Abstract
Directing attention helps to extract relevant information and suppress distracters. Alpha brain oscillations (8–12 Hz) are crucial for this process, with power decreases facilitating processing of important information and power increases inhibiting brain regions processing irrelevant information. Evidence for this phenomenon arises from visual attention studies (Worden et al., 2000); however, the effect also exists in other modalities, including the somatosensory system (Haegens et al., 2011) and intersensory attention tasks (Foxe and Snyder, 2011). We investigated in human participants (10 females, 10 males) the role of alpha oscillations in focused (0/100%) versus divided (40/60%) attention, both across modalities (visual/somatosensory; Experiment 1) and within the same modality (visual domain: across hemifields; Experiment 2) while recording EEG over 128 scalp electrodes. In Experiment 1, participants divided their attention between visual and somatosensory modality to determine the temporal/spatial frequency of a target stimulus (vibrotactile stimulus/Gabor grating). In Experiment 2, participants divided attention between two visual hemifields to identify the orientation of a Gabor grating. In both experiments, prestimulus alpha power in visual areas decreased linearly with increasing attention to visual stimuli. In contrast, prestimulus alpha power in parietal areas was lower when attention was divided between modalities/hemifields compared with focused attention. These results suggest there are two alpha sources, one of which reflects the “visual spotlight of attention” and the other reflects attentional effort. To our knowledge, this is the first study to show that attention recruits two spatially distinct alpha sources in occipital and parietal brain regions, acting simultaneously but serving different functions in attention.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Attention to one spatial location/sensory modality leads to power changes of alpha oscillations (∼10 Hz) with decreased power over regions processing relevant information and power increases to actively inhibit areas processing “to-be-ignored” information. Here, we used detailed source modeling to investigate EEG data recorded during separate unimodal (visual) and multimodal (visual and somatosensory) attention tasks. Participants either focused their attention on one modality/spatial location or directed it to both. We show for the first time two distinct alpha sources are active simultaneously but play different roles. A sensory (visual) alpha source was linearly modulated by attention representing the “visual spotlight of attention.” By contrast, a parietal alpha source was modulated by attentional effort, showing lowest alpha power when attention was divided.
Introduction
Allocation of attention helps to extract important and neglect irrelevant information. Alpha brain oscillations (8–13 Hz) potentially occupy this filtering role and lead to excitation or inhibition of sensory-specific regions, thereby facilitating or suppressing sensory processing (Klimesch et al., 2007; Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010; Mathewson et al., 2011). When attending to two spatial locations (right/left), a relative alpha power decrease is observed over brain regions processing relevant information compared with regions inhibiting irrelevant information. Such a hemispheric alpha power lateralization over occipitoparietal regions has been shown many times in visuospatial attention (Foxe et al., 1998; Worden et al., 2000; Kelly et al., 2006; Thut et al., 2006; Gould et al., 2011; Zumer et al., 2014). This has also been observed in the somatosensory system (Anderson and Ding, 2011; Haegens et al., 2011, 2012; van Ede et al., 2011) and in intersensory attention (Foxe and Snyder, 2011; Gomez-Ramirez et al., 2011; Bauer et al., 2012).
What happens if attention is divided between two sensory modalities simultaneously? Would this provoke an alpha-power imbalance between sensory-specific regions reflecting the peak location of attention like that recently observed for spatially divided visual attention (Gould et al., 2011)? Existing literature showed evidence for alpha-power modulation over sensory-specific brain regions; however, attention was not divided between two senses simultaneously (Foxe and Snyder, 2011; Gomez-Ramirez et al., 2011; Bauer et al., 2012). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) evidence suggests attention also modulates activity over higher-level frontal and parietal areas (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002) that modulate lower-level sensory regions via top-down control (Bressler et al., 2008). Inhibiting frontal eye field (FEF) and inferior parietal lobule (IPL) using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS), Capotosto et al. (2009) observed increased reaction times (RTs) and decreased accuracy for visual detection and thereby confirmed fMRI results. They concluded that inhibiting these regions disrupted the control over visual alpha oscillations and altered behavior. According to the authors, both primary sensory and parietal regions are important for controlling attention allocation. Hints of this in EEG are shown by the spatial and functional dissociation of occipital and parietal alpha sources during visual perception (Gulbinaite et al., 2017).
Here, we investigated potential differences in the role of alpha oscillations in focused (0/100%) versus divided (40/60%) attention, both across modalities (visual/somatosensory) and within a modality (visual: across hemifields). We used multimodal (visual/somatosensory, Experiment 1) and unimodal (left/right visual fields, Experiment 2) attention paradigms while recording scalp EEG over 128 electrodes.
A linearly constrained minimum variance (LCMV) beam former (Van Drongelen et al., 1996) source localized changes in prestimulus alpha power. Two alpha sources were identified in Experiment 1: A visual source decreased linearly in power with increasing attention to visual stimuli; a second source in the parietal cortex modulated by task difficulty showed lower alpha power when attention was divided between modalities. Experiment 2 shared the visual source with linear attention modulation however parietal brain regions were not as strongly modulated.
To our knowledge, this study is the first to reveal two spatially distinct alpha mechanisms acting simultaneously and yet performing different roles in attention: a sensory, visual alpha source reflecting the current location of attention and a parietal alpha source modulated by task difficulty and reflecting attentional effort.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Data were acquired from 20 healthy participants (all right-handed, 10 females, mean age 28.1 ± 3.8 years) with normal or corrected to normal vision. One participant was not included in final data analysis because of the absence of an anatomical MRI scan that prohibited complete data analysis. Fifteen of these 20 participants performed two attention paradigms (Experiments 1 and 2) and the remaining four subjects only participated in Experiment 1. Therefore, Experiment 1 had 19 subjects in total and Experiment 2 had 15 in total.
The study was approved by the University of Birmingham Research Ethics Committee. Before the start of the experiment(s), participants provided informed written consent.
Stimuli and task
Visual and somatosensory stimuli were presented using Psychophysics Toolbox (version 3; Brainard, 1997) running in MATLAB (version 2014b; The MathWorks) on a desktop computer (Windows 7 operating system). Participants sat comfortably in a dark room. To minimize head movement and to maintain a constant degree of visual angle for the visual stimuli, their heads were kept stable using a chin rest.
Visual stimuli were presented in Experiments 1 and 2 on a gray background. Gabor gratings were presented briefly (presentation time: 66.7 ms; radius: 1.75 degrees of visual angle, phase: 180°), on a gray background at a distance of 57 cm, using a cathode ray monitor (resolution: 600 × 800 pixels). These stimuli were presented vertically centered and with a horizontal eccentricity of ±8 degrees of visual angle from a horizontally centered white fixation point (radius: 0.1 degrees of visual angle). In Experiment 2, two Gabor gratings were presented to the left and right of the fixation point whereas in Experiment 1 a single visual stimulus was presented to the left of the fixation point, simultaneously with a 250-ms-long vibrotactile stimulus to the tip of the left index finger using a piezoelectric stimulator (Dancer Design).
In Experiment 1, the multimodal attention task was conducted (Fig. 1A). Although fixating on the fixation cross, subjects had to covertly divide their attention between two sensory modalities, attending either more to visual or more to somatosensory stimuli (0/100% or 40/60% attention toward somatosensory/visual domain and vice-versa). A visual cue (5 × 2.5 degrees of visual angle) was presented at fixation at the beginning of every trial for 250 ms, indicating how attention was to be divided. Cues took the form of black arrows indicating the likelihood of subsequent target appearance in each modality (cf. Fig. 1A). After an asynchronous interstimulus interval (aISI) of 1.3–1.6 s (aISIs were randomly chosen for every trial reaching from 1.3 s (minimum aISI) to 1.6 s (maximum aISI)), during which participants were required to divide their attention between modalities according to the prestimulus cue, visual and somatosensory stimuli were presented simultaneously. Gabor patterns were presented in a tilted orientation: for half of the participants they were tilted at 45° and for the other half at −45°. Stimuli with a low or high spatial frequency (0.025 cycles/pixel and 0.1 cycles/pixel) were visual targets and medium-frequency stimuli (0.05 cycles/pixel) were visual distracters. In the somatosensory domain, vibrotactile stimulation at a low or high temporal frequency (4 and 52 Hz) served as somatosensory targets and those at medium temporal frequency (16 Hz) as somatosensory distracters. In every trial, one target (e.g., a visual Gabor pattern with a high spatial frequency) and one distracter (e.g., a somatosensory stimulus with a medium temporal frequency) stimulus were presented simultaneously. After stimulus presentation, white question marks (5 × 1.5 degrees of visual angle) indicated an 850 ms response period where participants pressed a button with their right index finger to report the frequency of the target (two different keys: high or low frequency, regardless of probed modality) as quickly as possible. Even if participants were responding before the end of the response period, the next trial only started after 850 ms with an aISI.
In Experiment 2 the unimodal attention task was conducted (Fig. 1B). This second experiment had a similar structure to Experiment 1 but used only visual Gabor gratings (spatial frequency: 0.05 cycles/pixel), akin to a classic Posner task (Posner et al., 1980). Subjects had to covertly direct their attention in a graded fashion either more to the left or more to the right visual hemifield (0/100%, 20/80%, or 40/60%, attention toward left/right visual hemifields and vice-versa) while they fixated on a central fixation cross (similar to Gould et al., 2011). Trials started with the presentation of a visual cue (5 × 2.5 degrees of visual angle; presentation time: 250 ms) in the form of black arrows indicating where subjects should direct their spatial attention (cf. Fig. 1B). As in Experiment 1, this was followed by an aISI of 1.3–1.6 s, before visual stimuli were presented to the left and right of the fixation point. For half of the participants, horizontal and vertical gratings were target stimuli and rightwards (45°) and leftwards (−45°) tilted gratings served as distracters, whereas for the other half, the opposite was true. In every trial, one target (e.g., horizontal grating) and one distractor (e.g., rightwards tilted grating) appeared simultaneously at opposite sides of the fixation cross. After stimulus presentation, a white question mark was presented for 850 ms to indicate the response period. The task was to respond as quickly as possible to indicate the orientation of the target grating (two different keys: e.g., horizontal or vertical). Even if participants were responding before the end of the response period, the next trial only started after 850 ms with an aISI.
In both experiments, participants were given feedback on their performance (accuracy and RT), which was displayed after each experimental run to maintain their motivation for performing the tasks.
Participants completed a training run consisting of 10 trials per attention condition (resulting in a total of 40/60 trials for Experiments 1 and 2, respectively) before they performed the same task in a staircase experimental run, where the contrast of the visual stimuli was adapted according to participants' performance (60 trials per attention condition) to ensure an accuracy of ∼80% was achieved. For somatosensory stimuli, a similar procedure was used to adapt the amplitude of vibrotactile pulses.
The subjects then started the experiment and performed 150 trials per attention condition giving a total of 600/900 trials for Experiments 1 and 2, respectively. Experiments were divided into three individual runs; all runs contained equal number of trials of each attention condition (50 trials/condition/run). All trials of a given attention condition within a run were grouped together in one block, the order of the blocks between runs was varied pseudorandomly. The whole study took ∼1.5 h per participant, including short breaks that the participants took between runs.
EEG data acquisition
EEG data were recorded from 128 active scalp electrodes following an equi-radial montage at 1024 Hz sampling rate using a Biosemi EEG system with a reference electrode (common mode sense electrode) placed parietocentrally for the recording. In addition, EOG was recorded using three active ocular electrodes with the horizontal electrodes being placed near the two temples and the vertical electrode below the left eye. In Experiment 1, data were recorded in 3 runs of ∼9 min each; in Experiment 2, the 3 runs consisted of ∼12 min each.
After each EEG recording session, the individual electrode positions were digitized relative to the surface of the head with a Polhemus FASTRAK using Brainstorm software (Tadel et al., 2011) running in MATLAB. In addition, each subject attended a separate MRI session where a T1-weighted anatomical image (MPRAGE sequence) of the head, including the nose, with 1 mm isotropic resolution was acquired on either a 3 T or 7 T MRI system that was registered with the digitized head shape.
Data analysis
Behavioral
Behavioral parameters analyzed were RT and accuracy. To analyze significant differences between attention conditions, a repeated-measures two-way ANOVA was computed for both behavioral parameters and experiments separately, with the following factors: (1) attention condition (60% and 100% for Experiment 1 and 60%, 80%, and 100% for Experiment 2, and (2) attended modality (somatosensory and visual) or hemifield (left and right). Post hoc paired-sample t tests were used to identify individual differences between attention conditions and p-values were subsequently Bonferroni corrected to account for multiple comparisons.
EEG
All EEG data processing was performed using the MATLAB toolbox Fieldtrip (Oostenveld et al., 2011).
Preprocessing.
Data were read in as continuous data. For each channel, data were notch filtered (49–51 Hz) to reduce line noise, detrended to remove linear drifts, and demeaned (subtracting the average signal recorded over the whole time course at each channel) to remove between run baseline effects. By visual inspection, noisy channels (i.e., channels with obvious artifacts) were removed from further data analysis. This resulted in a group mean of (±SE) 117 ± 4/116 ± 5 channels remaining for further analysis for Experiments 1 and 2, respectively. Independent component analysis (ICA, logistic infomax ICA algorithm; cf. Bell and Sejnowski, 1995) was then performed to discard eye blinks from the recorded data, with an average of 1 ± 0.6 ICs for Experiment 1 and 1.5 ± 1.5 ICs for Experiment 2 removed from each dataset. The remaining ICs were reprojected to the channel level. Finally, data were rereferenced to the average of all the non-noisy channels that remained for each subject and run.
These data were subsequently used for time–frequency analysis on the sensor and source level.
Sensor-level analysis.
Data were epoched into 1.7 s (−1.5 s until +0.2 s relative to the stimulus presentation onset) segments for every trial and the separate runs of the experiment concatenated. All trial level data were visually inspected and noisy trials removed for each subject, resulting in 818 ± 12.4/539 ± 11.7 (number of trials ± SEM) trials of data remaining for Experiments 1 and 2. Furthermore, those trials where the subject had responded incorrectly to the target were subsequently removed such that 727 ± 16.5/471 ± 15.6 trials remained for Experiments 1 and 2.
Source level analysis.
Individual, four-layer (scalp, skull, CSF, and brain) boundary element (BEM) head models were constructed from the individual subject T1-weighted anatomical images using the Fieldtrip toolbox with the 'dipoli' method (http://www.ru.nl/neuroimaging/field-trip) (Oostenveld et al., 2011). Individual electrode positions were aligned to the scalp surface of the subject's T1 using the fiducial points and head shape to inform alignment. In four of the 19 participants, no individual electrode positions were recorded due to technical problems; therefore, in these subjects, the average electrode positions of the 11 other participants sharing the same electrode layout were used and warped to the scalp surface extracted from the segmented individual T1-weighted scans.
Beamforming analysis was performed using an LCMV beam former (Van Drongelen et al., 1996; Van Veen et al., 1997; Robinson and Vrba, 1998) implemented in the Fieldtrip toolbox to spatially localize changes in alpha power between different attention conditions. The continuous data for each run were first filtered into the alpha-frequency band (8–13 Hz), applying the default parameters for a FIR band-pass filter (which uses the MATLAB fir1 function with a two-pass filter direction, a hamming filter window type, and a filter order of 768 for 10 subjects [sampling rate: 2048 Hz) and 384 (sampling rate: 1024 Hz) for the remaining nine subjects]. The filtered data were subsequently investigated for temporal leakage of the peak of the ERP into the prestimulus period, with no leakage found. The data were then epoched −1.5 s to +0.2 s relative to stimulus onset. The noisy and incorrect response trials, identified from the broadband visual data inspection (see “Sensor level” section above) were removed. Remaining trials were then concatenated over runs, down-sampled to 500 Hz and beam former weights (also known as a spatial filter) (Van Veen et al., 1997) derived. All attention conditions within an experiment were considered together to calculate these weights as the spatial sources of the alpha power were not hypothesized to change between conditions but only their relative amplitude.
For each subject, the preprocessed, cleaned, and down-sampled sensor level data were then separated into trials for each of the attention conditions. The number of trials in each condition was reduced to match that of the condition with the minimum number of trials remaining. This data rejection process was done by randomly removing trials from conditions containing more trials than the minimum. This process ensured that all source localization comparisons were performed on equal amounts of data to avoid biases. An average of 105 ± 22 of the 150 trials per condition for Experiment 1 and 107 ± 16 of the 150 trials per conditions for Experiment 2 remained (mean ± SE over subjects) for further source analysis.
To enable alpha power to be calculated only during the aISI, trials were then segmented, resulting in a time window from −1.3 s to 0 s relative to stimulus onset and concatenated together for each condition to ensure no baseline effects within trials were removed. The source power at each location in the brain BEM (0.5 cm grid) was estimated for each condition using the previously derived weights from all conditions. These source power maps were then used to calculate the alpha modulation index (AMI) source maps for both experiments for each subject using Equation 1, where the source power estimates at each location in the brain for each condition were input, as described previously (Zumer et al., 2014): In Experiment 1, the AMI between trials where participants focused on one modality compared with focusing on the other, for example, between 100% attention to the visual domain vs 100% to the somatosensory domain, was calculated using Equation 1, where SPow (“source power”) was calculated for every location in the brain (on the 0.5 cm grid) and is the power estimate of the alpha band signal over the time period −1.3 to 0 s relative to stimulus onset for all trials in a given condition. Here, cond 1 denotes attend 100% to visual (and 0% to somatosensory) stimuli and cond 2 denotes attend 100% to somatosensory (and 0% to visual) stimuli.
Furthermore, the AMI between trials where participants focused on one modality (100% visual or somatosensory; cond 1 in Eq. 1) and those where attention was divided between modalities (60% visual (i.e., 60% visual and 40% somatosensory) or somatosensory (i.e., 60% somatosensory and 40% visual); cond 2 in Eq. 1) was computed. The equivalent AMIs were calculated for Experiment 2. First, attention conditions 100% left (cond 1 in Eq. 1) and 100% right (cond 2 in Eq. 1) were compared. Then, trials were compared according to whether subjects paid attention to only one side of the visual field (100%; cond 1 in Eq. 1) or divided their attention between left and right hemifields (60%; cond 2 in Eq. 1).
The AMI(100%,100%) contrasts “100% visual (V) vs 100% somatosensory (S)” and “100% left (L) versus 100% right (R)” for Experiments 1 and 2, respectively, were designed to investigate differences in alpha modulation depending on the attentional cue. The AMI(100%, 60%) contrasts “100% (visual/somatosensory) versus 60% (visual/somatosensory)” and “100% (left/right) versus 60% (left/right)” for Experiments 1 and 2, respectively, were designed to elucidate whether task difficulty was reflected by modulations in alpha power.
AMI source maps for each subject were spatially normalized to the MNI template before being averaged over subjects for each experiment to provide a grand average. The different grand average AMI source maps were visually inspected for local minima and maxima for the two experiments. In both experiments, local minima and maxima were observed over the visual cortex [AMI(100%,100%)] and the parietal cortex [AMI(100%,60%)], respectively. For Experiment 1, all stimuli were presented on the left thus hypothesized to recruit the right hemisphere of the brain primarily. Therefore, the maximum AMI value peak location in the right parietal cortex (anatomically defined) from the AMI(100%,60%) maps and a minimum AMI value peak location in the right visual cortex from the AMI(100%,100%) were found for each subject individually. For Experiment 2 bilateral stimulus presentation resulted in hypothesized responses in both hemispheres. Therefore, the AMI maxima were identified in the right and left parietal cortices [(AMI(100%,60%)], and in the left visual cortex [AMI(100%,100%)]. Furthermore, the AMI minimum in the right visual cortex was identified [AMI(100%,100%)]. All peak locations within the anatomically defined regions were identified for each subject individually.
Peak location analysis.
The identified peak locations were used as virtual electrode (VE) locations from which alpha frequency time courses were extracted for each participant individually. Time courses were obtained at each VE location by multiplying the cleaned, continuous, down-sampled channel level data (used to derive the initial weights) by the respective alpha beam former weights derived over all data (see above). Time courses were then demeaned before a Hilbert transform was performed to provide a measure of alpha power at each VE location interrogated for each subject. The data were then epoched −1.3 s to 0 s relative to stimulus onset (i.e., the aISI period) and separated into conditions (using the same balancing procedure used to derive the source maps). The average alpha power over trials for each condition was found and then averaged over the aISI period (−1.3–0 s) to provide a measure of mean alpha power per condition in the visual and parietal cortices.
For Experiment 2, data from left and right hemispheres were combined by flipping the attention conditions (attention left 100% = attention right 100% etc.) for the data recorded over the right hemisphere, effectively resulting in alpha power modulations from the left parietal and visual cortices (cf. Waldhauser et al., 2016). This procedure was designed to increase the signal-to-noise ratio.
To take into account between-subject variance, alpha power values were then normalized by the maximum average alpha power value in any condition for each subject. Subsequently, the grand average over subjects was computed. These were tested for linear and quadratic modulation over conditions by fitting the data first to linear and then to quadratic functions using the MATLAB function polyfitn.
Automated anatomical labeling (AAL) analysis.
To test whether the linear and quadratic modulations observed from the peak location analysis were statistically significant, we performed additional analyses based purely on anatomically parcellated brain regions and therefore not biased by the AMI source maps in identification of locations to interrogate. Brain regions were parcellated using the AAL atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). Only the anatomical regions in which an alpha related response was predicted were interrogated. Therefore 15 AAL regions in the right hemisphere, spanning from the visual cortex to the somatosensory cortex and 26 AAL regions in right and left hemisphere, reaching from visual to parietal cortex, were investigated in Experiments 1 and 2, respectively (Tables 1, 2).
The following analysis approach, previously used on MEG data (Brookes et al., 2016), was used here. For each subject, all AAL regions were warped onto the individual subject's T1-weighted image and time courses were then extracted from all VE locations (on a 0.5 cm grid) which fell within the AAL regions. The VE time courses were extracted using the same data and processes used for the peak location analysis. Time courses from VE locations (each grid point) were weighted according to the Euclidian distance of the VE location to the center of gravity of the respective AAL region. After applying the correct weighting, time course data were summed over all VEs per AAL region, to give one time course per AAL region containing all trials, which was then demeaned. The Hilbert transform was subsequently applied to time courses for each AAL region. The data were then epoched −1.3 to 0 s relative to stimulus onset (i.e., the aISI period) and separated into conditions (using the same trial balance used for the source maps and peak responses). The alpha power time courses for each AAL region were then averaged over trials and aISI time window within each attention condition and subject. The outcome of this processing was 15 × 4 (Experiment 1: 15 AAL regions and 4 attention conditions) or 26 × 6 (Experiment 2: 26 AAL regions—including AAL regions in the left hemisphere but excluding AAL regions within the somatosensory cortex—and six attention conditions) alpha power values per subject. Data of Experiment 2 were averaged between AAL regions across hemispheres by flipping the attention conditions, resulting in 13 AAL datasets per subject.
Before averaging over subjects, the resulting 4/6 alpha power values for the attention conditions in Experiments 1 and 2 per AAL region were normalized by the alpha power value of the attention condition that showed the maximum power, removing between-subject variance to ensure between condition variance was interrogated. Given the apparent linear and quadratic modulation patterns derived from the peak location analysis, for each AAL region the normalized alpha power averaged over all subjects (i.e., 15/19 data points per condition for Experiments 1 and 2, respectively) were first fit with a linear function. Subsequently, those AAL regions, where no significant linear modulation was observed, were investigated for potential quadratic modulations. This approach was chosen to circumvent the issue that quadratic models; being more complex, will always provide a better goodness of fit than a linear model. Significance of the fits obtained on the real data was determined through Monte Carlo permutation tests (25,000 repetitions). Here, for every AAL region, the real data fits were compared with surrogate distributions of linear and quadratic terms of the respective AAL regions, derived from shuffling data between the different attention conditions for every subject individually and performing new linear and quadratic fits over the 4/6 surrogate attention conditions. The p-values obtained were then corrected for multiple comparisons (i.e., AAL regions) using false discovery rate (FDR) correction (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995; Yekutieli and Benjamini, 1999).
For those regions where a significant quadratic modulation was found, we further interogated whether the quadratic model out-performed a linear model by computing the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1974) using the fitlm function implemented in MATLAB. This ruled out the possibility that the significant quadratic modulation was only based on the higher complexity of the model compared with a linear model. The “winner” of these different model types is the one that minimizes the AIC. An ANOVA implemented in the fitlm function tests whether the “winning” model explains the data better than a constant model. The resulting p-values were then Bonferroni corrected taking into account the number of AAL regions which showed a significant quadratic modulation.
Control time–frequency analysis.
To investigate power lateralization effects due to attentional modulation on a more broad spectrum of frequencies, we conducted a wavelet analysis for frequencies ranging from 1 to 48 Hz, using an increasing number of cycles (2 cycles at 1 Hz and 8 cycles at 48 Hz) in a time window ranging from −1.5 s until −0.1 s with respect to stimulus onset. In Experiment 1, this analysis was performed for four neighboring electrodes over right somatosensory areas and four neighboring electrodes over right visual areas (cf. topography plot in Fig. 6). For Experiment 2, four neighboring electrodes over left and four neighboring electrodes over right visual recording sites were chosen (cf. topography plot in Fig. 6). Power lateralization was calculated in the same way as the AMI using Equation 1 (see above). For Experiment 2, right hemisphere electrodes were mirrored to combine with data recorded over left electrodes.
Results
Behavior
Experiment 1 (multimodal task)
A two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with main factors of cue (100 or 60% attention) and modality (attention to visual or somatosensory modality) revealed that the accuracy for discrimination of spatial and temporal frequencies was significantly higher in the “attend 100%” condition than in the “attend 60%” condition (p-value = 1.3 × 10−7; F = 34.3, Fig. 2A, top). No significant effect of modality (p-value = 0.21; F = 1.6) and no interaction between cue and modality was observed (p-value = 0.4; F = 0.67; Fig. 2A, top).
When investigating potential differences of the second dependent variable, RTs across attention conditions, we could observe a main effect of cue (p-value = 1.1 × 10−8; F = 41.8). Furthermore, a significant interaction between factors cue and modality (p-value = 1.03 × 10−4; F = 16.9) revealed a stronger effect of cue on RTs when subjects attended to the somatosensory stimuli (Fig. 2A, bottom).
Experiment 2 (unimodal task)
A 2-way repeated-measures ANOVA with main factors of cue (60%, 80%, and 100% attention) and side of presentation (left or right visual hemifield) revealed as the main effect that the first dependent variable, accuracy for discriminating the orientation of Gabor gratings was significantly higher in the “attend 100%” than in the “attend 60%” condition (p-value = 3.37 × 10−4; F = 8.8; Fig. 2B, top). No significant effect of side of presentation (p-value = 0.63; F = 0.2) and no interaction between cue and side of presentation was observed (p-value = 0.64; F = 0.4; Fig. 2B, top).
Furthermore, RT was significantly shorter when subjects only attended to one side of the visual field (100 vs 0% attention), than when they divided their attention between hemifields (80 vs 20% and 60 vs 40% attention; p-value = 4.1 × 10−6; F = 14.4). There was no significant interaction between cue and side of presentation (p-value = 0.3; F = 1.1; Fig. 2B, bottom).
EEG responses
As there was hypothesized to be more than one alpha power response from different cortical areas, we focus the results on the source level where spatial localization aids interpretation of the data.
Experiment 1
To investigate potential differences in alpha power between the attention conditions, we first compared trials where subjects only paid attention to visual stimuli (100% V; cond 1 in Eq. 1) with trials where they only attended to somatosensory stimuli (100% S; cond 2 in Eq. 1). We computed the alpha modulation index ('AMI'; Eq. 1) on the beam former results, which revealed a negative response in right visual cortex, indicating an alpha power decrease in visual cortex with increasing attention to the visual domain as shown in Figure 3A. No alpha power modulation was found in somatosensory areas between these two attention conditions (Fig. 3A). The AAL analyses supported this observation revealing a significant linear modulation of alpha power (p-value = 0.02, FDR corrected; r2 = 0.056) observed in the right inferior occipital gyrus; Figure 3B.
To investigate whether differential alpha power modulation was observed in other brain regions in trials where attention was divided between modalities compared with those where attention was focused on one modality only, the AMI between the 100% (cond 1 in Eq. 1) and 60% (cond 2 in Eq. 1) attention conditions was computed. This contrast revealed a peak source location in the right parietal cortex, showing higher alpha power in the 100% than 60% attention conditions (Fig. 3C, denoted by red color). AAL analysis confirmed this result, showing significant quadratic modulations of alpha power in two superior parietal regions: right postcentral lobule (p-value = 4 × 10−5, FDR corrected; r2 = 0.12) and right precuneus (p-value = 0.01, FDR corrected; r2 = 0.068). Visual inspection of the alpha power across conditions showed that significantly lower alpha power was induced in these regions when attention was divided between modalities than when subjects paid attention to only one modality (Fig. 3D, left). No significant linear modulations were seen in these regions. To rule out that the significant quadratic modulations over these two AAL regions were purely a result of the higher complexity of quadratic models compared with linear models, we directly compared whether a linear or a quadratic model better explained the data, using the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1974). For both regions, the quadratic model minimized the AIC compared with a constant or linear model. Furthermore, in the right postcentral lobule, the quadratic model was significantly better than a constant model (corrected p = 0.022) but failed significance for the right precuneus (corrected p = 0.155).
Experiment 2
In this second experiment, AMI analysis in source space (Eq. 1) identified a maximum in left and a minimum in right visual cortices when contrasting trials where subjects attended 100% to the left (cond 1 in Eq. 1) with trials where subjects attended 100% to the right (cond 2 in Eq. 1) side of the visual field (Fig. 4A). Alpha power at the peak in the left visual cortex increased with increasing attention to the left visual hemifield, whereas the response in the right visual cortex showed a decrease in alpha power. For increasing attention to the right visual hemifield, the opposite was observed. Therefore, a decrease in alpha power could be observed over visual areas contralateral to the focus of attention whereas an increase in alpha power was present over visual areas of the hemisphere ipsilateral to attention. These responses were combined by inverting the responses across conditions measured from right hemisphere and then averaging with those measured from left hemisphere. The results of this analysis are shown in the bar plot in Figure 4A, and suggest a linear modulation of alpha power by attention. The fitting analysis in AAL regions confirmed this observation, showing that with increasing attention, alpha power decreased linearly over visual areas of the hemisphere contralateral to the focus of attention: significant linear fits were found in the angular gyri (p = 0.03, FDR corrected; r2 = 0.052) and superior occipital gyri (p = 0.03, FDR corrected; r2 = 0.047), as shown in Figure 4B.
The AMI maps comparing the conditions 100% attention (cond 1 in Eq. 1) and 60% attention (cond 2 in Eq. 1) revealed maxima in the parietal cortex in the hemisphere ipsilateral to where visual attention was directed (Fig. 4C, AMI maps), indicating higher alpha power in the 100% attention conditions than the 60% attention conditions. Interrogation of these responses over all conditions showed a quadratic (“u”-shaped) alpha power modulation pattern, as shown in the bar plot in Figure 4C. Further interrogation using the AAL analysis showed that a trend (p = 0.07, FDR-corrected; r2 = 0.039) for a quadratic fit was observed over the parietal region inferior parietal lobule (IPL). Visual inspection of the alpha power across conditions for this AAL region, revealed that the quadratic fit was a “u” shape (Fig. 4D), as seen in the peak analysis (Fig. 4C) and similar to that seen for the multimodal paradigm shown in Figure 3, C and D.
Discussion
Numerous EEG/MEG studies showed that posterior alpha power is modulated by attention. However, it is unclear whether these alpha power modulations reflect one or several attentional mechanisms. Here we show using EEG source analysis (LCMV beam former) that in unimodal and multimodal attention tasks, alpha power is differently modulated by attention in occipital and parietal areas (Fig. 5).
We found two alpha sources, visual and parietal, which can be separated spatially and experimentally. These sources are both modulated by attention, but play different functional roles depending on behavioral demands. The visual alpha source showed linear power decreases with increasing attention to visual stimuli or a given location, thus indicating the location of attention, i.e., the visual Spotlight of Attention (Posner et al., 1980; Crick, 1984; Eriksen and Yeh, 1985). In contrast, the parietal alpha source was quadratically modulated by attention showing lower alpha power when attention was divided, between modalities or spatial locations, rather than focused on either. Thus we suggest the parietal alpha source likely indicates attentional effort. Regions showing significant linear (pink) and quadratic (blue) alpha power modulations observed in Experiments 1 and 2 are summarized in Figure 5.
On the behavioral level, we have replicated previous findings (Gould et al., 2011) and show a robust effect of attention, modulating significantly accuracy and RTs in both experiments where higher accuracy and lower RTs are present when attention is focused on one modality (Exp. 1) or spatial location (Exp. 2).
Surprisingly, we did not find an alpha power lateralization effect over somatosensory regions, when comparing attention to visual and somatosensory targets in Experiment 1. We thus investigated the data using a broad frequency spectrum (1–48 Hz) and found that the power lateralization between visual and somatosensory recording sites seems to depend more on beta (∼16–17 Hz) than alpha oscillations. Although the data of the purely visual task of Experiment 2 show a prominent peak in the alpha frequency band (∼10 Hz; cf. Fig. 6, right), Experiment 1 shows a more broad effect, with a peak frequency in the beta band (cf. Fig. 6, left). We think that this could be the reason that we did not find any linear attention modulation effects on alpha power over somatosensory areas. Although this finding is interesting, the focus of this study was the role of alpha oscillations in different types of attention. Further analyses of this beta band effect are therefore subject to future reinvestigation of this dataset.
Functional significance
The two tasks used in this study show similar clustering of alpha activity in visual and parietal areas (Fig. 5). Although quadratic modulations over parietal areas that were observed in the purely visual task (Exp. 2) only showed a trend toward significance, this still suggests that the two alpha sources are a general phenomenon of attention rather than specific to the experimental task. If our assumption is true, the imprecise region of interest that has previously been reported as parietooccipital is actually composed of two distinct brain sources that act in different ways. We hypothesize that the sensory-specific source reflects “the visual spotlight of attention” and is controlled by top-down processes coming from a parietal alpha source, which in turn is modulated by attentional effort toward the task. Participants reported that the purely visual task of Experiment 2 was easier to accomplish than the multimodal task in Experiment 1. This discrepancy could contribute to the weaker effect of quadratic modulations over parietal areas in Experiment 2.
Previous fMRI studies showed that both visual and parietal regions show an increased BOLD response in the hemisphere contralateral to the direction of visual spatial attention (Sylvester et al., 2007; Bressler et al., 2008; Lauritzen et al., 2009), reflecting increased cortical excitability. Using Granger causality, Bressler et al. (2008) further showed that the FEF and IPS, both part of the dorsal attention network, were responsible for driving neural activity in early visual areas by top-down control. Other studies obtained similar results (Ruff et al., 2008; Marshall et al., 2015; Popov et al., 2017). Because EEG alpha activity and the BOLD signal are widely reported to be negatively correlated (Goldman et al., 2002; Laufs et al., 2006; Scheeringa et al., 2011), an increase in BOLD signal over contralateral visual and parietal areas in a visual spatial attention task could be related to a decrease in alpha activity over the same regions, which would agree with the findings of this study. Capotosto et al. (2009) hypothesized that top-down control from frontal and parietal areas mediates the occipital alpha rhythm and therewith the level of inhibition. Using rTMS to inhibit the previously identified regions FEF and IPS (Bressler et al., 2008), Capotosto et al. (2009) also showed increased RTs and decreased accuracy for target detection. Furthermore, they demonstrated that this inhibition abolished the prestimulus alpha-desynchronization, which can be typically observed over parietal and occipital electrodes contralateral to attention and concluded that this disruption in top-down control of the visual alpha rhythm led to a decrease in visual identification (Capotosto et al., 2009). However, they were not able to identify what neuronal rhythms from IPS and FEF were causing this top-down control of the occipitoparietal alpha rhythm from their experiment.
Previous studies support the idea for spatially distinct visual and parietal alpha sources mediating attention and visual perception. Van Dijk et al. (2008) showed that low prestimulus parietal alpha power was advantageous for visual discrimination (van Dijk et al., 2008). They concluded that this parietal alpha source regulates alpha power in low-level visual areas via top-down control. With our results we could reinterpret their findings and conclude that the parietal alpha power modulation rather reflects the attentional state than the level of inhibition of the occipital cortex. Thus, low parietal alpha power would indicate a state where the subject is engaged in the task, leading to the recruitment of top-down attention and an increase in performance in discriminating grating orientations or temporal and spatial frequencies. Another example is a recently published EEG study (Gulbinaite et al., 2017) on the triple-flash illusion, where a third visual flash is perceived upon presentation of only two stimuli. The illusion comes about when presenting the second stimulus after a specific interval; the authors could show that this interval and the illusory percept correlated with the individual alpha frequency at parietal but not occipital sources. Consistent with these findings, a recent intracranial EEG study shed more light onto these distinct alpha sources and their directionality, supporting the view of a top-down control of occipital alpha by parietal areas (Halgren et al., 2017). The authors recorded resting state data on epilepsy patients and found evidence for alpha generators in the parietal cortex. They further showed that alpha acts like a traveling wave, propagating in space from parietal to occipital brain regions (Halgren et al., 2017). Albeit the important evidence for the existence of two spatially distinct alpha sources, none of the above presented studies could experimentally dissociate them into occipital and parietal sources.
Crucially we extend these previous studies by showing that there are two distinct alpha sources which are modulated differentially by attention in two different sets of experiments and are thus likely to have different functional roles. These data adds to a growing body of evidence that there are multiple alpha sources present during a cognitive task with distinct roles (Nunez et al., 2001). Alpha oscillations have gained much interest in neuroscientific research and their image has changed from reflecting a passive idling state (Pfurtscheller et al., 1996) to actively regulating inhibition in the service of cognition (Klimesch et al., 2007; Palva and Palva, 2007; Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010; Mathewson et al., 2011). Given the ubiquity of alpha oscillations in the human brain, it makes sense to assume that the role played by alpha is a very general one like gating neural activity. If this assumption is true, then we should be able to dissociate different alpha oscillations in a complex cognitive task that recruits a number of cortical assemblies controlled by alpha. Our data represents such evidence where we spatially and experimentally dissociate an occipital/ventral parietal from a more superior parietal alpha source in two experiments requiring a complex interaction between top-down and bottom-up attention processes. Previous studies focused on the role of alpha in gating low level sensory information (Jensen et al., 2012). We add to this literature by showing that alpha not only indicates the locus of attention, but also the recruitment of higher order areas, which arguably control the shift of attention to lower-order, primary sensory locations/modalities. Our results open up the avenue for future non-invasive human EEG studies to investigate how alpha oscillations in these two regions coordinate their activity to implement attentional shifts, which so far has mostly been addressed by invasive animal recordings (von Stein et al., 2000; Buffalo et al., 2011; van Kerkoerle et al., 2014).
EEG source localization relies on whether the assumptions of its algorithm are met by the data. Our EEG results are corroborated by a separate, high resolution fMRI study conducted by our group at ultra-high field (7 T) on a subsample of the same subjects (7/10 participants also performed Experiment 1), using the same multimodal task as in Experiment 1. This study (Aquino et al., 2019) also reveals two fMRI sources modulated by attention: (1) quadratic BOLD-response modulations over parietal areas when contrasting attentional effort (100% vs 60%) and (2) linear modulations over visual areas when contrasting the location of attention (100% vs 100%). Due to the superior spatial resolution of fMRI, these results strongly suggest that the two alpha band sources measured with EEG are indeed distinct sources that need to be considered separately.
Conclusions
We show that two spatially distinct alpha sources execute different roles in unimodal and multimodal attention: (1) a parietal source modulated by attentional effort showed significantly lower alpha power when subjects divided their attention, which potentially exerts top-down control on alpha oscillations over lower-level visual areas; and (2) a visual alpha source that reflects the current spotlight of visual attention showing a significant linear power decrease with increasing attention to visual stimuli, possibly driven by top-down control from parietal alpha sources. Given that such a top-down control has been shown to exist (Ruff et al., 2008; Bressler et al., 2008; Capotosto et al., 2009), we hypothesize a similar top-down regulation from parietal toward visual areas; however, further exploration is needed to confirm this hypothesis.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Leverhulme Trust (Research Project Grant to K.J.M. RPG-2014-369).
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Rodika Sokoliuk at r.sokoliuk{at}bham.ac.uk