Abstract
Two types of mammalian direction-selective ganglion cells (DSGCs), ON and ONOFF, operate over different speed ranges. The directional axes of the ON-DSGCs are thought to align with the axes of the vestibular system and provide sensitivity at rotational velocities that are too slow to activate the semicircular canals. ONOFF-DSGCs respond to faster image velocities. Using natural images that simulate the natural visual inputs to freely moving animals, we show that simulated visual saccades suppress responses in ON-DSGCs but not ONOFF-DSGCs recorded in retinas of domestic rabbits of either gender. Analysis of the synaptic inputs shows that this saccadic suppression results from glycinergic inputs that are specific to ON-DSGCs and are absent in ONOFF-DSGCs. When this glycinergic input is blocked, both cell types respond similarly to visual saccades and display essentially identical speed tuning. The results demonstrate that glycinergic circuits within the retina can produce saccadic suppression of retinal ganglion cell activity. The cell-type-specific targeting of the glycinergic circuits further supports the proposed physiological roles of ON-DSGCs in retinal-image stabilization and of ONOFF-DSGCs in detecting local object motion and signaling optical flow.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT In the mammalian retina, ON direction-selective ganglion cells (DSGCs) respond preferentially to slow image motion, whereas ONOFF-DSGCs respond better to rapid motion. The mechanisms producing this different speed tuning remain unclear. Here we show that simulated visual saccades suppress ON-DSGCs, but not ONOFF-DSGCs. This selective saccadic suppression is because of the selective targeting of glycinergic inhibitory synaptic inputs to ON-DSGCs. The different saccadic suppression in the two cell types points to different physiological roles, consistent with their projections to distinct areas within the brain. ON-DSGCs may be critical for providing the visual feedback signals that contribute to stabilizing the image on the retina, whereas ONOFF-DSGCs may be important for detecting the onset of saccades or for signaling optical flow.
Introduction
Image motion on the retina can occur when an animal moves (global-motion), or when something in the environment moves (local or object-motion). The self-motion that produces global-image motion on the retina typically elicits powerful feedback systems that drive muscles in the neck and eyes that counter the effects of the motion and stabilize the image on the retina (Collewijn, 1977; Simpson, 1984). Motion of objects in the environment are also salient, but produce very different behavioral responses, often directing attention to a location (Lettvin et al., 1959), or eliciting avoidance (Münch et al., 2009; Wallace et al., 2013; Yilmaz and Meister, 2013). Such sensitivity to motion could arise at the earliest stages within the visual stream, within the so-called direction selective ganglion cells (DSGCs) in the retina.
DSGCs respond maximally to movement in a narrow range of “preferred” directions, and little if at all to motion in the opposite, “null” directions (Barlow and Hill, 1963; Barlow et al., 1964; Vaney et al., 2012). Mammalian retinas contain two types of DSGCs (Barlow et al., 1964): ONOFF-DSGCs respond to both positive and negative contrasts, whereas ON-DSGCs respond only to positive-contrast. Direction selectivity in both types is mediated largely by directional GABAergic inhibition (Caldwell et al., 1978; Kittila and Massey, 1997; Taylor et al., 2000), arising from specialized inhibitory interneurons called starburst amacrine cells (SBACs; Euler et al., 2002; Fried et al., 2002). The directional signals, arising from individual dendrites of the SBACs, are preserved by selective, spatially asymmetric connections between the SBACs and the DSGCs (Lee and Zhou, 2006; Briggman et al., 2011; Wei et al., 2011).
Although the synaptic mechanisms generating directional responses are similar for both the ON-DSGC and ONOFF-DSGC circuits (Yonehara et al., 2011), the two types have different speed tuning and distinct central projections (Huberman et al., 2009; Kay et al., 2011), consistent with different functional roles. ON-DSGCs have larger receptive fields and are tuned to lower speeds than the ONOFF-DSGCs (Oyster et al., 1972; Sivyer et al., 2010). The strong projection of ON-DSGCs to the non-image-forming accessory optic system (Soodak and Simpson, 1988), which underlies the visual drive of the optokinetic reflex (Simpson, 1984), suggests an important role for ON-DSGCs in image stabilization as part of the vestibular ocular reflex. The high sensitivity of ON-DSGCs to slow image velocities will complement the loss of vestibular drive at low acceleration that results from the inertia of the endolymph in the semicircular canals (Angelaki and Cullen, 2008). Indeed, selective lesion of DS circuits produces profound deficits in eye-stabilization (Yoshida et al., 2001; Amthor et al., 2002). ONOFF-DSGCs, on the other hand, respond to higher velocities (Oyster and Barlow, 1967; Wyatt and Daw, 1975; Sivyer et al., 2010), and their axons project primarily to the superior colliculus and the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN; Semm, 1978). The LGN relays the directional information to the superficial layers of V1 (Cruz-Martín et al., 2014), but the role of ONOFF-DSGCs in image-forming pathways remains unclear. Some studies suggest these cells are selective for small moving objects within the visual field (Chiao and Masland, 2003; Olveczky et al., 2003). A recent analysis has proposed that both types of DSGCs may be important for signaling global motion or optical flow, produced by rotational head motion or translational body motion (Sabbah et al., 2017).
Here we show that the difference in speed tuning between the ON-DSGCs and the ONOFF-DSGCs arises by virtue of differences in the inhibitory and excitatory inputs. In particular, a glycinergic input, that is specific to ON-DSGCs, suppresses signals produced by sudden contrast changes and visual saccades. The results are consistent with complementary physiological roles for the two types of DSGCs.
Materials and Methods
Ethical approval and tissue preparation.
Experiments involving animals were done in accordance with the National Institutes of Health guidelines, and all procedures were approved by the Oregon Health and Science University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Pigmented rabbits, 5 weeks or older of either sex were dark adapted overnight and anesthetized with ketamine (50 mg/kg) and xylazine (10 mg/kg) before being killed with an overdose of sodium pentobarbital. Under dim red illumination, the eyes were quickly enucleated, hemisected, and placed in Ames medium equilibrated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2, pH 7.4, at room temperature. Subsequent to enucleation, all procedures were performed under infrared illumination. The retina and attached pigment epithelium were dissected free from the sclera and placed in a recording chamber under a microscope and perfused with Ames medium maintained at 36°C. Ganglion cell somas were visualized though a 60× water-immersion objective and differential interference contrast optics. DSGCs were selectively targeted by their characteristic somatodendritic morphology (Sivyer et al., 2010; Sivyer and Williams, 2013).
Experimental design and statistical analysis.
Following the establishment of an extracellular recording, the 60× objective was replaced with a 10× water-immersion objective and DSGCs were identified by their responses to a moving bar projected on the receptive field. Whole-cell current-clamp or voltage-clamp recordings were made with the following internal solutions (in mm): 128 K-gluconate, 6 KCl, 10 Na-HEPES, 5 phosphocreatine-Na2, 1 EGTA, 2 Mg-ATP, 1 Na-GTP, and 3 QX-314. For voltage-clamp experiments the K was replaced with Cs. The pH was adjusted to 7.3 using the appropriate hydroxide. Synaptic conductances were estimated from current–voltage relations sampled at potentials between −95 and 0 mV in 15 mV increments. The visual stimulus was repeated for each potential. Excitatory and inhibitory contributions to the total light-evoked synaptic input were estimated by fitting the current–voltage relations as described previously (Borg-Graham, 2001; Taylor and Vaney, 2002; van Wyk et al., 2006; Venkataramani and Taylor, 2010). Drugs were diluted in Ames medium to a final concentration of 1 μm strychnine, 10 μm 6-Imino-3-(4-methoxyphenyl)-1(6H)-pyridazinebutanoic acid hydrobromide (SR 95531), 50 μm 1,2,5,6-tetrahydropyridin-4-yl)methylphosphinic acid (TPMPA). Because of its high affinity the time required to washout strychnine exceeded the attainable recording duration for whole-cell recordings. However, after allowing at least 60 min for washout, recovery of glycinergic IPSPs can be observed in retinal preparations, indicating that the drug effect is reversible.
Visual stimuli were produced using custom software based on the PsychoPy routines (Peirce, 2007; RRID:SCR_006571). The stimuli, generated on a Texas Instruments digital light projector (DLP; Lightcrafter 4500), were projected onto the photoreceptor layer through a 10× water-immersion objective (0.3 NA, Olympus). Only monochromatic light from the green LED was used, with a peak emission wavelength of 520 nm. The DLP intensity was linearized using a calibrated lookup table. DLP intensity was attenuated using neutral density filters to produce an adapting background photon density of ∼3.4 × 105 photons/μm2, which was maintained throughout the experiments. The image of the DLP array was magnified to fill the field-of-view of the 10× objective, producing a projected image of ∼2.6 mm diameter on the retina. Each pixel on the DLP corresponded to 3.2 μm on the retina. To convert distance on the retina to degrees of visual angle we used the conversion factor of 166 μm/degree measured for the rabbit eye (Hughes and Vaney, 1981).
Two stimuli were used to probe responses to global motion; a checkerboard stimulus, and a natural scene, “imk00955.iml”, obtained from the Van Harteren image database (van Hateren and van der Schaaf, 1998). The checkerboard comprised 100 μm squares with randomly assigned intensities of +80% or −80% relative to the mean luminance level (Fig. 1A). The natural scene was scaled to span the dynamic range of the stimulus monitor. The mean luminance was 52% lower than the interstimulus background level and the mean contrast (SD) was 28.7%. Saccadic stimuli were simulated by generating image trajectories based on the measurements of eye and head movements that were made in freely moving rabbits (Collewijn, 1977). For saccades the image moved at 90°/s, which corresponds to a displacement of 250 μm/stimulus frame. Thus, the image jumped between frames by less than one-quarter of the receptive field diameter of the ganglion cell. Saccade distance ranged from 4.5° to 19.7° with an average of 13.0° ± 6.8°. The average saccade duration was 160 ± 68 ms.
Statistical comparisons between cell types were made using unpaired two-tailed Student's t tests assuming normally distributed data. Comparison of pharmacological effects used paired t tests. Results were considered significant for p < 0.05. Custom routines in Igor Pro (Wavemetrics; RRID:SCR_000325) were used for data analysis. Unless otherwise specified, error bars or error-shading represent ±SEM.
Results
Saccade-like motion suppresses ON-type DSGCs selectively
To compare ON-DSGC and ONOFF-DSGC responses during rapid and slow eye movements we simulated saccades and slow image drift at velocities similar to those encountered during normal behavior (Collewijn, 1977). During simulation of global image motion on the retina that occurs during eye movement, random checkerboard images covered the full stimulus area of ∼2.6 mm diameter. To assess the effect of saccades on the ongoing neural activity, a maintained level of activity in the DSGCs was generated by continuously drifting the stimulus image at 0.6°/s in the cell's preferred direction during intervals between saccades. Linear saccades were generated in either the preferred or null direction (Fig. 1, stimulus trace). When the stimulus first appeared, it produced a burst of spikes that subsided to near zero within ∼1 s (Fig. 1B). The steady drift in the preferred direction (0.6°/s), produced sustained firing in the ON and ONOFF-DSGCs. Sustained firing was higher in ON-DSGCs than ONOFF-DSGCs (ON-DS, n = 10; ONOFF-DSGC, n = 7; unpaired t test, p = 0.0040). The simulated saccades strongly suppressed preferred-direction spiking in the ON-DSGCs. Spiking increased regardless of saccade-direction in the ONOFF-DSGCs indicating a loss of directional signaling at these high velocities (Fig. 1B). On return to the slow drift in the preferred direction at the end of each saccade, spiking in the ON-DSGCs recovered rapidly, indicating that the saccadic suppression is relatively transient.
Simulated saccades suppress preferred-direction responses in ON but not ONOFF-DSGCs. A, Images used for simulated saccades. Left, An example checkerboard. Right, the natural scene used. The image sizes were extended outside the projection area to allow for generation of saccades. The red rectangles show the size of the stimulus DLP array, and the blue circles show the area projected onto the retina. The yellow circles show the approximate extent of a typical ON-DSGC dendritic arbor. See Materials and Methods for details on intensities and dimensions. B, Average PSTHs from ONOFF- (red, n = 7) and ON- (black, n = 10) DSGCs during stimulation with the drifting checkerboard shown in A. Shading shows the SEM. The symbols show the timing and direction of saccades (blue, preferred; green, null). C, Average PSTHs for ONOFF- (red; n = 10) and ON- (black; n = 8) DSGCs during stimulation with the drifting natural image shown in A. Raster plots for several ON-DSGCs during the natural scene stimulation are shown. Each line of dots shows the timing of the spikes during a single trial. Different cells are shown alternately in gray and black. The trace at the bottom of the figure shows the position of the stimuli along the preferred-null axis as a function of time, i.e., the slope represents the speed. Speed of saccades was 90°/s. In the intervals between saccades the positive slope indicates the constant drift of the image in the preferred direction (0.6°/s). The same stimulus trajectory was used for natural scenes and checkerboards. The vertical broken lines mark the saccade onsets, with blue and green lines marking the preferred- and null-direction events, respectively.
Random checkerboard images over-represent high spatial frequencies relative to natural images, in which the power declines inversely with spatial frequency (Simoncelli and Olshausen, 2001). To ensure that the responses were pertinent to natural images, we repeated the global motion stimuli with the same velocity trajectories but using a natural scene instead of the checkerboard. We chose an image that a rabbit might conceivably experience, and that had fairly uniform contrast (Fig. 1A; see Materials and Methods). The ON-DSGCs responded to the appearance of the checkerboard, but not to the appearance of the scene (Fig. 1B,C, compare black traces), most likely because the scene produced a step decrease in mean luminance which offset the drive produced by the step increase in spatial contrast. Similar to the checkerboards, the identical motion trajectories for the natural scenes suppressed preferred-direction spiking in ON-DSGCs but not ONOFF-DSGCs (Fig. 1C). For the ON-DSGCs the initial slow drift during the conditioning phase elicited a lower average spike rate for the natural scene than for the checkerboards (scene: 17 ± 8.8 Hz, n = 8; checkerboard: 30.5 ± 12.1 Hz, n = 10, p < 0.017). The two stimuli produced similar sustained firing in the ONOFF-DSGCs (scene: 10.7 ± 8.2, n = 5; checkerboard: 12.7 ± 9.3, n = 7, p = 0.69).
Similar to the checkerboards, for the natural scenes the ONOFF-DSGCs showed an increase in spiking for both the preferred and null directions, indicating a reduction in the strength of the directional tuning. The average number of spikes produced by the preferred- and null-direction saccades was the same for either the checkerboards or scenes (checkerboards: n = 7, p = 0.23; scenes: n = 5, p = 0.094). We wanted to be certain that the lack of saccadic suppression in the ONOFF-DSGCs was not related to this loss in direction-selectivity. Therefore, we compared responses of ON-DSGCs and ONOFF-DSGCs to rapid global motion at lower velocities where the ONOFF-DSGCs remained strongly directional (10°/s). These stimuli strongly suppressed preferred-direction firing in the ON-DSGCs but not in the ONOFF-DSGCs (Fig. 2). Overall, these results demonstrate that rapid motion simulating saccadic eye movements selectively suppresses ON-DSGCs but not ONOFF-DSGCs. This selective suppression is produced by both spatially uncorrelated checkerboard stimuli and by highly correlated natural images.
The decrease in directional responses in ONOFF-DSGCs during rapid global motion only occurs at very high stimulus speeds. Averaged spike responses (8 cells) to simulated rapid eye movements in the preferred and null directions at image velocities producing clear directional responses of ONOFF-DSGCs (10°/s). The checkerboard stimulus and the times at which the rapid motion events occurred was the same as in Figure 1. In one set of trials, stimuli moved only in the preferred direction (blue) and in a second set, stimuli moved in the preferred and null directions (black).
Because ON-DSGCs and ONOFF-DSGCs both respond during low-speed global image motion but not at high speeds, we sought to quantify how steeply the responses of the two cell types diverged as the stimulus speed increased. We examined the speed tuning functions for both global and local image motion, because ONOFF-DSGCs are known to respond more strongly to local versus global image motion. Global motion refers to stimuli that are elicited for example by self-motion, when the eye or head moves and the image on the entire retina moves coherently. Local motion refers to stimuli that differentially activate the receptive field center, such as an object moving relative to the background. The stimulus paradigm for global motion was similar to that used in Figure 1. Figure 3A shows responses to full-field checkerboard stimuli. The stimulus appeared and remained static for 1 s before drifting at 0.6°/s in the preferred direction for 4 s and then abruptly increasing to a test speed for a further 4 s. The initial reference speed produced sustained firing in both types of DSGCs, whereas increasing the speed elicited progressively less spikes in the ON-DSGCs, but more spikes in the ONOFF cells (Fig. 3A,B). The speed-tuning of the two types of DSGCs for this global motion is shown in Figure 3C (solid squares). The mean firing rate over the 4 s stimulus interval declined for ON-DSGCs as the speed was increased above the initial drift rate (0.6°/s), and was 50% suppressed at ∼1.2°/s. We measured the maximum firing rate ONOFF-DSGCs, which occurred near the beginning of the 4 s stimulus interval. Peak firing rate increased steadily without saturation over this velocity range (Fig. 3C, solid red).
Speed tuning of ON- (black) and ONOFF- (red) DSGCs. A, Average PSTHs recorded for a moving full-field checkerboard stimulus. The stimulus appeared and after a 1 s delay began drifting in the preferred direction at 0.6°/s for 4 s before abruptly increasing speed. This stimulus timing is shown beneath the traces. ON-DSGCs shown in black (n = 3) and ONOFF-DSGCs in red (n = 4). B, Average PSTHs recorded for ON- (black; n = 11) and ONOFF- (red; n = 12) DSGCs in response to a bright bar on a gray background drifted across the receptive field center in the preferred direction at the speeds indicated. The length of the bar was adjusted to separate leading and trailing edge responses. Inset; replot of the PSTHs for the ON-DSGCs with the time axis rescaled so that the responses to the leading and trailing edges of the stimulus were superimposed. The calibration bars beneath the traces all denote 4 s, according to the speed and length of the stimulus bars. C, Speed-dependence of spike responses to global (filled symbols) and local motion (open symbols) for ON-DSGCs (black) and ONOFF-DSGCs (red; on response only). Data drawn from cells in A and B. D, Expanded segment of the PSTHs from A normalized in time to show the correspondence of the responses to the spatial structure of the stimuli.
ONOFF-DSGCs show surround inhibition, which suppresses responses to global motion and hence tunes cells to respond more robustly to small objects that selectively stimulate the receptive field center (Chiao and Masland, 2003; Olveczky et al., 2003; Hoggarth et al., 2015; Im and Fried, 2016). We asked whether the strong suppression of ON-DSGCS during rapid image motion might result from strong wide-field inhibition activated during global motion. To address this question, we used elongated bars to stimulate the center receptive fields of ON- and ONOFF-DSGCs without engaging the inhibitory surround. The length of the bar was adjusted so that the leading and trailing edges produced well separated ON and OFF responses in the ONOFF-DSGCs (see Materials and Methods). For these local motion stimuli, the peak ON-response increased monotonically as a function of the bar velocity for ONOFF-DSGCs, reaching half-maximal responses at ∼1.8°/s (Fig. 3B,C, open red symbols), but declined progressively for the ON-DSGCs (Fig. 3B,C, open black symbols). This stimulus also revealed small trailing-edge OFF responses in the ON-DSGCs (Fig. 3B, inset, arrow), as has been reported previously (Ariel and Daw, 1982; Nirenberg and Meister, 1997; Roska and Werblin, 2001; Rentería et al., 2006; Sun et al., 2006; Ackert et al., 2009). In agreement with the previous work, the spike rates for local motion tended to be higher than for global motion (Fig. 3C, compare corresponding red and black symbols; Chiao and Masland, 2003; Olveczky et al., 2003; Hoggarth et al., 2015; Im and Fried, 2016). Overall, these observations are in accord with previous work showing differences in the temporal/velocity tuning of ON- and ONOFF-DSGCs (Oyster, 1968; Wyatt and Daw, 1975; Sivyer et al., 2010).
The ONOFF-DSGC firing rate varied widely in response to the constant-velocity global motion stimuli (Fig. 3A, red), however, similarities in the structure of the peristimulus spike-time histograms (PSTHs) become evident when the they are normalized in time (Fig. 3D). After this transformation, the peaks and troughs in the PSTHs appeared to be remarkably reproducible, consistent with the idea that the cells respond similarly to the spatial structure of the checkerboard stimulus, regardless of velocity. Such invariance might allow the integrated spike counts in ONOFF-DSGCs to provide a measure of optical flow over a range of image velocities (Sabbah et al., 2017). Overall, the data indicated that the selective suppression of ON-DSGCs versus ONOFF-DSGCs during saccadic stimuli is consistent with a marked difference in velocity tuning over a range of stimulus speeds.
Saccadic motion activates glycinergic inhibition in ON-type DSGCs
Previous data indicated that differences in velocity tuning in ON-DSGCs versus ONOFF-DSGCs resulted in part from stronger activation of inhibition over excitation in ON-DSGCs at higher temporal frequencies (Sivyer et al., 2010). To further investigate the synaptic basis for such differences we recorded voltage-responses while stimulating cells with a centered spot that was sinusoidally modulated over a range of temporal frequencies. In agreement with our previous analysis (Sivyer et al., 2010), spiking in ON-DSGCs declined dramatically at stimulus frequencies >1 Hz, whereas the ONOFF-DSGCs continued to respond up to 8.5 Hz, the highest frequency tested (Fig. 4A). The loss of spiking in the ON-DSGCs was accompanied by the appearance of hyperpolarizing IPSPs that were time-locked to the stimulus frequency (Fig. 4A,B). In an overlapping ONOFF-DSGC recorded simultaneously we saw no evidence for hyperpolarizing IPSPs (Fig. 4A, red). Two other pairs of overlapping ON- and ONOFF-DSGCs recorded simultaneously showed similar results.
ON-DSGCs but not ONOFF-DSGCs receive a transient inhibitory input that is activated at high flicker frequencies. Responses to a flickering spot centered on the receptive field to illustrate the temporal tuning. A, Simultaneous voltage recordings from adjacent ON- and ONOFF-DSGCs with overlapping receptive fields at a range of temporal frequencies. Stimulus timing is shown by the overlying the traces. B, Average voltage responses from 10 ON-DSGCs during 1, 2, 4, and 8.5 Hz sinusoidal flicker stimulation. Shading shows the SD; stimulus timing is superimposed in gray. C, Phase of the peak IPSP versus stimulus frequency measured from the second stimulus cycle. The solid line shows the linear regression. The slope of the line corresponds to a fixed time delay of 19.3 ms.
The IPSPs were activated during the upswing of the stimulus intensity and were time-locked to the flicker (Fig. 4B), consistent with drive through the ON-pathway. The width at half-height of the IPSPs ranged from ∼30 ms at 8.5 Hz to ∼90 ms at 1 Hz. We examined the activation-latency by measuring the timing of the peak IPSP, expressed as the phase relative to the start of the stimulus cycle. The IPSPs activated sharply at a fixed phase of 155° (Fig. 4C, intercept of fitted line). The increase in the phase delay with increasing stimulus frequency was consistent with a fixed time-delay of ∼19.3 ms (Fig. 4C, slope of fitted line). A second, slow component to the IPSPs was also seen and is particularly evident at 4 and 8.5 Hz (Fig. 4B) as a slowly decaying hyperpolarization activated at the start of the stimulus. We next made current recordings under voltage-clamp to investigate the synaptic basis of these light-evoked postsynaptic potentials.
We used the same flickering spot stimulus (Fig. 5A1) and recorded light-evoked synaptic currents over a range of holding potentials and calculated the underlying excitatory and inhibitory conductance components (Taylor and Vaney, 2002; Venkataramani and Taylor, 2016). Inhibition comprised fast, transient stimulus-locked inputs (Fig. 5A2), which appeared to be superimposed on a slower, sustained component. It seems likely that the slower sustained component includes an inhibitory input activated during the Off-phase of the stimulus, which is clearly evident at the lower stimulus frequencies (Fig. 5A2, arrows), although one cannot rule out contribution from a slower ON inhibitory component with a longer duration. The time course of the sustained envelope is particularly evident at the highest stimulus frequency (Fig. 5A2, 8.5 Hz trace). It is largest at the start of the stimulus and declines markedly within a few hundred milliseconds. The excitatory conductance was modulated at the stimulus frequency, with a peak of ∼4–5 nS that remained constant with increasing stimulus frequency (Fig. 5A3,B, solid symbols). Note that the phasic inhibition turns on more sharply than the excitation (Fig. 5A, compare responses at 1 and 2 Hz). Overall, these results indicate that the hyperpolarizing PSPs seen in current-clamp are consistent with the presence of direct inhibitory inputs to the ON-DSGCs.
A rapid transient inhibitory synaptic conductance is observed ON-DSGCs. A1, The stimulus was a sinusoidally-modulated 300 μm diameter spot, 80% contrast centered on the receptive field. A2, A3, Net light-evoked inhibitory and excitatory synaptic conductances activated by a range of flicker frequencies (for details of analysis, see Materials and Methods). Within each trial the spot was flickered at three frequencies, as indicated by the stimulus timing beneath each trace. Shading shows the SEM (top, n = 9 cells; bottom, n = 6 cells). The arrows highlight the inhibition activated during the OFF-phase of the stimulus. The symbols above the 2 Hz responses illustrate the measurement points for B. The triangles represent the peak amplitude during the first half stimulus cycle, and the circles show the amplitude during the second cycle. B, Peak amplitude of the inhibitory and excitatory conductances at the stimulus-cycle time-points indicated in A2 and A3.
It is well established that DSGCs receive a strong asymmetric GABAergic input that is critical for generating directional responses (Taylor et al., 2000; Vaney et al., 2001; Fried et al., 2002). Previous work has shown that the inhibitory inputs to ONOFF-DSGCs are mediated by GABA receptors (Wyatt and Day, 1976; Caldwell and Daw, 1978; Massey et al., 1997; Fried et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2010). We next tested the hypothesis that the inhibitory inputs seen in the ON-DSGCs were also mediated entirely by GABAergic mechanisms by examining the pharmacological sensitivity of the inhibition. We recorded responses to a centered flickering spot in control and during application of 10 μm SR93551 and 50 μm TPMPA to block GABAA and GABAC receptors (GABA block). The flickering spot-stimulus was identical to that shown in Figure 5. GABA block appeared to slightly suppress the fast IPSCs, whereas excitation was enhanced relative to control (Fig. 6A, green). The subsequent addition of 1 μm strychnine to block glycine receptors completely blocked inhibition but had little additional effect on excitation (Fig. 6A, magenta). Similar experiments were performed in a second group of cells but using a stimulus bar moving at 10°/s in the cell's preferred direction to minimize the magnitude of the directional GABAergic input from SBACs. GABA block had little effect on the preferred-direction inhibition, but strongly enhanced the excitatory input, which also became more sustained (Fig. 6B, green). Similar to the flickering spot, blocking both GABA and glycine receptors completely suppressed the inhibitory input (Fig. 6, magenta). These results indicate that ON-DSGCs receive a strong direct glycinergic inhibitory input. The effects of the inhibitory blockers on excitation were complex; blocking GABA-receptors alone prolonged the time course of the excitation, whereas the addition of the glycinergic blocker produced excitation very similar to control.
Inhibitory synaptic inputs to ON-DSGCs comprise direct glycinergic inputs. A, Average inhibitory and excitatory conductances recorded in control (black) and during application of 10 μm SR95531 + 10 μm TPMPA (GABA block, green; n = 5). Top row shows inhibition; bottom row shows excitation. Sinusoidal flicker frequency indicated beneath. Three recordings remained stable enough for the subsequent addition of 1 μm strychnine (GABA + Glycine block, magenta; n = 3). Centered light spot, 300 μm diameter, 80% contrast. B, Average inhibitory and excitatory conductances generated by a bar (300 μm wide, 5000 μm long, 1600 μm/s, 80% contrast) moving through the center of the receptive field in the preferred motion. Traces show control (black), GABAergic block (green; n = 5), and subsequent GABAergic and glycinergic block (magenta; n = 4, TPMPA 10 μm, SR95531 10 μm, strychnine 1 μm). The grey lines show the control traces scaled to match the maximum amplitude during drug application to compare the time course of the excitation during inhibitory block.
These results suggest that glycinergic inhibition specific to ON-DSGCs largely determines the different temporal tuning of the two types of DSGCs, and most importantly, the different sensitivity to saccadic motion. This hypothesis predicts that blocking glycine receptors should allow ON-DSGCs to become more responsive at high temporal frequencies. Confirming this prediction, ON-DSGCs responded strongly to flickering stimuli at 2 and 8.5 Hz after glycinergic inhibition as blocked (Fig. 7A). These changes in excitability were paralleled by changes in the synaptic conductances. Inhibition was strongly suppressed during glycinergic block (Fig. 7B, averages of 4 cells). The sustained envelope disappeared, consistent with this component also relying on glycinergic receptors. The residual IPSCs in Figure 7B presumably reflect GABAergic input, largely from starburst amacrine cells. As predicted, the suppression of spiking observed during saccadic global motion of natural scenes was lost in the presence of the glycinergic blocker (Fig. 7C). The number of spikes recorded during preferred and null direction saccades increased significantly (paired t test, preferred: p = 2.4 × 10−3, null: p = 9.9 × 10−5). Note that with glycine receptors blocked, the ON-DSGCs responded in the preferred and null directions indicating that directional tuning was weaker at these high velocities (Fig. 7C, blue and green arrows), as was observed for the ONOFF-DSGCs (Fig. 1B). Finally, we tested the effects of strychnine on the speed tuning for local motion. After blocking glycinergic inputs, there was little effect on the ON-responses of the ONOFF-DSGCs (Fig. 7D, top), but a large potentiation of the ON-DSGC response (Fig. 7D, bottom), at least at high velocities. DSGCs receive excitatory input partially through cholinergic inputs (Ariel and Daw, 1982; Kittila and Massey, 1997), which can be suppressed by micromolar concentrations of strychnine (Renna et al., 2007). The major effect of strychnine in our experiments was consistent with suppression of glycine receptors, because excitatory drive was generally increased. Perhaps the slightly lower firing rate in response to slow drift of the visual scene could be attributable to cholinergic blockade (Fig. 7C), but further work would be required to test that hypothesis. Overall, after scaling for absolute spike rate, the responses for the two cell types became comparable in the presence of strychnine, and therefore the speed tuning became essentially indistinguishable (Fig. 7E).
Speed-tuning in ON- and ONOFF-DSGCs is similar after glycinergic block. A, Representative voltage responses during center flicker (diameter, 300 μm; contrast, 80%) at two frequencies as indicated in control (black; replotted from Fig. 4A) and in the presence of 1 μm strychnine (blue). B, Average synaptic conductances (n = 4) activated by a centered stimulus spot (diameter, 300 μm; contrast, 80%) modulated at 8.5 Hz, in control (black) and in the presence of 1 μm strychnine (blue). C, PSTHs in response to global drift of a random checkerboard stimulus similar to that shown in Figure 1. Average of six ON-DSGCs in control and during glycinergic block. The arrows highlight preferred and null direction saccades. Spiking increases in both cases. D, PSTHs averaged from four ONOFF-DSGCs and seven ON-DSGCs before (black) and during application of strychnine (blue). Shading shows SEM. Light stimulus was a 300 μm wide bar (same stimulus as Fig. 3B) drifting in the preferred direction at various velocities. Speed of the stimulus indicated next to the traces. E, Peak spike rates shown for the full range of stimulus velocities. Circles show the ON-response of the ONOFF-DSGCs. The scaling of the y-axes is set to normalize the amplitude.
Discussion
The results demonstrate that glycinergic inhibitory inputs specific to ON- but not ONOFF-DSGCs, are responsible for differences in the temporal response properties between the two classes of DSGCs. The inhibition is activated by both local and global visual stimuli and serves to suppress spike output from the ON-DSGCs during fast image motion (Figs. 1, 3). Unlike the directional GABAergic input to these cells, the glycinergic inhibition is activated regardless of the direction of image motion. Thus, at least two inhibitory mechanisms function independently within the dendrites of ON-DSGCs, a GABAergic component that produces directional responses, and glycinergic components that mediate saccadic suppression and tune the cells to respond best to relatively low speeds. During simulated saccades, ONOFF-DSGCs respond regardless of the direction of image motion, indicating that the directional GABAergic inputs were ineffective at high velocities (Fig. 1, red). The ON-DSGCs showed similar behavior when glycinergic inputs were blocked (Fig. 7C). Such high velocities may be functionally similar to static flashes, which cause DSGCs to spike because the GABAergic inhibition from starburst amacrine cells arrives slightly delayed relative to the excitation (Sivyer et al., 2010). The ability of the glycinergic circuit to suppress spiking for rapid contrast changes appears similar to the role of glycinergic receptors in temporal tuning of local-edge-detector ganglion cells (Venkataramani et al., 2014). The primary role for glycinergic circuits in shaping temporal tuning at high velocities is further evident in the similar speed tuning for ON- and ONOFF-DSGCs, seen when glycinergic transmission was blocked. At low velocities and stimulus frequencies, where spiking is more sustained, the properties of the excitatory inputs also appear to contribute to temporal tuning (Figs. 3A, 4A), because the sustained spiking likely reflects the more sustained excitatory inputs to ON-DSGCs relative to ONOFF-DSGCs (Sivyer et al., 2010). It is not known whether this excitatory difference results from different presynaptic bipolar cells or synaptic heterogeneity within a common type. Overall, the markedly different responses of ON-DSGCs and ONOFF-DSGCs during rapid visual motion appear attributable largely to a glycinergic input that targets ON-DSGCs but not ONOFF-DSGCs.
The different functional properties for the two types of DSGCs seem to be attuned to their distinct central projections. ONOFF-DSGCs project to the LGN and are thus likely involved in image-forming and perceptual vision. ON-DSGCs are conspicuous among ganglion cells by their primary projection to the accessory optic system (AOS; Collewijn, 1975; Oyster et al., 1980; Ellis et al., 2016; Martersteck et al., 2017), where their afferent signals drive the optokinetic reflex (OKR) that helps stabilize the image on the retina (Erickson and Barmack, 1980). Some ONOFF-DSGCs in mouse also project to the AOS in addition to their primary projections to the superior colliculus and LGN (Kay et al., 2011; Dhande et al., 2013). However, the optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) is abolished by selectively blocking the ON-pathway (Knapp et al., 1988; Iwakabe et al., 1997), which challenges a major role for ONOFF-DSGCs in generating OKN, because these cells still produce strong directional responses through the OFF-pathway when the ON-pathway is suppressed (Kittila and Massey, 1995). OFF ganglion cells are incapable of driving the optokinetic reflex in zebrafish (Emran et al., 2007).
Our current findings seem consistent with the idea that ON-DSGCs alone are critical for generating the OKN. The ON-DSGCs could correct for small, slow drift of the image on the retina, which might occur during steady gaze, by activating high-gain feedback pathways producing compensatory eye movements that stabilize the retinal image. Conversely, sudden or continuous motion producing strong optical flow across the retina will activate the glycinergic inputs and silence output from the ON-DSGCs, thus preventing inappropriate activation of the high-gain compensatory eye movements. ONOFF-DSGCs on the other hand, appear to well suited to signal saccade onset, or global optical flow (Sabbah et al., 2017). They modulate their firing continuously as a function of the speed (Fig. 7E), allowing them to signal changes in image motion on the retina. Moreover, a given image pattern passing across an ONOFF-DSGC receptive field appears to produce a fairly constant number of spikes over a range of velocities (Fig. 3D), suggesting that these cells may be good substrates for signaling the amount of optical flow. On the other hand, ONOFF-DSGCs are suppressed by wide-field stimuli in mouse and rabbit (Chiao and Masland, 2003; Olveczky et al., 2003; Hoggarth et al., 2015; Im and Fried, 2016), which would tend to reduce ability to signal optical flow. Nonetheless, the central projection patterns of ON- and ONOFF-DSGCs in mouse (Dhande et al., 2013; Gauvain and Murphy, 2015), seem to be broadly similar to rabbit consistent with conservation of the fundamental directional circuitry in these two species.
A previous analysis performed in head-fixed animals (Collewijn, 1969), concluded that the open loop gain of OKN in rabbits declines at frequencies >∼1°/s, and that the closed loop gain is lower than might be expected for the open-loop results (Collewijn, 1969). The suggestion was that “saccadic interruptions in visual input” might explain these findings. Indeed, during OKN experiments, rapid “stepwise” movement of the visual scene to a different location failed to drive compensatory eye movements (Collewijn, 1981). Moreover, higher closed-loop gain was found for sine-wave modulated motion, where saccades were relatively rare (Collewijn, 1969). Our results showing strong suppression of spiking during rapid saccades directly demonstrate saccadic interruption of retinal output, at least that arising from the ON-DSGCs. The higher spike output from ONOFF-DSGCs during saccades suggest that if these cells make a convergent contribution to the central OKN circuits in the rabbit, they likely play different roles. For example, in the wallaby, recordings from the nucleus of the optic tract and dorsal terminal nucleus revealed two types of direction-selective units, slow and fast (Ibbotson et al., 1994). The slow units were inhibited by saccades in either direction, similar to the ON-DSGCs illustrated here.
Glycinergic inhibition and temporal tuning
The results demonstrate a key role for inhibitory inputs in generating saccadic suppression of the ON-DSGCs. The flicker stimuli reveal two characteristic temporal components to the inhibition (Figs. 4, 5); a rapid phasic component that modulates at the stimulus frequency, and a slower component that generates a sustained envelope that peaks within a few tens of milliseconds and decays over a few hundred milliseconds (Figs. 4B, 5, 6A). The increasing phase-delay observed with increasing frequency (Fig. 4C), suggests a fixed time delay for synaptic transmission. The inferred delay, close to 20 ms, is consistent with many previous estimates of synaptic delays for surround-inhibition in the retina (Gouras and Zrenner, 1979; Enroth-Cugell et al., 1983; Benardete and Kaplan, 1997; Molnar and Werblin, 2007; Cui et al., 2016; Murphy-Baum and Taylor, 2018). However, previous studies estimated inhibitory delays relative to excitation, while the current results require that inhibition and excitation be coincident, and therefore exhibit similar synaptic delays. A previous analysis of temporal tuning in local-edge-detector ganglion cells point to a similar role for glycinergic transmission, and also predicts that inhibition and excitation should be coincident (Venkataramani et al., 2014). The circuit mechanisms that ensure that inhibition and excitation are either temporally coincident or delayed are not well understood.
The slow component of the glycinergic inhibition, which may result from temporal summation of relatively sustained inhibition arising from the both the ON- and OFF-pathways, is likely glycinergic because it is suppressed by strychnine (Fig. 7B). The slow component represents a considerable fraction of the total inhibition, and could produce sustained hyperpolarization of the ON-DSGCs during saccadic eye movements in the rabbit, which can be up to ∼250 ms (Collewijn, 1977). The faster transient component may be important for countermanding rapidly fluctuating excitatory inputs activated by a rapidly moving image. A further factor driving temporal tuning in the ON-DSGCs is the excitatory–inhibitory synaptic conductance ratio, which decreased with stimulus frequency because of increased inhibition (Fig. 5B). It is also noteworthy, that the inhibition shuts-off rapidly (Fig. 5A2, 4 and 8 Hz), which would allow the ON-DSGCs to recover rapidly from the saccadic-suppression (Fig. 1B). Thus, at the end of a saccade, the ON-DSGC will quickly resume encoding slow image-slip on retina and thus re-establish the signals required for image stabilization.
These results do not preclude the possibility that differences in GABAergic transmission also make some contribution to the temporal tuning of the two DSGC types. Understanding the functional roles of glycinergic and GABAergic inhibitory systems in the retina remains an ongoing challenge (Masland, 2012). Feedforward inhibition is known to convey specific computational features to several ganglion cells types including direction-selective (Vaney et al., 2012), orientation-selective (Nath and Schwartz, 2016; Venkataramani and Taylor, 2016), suppressed-by-contrast (Sivyer et al., 2011; Jacoby et al., 2015; Tien et al., 2016), and looming-sensitive cells (Münch et al., 2009). Our results further illustrate how targeted feedforward inhibition can bestow a computational property onto a specific ganglion cell type. A question for future research is whether the glycinergic amacrine cells involved are unique to this ON-DS circuit, or whether they produce similar saccadic suppression in other ganglion cell types.
Footnotes
This work was supported by NIH Grants R01 EY014888, R01 EY022070, T32 EY023211, P30 EY010572, and P30 NS061800; and by unrestricted departmental funding from Research to Prevent Blindness (New York, NY). We thank Marla Feller, Mathew Summers, David Vaney, and our anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on the paper.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to W. Rowland Taylor at wrtaylor{at}berkeley.edu