4). Epigenetic regulators that correlated with ARC transcription heavily regulated plasticity genes that were significantly changed by VNS (Fig. 4A,B). We also found that TNFRSF11B (OPG) correlated with genes from all of the other classes (Fig. 4D).

VNS induces widespread changes in hippocampal transcription when paired with a novelty preference task

To identify hippocampal changes induced by VNS, we analyzed RNA-seq data from rat samples extracted immediately after stimulation on day 4 of the NP behavioral task (N = 7 highest quality hippocampal RNA samples from cohort 3: n = 4 VNS, n = 3 sham). Hippocampal RNA-seq analysis identified 524 genes with significant transcription changes; 61% were decreased (N = 7 rats, n = 4 VNS and n = 3 sham; Fig. 5). This result was supported by ChIP results indicating reduced levels of hippocampal H3K4me3, a histone methylation mark associated with increased transcription (Fig. 5B). VNS strongly decreased transcription of WNT9B (logFC = −5.2, p = 0.001), known to be important for calcium signaling, cell proliferation and differentiation, PRKCD (protein kinase C delta, logFC = −4.1, p = 0.003), a cell cycle regulator that can play a role in activation of NF-κB pathways, and motilin receptor, MLNR (logFC = −3.4, p = 7.0E-5), important for both brain and gut motility. Increased transcripts included ubiquitin-associated chemokine receptor CXCR4 (logFC = 1.1, p = 0.0005) and CAMK2B (logFC = 1.1, p = 0.003) (Fig. 5D).

GO analysis revealed further diverse effects in the hippocampus (Fig. 5F), including changes in transcripts involved in increased chaperone-mediated protein folding, negative regulation of ER stress, negative regulation of STAT signaling, increased transcription elongation, and reduced transmembrane transport.

VNS changes hippocampal calcium signaling

Many significantly differentially transcribed genes in the hippocampus were related to calcium signaling, including CAMK2B, CAMK2N2, SYT2, and Car8 (Fig. 5D). CAMK2 (Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase 2) is involved in many signaling cascades, including stress response, and is an important mediator of learning and memory (Lisman et al., 2002). CAMK2B encodes the β chain of CAMK2 (one of four CAMK2 subunits), whereas CAMK2N2 inhibits CAMK2. The observed increase in CAMK2B and decrease in CAMK2N2 (logFC = −1.3, p = 0.002) suggests that VNS promotes net CAMK2 activity. CAMK2B correlated with increased BDNF (ρ = 0.76, p = 0.05). SYT2 (synaptotagmin 2) is a synaptic vesicle membrane protein and calcium sensor (logFC = −2.9, p = 0.0001). Finally, Car8 (carbonic anhydrase 8, logFC = −1.2, p = 8.8 × 10−7) inhibits a receptor that promotes intracellular calcium release. Previous studies have shown that histone modifiers can play an important role in calcium regulation through Wnt and other signaling pathways (Takada et al., 2009). In our study, we found multiple three-way correlations among novel object recognition, calcium signaling transcripts, and epigenetic regulation transcripts (Fig. 6).

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

VNS-induced transcription correlates with novel object preference. A, Pearson correlation between 50 hippocampal transcripts that correlate with novel object preference (abs(ρ) > 0.8). Note TNFRSF11 refers to TNFRSF11B. B, Cortex: IEG ARC transcription correlates with novel object preference (R2 = 0.8563, qRT-PCR). C, Hippocampus: Epigenetic regulator HDAC11 transcription is inversely correlated with novel object preference (R2 = 0.8982, RNA-seq). D, NF-κB1 transcription is inversely correlated with novel object preference (R2 = 0.8164, RNA-seq).

VNS alters the hippocampal epigenetic transcriptome

RNA-seq data from hippocampal tissues was separable into VNS and sham-stimulated populations based solely on changes in epigenetic modulator transcription (Fig. 5E,G). The separation was enabled using only the first two principal components from a set composed of histone deacetylases, histone acetyltransferases, histone methyltransferases, histone demethylases, DNA methyltransferases, and ten-eleven translocation (TET) enzyme gene transcripts from the rattus norvegicus rn6 reference genome.

The most reduced hippocampal epigenetic transcript was SETSIP (logFC = −2.7, p = 5.4E-06), a gene similar to SET that encodes a histone H4 acetylation (HAT) inhibitor. The encoded protein is part of a complex typically localized to the endoplasmic reticulum but also found in the nucleus. Decreased SETSIP correlated with reductions in TNFSF13B (ρ = 0.89, p = 0.007) and TNFSF14 (ρ = 0.92, p = 0.004). Other reduced transcripts included those for genes encoding HDAC11 (histone deacetylase enriched in output cells of the hippocampus CA1; Broide et al., 2007), histone H2A.Y2 (H2AFY2, a learning and memory-associated histone variant; Barrero et al., 2013), SETD4 (lysine methyltransferase), and KDM4D (histone lysine (H3K9) demethylase). HDAC11 and H2AFY2 correlated with many hippocampal transcriptional changes (ρ > 0.8, p < 0.01; Fig. 6). SETD4 correlated with NF-κB1 (ρ = 0.98, p = 0.0002), IL17RC (ρ = 0.98, p = 9.7E-5), RASGRP3 (ρ = 0.96, p = 0.0006), and GABRB2 (ρ = 0.89, p = 0.007). Reduced KDM4D correlated with reduced SYT2 (ρ = 0.91, p = 0.004), KCNH5 (ρ = 0.91, p = 0.005), Car2 (ρ = 0.96, p = 0.0008), CAMK2N2 (ρ = 0.93, p = 0.002), and GABRB2 (ρ = 0.94, p = 0.002). Clearly, VNS alters the epigenetic transcription landscape associated with stress–response signaling and neuronal plasticity. Because our interest was in epigenetic changes related to cognition, we sought to identify significantly changed genes involved in the observed enhanced novel object preference.

VNS-induced transcriptome alterations correlate with behavioral performance

To identify genes likely to be directly involved in learning and memory, we examined correlations between behavior and transcription in the most significantly changed transcripts. Pearson correlation analysis revealed that transcription levels in several genes were correlated with one another and with performance on the NP behavioral task (Fig. 6). In particular, cortical ARC transcription was strongly positively correlated with NP test performance (ρ = 0.94, p = 0.01, qRT-PCR; Fig. 6B).

In the hippocampus, transcription of 714 genes correlated with NP test performance (abs(ρ) > 0.8, p < 0.01, RNA-seq). The most correlated gene was SORCS1 (ρ = −0.98, p = 7.9E-5), which encodes a protein important for vacuolar protein sorting. SORCS1 is genetically associated with diabetes and Alzheimer's amyloid-β metabolism (Lane et al., 2010). Interestingly, NF-κB1 (ρ = −0.90, p = 0.005) and TNFRSF11B (OPG, ρ = −0.95, p = 0.001), found to be significantly changed in the cortical RNA-seq, were included in the top hippocampal genes that correlated with behavioral performance. Many of the 714 genes correlated significantly with one another (Fig. 6A). Hierarchical clustering of the Pearson correlations revealed two major clusters, one that contained HDAC11, and another that contained H2AFY2 and NF-κB1 (H2AFY2 | NF-κB1 ρ = 0.92, p = 0.0028).

Genes that clustered with HDAC11 included CAMK2N2 (ρ = −0.89, p = 0.007), STAT5A (ρ = −0.94, p = 0.001), folh1 (ρ = −0.97, p = 0.0004), and parasympathetically linked muscarinic acetylcholine receptor, CHRM2 (ρ = −0.91, p = 0.005). The top upregulated gene was plectin (PLEC, a protein that bins intermediate filaments, actin microfilaments and microtubules and forms links between other cellular components, observed in astrocytic end feet abutting on blood vessels). Genes that clustered with NF-κB1 and H2AFY2 included DNA damage checkpoint regulator, RAD54B (ρ = −0.88, p = 0.009), Car8 (ρ = −0.91, p = 0.004), and TNFRSF11B (OPG). These correlations suggest relationships among transcription of epigenetic regulators, neuronal plasticity, stress–response signaling, and other powerful signaling cascades that may be responsible for the enhanced cognition observed in the NP task.

The many correlated neuronal plasticity, stress–response signaling, and epigenetic regulation transcriptional changes in the VNS rats led us to investigate how these three categories of genes might interact. In particular, we were interested in the epigenetic regulation of stress–response signaling and neuronal plasticity genes important for VNS-enhanced learning and memory.

VNS downregulates stress–response signaling without significantly changing inflammatory cytokine transcripts

As previously discussed, cortical NF-κB1 transcripts were significantly reduced by VNS (see Figs. 3, 8A). NF-κB1 was also decreased in the blood (qRT-PCR, logFC = −1.6, p = 0.03) and inversely correlated with novel object preference in the hippocampus (Fig. 6D). NF-κB is a transcription factor known to be a master regulator of dynamic response mechanisms such as immune activation, cell proliferation, and neural plasticity. Our finding of reduced NF-κB1 in VNS rats is congruent with previous studies showing decreased inflammatory signaling during VNS treatment (Borovikova et al., 2000; Tracey, 2002). Interestingly, our analysis found decreased NF-κB1 without corresponding decreases in transcription of proinflammatory cytokines (Fig. 7, RNA-seq). Anti-inflammatory cytokine transcripts including IL10 and TGF-β also failed to show significant increases. We hypothesized that VNS may downregulate stress response signaling through other known NF-κB modulators such as hormones, histone modifiers, or DNA DSBs.

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Proinflammatory cytokine transcripts are not significantly changed after 4 d of VNS. A, Cortical IL12A and IL18 were reduced, but did not reach significance (p = 0.32, and p = 0.06, respectively, RNA-seq data, n = 8). B, Hippocampal proinflammatory transcripts were not significantly changed (IL6 p = 0.18, RNA-seq data, n = 7). Error bars indicate SEM.

The correlation between NF-κB1 and TNFRSF11B (OPG, reduced in cortex and hippocampus) suggested a potential involvement of thyroid and/or parathyroid hormone-mediated effects in reduced stress–response signaling. Although we did not find significant changes in thyroid or parathyroid hormone expression, we found increased hippocampal thyroid hormone receptor α (THRA, logFC = 1.1, p = 0.01) and reduced cortical thyroid hormone receptor interactor 10 (TRIP10, logFC = −2.5, p = 0.05). TRIP10 was also decreased in the hippocampus and correlated with NF-κB1 (ρ = 0.97, p = 0.0002), along with epigenetic modifiers H2AFY2, SETD4, and KDMD4. Interestingly, examination of a set of thyroid hormone-responsive genes, FOS (Fig. 2B,C), STAT5A (Fig. 5D, Fig. 6), lysine demethylase HR, and TXNIP (Chatonnet et al., 2015; Kyono et al., 2016), revealed increased transcription of the genes in the cortex and decreased transcription in the hippocampus (see Discussion).

Given the known link between DNA DSBs and stress–response signaling (Volcic et al., 2012; Madabhushi et al., 2015) and our evidence suggesting involvement of VNS epigenetic modulation in both, we examined epigenetic regulation of NF-κB1.

VNS decreases DNA methylation at the NF-κB1 promoter in both the cortex and hippocampus

Because CpG methylation at gene promoters is known to regulate transcription, we used bisulfite conversion followed by pyrosequencing to examine DNA methylation in the NF-κB1 promoter region in VNS and sham rats (N = 8, n = 4 sham and n = 4 VNS, cortex: cohort 2, hippocampus: cohort 3; Fig. 8). Although reduced gene body methylation can coincide with decreased transcription (Yang et al., 2014), gene promoter methylation is typically found to be increased with decreased transcription. Surprisingly, our results showed that NF-κB1 promoter CpG methylation was reduced in rats that received VNS compared with those that received sham stimulation (23% overall decrease in cortex, p = 0.01, unpaired t test; Fig. 8B). It is important to note that promoter methylation is usually a function of the history of cellular experience. Therefore, one interpretation of the reduced NF-κB1 methylation is that NF-κB1 may have been increased at points during the learning, stimulation, and consolidation cycle. However, although day 4 NF-κB1 transcript levels differed between the VNS cortical samples (logFC = −5.9, p = 9.6 × 10−6, cohort 2, no behavioral task) and the VNS hippocampal samples (logFC = −0.4, p = 0.08, cohort 3, NP task; Fig. 8A), the NF-κB1 DNA methylation did not reflect this difference. Instead, we found that NF-κB1 CpG methylation did not depend on whether rats performed the task (Fig. 8B), suggesting that the reduced methylation could instead be an independent effect of VNS.

Figure 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 8.

VNS modulates DNA methylation of NF-κB. A, NF-κB1 transcripts were reduced in rats receiving VNS. B, Cytosine methylation within the NF-κB1 promoter was reduced in both cortex and hippocampus. C, Bar chart showing the stim versus sham ratio of occurrence for each nucleotide examined (red bars indicate methylated cytosines). Cortical NF-κB1 methylation in this segment was reduced by 23% overall (N = 8; n = 4 sham, n = 4 stimulated, p = 0.01, unpaired t test). D, Segment of CpG-rich region examined (orange bar) in relation to exon 1 (blue) and full extent of CpG-rich region (green). *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001, box-and-whisker plots: central horizontal line indicates median, box indicates quartiles above and below median, whiskers indicate full range of data.

DNA DSBs in the ARC promoter are reduced by VNS

Continuing our investigation of the link between stress–response signaling and DNA DSBs, we turned our attention to the IEG ARC. Because cortical ARC transcription was increased by VNS and correlated strongly with behavioral performance (Fig. 6B), we examined the ARC promoter for evidence of DNA DSBs, which are known to be facilitators of IEG transcription (Suberbielle et al., 2013; Madabhushi et al., 2015). Histone 2A.X phosphorylation at serine 139 (γH2A.X) is known to colocalize with DSBs and provide a highly sensitive marker for examining DNA breaks and subsequent repair (Sharma et al., 2012). We examined γH2A.X both globally (through immunostaining) and locally at the promoter of ARC DNA using ChIP followed by qRT-PCR with ARC primers (cortex, cohort 2, n = 8). Although we did not find a significant global change in overall γH2A.X using immunostaining (Fig. 9C,D), ChIP revealed reduced γH2A.X at the ARC promoter in the cortex of VNS rats (Fig. 9B). Therefore, VNS reduced DSBs in the ARC promoter, possibly through NF-κB regulatory mechanisms (Krzyzaniak et al., 2011). VNS-enhanced repair of DSBs was further suggested by increased transcription of genes in DNA DSB repair pathways (ASCC1, XRCC1) in the cortex (logFC = 3.3, p = 0.017, logFC = 3.9, p = 0.001) of VNS rats. This is consistent with our findings regarding reduced cortical NF-κB1. However, previous research indicates that synaptic plasticity changes required for learning are accompanied by increased DSBs at IEG promoters (Madabhushi et al., 2015). Therefore, our finding of reduced promoter DSBs in the plasticity gene ARC after VNS suggests that non-monotonic changes may occur during the 4 d NP task. This is consistent with previous findings of activity-dependent oscillations in NF-κB and epigenetic modulators (Nelson et al., 2004; Clayton et al., 2006).

Figure 9.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 9.

VNS reduces DSBs at the ARC promoter. A, DSBs activate stress–response signaling (NF-kB), facilitate transcription of IEGs, and are colocated with γH2A.X. B, VNS reduces γH2A.X (DSB indicator) at the promoter of IEG ARC. C, Cells stained for γH2A.X show puncta (red) in the nucleus (blue). D, Quantification of global γH2A.X staining showed no significant difference between sham and VNS rats (p = 0.2). Error bars indicate SEM. N = 8 (n = 4 sham, n = 4 stimulated). **p < 0.01.

Discussion

Novel object preference tests are popular for measuring enhanced learning and memory (Bevins and Besheer, 2006; Broadbent et al., 2010; Gaskin et al., 2010; Antunes and Biala, 2012). We designed a 4 d protocol to allow analysis of longer-term and short-term VNS impacts on plasticity and behavior. Objects were placed in alternating locations to reduce the likelihood of confounding effects due to innate preference for a particular object or location. Consistent with the expected result of VNS-enhanced cognition, VNS rats preferred the novel object over the familiar object during all 3 d of testing, whereas sham rats, on average, spent a similar amount of time with each object.

Sham rats in our study preferred the familiar object more than most controls reported in the literature. To gain insight into this phenomenon, we reanalyzed our data to look for confounding factors. Because sham and VNS rats interacted with the novel objects for a similar amount of time, we did not find cause to implicate anxiety in the shams' lack of preference for the novel object. However, we noted that several of the objects in our study were compound/complex and thus possibly more engaging (or confusing) to the rats than the simple objects typically used. Indeed, in trials in which our simplest object (lid) was the familiar object (Fig. 1D, day 4), we found that both sham and stimulated rats preferred the novel object during the test period. Although this suggests that intrinsic object characteristics, and possibly other unknowns, may have played a role in the daily relative object preferences, the results unequivocally show that, compared with shams, VNS rats demonstrated significantly greater overall preference for novel objects.

Stress–response signaling

Reduced NF-κB1 in the cortex and blood, along with inversely correlated hippocampal NF-κB1 and cognitive performance, revealed a widespread VNS effect on stress–response signaling. VNS-induced reductions in NF-κB associated with cytokine changes such as reduced TNF have been reported previously (Borovikova et al., 2000; Tracey, 2002; Andersson and Tracey, 2012). However, the minimal change in cytokine transcription in the hippocampus and cortex in our rats suggests that NF-κB effects in these structures could be related to alternative stress–response modulators such as hormones, histone modifiers, or DNA DSBs.

Several immune receptors correlated with reduced NF-κB1. Importantly, TNFRSF11B (OPG) transcripts were reduced in the cortex and hippocampus and correlated with NF-κB1 transcription and increased novel object recognition. OPG, a cytokine receptor and negative regulator of bone resorption, is a thyroid and parathyroid hormone responsive gene. This evidence, combined with the VNS cuff proximity to the thyroid, points to potential involvement of these hormones in VNS-induced alterations to stress–response signaling, plasticity, and novel object preference.

For VNS rats that did not perform the behavioral task (cohort 2), reductions in NF-κB1 transcription accumulated, leading to a complete loss of cortical NF-κB1 transcripts. However, during the behavioral task (cohort 3), stress–response signaling likely increased as a result of exposure to novelty and then decreased by the next session of VNS, resulting in the modest cumulative NF-κB1 decrease observed in the hippocampus. Despite the large difference between transcriptional effects, VNS reduced NF-κB1 methylation similarly in the two groups. This suggests that DNA methylation did not drive NF-κB1 transcription and that reduced DNA methylation may be an independent effect of VNS. Potential mechanisms for this general demethylating effect include blood/CSF changes (suggested by reduced carbonic anhydrase transcription) and reduced TRIP10 (associated with increased methylated cytosine).

Increased BDNF

Not all of the VNS-induced transcriptional changes were linked to OPG effects. For example, BDNF transcripts and protein were increased in both the cortex and the hippocampus, but did not correlate with any of the stress–response signaling transcripts, including OPG. BDNF has been linked to successful memory consolidation (Bekinschtein et al., 2007; Lubin et al., 2008) and positive effects on treatment-resistant depression (Nemeroff et al., 2006; Aaronson et al., 2017). Consistent with previous studies reporting a link among electrical stimulation, BDNF transcription, and increased CAMK2 activity (Yan et al., 2016), we found correlations between transcription of hippocampal BDNF and learning and memory genes CAMK2A and CAMK2B. CAMK2B has been shown to facilitate ARC targeting to inactive synapses to prevent undesired enhancement of weak synapses in potentiated neurons (by reducing surface AMPA receptors) (Okuno et al., 2016).

Diverse plasticity effects

Although we observed increased BDNF and reduced OPG in both the hippocampus and cortex, overall transcription effects differed dramatically between the two structures. Investigation of a set of thyroid-responsive genes (STAT5A, FOS, HR, TXNIP; Chatonnet et al., 2015) revealed that these transcripts were reduced in the hippocampus but increased in the cortex. The cortical increase is consistent with VNS-increased thyroid hormone suggested by reduced OPG. The hippocampal decrease can be explained by the smaller reduction in OPG combined with an increase in THRA transcription because unliganded thyroid receptor represses transcription (Grøntved et al., 2015). Therefore, the dichotomy of hippocampal and cortical responses to VNS may have a common root, in part, in thyroid/OPG changes. Importantly, the different histone modifiers that correlated with reduced OPG in the cortex (KAT8 and SETD7) and hippocampus (HDAC11 and H2AFY2) likely contributed to the downstream transcriptional diversity.

ARC has been previously identified as a key synaptic plasticity gene with the capability to transfer mRNA across neurons (Bramham et al., 2008; Lonergan et al., 2010; Shepherd and Bear, 2011; Pastuzyn et al., 2018; Wall et al., 2018). In our study, ARC did not correlate with OPG, suggesting the observed thyroid/OPG effects were not primary drivers in its upregulation. Additionally, although increased BDNF has previously been shown to increase ARC (Wibrand et al., 2006), we did not find a correlation between ARC and BDNF.

The observed cortical ARC increase in VNS rats regardless of whether the cognitive task was performed suggests that VNS may induce preparatory cortical plasticity. Importantly, in rats that performed the task, cortical ARC transcription correlated with novel object preference. ARC also correlated with many epigenetic modifiers (HDAC3, DNMT3A, SETDB1, SETD7, and ANKRA2). HDAC3 is known to directly deacetylate NF-κB subunit Rel6 (Chen et al., 2001), causing NF-κB subunits to bind more strongly to IκB in the cytoplasm. Therefore, HDAC3 upregulation may play a role in downregulated NF-κB1.

The increased transcriptional repression suggested by the ARC-correlating epigenetic modifiers initially appeared incongruent with the observed increased cortical transcription. However, correlation mapping suggested that repression may have been directed toward transcriptional coregulators (SIN3B), cell growth inhibitors (RTN4R, neurite outgrowth inhibitor), and metastasis enablers (TCF12, MMP17). Therefore, the positive correlation of these transcriptional repressors with ARC may be due to their role in reducing competing metabolic programs.

Additional transcriptional evidence suggests VNS may facilitate synaptic localization of GABAA receptors (increased GABRG3), upregulate nicotinic cholinergic effects (CHRNB2), and increase the NMDA/AMPA ratio (increased NMDA-R subunits, GRIN2A and GRIN2D, decreased AMPA-R subunit GRIA4). Previous studies have shown these increases are implicated in enhanced cognition (Lane et al., 2010; Das et al., 2013; Kerti-Szigeti et al., 2014; Karasawa et al., 2008).

Blood IEG transcripts increased in VNS rats that did not perform the task, coinciding with cortical increases. However, rats that participated in the task displayed a decrease in IEG transcripts compared with rats that did not perform the task. This may indicate that, in the absence of learning-induced plasticity, VNS produces excess IEG transcripts (ARC and FOS in our study) that are cleared via the blood. Conversely, when IEG transcripts such as ARC are used to facilitate synaptic plasticity, fewer may be released into the blood. If this is the case, then blood ARC transcripts may be a useful measure of enhanced plasticity and learning.

Hippocampal neuronal changes that facilitate learning were also observed, such as reduced expression of potassium channels that may enable faster neuronal recovery after spiking and changes in calcium-related signaling that may alter the quantities and types of neurotransmitters released (Takács et al., 2018). Changes in blood calcium implied by downregulated OPG likely contributed to the large number of significantly changed hippocampal calcium signaling transcripts. Individual hippocampal calcium signaling transcripts correlated with histone modifiers HDAC11, H2AFY2, SETD4, and KDM4D, again suggesting involvement of epigenetic transcriptome changes in VNS-induced plasticity.

Significantly reduced hippocampal histone modifiers (HDAC11 and H2AFY2) correlated with reduced OPG and improved cognitive performance. HDAC11 is enriched in CA1, so reducing its deacetylase activity may facilitate consolidation by increasing hippocampal output to the subiculum (Broide et al., 2007), potentially through increased expression of PLEC (Fuchs et al., 2009).

Stress–response signaling, plasticity, and epigenetic mechanisms work together to enhance learning and memory

Our finding of reduced γH2A.X in rats that received VNS without a behavioral task suggests that VNS may enhance plasticity by rapid repair of DSBs, thus facilitating quick replenishment of IEG transcripts. Enhanced DSB repair is also supported by increased cortical transcription of repair genes ASCC1 and XRCC1 and by HDAC3, which, when complexed with silencing mediator for retinoid and thyroid hormone receptor, SMRT, facilitates recovery from DNA DSBs (Yu et al., 2006). This result exemplifies the convergence of the processes we found most important for VNS-enhanced cognition: neuronal plasticity (ARC), stress–response signaling (NF-κB1), and histone modifications (γH2A.X).

Cycles of learning and memory initiation in the hippocampus followed by cortical consolidation (and reconsolidation) are known to be important for effective cognition (Bekinschtein et al., 2007; Lubin et al., 2008; Tay et al., 2018). A cyclical interpretation of stress–response signaling and epigenetic regulation is also supported by previous reports of oscillations in NF-κB and related epigenetic modulators (Nelson et al., 2004; Vanden Berghe et al., 2006; Clayton et al., 2006). We constructed a proposed working model of VNS-induced cortical plasticity (Fig. 10) to illustrate how increased stress from IEG DSBs followed by VNS-induced DSB repair can cause cumulative oscillatory effects that are consistent with the observed data. This evolving landscape highlights one aspect of the difficulty inherent in optimizing VNS treatments: selecting appropriate stimulus parameters and timelines in a system that is continually adapting.

Figure 10.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 10.

Working model of cortical plasticity. A, Novelty presentation (N) induces stress–response signaling due to DSBs in IEG promoters. B, VNS (S) reduces NF-κB through multiple mechanisms, including increased DSB repair along with ACh and HDAC3 effects. C, Reduced NF-κB contributes to subsequent consolidation (C) by downregulating metabolic demand from competing activities such as proliferation and immune signaling, as well as facilitating a large pool of plasticity-enabling transcripts. Phases were repeated each day for 4 d, leading to cumulative effects in many genes, including those shown in D–F. D, NF-κB is increased by exposure to novelty, but is reduced by intermittent 30 Hz VNS. E, DSBs (indicated by γH2A.X) are also increased by novelty, but rapidly repaired subsequent to VNS. F, Rapid DSB repair enhances cells' ability to process new cycles of transcription necessary for synaptic plasticity.

Therapeutic relevance

Our findings support previously identified VNS therapeutic effects for conditions such as arthritis (Tracey, 2002; Andersson and Tracey, 2012; Bassi et al., 2017), treatment-resistant depression (Nemeroff et al., 2006; Aaronson et al., 2017), epilepsy, and stroke (Hwang et al., 2013). Downregulated cellular proliferation and modulation of cancer-relevant genes (Hymowitz and Wertz, 2010) suggest a potential new role for VNS in cancer treatment. The evidence also supports our study's main hypothesis that epigenetic alterations underlie VNS enhancement of cognition and plasticity. More specifically, we find that VNS induces preparatory cortical plasticity and upregulated hippocampal output that could be beneficial for treating Alzheimer's disease (Lane et al., 2010; Suberbielle et al., 2013), as well as other forms of cognitive impairment (Karvat and Kimchi, 2014; Kennedy et al., 2016). Importantly, we identified genes associated with enhanced plasticity and improved cognition that are promising independent therapeutic targets. Several targets and pathways are implicated by our discovery that OPG plays an important role in VNS-induced cognitive enhancements. Additionally, new cognitive epigenetic targets such as HDAC11, KAT8, and SIN3B are of interest because they may modulate gene expression important for enhanced learning and memory more specifically than currently prescribed broad-acting histone deacetylase inhibitors.

In conclusion, our study provides key insights into VNS-induced epigenetic alterations in pathways important for enhanced cognition. Our data reveal that VNS modulates epigenetic involvement in stress–response signaling, neuronal plasticity, and transcriptional modifications that facilitate consolidation. Our findings also identify genetic and epigenetic players in a noncanonical stress–response signaling/plasticity pathway that may be broadly important for understanding learning and memory mechanisms.

Footnotes

  • This work was sponsored by the Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency (DARPA) Biological Technologies Office (BTO) Targeted Neuroplasticity Training (TNT) program under the auspices of Dr. Tristan McClure-Begley (SPAWAR Cooperative Agreement N66001–17-2–4019). We thank Timothy Broderick for intellectual support throughout the design and implementation of these experiments; Lindsey Noble, Kris Loerwald, Kim Rahebi, Seth Hays, and Mike Kilgard at UT Dallas for training THS in vagus nerve cuff construction and surgical implantation; Ben Litt for assistance with cuff building; and Katie Sanders for brain tissue (qRT-PCR) and bioinformatic data extraction. The views, opinions, and/or findings contained in this article are those of the authors and should not be interpreted as representing the official views or policies of the Department of Defense or the U.S. Government.

  • All sequencing data that support the findings of this study have been deposited in the National Center for Biotechnology Information Gene Expression Omnibus (GEO) and are accessible through GEO. All other relevant data are available from the corresponding author on request.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Teresa H. Sanders at teresa.hinkle.sanders{at}gmail.com

References

  1. ↵
    1. Aaronson ST,
    2. Sears P,
    3. Ruvuna F,
    4. Bunker M,
    5. Conway CR,
    6. Dougherty DD,
    7. Reimherr FW,
    8. Schwartz TL,
    9. Zajecka JM
    (2017) A 5-year observational study of patients with treatment-resistant depression treated with vagus nerve stimulation or treatment as usual: comparison of response, remission, and suicidality. Am J Psychiatry 174:640–648. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.16010034 pmid:28359201
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Alvarez-Dieppa AC,
    2. Griffin K,
    3. Cavalier S,
    4. McIntyre CK
    (2016) Vagus nerve stimulation enhances extinction of conditioned fear in rats and modulates Arc protein, CaMKII, and GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors in the basolateral amygdala. Neural Plast 2016:4273280. doi:10.1155/2016/4273280 pmid:27957346
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Andersson U,
    2. Tracey KJ
    (2012) Reflex principles of immunological homeostasis. Annu Rev Immunol 30:313–335. doi:10.1146/annurev-immunol-020711-075015 pmid:22224768
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Antunes M,
    2. Biala G
    (2012) The novel object recognition memory: neurobiology, test procedure, and its modifications. Cogn Process 13:93–110. doi:10.1007/s10339-011-0430-z pmid:22160349
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Barrero MJ,
    2. Sese B,
    3. Kuebler B,
    4. Bilic J,
    5. Boue S,
    6. Martí M,
    7. Izpisua Belmonte JC
    (2013) Macrohistone variants preserve cell identity by preventing the gain of H3K4me2 during reprogramming to pluripotency. Cell Rep 3:1005–1011. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2013.02.029 pmid:23545500
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Bassi GS,
    2. Dias DPM,
    3. Franchin M,
    4. Talbot J,
    5. Reis DG,
    6. Menezes GB,
    7. Castania JA,
    8. Garcia-Cairasco N,
    9. Resstel LBM,
    10. Salgado HC,
    11. Cunha FQ,
    12. Cunha TM,
    13. Ulloa L,
    14. Kanashiro A
    (2017) Modulation of experimental arthritis by vagal sensory and central brain stimulation. Brain Behav Immun 64:330–343. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2017.04.003 pmid:28392428
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Bekinschtein P,
    2. Cammarota M,
    3. Igaz LM,
    4. Bevilaqua LR,
    5. Izquierdo I,
    6. Medina JH
    (2007) Persistence of long-term memory storage requires a late protein synthesis-and BDNF-dependent phase in the hippocampus. Neuron 53:261–277. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.025 pmid:17224407
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Berridge CW,
    2. Waterhouse BD
    (2003) The locus coeruleus-noradrenergic system: modulation of behavioral state and state-dependent cognitive processes. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 42:33–84. doi:10.1016/S0165-0173(03)00143-7 pmid:12668290
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Bevins RA,
    2. Besheer J
    (2006) Object recognition in rats and mice: a one-trial non-matching-to-sample learning task to study 'recognition memory'. Nat Protoc 1:1306–1311. doi:10.1038/nprot.2006.205 pmid:17406415
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Bolger AM,
    2. Lohse M,
    3. Usadel B
    (2014) Trimmomatic: a flexible trimmer for Illumina sequence data. Bioinformatics 30:2114–2120. doi:10.1093/bioinformatics/btu170 pmid:24695404
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Borland MS,
    2. Vrana WA,
    3. Moreno NA,
    4. Fogarty EA,
    5. Buell EP,
    6. Sharma P,
    7. Engineer CT,
    8. Kilgard MP
    (2016) Cortical map plasticity as a function of vagus nerve stimulation intensity. Brain Stimul 9:117–123. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2015.08.018 pmid:26460200
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Borovikova LV,
    2. Ivanova S,
    3. Zhang M,
    4. Yang H,
    5. Botchkina GI,
    6. Watkins LR,
    7. Wang H,
    8. Abumrad N,
    9. Eaton JW,
    10. Tracey KJ
    (2000) Vagus nerve stimulation attenuates the systemic inflammatory response to endotoxin. Nature 405:458–462. doi:10.1038/35013070 pmid:10839541
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Bramham CR,
    2. Worley PF,
    3. Moore MJ,
    4. Guzowski JF
    (2008) The immediate early gene arc/arg3. 1: regulation, mechanisms, and function. J Neurosci 28:11760–11767. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3864-08.2008 pmid:19005037
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Broadbent NJ,
    2. Gaskin S,
    3. Squire LR,
    4. Clark RE
    (2010) Object recognition memory and the rodent hippocampus. Learn Mem 17:5–11. doi:10.1101/lm.1650110 pmid:20028732
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Broide RS,
    2. Redwine JM,
    3. Aftahi N,
    4. Young W,
    5. Bloom FE,
    6. Winrow CJ
    (2007) Distribution of histone deacetylases 1–11 in the rat brain. J Mol Neurosci 31:47–58. doi:10.1007/BF02686117 pmid:17416969
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Cao B,
    2. Wang J,
    3. Shahed M,
    4. Jelfs B,
    5. Chan RH,
    6. Li Y
    (2016) Vagus nerve stimulation alters phase synchrony of the anterior cingulate cortex and facilitates decision making in rats. Sci Rep 6:35135. doi:10.1038/srep35135 pmid:27731403
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Chatonnet F,
    2. Flamant F,
    3. Morte B
    (2015) A temporary compendium of thyroid hormone target genes in brain. Biochim Biophys Acta 1849:122–129. doi:10.1016/j.bbagrm.2014.05.023 pmid:24882357
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Chen Lf,
    2. Fischle W,
    3. Verdin E,
    4. Greene WC
    (2001) Duration of nuclear NF-kappaB action regulated by reversible acetylation. Science 293:1653–1657. doi:10.1126/science.1062374 pmid:11533489
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Clark KB,
    2. Naritoku DK,
    3. Smith DC,
    4. Browning RA,
    5. Jensen RA
    (1999) Enhanced recognition memory following vagus nerve stimulation in human subjects. Nat Neurosci 2:94–98. doi:10.1038/4600 pmid:10195186
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Clayton AL,
    2. Hazzalin CA,
    3. Mahadevan LC
    (2006) Enhanced histone acetylation and transcription: a dynamic perspective. Mol Cell 23:289–296. doi:10.1016/j.molcel.2006.06.017 pmid:16885019
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Dai Z,
    2. Mentch SJ,
    3. Gao X,
    4. Nichenametla SN,
    5. Locasale JW
    (2018) Methionine metabolism influences genomic architecture and gene expression through H3K4me3 peak width. Nat Commun 9:1955. doi:10.1038/s41467-018-04426-y pmid:29769529
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Das I,
    2. Park JM,
    3. Shin JH,
    4. Jeon SK,
    5. Lorenzi H,
    6. Linden DJ,
    7. Worley PF,
    8. Reeves RH
    (2013) Hedgehog agonist therapy corrects structural and cognitive deficits in a Down syndrome mouse model. Sci Transl Med 5:201ra120–201ra120. doi:10.1126/scitranslmed.3005983 pmid:24005160
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    1. Day JJ,
    2. Sweatt JD
    (2010) DNA methylation and memory formation. Nat Neurosci 13:1319–1323. doi:10.1038/nn.2666 pmid:20975755
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Day JJ,
    2. Sweatt JD
    (2011) Epigenetic mechanisms in cognition. Neuron 70:813–829. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.019 pmid:21658577
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Engineer ND,
    2. Riley JR,
    3. Seale JD,
    4. Vrana WA,
    5. Shetake JA,
    6. Sudanagunta SP,
    7. Borland MS,
    8. Kilgard MP
    (2011) Reversing pathological neural activity using targeted plasticity. Nature 470:101–104. doi:10.1038/nature09656 pmid:21228773
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Feng J,
    2. Zhou Y,
    3. Campbell SL,
    4. Le T,
    5. Li E,
    6. Sweatt JD,
    7. Silva AJ,
    8. Fan G
    (2010) Dnmt1 and Dnmt3a maintain DNA methylation and regulate synaptic function in adult forebrain neurons. Nat Neurosci 13:423–430. doi:10.1038/nn.2514 pmid:20228804
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Frankland PW,
    2. Ding HK,
    3. Takahashi E,
    4. Suzuki A,
    5. Kida S,
    6. Silva AJ
    (2006) Stability of recent and remote contextual fear memory. Learn Mem 13:451–457. doi:10.1101/lm.183406 pmid:16882861
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    1. Fuchs P,
    2. Zörer M,
    3. Reipert S,
    4. Rezniczek GA,
    5. Propst F,
    6. Walko G,
    7. Fischer I,
    8. Bauer J,
    9. Leschnik MW,
    10. Lüscher B,
    11. Thalhammer JG,
    12. Lassmann H,
    13. Wiche G
    (2009) Targeted inactivation of a developmentally regulated neural plectin isoform leads to reduced motor nerve conduction velocity. J Biol Chem 284:26502–26509. doi:10.1074/jbc.M109.018150 pmid:19625254
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Gaskin S,
    2. Tardif M,
    3. Cole E,
    4. Piterkin P,
    5. Kayello L,
    6. Mumby DG
    (2010) Object familiarization and novel-object preference in rats. Behav Processes 83:61–71. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2009.10.003 pmid:19874876
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Grøntved L,
    2. Waterfall JJ,
    3. Kim DW,
    4. Baek S,
    5. Sung MH,
    6. Zhao L,
    7. Park JW,
    8. Nielsen R,
    9. Walker RL,
    10. Zhu YJ,
    11. Meltzer PS,
    12. Hager GL,
    13. Cheng SY
    (2015) Transcriptional activation by the thyroid hormone receptor through ligand-dependent receptor recruitment and chromatin remodeling. Nat Commun 6:7048. doi:10.1038/ncomms8048 pmid:25916672
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Haettig J,
    2. Stefanko DP,
    3. Multani ML,
    4. Figueroa DX,
    5. McQuown SC,
    6. Wood MA
    (2011) HDAC inhibition modulates hippocampus-dependent long-term memory for object location in a CBP-dependent manner. Learn Mem 18:71–79. doi:10.1101/lm.1986911 pmid:21224411
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Hays SA,
    2. Khodaparast N,
    3. Hulsey DR,
    4. Ruiz A,
    5. Sloan AM,
    6. Rennaker RL 2nd.,
    7. Kilgard MP
    (2014) Vagus nerve stimulation during rehabilitative training improves functional recovery after intracerebral hemorrhage. Stroke 45:3097–3100. doi:10.1161/STROKEAHA.114.006654 pmid:25147331
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    1. Hulsey DR,
    2. Riley JR,
    3. Loerwald KW,
    4. Rennaker RL 2nd.,
    5. Kilgard MP,
    6. Hays SA
    (2017) Parametric characterization of neural activity in the locus coeruleus in response to vagus nerve stimulation. Exp Neurol 289:21–30. doi:10.1016/j.expneurol.2016.12.005 pmid:27988257
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Hwang JY,
    2. Aromolaran KA,
    3. Zukin RS
    (2013) Epigenetic mechanisms in stroke and epilepsy. Neuropsychopharmacology 38:167–182. doi:10.1038/npp.2012.134 pmid:22892394
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Hymowitz SG,
    2. Wertz IE
    (2010) A20: from ubiquitin editing to tumour suppression. Nat Rev Cancer 10:332–341. doi:10.1038/nrc2775 pmid:20383180
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Itzhak Y,
    2. Liddie S,
    3. Anderson KL
    (2013) Sodium butyrate-induced histone acetylation strengthens the expression of cocaine-associated contextual memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem 102:34–42. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2013.03.007 pmid:23567105
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Jones PA
    (2012) Functions of DNA methylation: islands, start sites, gene bodies and beyond. Nat Rev Genet 13:484–492. doi:10.1038/nrg3230 pmid:22641018
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Karasawa J,
    2. Hashimoto K,
    3. Chaki S
    (2008) d-Serine and a glycine transporter inhibitor improve MK-801-induced cognitive deficits in a novel object recognition test in rats. Behav Brain Res 186:78–83. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2007.07.033 pmid:17854919
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Kaas GA,
    2. Zhong C,
    3. Eason DE,
    4. Ross DL,
    5. Vachhani RV,
    6. Ming GL,
    7. King JR,
    8. Song H,
    9. Sweatt JD
    (2013) TET1 controls CNS 5-methylcytosine hydroxylation, active DNA demethylation, gene transcription, and memory formation. Neuron 79:1086–1093. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.032 pmid:24050399
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Karvat G,
    2. Kimchi T
    (2014) Acetylcholine elevation relieves cognitive rigidity and social deficiency in a mouse model of autism. Neuropsychopharmacology 39:831–840. doi:10.1038/npp.2013.274 pmid:24096295
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Kennedy AJ,
    2. Sweatt JD
    (2016) Drugging the methylome: DNA methylation and memory. Crit Rev Biochem Mol Biol 51:185–194. doi:10.3109/10409238.2016.1150958 pmid:26915423
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Kennedy AJ,
    2. Rahn EJ,
    3. Paulukaitis BS,
    4. Savell KE,
    5. Kordasiewicz HB,
    6. Wang J,
    7. Lewis JW,
    8. Posey J,
    9. Strange SK,
    10. Guzman-Karlsson MC,
    11. Phillips SE,
    12. Decker K,
    13. Motley ST,
    14. Swayze EE,
    15. Ecker DJ,
    16. Michael TP,
    17. Day JJ,
    18. Sweatt JD
    (2016) Tcf4 regulates synaptic plasticity, DNA methylation, and memory function. Cell Rep 16:2666–2685. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2016.08.004 pmid:27568567
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Kerti-Szigeti K,
    2. Nusser Z,
    3. Eyre MD
    (2014) Synaptic GABAA receptor clustering without the γ2 subunit. J Neurosci 34:10219–10233. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1721-14.2014 pmid:25080584
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    1. Krzyzaniak MJ,
    2. Peterson CY,
    3. Cheadle G,
    4. Loomis W,
    5. Wolf P,
    6. Kennedy V,
    7. Putnam JG,
    8. Bansal V,
    9. Eliceiri B,
    10. Baird A,
    11. Coimbra R
    (2011) Efferent vagus nerve stimulation attenuates acute lung injury following burn: the importance of the gut-lung axis. Surgery 150:379–389. doi:10.1016/j.surg.2011.06.008 pmid:21783215
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Kyono Y,
    2. Subramani A,
    3. Ramadoss P,
    4. Hollenberg AN,
    5. Bonnett RM,
    6. Denver RJ
    (2016) Liganded thyroid hormone receptors transactivate the DNA methyltransferase 3a gene in mouse neuronal cells. Endocrinology 157:3647–3657. doi:10.1210/en.2015-1529 pmid:27387481
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Lane RF,
    2. Raines SM,
    3. Steele JW,
    4. Ehrlich ME,
    5. Lah JA,
    6. Small SA,
    7. Tanzi RE,
    8. Attie AD,
    9. Gandy S
    (2010) Diabetes-associated SorCS1 regulates Alzheimer's amyloid-β metabolism: evidence for involvement of SorL1 and the retromer complex. J Neurosci 30:13110–13115. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3872-10.2010 pmid:20881129
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  47. ↵
    1. Langmead B,
    2. Salzberg SL
    (2012) Fast gapped-read alignment with bowtie 2. Nat Methods 9:357–359. doi:10.1038/nmeth.1923 pmid:22388286
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Levenson JM,
    2. O'Riordan KJ,
    3. Brown KD,
    4. Trinh MA,
    5. Molfese DL,
    6. Sweatt JD
    (2004) Regulation of histone acetylation during memory formation in the hippocampus. J Biol Chem 279:40545–40559. doi:10.1074/jbc.M402229200 pmid:15273246
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  49. ↵
    1. Levenson JM,
    2. Roth TL,
    3. Lubin FD,
    4. Miller CA,
    5. Huang IC,
    6. Desai P,
    7. Malone LM,
    8. Sweatt JD
    (2006) Evidence that DNA (cytosine-5) methyltransferase regulates synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus. J Biol Chem 281:15763–15773. doi:10.1074/jbc.M511767200 pmid:16606618
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  50. ↵
    1. Lisman J,
    2. Schulman H,
    3. Cline H
    (2002) The molecular basis of CaMKII function in synaptic and behavioural memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:175–190. doi:10.1038/nrn753 pmid:11994750
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Livak KJ,
    2. Schmittgen TD
    (2001) Analysis of relative gene expression data using real-time quantitative PCR and the 2- ΔΔCT method. Methods 25:402–408. doi:10.1006/meth.2001.1262 pmid:11846609
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Lonergan ME,
    2. Gafford GM,
    3. Jarome TJ,
    4. Helmstetter FJ
    (2010) Time-dependent expression of Arc and zif268 after acquisition of fear conditioning. Neural Plast 2010:139891. pmid:20592749
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Lubin FD,
    2. Roth TL,
    3. Sweatt JD
    (2008) Epigenetic regulation of BDNF gene transcription in the consolidation of fear memory. J Neurosci 28:10576–10586. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1786-08.2008 pmid:18923034
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  54. ↵
    1. Madabhushi R,
    2. Gao F,
    3. Pfenning AR,
    4. Pan L,
    5. Yamakawa S,
    6. Seo J,
    7. Rueda R,
    8. Phan TX,
    9. Yamakawa H,
    10. Pao PC,
    11. Stott RT,
    12. Gjoneska E,
    13. Nott A,
    14. Cho S,
    15. Kellis M,
    16. Tsai LH
    (2015) Activity-induced DNA breaks govern the expression of neuronal early-response genes. Cell 161:1592–1605. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2015.05.032 pmid:26052046
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Maere S,
    2. Heymans K,
    3. Kuiper M
    (2005) BiNGO: a Cytoscape plugin to assess overrepresentation of gene ontology categories in biological networks. Bioinformatics 21:3448–3449. doi:10.1093/bioinformatics/bti551 pmid:15972284
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Meadows JP,
    2. Guzman-Karlsson MC,
    3. Phillips S,
    4. Holleman C,
    5. Posey JL,
    6. Day JJ,
    7. Hablitz JJ,
    8. Sweatt JD
    (2015) DNA methylation regulates neuronal glutamatergic synaptic scaling. Sci Signal 8:ra61. doi:10.1126/scisignal.aab0715 pmid:26106219
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    1. Miller CA,
    2. Campbell SL,
    3. Sweatt JD
    (2008) DNA methylation and histone acetylation work in concert to regulate memory formation and synaptic plasticity. Neurobiol Learn Mem 89:599–603. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2007.07.016 pmid:17881251
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Miller CA,
    2. Gavin CF,
    3. White JA,
    4. Parrish RR,
    5. Honasoge A,
    6. Yancey CR,
    7. Rivera IM,
    8. Rubio MD,
    9. Rumbaugh G,
    10. Sweatt JD
    (2010) Cortical DNA methylation maintains remote memory. Nat Neurosci 13:664–666. doi:10.1038/nn.2560 pmid:20495557
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Mumby DG,
    2. Gaskin S,
    3. Glenn MJ,
    4. Schramek TE,
    5. Lehmann H
    (2002) Hippocampal damage and exploratory preferences in rats: memory for objects, places, and contexts. Learn Mem 9:49–57. doi:10.1101/lm.41302 pmid:11992015
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  60. ↵
    1. Nelson DE,
    2. Ihekwaba AE,
    3. Elliott M,
    4. Johnson JR,
    5. Gibney CA,
    6. Foreman BE,
    7. Nelson G,
    8. See V,
    9. Horton CA,
    10. Spiller DG,
    11. Edwards SW,
    12. McDowell HP,
    13. Unitt JF,
    14. Sullivan E,
    15. Grimley R,
    16. Benson N,
    17. Broomhead D,
    18. Kell DB,
    19. White MR
    (2004) Oscillations in NF-kappaB signaling control the dynamics of gene expression. Science 306:704–708. doi:10.1126/science.1099962 pmid:15499023
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  61. ↵
    1. Nemeroff CB,
    2. Mayberg HS,
    3. Krahl SE,
    4. McNamara J,
    5. Frazer A,
    6. Henry TR,
    7. George MS,
    8. Charney DS,
    9. Brannan SK
    (2006) VNS therapy in treatment-resistant depression: clinical evidence and putative neurobiological mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 31:1345–1355. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301082 pmid:16641939
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Okuno H,
    2. Araki A,
    3. Minatohara K
    (2016) Inverse Synaptic Tagging by Arc. In Novel Mechanisms of Memory (Giese KP, Radwanska k, eds) pp. 99–117. Springer, Cham:Springer.
  63. ↵
    1. Pastuzyn ED,
    2. Day CE,
    3. Kearns RB,
    4. Kyrke-Smith M,
    5. Taibi AV,
    6. McCormick J,
    7. Yoder N,
    8. Belnap DM,
    9. Erlendsson S,
    10. Morado DR,
    11. Briggs JAG,
    12. Feschotte C,
    13. Shepherd JD
    (2018) The neuronal gene arc encodes a repurposed retrotransposon gag protein that mediates intercellular RNA transfer. Cell 172:275–288.e18.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  64. ↵
    1. Patel YA,
    2. Saxena T,
    3. Bellamkonda RV,
    4. Butera RJ
    (2017) Kilohertz frequency nerve block enhances anti-inflammatory effects of vagus nerve stimulation. Sci Rep 7:39810. doi:10.1038/srep39810 pmid:28054557
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Pinto DM,
    2. Flaus A
    (2010) Structure and function of histone H2AX. Subcell Biochem 50:55–78. doi:10.1007/978-90-481-3471-7_4 pmid:20012577
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Robinson MD,
    2. McCarthy DJ,
    3. Smyth GK
    (2010) edgeR: a bioconductor package for differential expression analysis of digital gene expression data. Bioinformatics 26:139–140. doi:10.1093/bioinformatics/btp616 pmid:19910308
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Roosevelt RW,
    2. Smith DC,
    3. Clough RW,
    4. Jensen RA,
    5. Browning RA
    (2006) Increased extracellular concentrations of norepinephrine in cortex and hippocampus following vagus nerve stimulation in the rat. Brain Res 1119:124–132. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.048 pmid:16962076
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Rosas-Ballina M,
    2. Olofsson PS,
    3. Ochani M,
    4. Valdés-Ferrer SI,
    5. Levine YA,
    6. Reardon C,
    7. Tusche MW,
    8. Pavlov VA,
    9. Andersson U,
    10. Chavan S,
    11. Mak TW,
    12. Tracey KJ
    (2011) Acetylcholine-synthesizing T cells relay neural signals in a vagus nerve circuit. Science 334:98–101. doi:10.1126/science.1209985 pmid:21921156
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  69. ↵
    1. Rosenfeld MG,
    2. Lunyak VV,
    3. Glass CK
    (2006) Sensors and signals: a coactivator/corepressor/epigenetic code for integrating signal-dependent programs of transcriptional response. Genes Dev 20:1405–1428. doi:10.1101/gad.1424806 pmid:16751179
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  70. ↵
    1. Savell KE,
    2. Gallus NV,
    3. Simon RC,
    4. Brown JA,
    5. Revanna JS,
    6. Osborn MK,
    7. Song EY,
    8. O'Malley JJ,
    9. Stackhouse CT,
    10. Norvil A,
    11. Gowher H,
    12. Sweatt JD,
    13. Day JJ
    (2016) Extra-coding RNAs regulate neuronal DNA methylation dynamics. Nat Commun 7:12091. doi:10.1038/ncomms12091 pmid:27384705
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Shannon P,
    2. Markiel A,
    3. Ozier O,
    4. Baliga NS,
    5. Wang JT,
    6. Ramage D,
    7. Amin N,
    8. Schwikowski B,
    9. Ideker T
    (2003) Cytoscape: a software environment for integrated models of biomolecular interaction networks. Genome Res 13:2498–2504. doi:10.1101/gr.1239303 pmid:14597658
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  72. ↵
    1. Sharma A,
    2. Singh K,
    3. Almasan A
    (2012) Histone H2AX phosphorylation: a marker for DNA damage. Methods Mol Biol 920:613–626. doi:10.1007/978-1-61779-998-3_40 pmid:22941631
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Shepherd JD,
    2. Bear MF
    (2011) New views of arc, a master regulator of synaptic plasticity. Nat Neurosci 14:279–284. doi:10.1038/nn.2708 pmid:21278731
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Suberbielle E,
    2. Sanchez PE,
    3. Kravitz AV,
    4. Wang X,
    5. Ho K,
    6. Eilertson K,
    7. Devidze N,
    8. Kreitzer AC,
    9. Mucke L
    (2013) Physiologic brain activity causes DNA double-strand breaks in neurons, with exacerbation by amyloid-β. Nat Neurosci 16:613–621. doi:10.1038/nn.3356 pmid:23525040
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Subramanian A,
    2. Tamayo P,
    3. Mootha VK,
    4. Mukherjee S,
    5. Ebert BL,
    6. Gillette MA,
    7. Paulovich A,
    8. Pomeroy SL,
    9. Golub TR,
    10. Lander ES,
    11. Mesirov JP
    (2005) Gene set enrichment analysis: a knowledge-based approach for interpreting genome-wide expression profiles. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:15545–15550. doi:10.1073/pnas.0506580102 pmid:16199517
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  76. ↵
    1. Sun L,
    2. Peräkylä J,
    3. Holm K,
    4. Haapasalo J,
    5. Lehtimäki K,
    6. Ogawa KH,
    7. Peltola J,
    8. Hartikainen KM
    (2017) Vagus nerve stimulation improves working memory performance. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 39:954–964. doi:10.1080/13803395.2017.1285869 pmid:28492363
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Sweatt JD
    (2013) The emerging field of neuroepigenetics. Neuron 80:624–632. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.023 pmid:24183015
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. Takács VT,
    2. Cserép C,
    3. Schlingloff D,
    4. Pósfai B,
    5. Szőnyi A,
    6. Sos KE,
    7. Környei Z,
    8. Dénes Á,
    9. Gulyás AI,
    10. Freund TF,
    11. Nyiri G
    (2018) Co-transmission of acetylcholine and GABA regulates hippocampal states. Nat Commun 9:2848. doi:10.1038/s41467-018-05136-1 pmid:30030438
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Takada I,
    2. Kouzmenko AP,
    3. Kato S
    (2009) Wnt and PPARγ signaling in osteoblastogenesis and adipogenesis. Nat Rev Rheumatol 5:442–447. doi:10.1038/nrrheum.2009.137 pmid:19581903
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Tay KR,
    2. Flavell CR,
    3. Cassini L,
    4. Wimber M,
    5. Lee JL
    (2018) Post-retrieval re-learning strengthens hippocampal memories via destabilization and reconsolidation. J Neurosci 2618–18.
  81. ↵
    1. Tracey KJ
    (2002) The inflammatory reflex. Nature 420:853–859. doi:10.1038/nature01321 pmid:12490958
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    1. Turner BM
    (2000) Histone acetylation and an epigenetic code. Bioessays 22:836–845. doi:10.1002/1521-1878(200009)22:9%3C836::AID-BIES9%3E3.0.CO;2-X pmid:10944586
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  83. ↵
    1. Vanden Berghe WV,
    2. Ndlovu MN,
    3. Hoya-Arias R,
    4. Dijsselbloem N,
    5. Gerlo S,
    6. Haegeman G
    (2006) Keeping up NF-κB appearances: epigenetic control of immunity or inflammation-triggered epigenetics. Biochem Pharmacol 72:1114–1131. doi:10.1016/j.bcp.2006.07.012 pmid:16934762
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  84. ↵
    1. Volcic M,
    2. Karl S,
    3. Baumann B,
    4. Salles D,
    5. Daniel P,
    6. Fulda S,
    7. Wiesmüller L
    (2012) NF-κB regulates DNA double-strand break repair in conjunction with BRCA1-CtIP complexes. Nucleic Acids Res 40:181–195. doi:10.1093/nar/gkr687 pmid:21908405
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  85. ↵
    1. Wall MJ,
    2. Collins DR,
    3. Chery SL,
    4. Allen ZD,
    5. Pastuzyn ED,
    6. George AJ,
    7. Nikolova VD,
    8. Moy SS,
    9. Philpot BD,
    10. Shepherd JD,
    11. Müller J,
    12. Ehlers MD,
    13. Mabb AM,
    14. Corrêa SAL
    (2018) The temporal dynamics of arc expression regulate cognitive flexibility. Neuron 98:1124–1132.e7 doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2018.05.012 pmid:29861284
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  86. ↵
    1. Wibrand K,
    2. Messaoudi E,
    3. Håvik B,
    4. Steenslid V,
    5. Løvlie R,
    6. Steen VM,
    7. Bramham CR
    (2006) Identification of genes co-upregulated with arc during BDNF-induced long-term potentiation in adult rat dentate gyrus in vivo. Eur J Neurosci 23:1501–1511. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04687.x pmid:16553613
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  87. ↵
    1. Yan X,
    2. Liu J,
    3. Ye Z,
    4. Huang J,
    5. He F,
    6. Xiao W,
    7. Hu X,
    8. Luo Z
    (2016) CaMKII-mediated CREB phosphorylation is involved in Ca2+ induced BDNF mRNA transcription and neurite outgrowth promoted by electrical stimulation. PLoS One 11:e0162784. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0162784 pmid:27611779
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  88. ↵
    1. Yang X,
    2. Han H,
    3. De Carvalho DD,
    4. Lay FD,
    5. Jones PA,
    6. Liang G
    (2014) Gene body methylation can alter gene expression and is a therapeutic target in cancer. Cancer Cell 26:577–590. doi:10.1016/j.ccr.2014.07.028 pmid:25263941
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  89. ↵
    1. Yu J,
    2. Palmer C,
    3. Alenghat T,
    4. Li Y,
    5. Kao G,
    6. Lazar MA
    (2006) The corepressor silencing mediator for retinoid and thyroid hormone receptor facilitates cellular recovery from DNA double-strand breaks. Cancer Res 66:9316–9322. doi:10.1158/0008-5472.CAN-06-1902 pmid:16982777
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  90. ↵
    1. Zentner GE,
    2. Henikoff S
    (2013) Regulation of nucleosome dynamics by histone modifications. Nat Struct Mol Biol 20:259–266. doi:10.1038/nsmb.2470 pmid:23463310
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  91. ↵
    1. Zovkic IB,
    2. Guzman-Karlsson MC,
    3. Sweatt JD
    (2013) Epigenetic regulation of memory formation and maintenance. Learn Mem 20:61–74. doi:10.1101/lm.026575.112 pmid:23322554
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 39 (18)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 39, Issue 18
1 May 2019
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Cognition-Enhancing Vagus Nerve Stimulation Alters the Epigenetic Landscape
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Cognition-Enhancing Vagus Nerve Stimulation Alters the Epigenetic Landscape
Teresa H. Sanders, Joseph Weiss, Luke Hogewood, Lan Chen, Casey Paton, Rebekah L. McMahan, J. David Sweatt
Journal of Neuroscience 1 May 2019, 39 (18) 3454-3469; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2407-18.2019

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Cognition-Enhancing Vagus Nerve Stimulation Alters the Epigenetic Landscape
Teresa H. Sanders, Joseph Weiss, Luke Hogewood, Lan Chen, Casey Paton, Rebekah L. McMahan, J. David Sweatt
Journal of Neuroscience 1 May 2019, 39 (18) 3454-3469; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2407-18.2019
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • chromatin
  • DNA double-strand breaks
  • epigenetic
  • learning and memory
  • plasticity
  • vagus nerve stimulation

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • The Representational Organization of Static and Dynamic Visual Features in the Human Cortex
  • EEG Correlates of Active Removal from Working Memory
  • Diverse Firing Profiles of Crhbp-positive Neurons in the Dorsal Pons Suggestive of Their Pleiotropic Roles in REM Sleep Regulation in Mice
Show more Research Articles

Systems/Circuits

  • The Representational Organization of Static and Dynamic Visual Features in the Human Cortex
  • EEG Correlates of Active Removal from Working Memory
  • Diverse Firing Profiles of Crhbp-positive Neurons in the Dorsal Pons Suggestive of Their Pleiotropic Roles in REM Sleep Regulation in Mice
Show more Systems/Circuits
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.