Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Learning What Is Irrelevant or Relevant: Expectations Facilitate Distractor Inhibition and Target Facilitation through Distinct Neural Mechanisms

Dirk van Moorselaar and Heleen A. Slagter
Journal of Neuroscience 28 August 2019, 39 (35) 6953-6967; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0593-19.2019
Dirk van Moorselaar
1Department of Psychology,
2Amsterdam Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, 1001 NK Amsterdam, The Netherlands,
3Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, 1081BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands, and
4Institute of Brain and Behaviour Amsterdam, 1081BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Dirk van Moorselaar
Heleen A. Slagter
1Department of Psychology,
2Amsterdam Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, 1001 NK Amsterdam, The Netherlands,
3Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, 1081BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands, and
4Institute of Brain and Behaviour Amsterdam, 1081BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Heleen A. Slagter
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Task design and behavioral findings of Experiments 1–3. A, A trial sequence of Experiments 1–3. In each condition, in each trial of a sequence of trials, participants had to indicate the orientation (left or right) of a target Gabor. In all conditions except one, a distractor (a Gabor that was horizontally or vertically oriented) was concurrently presented. Across a sequence of trials, the distractor location could repeat, the target location could repeat, or target and distractor locations varied across trials. The search display was presented for 200 ms, and participants had 1000 ms to respond. In all conditions, except the target-only variable (Tv) condition, the target was accompanied by a distractor (a horizontally or vertically oriented Gabor). In the target-repeat (DvTr) and distractor-repeat (DrTv) conditions, the location of the target (Tr) or the distractor (Dr) was repeated over trials in a sequence. In the baseline (DvTv) condition, the target and distractor location varied from trial to trial. In the target-only variable condition (Tv), the location of the target also varied from trial to trial. The number of trials in a sequence ranged between 4 and 12, and the number of search locations between 4 and 8 across experiments. Further, the colors of each condition correspond to condition specific colors in subsequent plots. B–D, RTs as a function of condition and trial position for (B) Experiment 1, (C) Experiment 2, and (D) Experiment 3. E, Boxplot showing benefits of distractor location repetition in distractor-repeat sequences in Experiment 3 as a function of trial position contrasted to random distractor location repetitions in baseline sequences. Solid lines inside boxes indicate the mean. Dashed lines indicate the median.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    Topographic power in a range of low frequencies tracks both the location of the target and the distractor. A, Total power CTF slopes tuned to the target location across a range of frequencies and collapsed across all conditions of interest (i.e., DvTv1, DvTv4, DvTr1, DvTr4). All nonsignificant values were set to zero in a two-step procedure. First, each individual data point was tested against zero with a paired-sampled t test. After setting nonsignificant values to zero, data were evaluated using cluster-based permutation. B, Total power CTF slopes tuned to the distractor location across a range of frequencies and collapsed across all conditions of interest (i.e., DvTv1, DvTv4, DrTv1, DrTv4).

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    Target repetition increased anticipatory and poststimulus spatial tuning to target locations. All plots represent the CTF slope, which here quantifies the location specificity of the topographic distribution of activity in the alpha band. A, Evoked power CTF slopes tuned to the target location at the first (left) and final (right) trial in baseline (DvTv) and target-repeat sequences (DvTr). B, Total power CTF slopes tuned to the target location at the first (left) and final (right) trial in baseline and target-repeat sequences. Target repetition increased spatial tuning to the predictable target location already in advance of target presentation. A control (dotted black line) analysis showed that this effect cannot be attributed to lingering effects from the preceding trial. Shaded error bars represent bootstrapped SEM (same applies to subsequent figures). Colored bars on the x axis (blue; green) represent time points where conditions differ significantly from 0 after cluster correction (p < 0.05). Double-colored thick lines indicate time points with a significant difference between the respective conditions after cluster correction (p < 0.05).

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Estimated CTFs on the first and last trial of the sequence across three time windows of interest. A, Fitted CTFs tuned to the target location in baseline (blue; DvTv) and target-repeat sequences (green; DvTr). B, Fitted CTFs tuned to the distractor location in baseline (blue; DvTv) and distractor-repeat sequences (red; DrTv). Estimates were based on a fit to an exponential cosine function (for details, see Results). CTFs are shown separately for the first (top row) and final trial (bottom row) in the repetition sequence. From left to right, CTFs are averaged across three windows of interest (i.e., anticipation: −550 to 0 ms; search display: 0–200 ms; response: 200–550 ms). **p < 0.01, significant difference between CTF amplitudes in baseline and repeat sequences. Although there was a significant difference at the final trial position for target location tuned CTF amplitudes both in anticipation and during search, no such differences were observed in distractor location tuned CTF amplitudes.

  • Figure 5.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 5.

    Distractor repetition did not change spatial tuning to distractor locations. A, Evoked alpha power CTF slopes tuned to the distractor location at the first (left) and final (right) trial in baseline (DvTv) and distractor-repeat sequences (DrTv). B, Total alpha power CTF slopes tuned to the distractor location at the first (left) and final (right) trial in baseline and distractor-repeat sequences. Colored bars on the x axis (blue; red) represent time points where conditions differ significantly from 0 after cluster correction (p < 0.05).

  • Figure 6.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 6.

    Target repetition reduced the amplitude of the target-evoked N2pc. ERPs evoked by targets were computed only using trials where the target was presented on the bottom left or right from fixation, with a distractor on the midline. A, Difference waveforms (contralateral − ipsilateral) revealing the N2pc are shown separately for the baseline (DvTv) and target-repeat (DvTr) condition on the first (1) and final (4) repetition in the sequence. Double-colored thick lines indicate time points with a significant difference between the respective conditions after cluster correction (p < 0.05). Gray thick lines indicate time points with a significant condition difference after baseline correction (p < 0.05). B, Boxplots represent the difference between conditions (DvTv − DvTr) on the first (dashed) and final (solid) trial of a target repetition block within the N2pc window (170–230 ms). Solid lines inside boxes indicate the mean. Dashed lines indicate the median.

  • Figure 7.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 7.

    Distractor repetition reduced the distractor-evoked Pd. ERPs evoked by distractors were computed based on trials where the distractor was presented on the bottom left or right from fixation, with a target on the midline. A, Difference waveforms (contralateral − ipsilateral) revealing that the N2pc and Pd are shown separately for the baseline (DvTv) and distractor-repeat (DrTv) conditions on the first (1) and final (4) repetition in the sequence. Double-colored thick lines indicate time points with a significant difference between the respective conditions after cluster correction (p < 0.05). B, Boxplots represent the difference between conditions (DvTv − DrTv) on the first (dashed) and final (solid) repetition within the Pd window (280–360 ms). Solid lines inside boxes indicate the mean. Dashed lines indicate the median.

  • Figure 8.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 8.

    Target repetition was associated with a shortening of the representation of the target location within the N2pc time window, as reflected in decoding accuracy. Shown are target-location decoding accuracies of broadband EEG using all 64 electrodes separately for baseline (DvTv) and target-repeat (DvTr) sequences and the first (1) and last (4) trial in a sequence. Colored bars on the x axis (blue; green) represent time points where conditions differ significantly from 0 after cluster correction (p < 0.05). Double-colored thick lines indicate time points with a significant difference between the respective conditions after cluster correction (p < 0.05). Gray thick lines indicate time points with a significant condition difference after baseline correction (p < 0.05).

  • Figure 9.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 9.

    Distractor repetition was not associated with a change in the distractor representation, as reflected in decoding accuracy. Shown are distractor-location decoding accuracies of broadband EEG using all 64 electrodes separately for baseline (DvTv) and distractor-repeat (DrTv) sequences and the first and last trial in a sequence. Colored bars on the x axis (blue; red) represent time points where conditions differ significantly from 0 after cluster correction (p < 0.05).

Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 39 (35)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 39, Issue 35
28 Aug 2019
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Learning What Is Irrelevant or Relevant: Expectations Facilitate Distractor Inhibition and Target Facilitation through Distinct Neural Mechanisms
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Learning What Is Irrelevant or Relevant: Expectations Facilitate Distractor Inhibition and Target Facilitation through Distinct Neural Mechanisms
Dirk van Moorselaar, Heleen A. Slagter
Journal of Neuroscience 28 August 2019, 39 (35) 6953-6967; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0593-19.2019

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Learning What Is Irrelevant or Relevant: Expectations Facilitate Distractor Inhibition and Target Facilitation through Distinct Neural Mechanisms
Dirk van Moorselaar, Heleen A. Slagter
Journal of Neuroscience 28 August 2019, 39 (35) 6953-6967; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0593-19.2019
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Experiment 1: the time course of distractor learning
    • Experiment 2: the time course of learned suppression and learned facilitation
    • Experiment 3: neural mechanisms underlying learned facilitation and inhibition
    • General Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • attention
  • brain
  • EEG
  • expectation
  • inhibition
  • statistical learning

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Early-Life Stress Impairs Perception and Neural Encoding of Rapid Signals in the Auditory Pathway
  • Semantic representations during language comprehension are affected by context
  • The human centromedial amygdala contributes to negative prediction error signaling during appetitive and aversive Pavlovian gustatory learning
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Semantic representations during language comprehension are affected by context
  • Perceptual Difficulty Regulates Attentional Gain Modulations in Human Visual Cortex
  • Concurrent- and after-effects of medial temporal lobe stimulation on directed information flow to and from prefrontal and parietal cortices during memory formation
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.