Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Systems/Circuits

A Sound-Sensitive Source of Alpha Oscillations in Human Non-Primary Auditory Cortex

Alexander J. Billig, Björn Herrmann, Ariane E. Rhone, Phillip E. Gander, Kirill V. Nourski, Beau F. Snoad, Christopher K. Kovach, Hiroto Kawasaki, Matthew A. Howard III and Ingrid S. Johnsrude
Journal of Neuroscience 30 October 2019, 39 (44) 8679-8689; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0696-19.2019
Alexander J. Billig
1The Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Alexander J. Billig
Björn Herrmann
1The Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Björn Herrmann
Ariane E. Rhone
2Department of Neurosurgery,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Ariane E. Rhone
Phillip E. Gander
2Department of Neurosurgery,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kirill V. Nourski
2Department of Neurosurgery,
3Iowa Neuroscience Institute,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Kirill V. Nourski
Beau F. Snoad
2Department of Neurosurgery,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Christopher K. Kovach
2Department of Neurosurgery,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Christopher K. Kovach
Hiroto Kawasaki
2Department of Neurosurgery,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Hiroto Kawasaki
Matthew A. Howard III
2Department of Neurosurgery,
3Iowa Neuroscience Institute,
4Pappajohn Biomedical Institute, The University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa 52242, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Matthew A. Howard III
Ingrid S. Johnsrude
1The Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada,
5School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6A 5B7, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Ingrid S. Johnsrude
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The functional organization of human auditory cortex can be probed by characterizing responses to various classes of sound at different anatomical locations. Along with histological studies this approach has revealed a primary field in posteromedial Heschl's gyrus (HG) with pronounced induced high-frequency (70–150 Hz) activity and short-latency responses that phase-lock to rapid transient sounds. Low-frequency neural oscillations are also relevant to stimulus processing and information flow, however, their distribution within auditory cortex has not been established. Alpha activity (7–14 Hz) in particular has been associated with processes that may differentially engage earlier versus later levels of the cortical hierarchy, including functional inhibition and the communication of sensory predictions. These theories derive largely from the study of occipitoparietal sources readily detectable in scalp electroencephalography. To characterize the anatomical basis and functional significance of less accessible temporal-lobe alpha activity we analyzed responses to sentences in seven human adults (4 female) with epilepsy who had been implanted with electrodes in superior temporal cortex. In contrast to primary cortex in posteromedial HG, a non-primary field in anterolateral HG was characterized by high spontaneous alpha activity that was strongly suppressed during auditory stimulation. Alpha-power suppression decreased with distance from anterolateral HG throughout superior temporal cortex, and was more pronounced for clear compared to degraded speech. This suppression could not be accounted for solely by a change in the slope of the power spectrum. The differential manifestation and stimulus-sensitivity of alpha oscillations across auditory fields should be accounted for in theories of their generation and function.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT To understand how auditory cortex is organized in support of perception, we recorded from patients implanted with electrodes for clinical reasons. This allowed measurement of activity in brain regions at different levels of sensory processing. Oscillations in the alpha range (7–14 Hz) have been associated with functions including sensory prediction and inhibition of regions handling irrelevant information, but their distribution within auditory cortex is not known. A key finding was that these oscillations dominated in one particular non-primary field, anterolateral Heschl's gyrus, and were suppressed when subjects listened to sentences. These results build on our knowledge of the functional organization of auditory cortex and provide anatomical constraints on theories of the generation and function of alpha oscillations.

  • alpha
  • auditory cortex
  • functional inhibition
  • high gamma
  • intracranial
  • speech

Introduction

Human primary auditory cortex occupies the posteromedial portion of Heschl's gyrus (HG) and can be distinguished from neighboring non-primary fields based on architectonic and electrophysiological features (Hackett et al., 2001; Clarke and Morosan, 2012; Nourski and Howard, 2015). Responses to sound in primary auditory cortex phase-lock to rates beyond 100 Hz and include characteristic short-latency evoked components (Liégeois-Chauvel et al., 1991; Howard et al., 2000; Brugge et al., 2008, 2009). They also contain pronounced induced power in the high gamma range (Steinschneider et al., 2008; Brugge et al., 2009; 70–150 Hz), which is considered a proxy for multiunit spiking activity (Crone et al., 2001; Mukamel et al., 2005; Manning et al., 2009).

The anatomical distribution and stimulus-sensitivity of ongoing low-frequency oscillations in auditory cortex have been less well characterized, but there are several reasons to expect the strength of alpha-band (broadly defined as 7–14 Hz) activity in particular also to vary across primary and non-primary auditory cortex. First, cortical high-gamma and alpha activity often exhibit an antagonistic relationship (Crone et al., 1998; Mukamel et al., 2005; Ramot et al., 2012). For example, alpha power in visual cortex is suppressed as high gamma activity increases upon afferent stimulation (Hoogenboom et al., 2006; Bauer et al., 2014). Second, primary and non-primary auditory fields have distinct architectonic and connectivity profiles (Clarke and Morosan, 2012), which may give rise to intrinsic dynamics at different timescales (Başar and Güntekin, 2008; Honey et al., 2012; Murray et al., 2014). Third, alpha oscillations have been associated with a range of neural operations and processes that may differentially engage lower versus higher levels of the cortical hierarchy (Clayton et al., 2018). These include functional inhibition (Klimesch et al., 2007; Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010), inter-areal synchronization (Palva et al., 2010), and the communication of sensory predictions (Bauer et al., 2014; Sedley et al., 2016; Auksztulewicz et al., 2017; Chao et al., 2018).

Most of what we know of human alpha oscillations derives from study of the dominant occipital and parietal sources detectable in scalp electroencephalography (EEG), particularly when the eyes are closed or during manipulations of visuospatial attention (Berger, 1931; Clayton et al., 2018). However, ongoing oscillations at the lower end of the alpha frequency range (7–10 Hz) have also been recorded from superior temporal cortex. These show power decreases during auditory stimulation (Niedermeyer, 1990; Tiihonen et al., 1991; Lehtelä et al., 1997; Gomez-Ramirez et al., 2011; Weisz et al., 2011; Fontolan et al., 2014; A. Keitel and Gross, 2016), the extent of which is sensitive to the nature of the stimulus, for example with clear speech eliciting a larger power suppression compared with noisy speech (Obleser and Weisz, 2012; de Pesters et al., 2016). There are indications that the magnitude of ongoing alpha oscillations (Frauscher et al., 2018) and of their suppression during stimulation (Fontolan et al., 2014) is lower on HG than elsewhere in the temporal lobe. However, recording sites in the relevant studies were not localized to primary versus non-primary auditory cortex based on known electrophysiological response properties.

To more precisely characterize the spatial distribution and time course of low-frequency oscillations, including alpha, in human auditory cortex, we recorded from eight hemispheres in seven adult patients implanted with electrodes along the length of HG and elsewhere on the superior temporal plane and lateral superior temporal gyrus, during clinical monitoring for epilepsy. This coverage allowed analysis of local field potentials in both primary and non-primary auditory fields, defined both anatomically and based on responses to click trains, while subjects listened to clear and degraded speech. Our objective was not to directly test particular accounts of alpha generation or function, but rather to provide anatomical specificity that may constrain the development of these theories, particularly as they relate to auditory processing.

Materials and Methods

Subjects.

Subjects were seven individuals (4 females; median age: 33 years; age range 22–56 years) undergoing intracranial monitoring for diagnosis and treatment of medically intractable epilepsy. Recordings were made in an electromagnetically shielded hospital room at the Epilepsy Monitoring Unit at the University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics. Three individuals had electrode placement in the left hemisphere only (Subjects L357, L403, L442), three in the right hemisphere only (Subjects R369, R399, R429), and one bilaterally (Subject B335). Further details of electrode placement are provided in the “Data recording” section and in Figure 1. All subjects were right-handed with left-hemisphere language dominance as determined by preimplantation Wada testing. Three subjects (B335, R399, L442) had normal hearing (pure tone thresholds <20 dB hearing level for frequencies between 250 and 4000 Hz) and four (L357, R369, L403, R429) had mild hearing loss at isolated frequencies (there was no systematic difference in results between these two subgroups). Vision was self-reported as normal or corrected to normal; participants who required glasses wore them during the task. All participants were native speakers of English. Subject details, including demographic data, seizure focus, details of hearing loss, and the number of contacts in each studied field, are provided in Table 1.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Demographics, hearing, seizure focus, and electrode coverage

Research protocols were approved by the University of Iowa Institutional Review Board, and subjects signed informed consent documents before any recordings. Research did not interfere with acquisition of clinical data, and subjects could withdraw from research at any time without consequence for their clinical monitoring or treatment. Subjects initially remained on their antiepileptic medications but these were typically decreased in dosage during the monitoring period at the direction of neurologists until sufficient seizure activity had been recorded for localization, at which point antiepileptic medications were resumed. No research occurred within 3 h following seizure activity. Electrode contacts at seizure foci were excluded from all analyses.

Stimuli.

Stimuli for determining primary versus non-primary sites were 160 ms long click trains, presented at rates of 25, 50, 100, 125, 150, and 200 Hz. 50 click trains were presented at each rate, in random order and with intervals between onsets of successive click trains drawn from a normal distribution with mean 2000 ms and SD 10 ms. Stimuli for the main experiment were clear and noise-vocoded versions of English sentences previously used by Wild et al. (2012). They were recorded by a female native speaker of North American English in a soundproof booth using an AKG C1000S microphone with 16-bit sampling at 44.1 kHz using an RME Fireface 400 audio interface. Three-band noise-vocoding was performed as described by Shannon et al. (1995) using a custom-designed vocoder implemented in MATLAB (MathWorks; RRID:SCR_001622). In detail, items were filtered into three contiguous frequency bands (50–558, 558–2264, and 2264–8000 Hz, selected to have equal spacing along the basilar membrane), using FIR Hann bandpass filters with an 801-sample-window length. The amplitude envelope in each frequency band was extracted using full-wave rectification followed by low-pass filtering at 30 Hz with a fourth-order Butterworth filter. These envelopes were applied to bandpass filtered noise in the same frequency ranges, the results of which processing were summed to produce the noise-vocoded sentence. Clear speech remained unprocessed, containing all frequencies up to 22.05 kHz. Finally, the entire set of clear and noise-vocoded sentences was normalized for root-mean-square intensity.

Procedure.

Subjects took part while sitting upright in their hospital bed. Sounds were presented diotically at a comfortable listening level via insert earphones (ER4B, Etymotic Research) integrated into custom-fit earmolds. For the presentation of click trains, subjects relaxed and performed no task. For the main experiment, subjects fixated on the center of a screen (ViewSonic VX922 or Dell 1707FPc) positioned ∼60 cm in front of them and made responses on a computer keyboard. Each trial consisted of an initial auditory sentence presentation (either clear or degraded; duration 1223–4703 ms), a delay (1250–1500 ms), a written sentence (2500–2750 ms), a further delay (1250–1500 ms), a repeat presentation of the auditory sentence, and a further delay (2300–2800 ms). During the delay periods a fixation cross was displayed in the center of the screen. The written sentence either matched, or was completely different to, the content of the spoken sentence. To facilitate attention to the auditory stimuli, subjects were told that they would be asked about the content of the spoken sentences at the end of the experiment. To encourage attention to the text, subjects were asked to report occasional capital letters that occurred in 9% of trials (excluded from analysis) using the hand ipsilateral to the hemisphere with the most electrode contacts. Stimulus presentation and response collection was controlled by Presentation (Neurobehavioral Systems; RRID:SCR_002521) running on a Windows PC. The speech experiment lasted between 30 and 45 min, depending on the number of trials completed (range 120–136) and whether the subject opted to take a break. The manipulation of the visual cue was included to answer a separate research question concerning the neural basis of perceptual pop-out of degraded speech; related results including performance in the behavioral tasks will be reported elsewhere.

Data recording.

Electrophysiological activity was recorded using depth and subdural electrodes (Ad-Tech Medical). Subdural grids with coverage including superior temporal gyrus consisted of platinum-iridium discs (2.3 mm diameter, 5–10 mm inter-contact distance) embedded in a silicon membrane. Depth arrays of 8–12 recording contacts spaced at distances of 5 mm were implanted stereotactically, targeting HG. In some patients, additional arrays targeting the insula provided coverage of other sites in the superior temporal plane (Nagahama et al., 2018). A subgaleal contact served as a reference. Electrode placement was determined solely on the basis of clinical requirements, as determined by the neurosurgery and epileptology team (Nourski and Howard, 2015). Data acquisition was via a TDT RZ2 real-time processor (Tucker-Davis Technologies) for subjects B335 and L357, and via a NeuraLynx Atlas System (NeuraLynx) for the remaining subjects. Data were amplified, filtered (TDT: 0.7–800 Hz bandpass, 12 dB/octave roll-off; NeuraLynx: 0.1–500 Hz, 5 dB/octave roll-off, with the exception of subject R369 for whom filtering was at 0.8–800 Hz), and digitized with a sampling rate of 2034.5 Hz (TDT) or 2000 Hz (NeuraLynx).

Anatomical reconstruction of implanted electrode locations, mapping to a standardized coordinate space, and assignment to regions of interest were performed using FreeSurfer image analysis suite v5.3 (Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging; RRID:SCR_001847) and custom software, as described previously (Nourski et al., 2014). In brief, whole-brain high-resolution T1-weighted structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans (resolution and slice thickness ≤1.0 mm) were obtained from each subject before electrode implantation. After implantation, subjects underwent MRI and thin-slice volumetric computerized tomography (CT; resolution and slice thickness ≤1.0 mm) scanning. Electrode locations were initially extracted from post-implantation MRI and CT scans, then projected onto preoperative MRI scans using nonlinear three-dimensional thin-plate spline morphing, aided by intraoperative photographs. Where required, standard Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates were found for each contact using linear coregistration to the ICBM152 atlas, as implemented in FMRIB Software Library v5.0 (FMRIB Analysis Group; RRID:SCR_002823).

Data analysis.

Off-line data analysis was performed in MATLAB v2012b (MathWorks; RRID:SCR_001622) using the Fieldtrip Toolbox v20131231 (http://www.fieldtriptoolbox.org/; RRID:SCR_004849; Oostenveld et al., 2011) and custom MATLAB scripts. Power line noise was removed using a filter based on the demodulated band transform (Kovach and Gander, 2016). Data were then downsampled to 1000 Hz, and recordings divided into epochs from −1500 to 6300 ms relative to sentence onset.

Responses to click trains were investigated using intertrial phase coherence (ITPC; Lachaux et al., 1999), which indexes the strength with which neural activity synchronizes (phase locks) with temporally regular patterns in acoustic stimulation (Herrmann and Johnsrude, 2018; C. Keitel et al., 2019). To this end, a fast Fourier transform (including a Hann window taper and zero-padding) was calculated for frequencies ranging from 20 to 210 Hz using the data in the 0–0.2 s time window (i.e., the click-train duration). The resulting complex numbers were normalized by dividing each by its magnitude. ITPC was then calculated as the absolute value of the mean normalized complex number across trials. ITPC can take on values between 0 (no coherence) and 1 (maximum coherence). ITPC was calculated separately for each contact and repetition rate (50, 100, 125, 150, 200 Hz). We also calculated ITPC based on surrogate data to compare the empirically observed ITPC values against ITPC chance levels (Stam, 2005). In detail, time series data (0–0.2 s after stimulus onset) were converted to the frequency domain using an FFT; the phase of each frequency component was randomized, and the data were subsequently converted back to the time domain. ITPC was then calculated for the surrogate time series. Calculation of surrogate data and ITPC was repeated 100 times, and ITPC values were averaged subsequently, leading to a chance-level ITPC.

For the main experiment, time-frequency analysis of oscillatory activity was conducted separately for low-frequency (2–30 Hz) and high-frequency (40–180 Hz) activity using Morlet wavelets (Tallon-Baudry et al., 1996; Tallon-Baudry and Bertrand, 1999). In detail, for each sentence and contact, time-frequency representations were calculated for the −0.6 to 1.2 s time window relative to stimulus onset in 10 ms steps. We limited the endpoint of our analysis time window to 1.2 s to ensure that our analysis would be related to ongoing activity during stimulus presentation (1.223 s was the duration of the shortest sentence). For frequencies between 2 and 30 Hz (calculated in steps of 0.2 Hz), wavelet size linearly increased from 3 to 12 cycles as a function of frequency. For frequencies between 40 and 180 Hz (calculated in steps of 1 Hz), a wavelet size of 12 cycles was used uniformly. Power was calculated as the squared magnitude of the complex wavelet coefficients and averaged across trials. Time-frequency power was baseline-corrected by dividing the power at each time point by the mean power in the −0.6 to −0.1 s prestimulus time window, taking the logarithm (base 10), and multiplying the result by 10 (separately for each frequency). The result is power in decibel units, reflecting the signal change from baseline.

Functional localization.

Recording sites were identified as belonging to a primary region of interest in posteromedial HG (pmHG) if they showed significant phase-locked responses to 100 Hz click trains, and if averaged click-evoked potentials included short-latency (<20 ms) components (Brugge et al., 2009; Nourski et al., 2016). Other sites along the gyrus that did not demonstrate these properties were deemed to be in non-primary cortex and assigned to the anterolateral HG (alHG) region of interest.

Experimental design and statistical analysis.

Nonparametric Wilcoxon signed rank tests were calculated using the signrank function in MATLAB. False discovery rate was used to correct for multiple comparisons (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995; Genovese et al., 2002), if not indicated otherwise. Effect sizes are reported as re (requivalent; Rosenthal and Rubin, 2003), which is equivalent to a Pearson product-moment correlation for two continuous variables, to a point-biserial correlation for one continuous and one dichotomous variable, and to the square root of partial η2 for ANOVAs. Data are available on request. The experiment was not preregistered.

Software accessibility

Custom code is available on request.

Results

Functional segregation into primary and non-primary auditory cortex

A posteromedial-anterolateral boundary along HG between primary and non-primary auditory cortex was identified for each hemisphere (Fig. 1A), based on responses to click trains. As per our definition, phase-locking at 100–150 Hz click stimulation rates was significantly greater at posteromedial than at anterolateral contacts (Fig. 1B,C). It is worth re-emphasizing previous work showing that neural activity in cortical as well as subcortical brain regions synchronizes with such high stimulation rates (Nourski et al., 2013, 2016; Coffey et al., 2016; Holmes and Herrmann, 2017).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Anatomical and functional separation of primary and non-primary auditory cortex on HG. A, Recording contacts functionally identified as primary auditory cortex in pmHG are in purple and those functionally identified as non-primary auditory cortex in alHG are in green. Pairs of contacts circled in red for L335, L357, and R369 are those for which sample trials are plotted in Figure 2. Contacts outside of HG are not shown. The lateral surface of the superior temporal gyrus forms the lower bound of each image. The arrowhead indicates the anterior direction. The size of symbols depicting recording contacts has been increased for clarity. B, ITPC for two example click rates (100 and 150 Hz; median across hemispheres). C, ITPC at the neural frequencies corresponding to the click repetition rate, for the six different click rates. Data points reflect individual hemispheres, and box-and-whiskers indicate the semi-interquartile and full range. The horizontal red line close to zero indicates chance level. Asterisks below indicate a significant ITPC difference from chance level, and asterisks above reflect a significant difference between pmHG and alHG (p ≤ 0.05, FDR-corrected; n.s., not significant). These differences arise by definition: significant phase-locking at high stimulus rates was one of the criteria used to assign recording sites to areas. ITPC, intertrial phase coherence; pmHG, posteromedial Heschl's gyrus; alHG, anterolateral Heschl's gyrus.

Alpha-power suppression and high gamma-power enhancement dominate in anterolateral and posteromedial HG, respectively

Figure 2 shows activity from 1 s before sentence onset until 1 s after sentence onset for two exemplary trials at three pairs of contacts, each consisting of one pmHG and one alHG contact from a single subject and hemisphere. A striking feature is the prominent low-frequency prestimulus activity in alHG that is reduced following sentence onset. This observation is supported by a group analysis. Figure 3A displays time-frequency power for the low-frequency range (2–30 Hz) for pmHG and alHG, averaged across all recording sites. Motivated by previous research on alpha oscillatory activity in the temporal lobe, we focused on the 7–10 Hz frequency band (Tiihonen et al., 1991; Lehtelä et al., 1997); note however that low-frequency suppression was not limited to this band (Fig. 3A).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Exemplary single trial activity at pairs of pmHG and alHG contacts. Raw voltage traces from 1 s before sentence onset to 1 s after sentence onset for two representative trials as recorded in three hemispheres. For each hemisphere, traces at one pmHG and one alHG contact are plotted; these contacts are outlined in red in Figure 1A. Vertical line indicates stimulus onset.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Alpha power and high gamma power changes time-locked to sentence onset. A, Spectrogram (time-frequency) representations of power in the 2–30 Hz frequency range across all pmHG (left) and alHG (right) contacts in all hemispheres. White outlines indicate a significant difference from zero (p ≤ 0.05, uncorrected). B, Power in the alpha (7–10 Hz) range. Data points reflect individual hemispheres, and box-and-whiskers indicate the semi-interquartile and full range (*p ≤ 0.05). C, Alpha power time course (median across hemispheres; error bar reflects the semi-interquartile range). D, Low-frequency power spectrum (median across hemispheres; error bar reflects the semi-interquartile range). Black line marks significant differences between pmHG and alHG (p ≤ 0.05; FDR-corrected). E–H, Same as A–D but for the high-frequency range.

Alpha-power was significantly lower in alHG compared with pmHG (p = 0.008, re = 0.812; Fig. 3B). At the individual hemisphere level, this effect was significant in 7 of 8 cases (p ≤ 0.05, FDR-corrected; permutation procedure, where the pmHG and alHG labels were shuffled for single trials, with 1000 repetitions). The time course for the alpha frequency band was extracted and is shown in Figure 3C. Alpha-power suppression after sentence onset (relative to the prestimulus period) was significantly stronger in alHG compared with pmHG for the duration of the 0.5–1.2 s analysis window (p ≤ 0.05, FDR-corrected; Fig. 3C, black solid line). Note that although our analysis focused on 7–10 Hz, motivated by previous work and by the peak in the spectrum (Fig. 3D), the suppression of power in alHG relative to pmHG was also present for lower frequencies. To investigate whether the suppression of alpha power persisted for the duration of the sentences, which were of variable length, we also analyzed the data time-locked to sentence offset (baseline-corrected using the 0.1–0.6 s time window post-sentence offset). Alpha power (7–10 Hz; −1.2 to −0.5 s) remained significantly lower in alHG than in pmHG (p = 0.023, re = 0.737; not plotted) and was below the post-sentence baseline.

The pattern of activity across primary and non-primary cortex in the high gamma range had a different profile. Figure 3E–H shows that the strength of high gamma responses (70–150 Hz) to sentences was greater in pmHG compared with alHG (p = 0.023, re = 0.737) from shortly after stimulus onset, consistent with previous findings for click trains and single syllables (Brugge et al., 2009; Steinschneider et al., 2014).

Alpha-power suppression throughout superior temporal cortex decreases with distance from anterolateral HG

We wanted to establish whether alpha-power suppression decreases with distance from alHG throughout the superior temporal plane and superior temporal gyrus, which would suggest that the main local source of alpha oscillations is located in alHG. Figure 4A shows alpha power (0.5–1.2 s; 7–10 Hz) relative to the prestimulus time window for all superior-temporal-cortex contacts as a function of spatial (Euclidean) distance to the mean coordinate of each subject's alHG contacts. For each hemisphere, we fitted a linear function to the alpha-power values as a function of spatial distance from this point and tested the estimated linear coefficient against zero. Alpha-power suppression decreased as spatial distance from alHG increased (p = 0.023, re = 0.737), suggesting that the main source of auditory alpha-power suppression lies in alHG.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Spatial distribution of alpha-power suppression and high gamma-power enhancement throughout superior temporal cortex. A, Baseline-corrected alpha (7–10 Hz; 0.5–1.2 s) power for each contact as a function of distance from the mean coordinate of each subject's alHG contacts. The black line reflects the best-fitting linear function, estimated separately for each hemisphere. STG, superior temporal gyrus; STP, superior temporal plane. B, High gamma-power enhancement (70–150 Hz) and alpha-power suppression (7–10 Hz) mapped onto four equally spaced axial slices of a template brain. Data from the left hemisphere were project to the right hemisphere before averaging across subjects. High gamma power is greatest in posterior auditory areas, including on the lateral surface, whereas alpha power is suppressed most strongly in anterior areas.

To further visualize activity centers of alpha-power suppression and compare them to centers of high gamma power enhancement, alpha- and high gamma-power for each contact were projected onto a template brain (and left-hemisphere contacts were mapped onto the right hemisphere; smoothing 6 mm FWHM; Fig. 4B). High gamma-power enhancement (relative to baseline) was most prominent in posteromedial auditory cortex, but also present in the posterior part of the lateral surface of the temporal cortex. Alpha-power suppression (relative to baseline) was strongest in more anterior areas.

Alpha-power suppression in anterolateral HG is greater for clear than for noise-vocoded speech

Previous EEG work suggests that alpha-power suppression is stronger for clear compared with noise-vocoded speech (Obleser and Weisz, 2012). Estimated sources of this effect include extensive parieto-occipital and anterior temporal regions, but the resolution of this localization using scalp recordings is limited. Figure 5 shows that alpha-power suppression in the present data was larger for clear speech compared with noise-vocoded speech in alHG (p = 0.039, re = 0.692), but not in pmHG (p = 0.844, re = 0.077), although the stimulus by region interaction was not significant (p = 0.148, re = 0.523). This indicates that alpha suppression in at least one auditory cortical field is dependent on the spectral quality of the stimulus. At the individual hemisphere level, larger alpha-power suppression for clear speech in pmHG was not significant in any cases and in alHG was significant in 3 of 8 cases (p ≤ 0.05, FDR-corrected; permutation procedure, where the speech and noise labels were shuffled for single trials, with 1000 repetitions).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Low-frequency responses for clear speech compared with noise-vocoded speech. A, Spectrogram (time-frequency) representations of power in the 2–30 Hz frequency range across all pmHG (top) and alHG (bottom) contacts in all hemispheres for clear (left) and noise-vocoded (right) speech. B, Time courses of alpha (7–10 Hz) power. Boxplots show alpha power for the 0.5–1.2 s time interval. Data points reflect individual hemispheres, and box-and-whiskers indicate the semi-interquartile and full range. *p ≤ 0.05; n.s., not significant.

Prestimulus alpha power is greater in anterolateral than in posteromedial HG

Next, we examined the degree to which prestimulus power contributes to power differences between pmHG and alHG observed after stimulus onset. Power in the alpha-frequency band (7–10 Hz) was larger in alHG than in pmHG during the prestimulus time window (−0.6 to −0.1 s; p = 0.008, re = 0.812), whereas there was no difference in alpha power between alHG and pmHG for the poststimulus-onset time window (0.5–1.2 s; p = 0.742, re = 0.128; Fig. 6A). The baseline-corrected results presented in Figure 3B demonstrate that the interaction between time period and region was significant.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Contrast of pmHG versus alHG for non-baseline-corrected data. A, Spectrogram representation of the difference in low-frequency power between pmHG and alHG. Boxplots and individual data points reflect the power in the alpha (7–10 Hz) range for the prestimulus time window (−0.6 to −0.1 s) and the poststimulus-onset time window (0.5–1.2 s) for pmHG and alHG. B, Same as A, but for the high-frequency range, and the poststimulus-onset time window was 0–1.2 s. *p ≤ 0.05, n.s., not significant.

High gamma power (70–150 Hz) was larger in pmHG compared with alHG in both the prestimulus (−0.6 to −0.1 s; p = 0.039, re = 0.692) and in the poststimulus-onset (0–1.2 s; p = 0.039, re = 0.692) intervals (Fig. 6B). The baseline-corrected results presented in Figure 3F show that the time period by region interaction was also significant for high gamma power, with the difference in high gamma power between the two regions increasing after stimulus onset.

Alpha-power suppression in anterolateral HG reflects changes in both spectral slope and narrowband alpha power

Finally, we wanted to establish whether the suppressed low-frequency activity in alHG reflected a reduction in the strength of a narrowband oscillation that is present before stimulus onset, or could be better characterized as a change in the exponent χ (“slope”) of the scale-free 1/fχ component of the power spectrum. The latter may reflect properties of neural circuits distinct from those that generate narrowband oscillations (He, 2014; Gao, 2016; Podvalny et al., 2015; Voytek et al., 2015; Becker et al., 2018). The analyses described so far, in which power at each frequency was normalized by its value during a prestimulus baseline, cannot differentiate between these two contributions to neural power spectra.

To address this issue, for each hemisphere and region (pmHG, alHG; power averaged across contacts), we separately estimated the linear slope of the spectrum (on a log-log scale) and the residual narrowband oscillatory power after whitening by removal of the 1/fχ component (i.e., the slope). The slope of the spectrum was shallower during sound presentation compared with the prestimulus period in alHG (p = 0.039, re = 0.692), but not in pmHG (p = 0.742, re = 0.128; interaction: p = 0.008, re = 0.812; Fig. 7A), accounting for some proportion of the low-frequency power changes described earlier. Critically, an additional narrowband oscillatory peak at ∼8 Hz was present in the whitened power spectrum (i.e., after slope removal) in both regions during the prestimulus period (Fig. 7B). Power in the alpha (7–10 Hz) frequency band was larger in the prestimulus-onset time window (−0.6 to −0.1 s) compared with power during sound presentation (0.5–1.2 s) in pmHG (p = 0.023, re = 0.768) and alHG (p = 0.016, re = 0.768), and this effect was larger in alHG compared with pmHG (interaction: p = 0.023, re = 0.737; Fig. 7B).

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Separation of 1/fχ slope and narrowband oscillatory power. A, Low-frequency (2–30 Hz) power spectrum for pmHG and alHG during the prestimulus-onset (−0.6 to −0.1 s) and poststimulus-onset (0.5–1.2 s) time windows, plotted on a log-log scale. The dotted lines reflect the best linear fit to the log-transformed data (individual fits averaged across hemispheres). Box plots and individual data points show the slope of the best linear fit in the prestimulus and poststimulus time windows for the pmHG and alHG. B, Low-frequency (2–30 Hz) power spectrum after removal of the 1/fχ component for the pmHG and alHG during the prestimulus and poststimulus time windows, plotted on a linear scale. Boxplots and individual data points show the power in the 7–10 Hz band in the prestimulus and poststimulus time windows for pmHG and alHG after removal of the 1/fχ component. *p ≤ 0.05; n.s., not significant.

Discussion

Intracranial recordings revealed distinct profiles of spectral power across anatomically and functionally defined auditory cortical fields, both before, and during, auditory stimulation. Stimulus-related activity in the high gamma range was greater in pmHG (a primary field) than in alHG (a non-primary field; Brugge et al., 2009; Steinschneider et al., 2014). Lower frequency oscillations also differed across these areas, most markedly in the 7–10 Hz (“alpha”) range. In the absence of auditory stimulation (i.e., before stimulus onset), alpha power was stronger in alHG than in pmHG; this difference began to diminish following the onset of speech as alHG alpha power decreased. The suppression persisted until auditory stimulation ended, and could not be accounted for by a change in the exponent χ (“slope”) of the 1/fχ component of the spectrum alone (Gao, 2016; Podvalny et al., 2015).

It has been proposed that one function of alpha oscillations throughout the brain is to reduce cortical excitability, limiting spontaneous or task-irrelevant processing (Klimesch et al., 2007; Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010). One might expect such a diminution in activity to manifest as reduced high gamma power, a proxy for asynchronous neural spiking (Crone et al., 2001; Mukamel et al., 2005; Manning et al., 2009). The anatomical dissociation we observed between alpha-power suppression (more anterolateral) and high gamma-power increases (more posteromedial) implies that any such antagonistic relationship does not hold as a fixed ratio across all sites. One interpretation is that primary cortex is ready to process sensory input somewhat indiscriminately, reflected in low (but non-zero) spontaneous alpha activity, whereas non-primary areas are selectively engaged through suppression of ongoing alpha oscillations following the onset of stimuli with particular acoustic properties (de Pesters et al., 2016) or of greatest task-relevance (Gomez-Ramirez et al., 2011; Lakatos et al., 2016; Wöstmann et al., 2017). We observed such a dependency: alpha-power suppression for clear speech began earlier and was more pronounced than for spectrally degraded speech. Here we have shown that this effect, previously reported in scalp EEG (Obleser and Weisz, 2012), holds in non-primary auditory cortex. The extent to which the difference in alpha-power suppression arises directly from the acoustic properties of the stimuli as opposed to their intelligibility or the level of attentional resources deployed cannot be determined from the current data. Independent manipulations of these factors in future studies would be valuable in ascertaining the functional significance of local alpha power changes.

Alpha oscillations have also been linked with predictive coding accounts of brain function (Friston and Kiebel, 2009). Activity in low-frequency bands is thought to carry predictions of the causes of sensory data from higher to lower hierarchical levels (von Stein et al., 2000; Bastos et al., 2012; Fontolan et al., 2014; Chao et al., 2018), or to reflect the precision of such predictions (Bauer et al., 2014; Sedley et al., 2016; Auksztulewicz et al., 2017). The finding that alpha oscillations propagate from non-primary to primary visual cortex in monkeys (van Kerkoerle et al., 2014) and their dominance in infragranular layers containing dense descending projections (Maier et al., 2010; Buffalo et al., 2011; but see Haegens et al., 2015; Bastos et al., 2018; Bonaiuto et al., 2018; Halgren et al., 2018) are consistent with a feedback role. However, to adequately describe electrophysiological activity during auditory processing, predictive coding models will need to account for the differences we report between auditory fields both in baseline alpha power and in its stimulus-related suppression. Simultaneous laminar recordings from core and belt areas in nonhuman primates would be beneficial in isolating input and output layers at different hierarchical levels.

Linking the observed spectral signatures of primary and non-primary cortex to underlying cytoarchitectural, myeloarchitectural, and chemoarchitectural properties is complicated by the lack of a single agreed parcellation of the human superior temporal plane, and the variability of HG morphology across individuals (von Economo and Horn, 1930). Our pmHG sites probably belong to core regions termed Te1.1/Te1.0 (Morosan et al., 2001) or AI (Rivier and Clarke, 1997; Wallace et al., 2002) and the alHG sites to Te1.2 (Morosan et al., 2001) or ALA (Wallace et al., 2002). Regardless of nomenclature, the former likely had a more developed granular layer with denser myelinated input from thalamus, and the latter more extensive long-range connections to non-sensory cortices (Hackett et al., 2001, 2014; Munoz-Lopez et al., 2010). These properties may give rise to preferred timescales of activity corresponding to different spectral profiles (Honey et al., 2012; Murray et al., 2014). In terms of chemoarchitecture, the distribution of receptors and enzymes underlying cholinergic transmission in particular differs across the two fields (Hutsler and Gazzaniga, 1996; Zilles et al., 2002). This is relevant as the cholinergic system influences cellular excitability (Cox et al., 1994; Hsieh et al., 2000) as well as the stimulus coding capacity of a neural population (Minces et al., 2017; Schmitz and Duncan, 2018), and disrupting it pharmacologically can reduce alpha power (Osipova et al., 2003; Bauer et al., 2012; Eckart et al., 2016).

Alpha-power suppression was observed not only in HG. Proximal contacts on the superior temporal plane and gyrus showed similar activity patterns, with suppression decreasing with distance from the center of alHG. Whether these measurements reflect a single volume-conducted source of alpha activity or separate oscillators has yet to be established. A related observation is that the spectral profiles in posterolateral superior temporal gyrus and pmHG are more similar to each other than are profiles between the two subdivisions of HG. This is consistent with the identification of a posterolateral superior temporal auditory area with similar functional properties to and direct connections with core auditory cortex (Howard et al., 2000; Brugge et al., 2003; Nourski et al., 2013, 2014).

The dominance of posterior alpha sources in scalp recordings hinders detection of distinct alpha activity from the temporal lobe. Furthermore, alpha-power changes following presentation of brief auditory stimuli, such as clicks and syllables, are likely dominated by the broadband low-frequency component of the evoked response (also visible in Fig. 3). Such stimuli may be too short to elicit the sustained suppression we observed, which reached a maximum ∼500 ms after stimulus onset and continued for the duration of the sentence. Nonetheless, magnetoencephalography, which is more selectively sensitive to tangentially-oriented dipoles that dominate on the superior temporal plane than EEG (Hämäläinen et al., 1993), has revealed likely temporal-lobe alpha sources without establishing their precise location (Tiihonen et al., 1991; Lehtelä et al., 1997; Weisz et al., 2011; Leske et al., 2015; Magazzini et al., 2016). Depth electrode recordings from HG in humans are relatively rare, and analysis of such data has largely focused on the evoked LFP or on high gamma activity (Nourski and Howard, 2015). Relevant exceptions include a multicenter study of resting state oscillatory activity (Frauscher et al., 2018). In that study, alpha band peaks were present in activity from superior temporal gyrus but not HG, nor elsewhere on the posterior superior temporal plane. These findings are somewhat consistent with our prestimulus results but included little if any data from alHG. Another study, in which pmHG was sparsely sampled, did not consistently reveal changes in alpha activity in response to sound, although broadband low-frequency activity in one patient was suppressed in auditory association cortex on the lateral surface (Fontolan et al., 2014). Finally, Podvalny et al. (2015) noted stimulus-related alpha suppression beyond a 1/fχ slope change in intracranially-implanted auditory areas, but did not report anatomical details. One contribution of the current report is to functionally identify sites as primary versus non-primary auditory cortex in all subjects, based on known neurophysiological response properties.

For our main analyses we compared power prestimulus to that >500 ms after sentence onset, excluding transient evoked activity. We did not consider oscillatory phase, known to be important for perception (Busch et al., 2009; van Rullen, 2016) and possibly underpinning communication between brain regions (Palva and Palva, 2011). Indeed, a reduction in alpha power such as we observed in non-primary cortex may occur alongside increased inter-areal alpha-band phase synchrony that has been argued to support cognitive function such as working memory (Freunberger et al., 2008, 2009; Doesburg et al., 2009). A full account linking alpha activity in different cortical fields to auditory processing will need to take both power and phase into consideration.

In summary, we report strong evidence for a source of alpha oscillations in alHG that is suppressed during prolonged auditory stimulation. This suppression is more pronounced when subjects listen to clear than to degraded speech, and drops off with distance from alHG throughout superior temporal cortex. The suppression cannot be explained solely by a change in the exponent χ (“slope”) of the 1/fχ component of the spectrum, and has a different anatomical distribution to induced high gamma activity, which is strongest in pmHG. Theories concerning the generation and function of alpha oscillations should account for their differential manifestation and stimulus-sensitivity in primary and non-primary auditory cortex.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by NIH Grants R01-DC04290 and UL1RR024979, and by the Hoover Fund to M.A.H.; by CIHR Grant MOP 133450 and by the Canada First Research Excellence Fund (CFREF) award to BrainsCAN at Western University to A.J.B and I.S.J.; and by a BrainsCAN postdoctoral fellowship (CFREF) to B.H. We thank Haiming Chen for assistance with data collection, and Maria Chait, Tim Griffiths, and two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on previous versions of the paper.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Alexander J. Billig at ajbillig{at}gmail.com

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Auksztulewicz R,
    2. Barascud N,
    3. Cooray G,
    4. Nobre AC,
    5. Chait M,
    6. Friston K
    (2017) The cumulative effects of predictability on synaptic gain in the auditory processing stream. J Neurosci 37:6751–6760. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0291-17.2017 pmid:28607165
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Başar E,
    2. Güntekin B
    (2008) A review of brain oscillations in cognitive disorders and the role of neurotransmitters. Brain Res 1235:172–193. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.06.103 pmid:18640103
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Bastos AM,
    2. Usrey WM,
    3. Adams RA,
    4. Mangun GR,
    5. Fries P,
    6. Friston KJ
    (2012) Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding. Neuron 76:695–711. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.038 pmid:23177956
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Bastos AM,
    2. Loonis R,
    3. Kornblith S,
    4. Lundqvist M,
    5. Miller EK
    (2018) Laminar recordings in frontal cortex suggest distinct layers for maintenance and control of working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 115:1117–1122. doi:10.1073/pnas.1710323115 pmid:29339471
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Bauer M,
    2. Kluge C,
    3. Bach D,
    4. Bradbury D,
    5. Heinze HJ,
    6. Dolan RJ,
    7. Driver J
    (2012) Cholinergic enhancement of visual attention and neural oscillations in the human brain. Curr Biol 22:397–402. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.022 pmid:22305751
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Bauer M,
    2. Stenner MP,
    3. Friston KJ,
    4. Dolan RJ
    (2014) Attentional modulation of alpha/beta and gamma oscillations reflect functionally distinct processes. J Neurosci 34:16117–16125. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3474-13.2014 pmid:25429152
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    1. Becker R,
    2. Van de Ville D,
    3. Kleinschmidt A
    (2018) Alpha oscillations reduce temporal long-range dependence in spontaneous human brain activity. J Neurosci 38:755–764. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0831-17.2017 pmid:29167403
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Benjamini Y,
    2. Hochberg Y
    (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B 57:289–300.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Berger H
    (1931) Über das elektrenkephalogramm des menschen. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr 94:16–60. doi:10.1007/BF01835097
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  10. ↵
    1. Bonaiuto JJ,
    2. Meyer SS,
    3. Little S,
    4. Rossiter H,
    5. Callaghan MF,
    6. Dick F,
    7. Barnes GR,
    8. Bestmann S
    (2018) Laminar-specific cortical dynamics in human visual and sensorimotor cortices. eLife 7:e33977. doi:10.7554/eLife.33977 pmid:30346274
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Brugge JF,
    2. Volkov IO,
    3. Garell PC,
    4. Reale RA,
    5. Howard MA 3rd.
    (2003) Functional connections between auditory cortex on Heschl's gyrus and on the lateral superior temporal gyrus in humans. J Neurophysiol 90:3750–3763. doi:10.1152/jn.00500.2003 pmid:12968011
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Brugge JF,
    2. Volkov IO,
    3. Oya H,
    4. Kawasaki H,
    5. Reale RA,
    6. Fenoy A,
    7. Steinschneider M,
    8. Howard MA 3rd.
    (2008) Functional localization of auditory cortical fields of human: click-train stimulation. Hear Res 238:12–24. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2007.11.012 pmid:18207680
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Brugge JF,
    2. Nourski KV,
    3. Oya H,
    4. Reale RA,
    5. Kawasaki H,
    6. Steinschneider M,
    7. Howard MA 3rd.
    (2009) Coding of repetitive transients by auditory cortex on Heschl's gyrus. J Neurophysiol 102:2358–2374. doi:10.1152/jn.91346.2008 pmid:19675285
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Buffalo EA,
    2. Fries P,
    3. Landman R,
    4. Buschman TJ,
    5. Desimone R
    (2011) Laminar differences in gamma and alpha coherence in the ventral stream. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:11262–11267. doi:10.1073/pnas.1011284108 pmid:21690410
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Busch NA,
    2. Dubois J,
    3. VanRullen R
    (2009) The phase of ongoing EEG oscillations predicts visual perception. J Neurosci 29:7869–7876. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0113-09.2009 pmid:19535598
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Chao ZC,
    2. Takaura K,
    3. Wang L,
    4. Fujii N,
    5. Dehaene S
    (2018) Large-scale cortical networks for hierarchical prediction and prediction error in the primate brain. Neuron 100:1252–1266.e3. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.004 pmid:30482692
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Clarke S,
    2. Morosan P
    (2012) Architecture, connectivity, and transmitter receptors of human auditory cortex. In: The human auditory cortex, Ed 2 (Poeppel D, Overath T, Popper A, Fay R, eds), pp 11–38. New York: Springer.
  18. ↵
    1. Clayton MS,
    2. Yeung N,
    3. Cohen Kadosh R
    (2018) The many characters of visual alpha oscillations. Eur J Neurosci 48:2498–2508. doi:10.1111/ejn.13747 pmid:29044823
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Coffey EB,
    2. Herholz SC,
    3. Chepesiuk AM,
    4. Baillet S,
    5. Zatorre RJ
    (2016) Cortical contributions to the auditory frequency-following response revealed by MEG. Nat Commun 7:11070. doi:10.1038/ncomms11070 pmid:27009409
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Cox CL,
    2. Metherate R,
    3. Ashe JH
    (1994) Modulation of cellular excitability in neocortex: muscarinic receptor and second messenger-mediated actions of acetylcholine. Synapse 16:123–136. doi:10.1002/syn.890160206 pmid:7910986
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Crone NE,
    2. Miglioretti DL,
    3. Gordon B,
    4. Sieracki JM,
    5. Wilson MT,
    6. Uematsu S,
    7. Lesser RP
    (1998) Functional mapping of human sensorimotor cortex with electrocorticographic spectral analysis I. alpha and beta event-related desynchronization. Brain 121:2271–2299. doi:10.1093/brain/121.12.2271 pmid:9874480
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Crone NE,
    2. Boatman D,
    3. Gordon B,
    4. Hao L
    (2001) Induced electrocorticographic gamma activity during auditory perception. Clin Neurophysiol 112:565–582. doi:10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00545-9 pmid:11275528
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. de Pesters A,
    2. Coon WG,
    3. Brunner P,
    4. Gunduz A,
    5. Ritaccio AL,
    6. Brunet NM,
    7. de Weerd P,
    8. Roberts MJ,
    9. Oostenveld R,
    10. Fries P,
    11. Schalk G
    (2016) Alpha power indexes task-related networks on large and small scales: a multimodal ECoG study in humans and a non-human primate. Neuroimage 134:122–131. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.074 pmid:27057960
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Doesburg SM,
    2. Green JJ,
    3. McDonald JJ,
    4. Ward LM
    (2009) From local inhibition to long-range integration: a functional dissociation of alpha-band synchronization across cortical scales in visuospatial attention. Brain Res 1303:97–110. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.069 pmid:19782056
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Eckart C,
    2. Woźniak-Kwaśniewska A,
    3. Herweg NA,
    4. Fuentemilla L,
    5. Bunzeck N
    (2016) Acetylcholine modulates human working memory and subsequent familiarity based recognition via alpha oscillations. Neuroimage 137:61–69. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.049 pmid:27222217
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Fontolan L,
    2. Morillon B,
    3. Liégeois-Chauvel C,
    4. Giraud AL
    (2014) The contribution of frequency-specific activity to hierarchical information processing in human auditory cortices. Nat Commun 5:4694. doi:10.1038/ncomms5694 pmid:25178489
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Frauscher B,
    2. von Ellenrieder N,
    3. Zelmann R,
    4. Doležalová I,
    5. Minotti L,
    6. Olivier A,
    7. Hall J,
    8. Hoffmann D,
    9. Nguyen DK,
    10. Kahane P,
    11. Dubeau F,
    12. Gotman J
    (2018) Atlas of the normal intracranial electroencephalogram: neurophysiological awake activity in different cortical areas. Brain 141:1130–1144. doi:10.1093/brain/awy035 pmid:29506200
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Freunberger R,
    2. Klimesch W,
    3. Griesmayr B,
    4. Sauseng P,
    5. Gruber W
    (2008) Alpha phase coupling reflects object recognition. Neuroimage 42:928–935. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.020 pmid:18595738
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Freunberger R,
    2. Fellinger R,
    3. Sauseng P,
    4. Gruber W,
    5. Klimesch W
    (2009) Dissociation between phase-locked and nonphase-locked alpha oscillations in a working memory task. Hum Brain Mapp 30:3417–3425. doi:10.1002/hbm.20766 pmid:19384888
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Friston K,
    2. Kiebel S
    (2009) Predictive coding under the free-energy principle. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364:1211–1221. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0300 pmid:19528002
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Gao R
    (2016) Interpreting the electrophysiological power spectrum. J Neurophysiol 115:628–630. doi:10.1152/jn.00722.2015 pmid:26245320
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Genovese CR,
    2. Lazar NA,
    3. Nichols T
    (2002) Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage 15:870–878. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.1037 pmid:11906227
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Gomez-Ramirez M,
    2. Kelly SP,
    3. Molholm S,
    4. Sehatpour P,
    5. Schwartz TH,
    6. Foxe JJ
    (2011) Oscillatory sensory selection mechanisms during intersensory attention to rhythmic auditory and visual inputs: a human electrocorticographic investigation. J Neurosci 31:18556–18567. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2164-11.2011 pmid:22171054
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    1. Hackett TA,
    2. Preuss TM,
    3. Kaas JH
    (2001) Architectonic identification of the core region in auditory cortex of macaques, chimpanzees, and humans. J Comp Neurol 441:197–222. doi:10.1002/cne.1407 pmid:11745645
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Hackett TA,
    2. de la Mothe LA,
    3. Camalier CR,
    4. Falchier A,
    5. Lakatos P,
    6. Kajikawa Y,
    7. Scott BH,
    8. Schroeder CE
    (2014) Feedforward and feedback projections of caudal belt and parabelt areas of auditory cortex: refining the hierarchical model. Front Neurosci 8:72. doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00072 pmid:24795550
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Haegens S,
    2. Barczak A,
    3. Musacchia G,
    4. Lipton ML,
    5. Mehta AD,
    6. Lakatos P,
    7. Schroeder CE
    (2015) Laminar profile and physiology of the α rhythm in primary visual, auditory, and somatosensory regions of neocortex. J Neurosci 35:14341–14352. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0600-15.2015 pmid:26490871
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    1. Halgren M,
    2. Fabó D,
    3. Ulbert I,
    4. Madsen JR,
    5. Erőss L,
    6. Doyle WK,
    7. Devinsky O,
    8. Schomer D,
    9. Cash SS,
    10. Halgren E
    (2018) Superficial slow rhythms integrate cortical processing in humans. Sci Rep 8:2055. doi:10.1038/s41598-018-20662-0 pmid:29391596
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Hämäläinen MS,
    2. Hari R,
    3. Ilmoniemi RJ,
    4. Knuutila J,
    5. Lounasmaa OV
    (1993) Magnetoencephalography: theory, instrumentation, and applications to noninvasive studies of the working human brain. Rev Mod Phys 65:413–505. doi:10.1103/RevModPhys.65.413
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  39. ↵
    1. He BJ
    (2014) Scale-free brain activity: past, present, and future. Trends Cogn Sci 18:480–487. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.003 pmid:24788139
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Herrmann B,
    2. Johnsrude IS
    (2018) Neural signatures of the processing of temporal patterns in sound. J Neurosci 38:5466–5477. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0346-18.2018 pmid:29773757
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    1. Holmes E,
    2. Herrmann B
    (2017) Revisiting the contribution of auditory cortex to frequency-following responses. J Neurosci 37:5218–5220. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0794-17.2017 pmid:28539348
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  42. ↵
    1. Honey CJ,
    2. Thesen T,
    3. Donner TH,
    4. Silbert LJ,
    5. Carlson CE,
    6. Devinsky O,
    7. Doyle WK,
    8. Rubin N,
    9. Heeger DJ,
    10. Hasson U
    (2012) Slow cortical dynamics and the accumulation of information over long timescales. Neuron 76:423–434. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.011 pmid:23083743
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Hoogenboom N,
    2. Schoffelen JM,
    3. Oostenveld R,
    4. Parkes LM,
    5. Fries P
    (2006) Localizing human visual gamma-band activity in frequency, time and space. Neuroimage 29:764–773. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.043 pmid:16216533
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Howard MA,
    2. Volkov IO,
    3. Mirsky R,
    4. Garell PC,
    5. Noh MD,
    6. Granner M,
    7. Damasio H,
    8. Steinschneider M,
    9. Reale RA,
    10. Hind JE,
    11. Brugge JF
    (2000) Auditory cortex on the human posterior superior temporal gyrus. J Comp Neurol 416:79–92. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(20000103)416:1%3C79::AID-CNE6%3E3.0.CO;2-2 pmid:10578103
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Hsieh CY,
    2. Cruikshank SJ,
    3. Metherate R
    (2000) Differential modulation of auditory thalamocortical and intracortical synaptic transmission by cholinergic agonist. Brain Res 880:51–64. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02766-9 pmid:11032989
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Hutsler JJ,
    2. Gazzaniga MS
    (1996) Acetylcholinesterase staining in human auditory and language cortices: regional variation of structural features. Cereb Cortex 6:260–270. doi:10.1093/cercor/6.2.260 pmid:8670655
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Jensen O,
    2. Mazaheri A
    (2010) Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by inhibition. Front Hum Neurosci 4:186. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186 pmid:21119777
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Keitel A,
    2. Gross J
    (2016) Individual brain areas can be identified from their characteristic spectral activation fingerprints. PLoS Biol 14:e1002498. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002498 pmid:27355236
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Keitel C,
    2. Keitel A,
    3. Benwell CSY,
    4. Daube C,
    5. Thut G,
    6. Gross J
    (2019) Stimulus-driven brain rhythms within the alpha band: the attentional-modulation conundrum. J Neurosci 39:3119–3129. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1633-18.2019 pmid:30770401
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  50. ↵
    1. Klimesch W,
    2. Sauseng P,
    3. Hanslmayr S
    (2007) EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Res Rev 53:63–88. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003 pmid:16887192
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Kovach CK,
    2. Gander PE
    (2016) The demodulated band transform. J Neurosci Methods 261:135–154. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.12.004 pmid:26711370
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Lachaux JP,
    2. Rodriguez E,
    3. Martinerie J,
    4. Varela FJ
    (1999) Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum Brain Mapp 8:194–208. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4%3C194::AID-HBM4%3E3.0.CO;2-C pmid:10619414
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Lakatos P,
    2. Barczak A,
    3. Neymotin SA,
    4. McGinnis T,
    5. Ross D,
    6. Javitt DC,
    7. O'Connell MN
    (2016) Global dynamics of selective attention and its lapses in primary auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci 19:1707–1717. doi:10.1038/nn.4386 pmid:27618311
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Lehtelä L,
    2. Salmelin R,
    3. Hari R
    (1997) Evidence for reactive magnetic 10-hz rhythm in the human auditory cortex. Neurosci Lett 222:111–114. doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(97)13361-4 pmid:9111741
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Leske S,
    2. Ruhnau P,
    3. Frey J,
    4. Lithari C,
    5. Müller N,
    6. Hartmann T,
    7. Weisz N
    (2015) Prestimulus network integration of auditory cortex predisposes near-threshold perception independently of local excitability. Cereb Cortex 25:4898–4907. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv212 pmid:26408799
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Liégeois-Chauvel C,
    2. Musolino A,
    3. Chauvel P
    (1991) Localization of the primary auditory area in man. Brain 114:139–151. pmid:1900211
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Magazzini L,
    2. Ruhnau P,
    3. Weisz N
    (2016) Alpha suppression and connectivity modulations in left temporal and parietal cortices index partial awareness of words. Neuroimage 133:279–287. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.025 pmid:27001501
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Maier A,
    2. Adams GK,
    3. Aura C,
    4. Leopold DA
    (2010) Distinct superficial and deep laminar domains of activity in the visual cortex during rest and stimulation. Front Syst Neurosci 4:31. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2010.00031 pmid:20802856
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Manning JR,
    2. Jacobs J,
    3. Fried I,
    4. Kahana MJ
    (2009) Broadband shifts in local field potential power spectra are correlated with single-neuron spiking in humans. J Neurosci 29:13613–13620. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2041-09.2009 pmid:19864573
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  60. ↵
    1. Minces V,
    2. Pinto L,
    3. Dan Y,
    4. Chiba AA
    (2017) Cholinergic shaping of neural correlations. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 114:5725–5730. doi:10.1073/pnas.1621493114 pmid:28507133
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  61. ↵
    1. Morosan P,
    2. Rademacher J,
    3. Schleicher A,
    4. Amunts K,
    5. Schormann T,
    6. Zilles K
    (2001) Human primary auditory cortex: cytoarchitectonic subdivisions and mapping into a spatial reference system. Neuroimage 13:684–701. doi:10.1006/nimg.2000.0715 pmid:11305897
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Mukamel R,
    2. Gelbard H,
    3. Arieli A,
    4. Hasson U,
    5. Fried I,
    6. Malach R
    (2005) Coupling between neuronal firing, field potentials, and fMR1 in human auditory cortex. Science 309:951–954. doi:10.1126/science.1110913 pmid:16081741
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  63. ↵
    1. Munoz-Lopez MM,
    2. Mohedano-Moriano A,
    3. Insausti R
    (2010) Anatomical pathways for auditory memory in primates. Front Neuroanat 4:129. doi:10.3389/fnana.2010.00129 pmid:20976037
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Murray JD,
    2. Bernacchia A,
    3. Freedman DJ,
    4. Romo R,
    5. Wallis JD,
    6. Cai X,
    7. Padoa-Schioppa C,
    8. Pasternak T,
    9. Seo H,
    10. Lee D,
    11. Wang XJ
    (2014) A hierarchy of intrinsic timescales across primate cortex. Nat Neurosci 17:1661–1663. doi:10.1038/nn.3862 pmid:25383900
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Nagahama Y,
    2. Schmitt AJ,
    3. Dlouhy BJ,
    4. Vesole AS,
    5. Gander PE,
    6. Kovach CK,
    7. Nakagawa D,
    8. Granner MA,
    9. Howard MA,
    10. Kawasaki H
    (2018) Utility and safety of depth electrodes within the supratemporal plane for intracranial EEG. J Neurosurg 131:772–780. doi:10.3171/2018.4.JNS171812 pmid:30192197
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Niedermeyer E
    (1990) Alpha-like rhythmical activity of the temporal lobe. Clin Electroencephalogr 21:210–224. doi:10.1177/155005949002100410 pmid:2225471
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Nourski KV,
    2. Howard MA
    (2015) Invasive recordings in the human auditory cortex. In: handbook of clinical neurology: the human auditory system (Aminoff MJ, Boller F, Swaab DF, eds), pp 225–244. Philadelphia: Elsevier.
  68. ↵
    1. Nourski KV,
    2. Brugge JF,
    3. Reale RA,
    4. Kovach CK,
    5. Oya H,
    6. Kawasaki H,
    7. Jenison RL,
    8. Howard MA 3rd.
    (2013) Coding of repetitive transients by auditory cortex on posterolateral superior temporal gyrus in humans: an intracranial electrophysiology study. J Neurophysiol 109:1283–1295. doi:10.1152/jn.00718.2012 pmid:23236002
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Nourski KV,
    2. Steinschneider M,
    3. McMurray B,
    4. Kovach CK,
    5. Oya H,
    6. Kawasaki H,
    7. Howard MA 3rd.
    (2014) Functional organization of human auditory cortex: investigation of response latencies through direct recordings. Neuroimage 101:598–609. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.004 pmid:25019680
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Nourski KV,
    2. Steinschneider M,
    3. Rhone AE
    (2016) Electrocorticographic activation within human auditory cortex during dialog-based language and cognitive testing. Front Hum Neurosci 10:202. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2016.00202 pmid:27199720
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Obleser J,
    2. Weisz N
    (2012) Suppressed alpha oscillations predict intelligibility of speech and its acoustic details. Cereb Cortex 22:2466–2477. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr325 pmid:22100354
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Oostenveld R,
    2. Fries P,
    3. Maris E,
    4. Schoffelen JM
    (2011) FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput Intell Neurosci 2011:156869. doi:10.1155/2011/156869 pmid:21253357
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Osipova D,
    2. Ahveninen J,
    3. Kaakkola S,
    4. Jääskeläinen IP,
    5. Huttunen J,
    6. Pekkonen E
    (2003) Effects of scopolamine on MEG spectral power and coherence in elderly subjects. Clin Neurophysiol 114:1902–1907. doi:10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00165-2 pmid:14499752
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Palva JM,
    2. Monto S,
    3. Kulashekhar S,
    4. Palva S
    (2010) Neuronal synchrony reveals working memory networks and predicts individual memory capacity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:7580–7585. doi:10.1073/pnas.0913113107 pmid:20368447
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  75. ↵
    1. Palva S,
    2. Palva JM
    (2011) Functional roles of alpha-band phase synchronization in local and large-scale cortical networks. Front Psychol 2:204. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00204 pmid:21922012
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Podvalny E,
    2. Noy N,
    3. Harel M,
    4. Bickel S,
    5. Chechik G,
    6. Schroeder CE,
    7. Mehta AD,
    8. Tsodyks M,
    9. Malach R
    (2015) A unifying principle underlying the extracellular field potential spectral responses in the human cortex. J Neurophysiol 114:505–519. doi:10.1152/jn.00943.2014 pmid:25855698
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Ramot M,
    2. Fisch L,
    3. Harel M,
    4. Kipervasser S,
    5. Andelman F,
    6. Neufeld MY,
    7. Kramer U,
    8. Fried I,
    9. Malach R
    (2012) A widely distributed spectral signature of task-negative electrocorticography responses revealed during a visuomotor task in the human cortex. J Neurosci 32:10458–10469. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0877-12.2012 pmid:22855795
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  78. ↵
    1. Rivier F,
    2. Clarke S
    (1997) Cytochrome oxidase, acetylcholinesterase, and NADPH-diaphorase staining in human supratemporal and insular cortex: evidence for multiple auditory areas. Neuroimage 6:288–304. doi:10.1006/nimg.1997.0304 pmid:9417972
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Rosenthal R,
    2. Rubin DB
    (2003) r(equivalent): a simple effect size indicator. Psychol Methods 8:492–496. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.8.4.492 pmid:14664684
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Schmitz TW,
    2. Duncan J
    (2018) Normalization and the cholinergic microcircuit: a unified basis for attention. Trends Cogn Sci 22:422–437. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2018.02.011 pmid:29576464
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    1. Sedley W,
    2. Gander PE,
    3. Kumar S,
    4. Kovach CK,
    5. Oya H,
    6. Kawasaki H,
    7. Howard MA,
    8. Griffiths TD
    (2016) Neural signatures of perceptual inference. eLife 5:e11476. doi:10.7554/eLife.11476 pmid:26949254
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    1. Shannon RV,
    2. Zeng FG,
    3. Kamath V,
    4. Wygonski J,
    5. Ekelid M
    (1995) Speech recognition with primarily temporal cues. Science 270:303–304. doi:10.1126/science.270.5234.303 pmid:7569981
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  83. ↵
    1. Stam CJ
    (2005) Nonlinear dynamical analysis of EEG and MEG: review of an emerging field. Clin Neurophysiol 116:2266–2301. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2005.06.011 pmid:16115797
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  84. ↵
    1. Steinschneider M,
    2. Fishman YI,
    3. Arezzo JC
    (2008) Spectrotemporal analysis of evoked and induced electroencephalographic responses in primary auditory cortex (A1) of the awake monkey. Cereb Cortex 18:610–625. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm094 pmid:17586604
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  85. ↵
    1. Steinschneider M,
    2. Nourski KV,
    3. Rhone AE,
    4. Kawasaki H,
    5. Oya H,
    6. Howard MA 3rd.
    (2014) Differential activation of human core, non-core and auditory-related cortex during speech categorization tasks as revealed by intracranial recordings. Front Neurosci 8:240. pmid:25157216
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  86. ↵
    1. Tallon-Baudry C,
    2. Bertrand O
    (1999) Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its role in object representation. Trends Cogn Sci 3:151–162. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01299-1 pmid:10322469
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  87. ↵
    1. Tallon-Baudry C,
    2. Bertrand O,
    3. Delpuech C,
    4. Pernier J
    (1996) Stimulus specificity of phase-locked and non-phase-locked 40 hz visual responses in human. J Neurosci 16:4240–4249. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-13-04240.1996 pmid:8753885
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  88. ↵
    1. Tiihonen J,
    2. Hari R,
    3. Kajola M,
    4. Karhu J,
    5. Ahlfors S,
    6. Tissari S
    (1991) Magnetoencephalographic 10-hz rhythm from the human auditory cortex. Neurosci Lett 129:303–305. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(91)90486-D pmid:1745412
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  89. ↵
    1. van Kerkoerle T,
    2. Self MW,
    3. Dagnino B,
    4. Gariel-Mathis MA,
    5. Poort J,
    6. van der Togt C,
    7. Roelfsema PR
    (2014) Alpha and gamma oscillations characterize feedback and feedforward processing in monkey visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111:14332–14341. doi:10.1073/pnas.1402773111 pmid:25205811
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  90. ↵
    1. Van Rullen R
    (2016) Perceptual cycles. Trends Cogn Sci 20:723–735. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.07.006 pmid:27567317
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  91. ↵
    1. von Economo C,
    2. Horn L
    (1930) Über windungsrelief, Maβe und Rindenarchitektonik der Supratemporalfläche, ihre individuellen und ihre Seitenunterschiede. Zeitschrift für die gesamte Neurol und Psychiatr 130:678–757. doi:10.1007/BF02865945
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  92. ↵
    1. von Stein A,
    2. Chiang C,
    3. König P
    (2000) Top-down processing mediated by interareal synchronization. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:14748–14753. doi:10.1073/pnas.97.26.14748 pmid:11121074
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  93. ↵
    1. Voytek B,
    2. Kramer MA,
    3. Case J,
    4. Lepage KQ,
    5. Tempesta ZR,
    6. Knight RT,
    7. Gazzaley A
    (2015) Age-related changes in 1/f neural electrophysiological noise. J Neurosci 35:13257–13265. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2332-14.2015 pmid:26400953
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  94. ↵
    1. Wallace MN,
    2. Johnston PW,
    3. Palmer AR
    (2002) Histochemical identification of cortical areas in the auditory region of the human brain. Exp Brain Res 143:499–508. doi:10.1007/s00221-002-1014-z pmid:11914796
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  95. ↵
    1. Weisz N,
    2. Hartmann T,
    3. Müller N,
    4. Lorenz I,
    5. Obleser J
    (2011) Alpha rhythms in audition: cognitive and clinical perspectives. Front Psychol 2:73. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00073 pmid:21687444
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  96. ↵
    1. Wild CJ,
    2. Davis MH,
    3. Johnsrude IS
    (2012) Human auditory cortex is sensitive to the perceived clarity of speech. Neuroimage 60:1490–1502. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.035 pmid:22248574
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  97. ↵
    1. Wöstmann M,
    2. Lim SJ,
    3. Obleser J
    (2017) The human neural alpha response to speech is a proxy of attentional control. Cereb Cortex 27:3307–3317. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhx074 pmid:28334352
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  98. ↵
    1. Zilles K,
    2. Palomero-Gallagher N,
    3. Grefkes C,
    4. Scheperjans F,
    5. Boy C,
    6. Amunts K,
    7. Schleicher A
    (2002) Architectonics of the human cerebral cortex and transmitter receptor fingerprints: reconciling functional neuroanatomy and neurochemistry. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 12:587–599. doi:10.1016/S0924-977X(02)00108-6 pmid:12468022
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 39 (44)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 39, Issue 44
30 Oct 2019
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
A Sound-Sensitive Source of Alpha Oscillations in Human Non-Primary Auditory Cortex
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
A Sound-Sensitive Source of Alpha Oscillations in Human Non-Primary Auditory Cortex
Alexander J. Billig, Björn Herrmann, Ariane E. Rhone, Phillip E. Gander, Kirill V. Nourski, Beau F. Snoad, Christopher K. Kovach, Hiroto Kawasaki, Matthew A. Howard III, Ingrid S. Johnsrude
Journal of Neuroscience 30 October 2019, 39 (44) 8679-8689; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0696-19.2019

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
A Sound-Sensitive Source of Alpha Oscillations in Human Non-Primary Auditory Cortex
Alexander J. Billig, Björn Herrmann, Ariane E. Rhone, Phillip E. Gander, Kirill V. Nourski, Beau F. Snoad, Christopher K. Kovach, Hiroto Kawasaki, Matthew A. Howard III, Ingrid S. Johnsrude
Journal of Neuroscience 30 October 2019, 39 (44) 8679-8689; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0696-19.2019
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • alpha
  • auditory cortex
  • functional inhibition
  • high gamma
  • intracranial
  • speech

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Brain-behavior differences in pre-modern and modern lineages of domestic dogs
  • Microglia supports both the singular form of LTP expressed by the lateral perforant path and episodic memory
  • Inhibition of Rho-associated kinases ROCK1 and ROCK2 as a Therapeutic Strategy to Reactivate the Repressed FXN Gene in Friedreich Ataxia
Show more Research Articles

Systems/Circuits

  • Functional Roles of Gastrin-Releasing Peptide-Producing Neurons in the Suprachiasmatic Nucleus: Insights into Photic Entrainment and Circadian Regulation
  • Macroscale Traveling Waves Evoked by Single-Pulse Stimulation of the Human Brain
  • Multimodal Correspondence between Optogenetic fMRI, Electrophysiology, and Anatomical Maps of the Secondary Somatosensory Cortex in Nonhuman Primates
Show more Systems/Circuits
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.