Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Viewpoints

Synthesis of Hemispheric ITD Tuning from the Readout of a Neural Map: Commonalities of Proposed Coding Schemes in Birds and Mammals

Jose L. Peña, Fanny Cazettes, Michael V. Beckert and Brian J. Fischer
Journal of Neuroscience 13 November 2019, 39 (46) 9053-9061; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0873-19.2019
Jose L. Peña
1Dominick P. Purpura Department of Neuroscience, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York, New York 10461,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Fanny Cazettes
2Champalimaud Neuroscience Programme, Champalimaud Center for the Unknown, 1400-038, Lisbon, Portugal, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Fanny Cazettes
Michael V. Beckert
1Dominick P. Purpura Department of Neuroscience, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, New York, New York 10461,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Michael V. Beckert
Brian J. Fischer
3Department of Mathematics, Seattle University, Seattle, Washington 98122
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Brian J. Fischer
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

A major cue to infer sound direction is the difference in arrival time of the sound at the left and right ears, called interaural time difference (ITD). The neural coding of ITD and its similarity across species have been strongly debated. In the barn owl, an auditory specialist relying on sound localization to capture prey, ITDs within the physiological range determined by the head width are topographically represented at each frequency. The topographic representation suggests that sound direction may be inferred from the location of maximal neural activity within the map. Such topographical representation of ITD, however, is not evident in mammals. Instead, the preferred ITD of neurons in the mammalian brainstem often lies outside the physiological range and depends on the neuron's best frequency. Because of these disparities, it has been assumed that how spatial hearing is achieved in birds and mammals is fundamentally different. However, recent studies reveal ITD responses in the owl's forebrain and midbrain premotor area that are consistent with coding schemes proposed in mammals. Particularly, sound location in owls could be decoded from the relative firing rates of two broadly and inversely ITD-tuned channels. This evidence suggests that, at downstream stages, the code for ITD may not be qualitatively different across species. Thus, while experimental evidence continues to support the notion of differences in ITD representation across species and brain regions, the latest results indicate notable commonalities, suggesting that codes driving orienting behavior in mammals and birds may be comparable.

Introduction

A sound originating at a specific location horizontally away from the midline of the head will reach the two ears at different times. The timing primarily depends on both the sound location relative to the two ears and the distance between the ears. The binaural difference in the arrival time of the sound, referred to as interaural time difference (ITD), is a major cue for sound localization. Since the original proposal by Jeffress (1948) of a neural circuit to account for human sound localization, the neural representation of ITD has been the source of much debate (Schnupp and Carr, 2009; Grothe et al., 2010; Ashida and Carr, 2011). Jeffress (1948) proposed that the auditory system contains a circuit consisting of delay lines and coincidence detector neurons that creates a map of ITD across the physiological range, which is the range of ITD naturally encountered given the spacing of the ears. A topographical arrangement of ITD-sensitive neurons consistent with the Jeffress model has been observed in the avian brainstem (Sullivan and Konishi, 1986; Carr and Konishi, 1990; Peña et al., 2001; Köppl and Carr, 2008; Carr et al., 2015). However, such a topographic arrangement of ITD-sensitive neurons has not been found in mammals. This discrepancy has led to a prevalent view that the ITD code supporting sound localization is fundamentally different in birds and mammals (Schnupp and Carr, 2009; Grothe et al., 2010; Ashida and Carr, 2011; Palanca-Castan and Köppl, 2015).

In both birds and mammals, the timing of acoustic signals is sensed by receptor cells of the cochlea and transmitted to the cochlear nuclei through the auditory nerve fibers. ITD is first detected by coincidence detector neurons receiving bilateral projections from the cochlear nuclei, located in the nucleus laminaris in birds and alligators (Carr and Konishi, 1990; Köppl and Carr, 2008; Kettler and Carr, 2019) and the medial superior olive (MSO) in mammals (Goldberg and Brown, 1969; Yin and Chan, 1990; Spitzer and Semple, 1995). Information about ITD is conveyed to the inferior colliculus (IC) across vertebrate species.

Qualitative differences between mammals and birds have been reported in central regions of the IC and in brainstem ITD-detector neurons (McAlpine et al., 2001; Brand et al., 2002; Joris et al., 2006). Neurons in the central regions of the IC and brainstem ITD-detector neurons are narrowly tuned to frequency in both birds and mammals, but ITD tuning differs, as shown in Figure 1. In birds, IC (Wagner et al., 2007) and brainstem ITD-detector cells (Carr and Konishi, 1990; Köppl and Carr, 2008) are largely tuned to ITDs across the physiological range for all sound frequencies, suggesting a labeled line code where sound location is inferred from the location in the brain where ITD-selective cells are active (i.e., a place code) (Fig. 1A). In contrast, ITD-tuned neurons in the mammalian IC and MSO, especially those recorded in smaller rodents and tuned to low frequencies, are tuned to ITDs outside the physiological range (McAlpine et al., 2001; Brand et al., 2002; Grothe et al., 2010), and ITD tuning in the MSO and IC of mammals is markedly frequency-dependent: neurons tuned to lower frequencies respond best to larger ITDs than neurons tuned to higher frequencies (McAlpine et al., 2001; Hancock and Delgutte, 2004; Day and Semple, 2011; Bremen and Joris, 2013). These differences across species have led to the proposition that there are divergent codes for converting ITD to spatial position (Schnupp and Carr, 2009; Grothe et al., 2010). Instead of a place code, it has been hypothesized that mammals might use an opponent-channel code for detecting the location of sounds across the horizontal plane. In the opponent-channel code, neurons respond best to sounds in the contralateral hemifield and have ITD tuning curves with peaks outside the physiological range. Additionally, their responses change most rapidly for sounds arising near the midline, leading to a region of highest sensitivity in the front. This type of tuning to ITD is referred to as hemispheric tuning. It has been proposed that sound location can be decoded from the relative firing rates or the pattern of firing rates in two opposed hemispheric tuned channels created by populations of ITD-sensitive neurons in the two brain hemispheres (Fig. 1B) (McAlpine et al., 2001; Harper and McAlpine, 2004; Grothe et al., 2010; Lüling et al., 2011).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Postulated ITD coding schemes in birds and mammals. A, Top, In the owl's midbrain space map, binaural neurons are sharply tuned to different ITDs (indicated by different colors) within the physiological range given by the head size (dashed lines). Bottom, This organization suggests a labeled-line code for sound localization, where the position of maximal activity within the map represents the source location. B, Top, ITD tuning of two hypothetical hemispheric populations tuned to ITDs within the contralateral space in the mammalian brain. The steepest slopes of their broad ITD tuning curves are positioned within the physiological range (dashed lines), but peak responses may be positioned beyond that range. Bottom, The relative activation of the two brain hemispheres provides an opponent-channel code for sound source location (McAlpine and Grothe, 2003). C, Population vector readout of the owl's midbrain space map. Each neuron in the space map is represented by a vector pointing toward its preferred direction (colored arrows). The overrepresentation of frontal space (dashed line) drives the resultant vector slightly to the front of the actual location, matching the underestimation of sound sources observed across species. Data from Fischer and Peña (2011).

As we discuss later, different explanations have been proposed for the frequency dependence of ITD tuning in mammals. In the opponent-channel code scheme, the dependency between ITD and frequency tuning observed in the mammalian MSO and IC may be fundamental for achieving high spatial acuity near the midline by positioning the steepest slopes of ITD-response functions over this region across neurons tuned to different frequencies (McAlpine et al., 2001; Harper and McAlpine, 2004; Schnupp and Carr, 2009). Complementary to the issue of decoding sound direction, recent theoretical work based on spike-timing-dependent plasticity underlying the emergence of ITD selectivity detector neurons suggests that Hebbian learning may explain the observed relationship between ITD and frequency tuning (Fontaine and Brette, 2011). The coding principles underlying the frequency dependence of ITD tuning in mammals remains an open question.

In this review, we first provide a panoramic view of the literature describing spatial tuning of midbrain and forebrain neurons in mammalian and avian species. Next, we discuss recent findings in the avian auditory system that reveal similarities with coding schemes proposed for mammals. We then discuss how this coding emerges from a midbrain representation historically believed to be qualitatively different between birds and mammals. Finally, we conclude providing a view that highlights similarities, yet considers differences across species.

Midbrain space maps for gaze orientation

In barn owls, a map of auditory space emerges in the external nucleus of the IC (ICx), which is directly downstream of the central IC. The owl's midbrain map of auditory space and the neural processing leading to it have influenced the field for decades, due to its computational elegance and profound implications for neural coding (Moiseff and Konishi, 1981, 1983; Takahashi et al., 1984; Takahashi and Konishi, 1986; Moiseff, 1989; Carr and Konishi, 1990; Peña and Konishi, 2001; Fischer and Peña, 2011). The emergence of spatial tuning in neurons of the owl's space map relies on the combination selectivity to ITD and an additional binaural cue, the interaural level difference (ILD) (Moiseff and Konishi, 1983; Peña and Konishi, 2001). The combination selectivity to ITD and ILD results from a multiplicative integration of the tunings to ITD and ILD (Peña and Konishi, 2001). The emergence of spatial tuning in ICx also depends on convergence across frequency, which resolves the ambiguity in responses of ITD-tuned neurons at high frequencies (Takahashi and Konishi, 1986; Mazer, 1998; Peña and Konishi, 2000). The optic tectum (OT) receives input from ICx and has a similar map of auditory space (Knudsen and Konishi, 1978; Knudsen, 1982). The OT, called the superior colliculus (SC) in mammals, represents the initial hub of the midbrain selection network (Knudsen, 2018). This laminar structure of space-specific neurons sends output to the forebrain (Wurtz et al., 2005; Knudsen, 2018) and to the midbrain premotor nuclei that mediate ballistic-orienting movements (du Lac and Knudsen, 1990; Masino, 1992). The topographic organization of spatial tuning observed in the owl's ICx and OT has historically suggested a local labeled-line coding, where spatial positions are inferred from the location of the peak activity of single or few ITD-sensitive neurons (Fig. 1A) (Konishi, 2003; Harper and McAlpine, 2004; Köppl and Carr, 2008; Schnupp and Carr, 2009).

In mammals, tuning to multiple sound localization cues that are associated with spatial locations emerges in the brachium of the IC (Slee and Young, 2014) where there is a coarse map of azimuth (i.e., horizontal coordinate) (Schnupp and King, 1997). There is some similarity with ICx in the barn owl in that neurons in the brachium of the IC are selective for multiple sound localization cues across frequency (Slee and Young, 2013, 2014). However, neurons in the brachium of the IC have additive, not multiplicative, tuning to ITD and ILD (Slee and Young, 2014). Additionally, their responses are dominated by ILD tuning, with ITD playing a weak role in spatial tuning (Slee and Young, 2013). An auditory space map of varied coarseness across species has been reported in the SC (Palmer and King, 1982; Middlebrooks and Knudsen, 1984; King, 1993; King et al., 1996; Gaese and Johnen, 2000; Lee and Groh, 2014). The SC receives multimodal sensory inputs (Meredith and Stein, 1983), including a space-selective one from the brachium of the IC, but shows a clearer topographic representation of space, suggesting further refinement of spatial tuning in this region (Schnupp and King, 1997). While there are differences in the characteristics and synthesis of the midbrain space maps between barn owls and mammals, the combination of multiple sound localization cues to support direction of gaze is a common underlying mechanism.

Spatial tuning in the auditory cortex

Differences between avian and mammalian ITD coding have focused on the topographical representation of ITD in birds and the opponent-channel hemispheric representation in mammals. However, studies of spatial tuning in the mammalian auditory cortex have described a range of representations of sound location, not always consistent with the opponent-channel theory. While there is evidence in cats and primates of coarse spatial selectivity to peripheral locations in the contralateral space (Middlebrooks and Pettigrew, 1981; Middlebrooks et al., 1994, 1998; Brugge et al., 1996; Woods et al., 2006) that is consistent with the opponent-channel code, there are also reports of more heterogeneous cortical representations, with neurons tuned not only to peripheral, but also to frontal auditory space (Middlebrooks and Pettigrew, 1981; Stecker et al., 2003; Woods et al., 2006; Lee and Middlebrooks, 2013; Remington and Wang, 2019). Studies in humans, both physiological (Magezi and Krumbholz, 2010; Salminen et al., 2010; Briley et al., 2013) and psychophysical (Boehnke and Phillips, 1999; Phillips and Hall, 2005; Vigneault-MacLean et al., 2007), also support the idea of the opponent-channel model with two opposing broadly tuned sets of neurons encoding azimuthal space, consistent with studies in nonhuman mammals (McAlpine et al., 2001; Stecker et al., 2005; Werner-Reiss and Groh, 2008). However, mammalian physiological studies have reported a substantial number of neurons in the auditory cortex displaying sensitivity to sound location near the midline at lower sound levels (Zhang et al., 2004; Higgins et al., 2010; Razak and Fuzessery, 2010; Zhou and Wang, 2012; Lee and Middlebrooks, 2013; Belliveau et al., 2014). Similarly, EEG studies in humans have provided evidence of a midline spatial channel in the auditory cortex (Briley et al., 2016), suggesting that the human brain may contain a representation dedicated specifically to azimuth positions near the midline. Furthermore, studies in several mammalian species have shown that unilateral lesions and cooling of cortical and subcortical areas induce localization deficits restricted to the contralateral space (Jenkins and Masterton, 1982; Thompson and Cortez, 1983; Kavanagh and Kelly, 1987; Malhotra et al., 2008; Wood et al., 2017). These findings indicate that one side of the brain is necessary and sufficient to localize sound in the opposite side of space. Although not inconsistent with data showing left and right broadly tuned channels, they are inconsistent with the hypothesis that localization depends on the comparison of activity between the two sides of the brain, and have therefore motivated sound localization models that depart from the opponent-channel theory, including a third channel representing midline locations in models of azimuth localization (Dingle et al., 2010) or a labeled line code (Belliveau et al., 2014).

Like studies that investigated spatial tuning of cortical neurons along the azimuth, the examination of ITD tuning in the auditory cortex has produced conflicting results. While some studies in mammals, including humans, reported preferred ITDs corresponding to sounds arising far within the contralateral hemifield (Reale and Brugge, 1990; Magezi and Krumbholz, 2010; Salminen et al., 2010), others reported preferred ITDs largely covering the physiological range (Fitzpatrick et al., 2000; Scott et al., 2009; Belliveau et al., 2014). The nature of the ITD code supporting sound localization in the cortex may still be more complex than the two-channel, three-channel, or distributed coding schemes, in light of reported changes in temporal spiking pattern with sound location (Mickey and Middlebrooks, 2003) and task dependency of spatial tuning (Lee and Middlebrooks, 2013). The effect of anesthesia, stimulation paradigms, behavioral state, and hierarchy within cortical auditory pathways are also important questions regarding how spatial information is represented and readout in the mammalian auditory cortex.

New evidence on the coding of ITD in the owl's midbrain and forebrain

The overlap of new experimental evidence in the owl's auditory system with both observed response properties and proposed coding theories for mammals brings into question whether coding schemes downstream from sites of ITD detection are fundamentally different across mammalian and avian species. The dominant theory of sound localization in owls has been that a place code is used to infer sound location, where sound location is estimated from the place of highest activity in the map of space (Jeffress, 1948; Konishi, 2003; Schnupp and Carr, 2009). This theory has been challenged, however, for the midbrain map of space by recent evidence that it does not predict the systematic errors that owls make when locating sound at eccentric directions (Fischer and Peña, 2011; Cazettes et al., 2016). These studies have proposed that the owl's sound-localizing behavior could indeed be explained by a population vector decoding of the midbrain map, where the larger number of neurons tuned to frontal locations drives the vector to the front of the actual location, explaining these errors (Fig. 1C). According to this theory, each neuron in the space map is associated with a vector pointing toward its preferred direction, corresponding to the peak of the spatial tuning curve. The length of this vector is scaled to the size of the neuron's response at each direction. Averaging over the population results in a vector pointing toward the decoded direction. Population vector decoding, originally developed for predicting arm movements (Georgopoulos et al., 1986), has been motivated by its algorithmic simplicity. For decades, studies have reported that the firing rate of neurons can be correlated with vector quantities in many neural systems, such as the premotor and motor cortices (Georgopoulos et al., 1986; Caminiti et al., 1991) and cerebellum (Fortier et al., 1989) for monkey arm movement, the cercal system of the cricket for wind detection (Theunissen and Miller, 1991), the mammalian visual system for motion perception (Steinmetz et al., 1987; Gilbert and Wiesel, 1990; Churchland and Lisberger, 2001), or in the SC of vertebrates for saccade direction (Van Gisbergen et al., 1987; Lee et al., 1988). Recently, a population-vector readout of a hemispheric ITD representation has also been invoked to explain the repulsive effect of adaptation on history-dependent azimuth estimation in human listeners (Lingner et al., 2018). Yet, a limited number of studies have proposed theories for where and how the brain could implement such vector quantities (McNaughton et al., 1994; Salinas and Abbott, 1994; Burgess and O'Keefe, 1996), and even fewer studies have attempted to investigate these questions experimentally (Gonzalez-Bellido et al., 2013).

Other findings challenge the notion that the selectivity of neurons in the owl's midbrain map of auditory space is purely defined by their tuning to interaural time and level differences (Konishi, 2003). Because convergence across frequency is a primary mechanism underlying spatial tuning of space-specific neurons (Takahashi and Konishi, 1986; Mazer, 1998), an implicit notion is that the emergence of midbrain maps of auditory space correlates with a fading of the tonotopic organization widely observed in upstream areas of the owl's auditory system (Konishi, 2003). However, recent studies suggest a less simplistic view. Since the discovery of the space map, it has been known that the spatial tuning of neurons in the front of the space map is sharper than in the periphery (Knudsen and Konishi, 1978). Recent work shows that the basis of this nonuniform tuning is the varying frequency selectivity of neurons (Cazettes et al., 2016). Correlation between ITD and frequency tuning has been reported in the owl's space map (Knudsen, 1984; Cazettes et al., 2014). These findings indicate that the classic map of auditory space in the owl can still be viewed as a tonotopic representation, as high frequencies are represented in the front and low frequencies in the periphery of the map (Fig. 2A). Indeed, the best ITD increases as the preferred frequency decreases, in a manner that, albeit different in range, is reminiscent of that observed in mammals (Fig. 2B) (McAlpine et al., 2001; Hancock and Delgutte, 2004; Day and Semple, 2011; Bremen and Joris, 2013). This relationship between best ITD and preferred frequency leads to predictable changes in the owl's sound localization behavior, such as better accuracy localizing low-frequency than high-frequency narrowband sounds in the periphery (Cazettes et al., 2018).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Frequency tuning of ITD-selective cells in birds and mammals. A, Best ITD (absolute value) is correlated with best frequency in neurons of the barn owl's map of auditory space, where neurons selective to ITDs corresponding to sounds located in frontal positions respond to higher frequencies. Adapted from Cazettes et al. (2014). B, Best ITD (absolute value) also varies as a function of best frequency in neurons of the mammalian auditory pathway. Adapted from McAlpine et al. (2001). Neurons that prefer small ITDs are also tuned to higher frequencies.

Additionally, recent evidence suggests different coding schemes of sound location downstream of the midbrain maps. Studies in the owl's forebrain have reported evidence of a transformation in the neural code of ITD, where convergence across ITD channels leads to quasi-hemispherical ITD tuning (Vonderschen and Wagner, 2009, 2012). Furthermore, recent comparative population recordings in the owl's midbrain and forebrain showed differing correlation structure of nearby cells, where the hemispherical tuning of forebrain cells was associated with a significant drop in correlated variability of nearby cells, eliminating the potentially detrimental effect of noise correlation on information in a putative forebrain opponent-channel code (Beckert et al., 2017).

Emergence of hemispheric tuning from a brain map of auditory space

A critical question remains of how seemingly different ITD coding schemes observed in different parts of the brain of mammals and birds are functionally connected. Recent studies investigated the processing that links the sensory representation of ITD in the barn owl's space map to the motor command guiding head-orienting responses. The spatial tuning of neurons of a nucleus downstream from the space map, the midbrain tegmentum, which commands the owl's gaze via projections to the spinal cord (Masino, 1992; Masino and Knudsen, 1993), was measured for the first time (Cazettes et al., 2018). Consistent with previous reports (Masino, 1992; Masino and Knudsen, 1993), a topographic organization of ITD tuning was not observed in the midbrain tegmentum, suggesting that the map of auditory space disappears in this nucleus. Surprisingly, however, these premotor neurons displayed hemispheric ITD tuning, as predicted by the opponent-channel theory. Unlike neurons in the space map, which are sharply tuned to ITD, and therefore space (Fig. 3A,B), midbrain tegmentum neurons responded to the whole contralateral hemifield and displayed sigmoid-like ITD tuning curves (Fig. 3C,D). The peak response of their ITD tuning curves lay on the contralateral side, away from the midline (Fig. 3D). Additionally, the premotor neurons efficiently accounted for the owl's behavior with responses that are consistent with a population-vector readout of the midbrain map (Cazettes et al., 2018), as predicted by Fischer and Peña (2011).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Spatial and ITD tuning in the owl's midbrain map and midbrain tegmentum. A, Spatial tuning of an example neuron in the midbrain map. B, Example ITD tuning curves of three neurons in the midbrain map selective for different ITDs. C, Spatial tuning of an example midbrain tegmentum cell. D, Example ITD tuning curves of three midbrain tegmentum neurons. Firing rate is normalized from maximum to minimum in all plots. Different colors represent different neurons. Modified from Cazettes et al. (2018).

Average midbrain tegmentum responses in each brain hemisphere increased for ITDs corresponding to the contralateral side of space (Fig. 4A). These tuning curves are consistent with hypothesized hemispheric population responses to ITD underlying the inference of sound location in mammals (McAlpine and Grothe, 2003) (Fig. 1B). Additionally, the ITD tuning of midbrain tegmentum neurons varied with frequency (Fig. 4B) in a manner that matches the dependency between frequency and ITD tuning in the midbrain map, from which it receives projections (Knudsen, 1984; Cazettes et al., 2014, 2018). When neurons were stimulated with high-frequency tones, the ITD tuning peaked at small ITDs; whereas when neurons were stimulated with low-frequency tones, the ITD tuning peaked at larger ITDs (Fig. 4B) (Cazettes et al., 2018). This pattern is consistent with responses of ITD-sensitive neurons in the mammalian IC (Fig. 4C) (McAlpine et al., 2001).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

ITD tuning in midbrain tegmentum: emergence of neural responses reminiscent of the opponent-channel code hypothesized in mammals. A, Averaged ITD tuning curves (mean ± SE) of midbrain tegmentum neurons in left (red) and right (blue) hemispheres. The curves cross at the midline and the steepest slopes are positioned within the physiological range (black dashed lines). Note the similarity with postulated population responses in Figure 1B. Modified from Cazettes et al. (2018). B, Example ITD tuning curves of a single midbrain tegmentum cell, stimulated with tones of different frequencies. Adapted from Cazettes et al. (2018). C, Average rate-ITD curves of sets of inferior colliculus cells with best frequencies within frequency bands centered on 250 Hz (purple), 325 Hz (blue), 500 Hz (green), 700 Hz (yellow), 1.0 kHz (orange), and 1.4 kHz (red). Adapted from McAlpine et al. (2001). D, Convergence of coding schemes. The opponent-channel coding scheme proposed for mammals (left) emerges downstream the map of auditory space in the owl (right).

Notably, midbrain tegmentum responses could be explained by a weighted convergence of midbrain map responses (Cazettes et al., 2018), consistent with the population-vector readout previously predicted (Fischer and Peña, 2011; Cazettes et al., 2018) (Fig. 1C). In sum, the ITD tuning of midbrain tegmentum premotor neurons shows striking resemblance with proposed neural responses supporting the opponent-channel sound localization theory in mammals. However, in barn owls, this code emerges from a map of auditory space generated upstream (Fig. 4D).

Functional role of frequency-dependent ITD tuning: integrating natural statistics into the space map readout

Contextual factors of acoustic environments can significantly affect the information about sound azimuth carried by the ITD cue. For example, for a given location, the squeaks of a mouse in a barn full of noisy livestock will be distorted differently compared with the sound emitted by the same mouse in a calm open meadow. This effect was quantified in owls by measuring the variability of the interaural phase difference (IPD) as concurrent sounds were played from different locations. This showed that concurrent sounds are expected to corrupt the IPD of a target signal in a location- and frequency-dependent manner (Cazettes et al., 2014). The variance of IPD, a statistical quantity of how much this critical spatial cue of a sound's azimuthal position varies across directions of secondary concurrent sounds, is a measure of sensory cue reliability. If a sound frequency from a given location carries an IPD that does not vary much across contexts, then this auditory cue can be relied upon. Consistent with this, Cazettes et al. (2014) showed that neurons in the owl's map of auditory space are selective to the frequencies that carry the most reliable IPDs at the azimuthal location that each neuron prefers. Notably, by virtue of the frequency-convergence mechanism underlying the emergence of spatial selectivity, the relationship between frequency tuning and cue reliability results in a correlation between the sharpness of spatial selectivity and sensory reliability. In particular, the frontal locations, which are associated with more reliable ITDs, are represented by cells with sharper spatial tuning (Cazettes et al., 2016). Furthermore, the coincidence detection mechanism underlying ITD detection (Carr and Konishi, 1990; Fischer et al., 2008) results in a relationship between the width of spatial tuning and signal-to-noise ratio, which allows the tuning width to be a correlate of ongoing sensory reliability (Cazettes et al., 2016).

Theoretical studies have shown that the nonuniform spatial acuity of neurons across the map and the overrepresentation of frontal space may be used by a population vector decoder to perform optimal inference of sound location (Fischer and Peña, 2011; Cazettes et al., 2016). In this framework, the overrepresentation of frontal space acts as a prior distribution that emphasizes frontal directions, and the pattern of activity across the population represents a likelihood function for direction, where narrow tuning reflects high reliability of sensory information and wide tuning reflects low reliability of sensory information. As a result, the population vector combines information about a prior distribution for direction and the reliability of the sensory evidence to perform Bayesian inference. Within this context, the multiplicative combination selectivity to spatial cues of neurons in the map (Peña and Konishi, 2001) is optimal for integrating multiple spatial cues. As these cues can be assumed statistically independent, their multiplicative integration results in sensory information carried by each cue having greater influence on the estimate of sound direction (Fischer and Peña, 2017). The above response properties result in a critical implication for coding: the nonuniform frequency and spatial selectivity of space map neurons can represent the statistics of spatial cues (Fischer and Peña, 2011; Rich et al., 2015; Cazettes et al., 2016). Interestingly, decoding population of neurons with a population vector can also explain optimal inference in human visual orientation perception (Girshick et al., 2011).

A fundamental question has been whether and how statistical information is conveyed from the sensory midbrain map into premotor responses. Recent work has found that the firing rate of neurons in the owl's midbrain tegmentum captures sensory cue statistics through a weighted sum of responses of neurons in the space map (Cazettes et al., 2018). Thus, information on the statistics of sensory cues contained in population responses of the space map is represented by the firing rate of midbrain tegmentum premotor neurons, providing experimental evidence that optimal behavioral commands can be generated from the decoding of a sensory representation. The notion that sound-localizing behavior follows rules of optimality is not limited to owls, having been invoked in mammals, including humans (Harper and McAlpine, 2004; Dosso and Wilmut, 2012; Parise et al., 2014; Reijniers et al., 2014; Mlynarski, 2015; Pavao et al., 2018).

Several pieces of evidence suggest a common role of sensory statistics in driving neural coding of sound localization across species. The finding that the owl's midbrain space map reflects optimal cue combination (Fischer and Peña, 2017) may also apply to mammals, even though the way that ITD, ILD, and spectral sound localization cues contribute to the map formation may differ in birds and mammals. Indeed, the predominant use of ITD in the fine structure of sounds at low frequencies, and ILD and envelope ITD at higher frequencies as the cues for azimuth (Lord Rayleigh, 1876; Stevens and Newman, 1936; Henning, 1974) is consistent with the reliability of those cues as computed by MSO and lateral superior olive neurons. In addition, the multiplicative combination of ITD and ILD cues demonstrated in the barn owl's midbrain neurons (Peña and Konishi, 2001), an optimal operation for integration of statistically independent cues (Fischer and Peña, 2017), was first proposed for human binaural cue integration (Stern and Colburn, 1978). Furthermore, recent human studies indicate that ILD cue statistics explain high-frequency spatial acuity in humans (Brown et al., 2018). While the population vector may not describe behavior in all species, other implementations of optimal inference may be used (Day and Delgutte, 2013; Goodman et al., 2013). Future work will elucidate the fundamental question how sensory statistics are integrated into behavioral commands across species and sensory modalities.

Conclusions

Experimental evidence shows that auditory responses of premotor tegmentum neurons in the owl's midbrain meet predictions of an opponent-channel coding scheme supporting the owl's orienting behavior (Cazettes et al., 2018). Computing this opponent-channel code requires a weighted sum over the space map, therefore departing from the classical place code model of sound localization in the barn owl (Fischer and Peña, 2011; Cazettes et al., 2016, 2018). A transformation from a place code to an opponent-channel code has previously been proposed for midbrain control of eye movements (Groh, 2001). Thus, the observation of a similar conversion in the midbrain tegmentum, a premotor area, supports the validity of this hypothesis. This conversion also reveals similarities between the ITD tuning in the avian brains and the opponent-channel code proposed for the mammalian brains. Even though the opponent-channel code is not universally accepted across mammalian species and brain regions, these results indicate points of overlap between avian and mammalian species suggesting unifying principles in the representation of ITD (Fig. 4D). A recent study also suggests that the basis for auditory spatial adaptation may be comparable in ferret and owls (Keating et al., 2015). These findings account for growing evidence connecting neural coding of auditory spatial cues across species.

That responses consistent with the opponent-channel theory of sound localization emerge in premotor neurons of the owl's midbrain suggests that codes driving orienting behavior in mammals and birds may be comparable. A critical factor in barn owl studies is that an adaptive behavioral command emerges from the direct readout of a sensory representation. This evidence is consistent with predictions of how the retinotopic and auditory maps in the SC could be read out to generate an opponent-channel code guiding orienting behavior (Groh, 2001; Lee and Groh, 2014). More generally, the transformation of an ITD-map in the OT into a representation where ITD tuning is quasi-monotonic, reaching maxima at peripheral locations, in premotor neurons of the barn owl provides further evidence that generating sound-driven movements requires converting a place code into an opponent-channel code. To the extent that animal behavior is used as a metric for both perceptual and motor functions, how such transformation is accomplished by the brain may be significant for understanding both motor coordination and sensory perception.

Owls are auditory specialists that have evolved high capability for capturing prey using acoustic cues. If mammals and owls share coding strategies supporting sound azimuth localization, a question arises regarding the functional benefit of a space map in the owl's midbrain. The existence of a space map that recapitulates Jeffress model is uncontested in the barn owl. Seminal work by Harper and McAlpine (2004) provided an explanation of how topographic ITD representations could maximize information, based on the species' head size and frequency range over which ITD is encoded. Although this theory may not apply to all species (Kettler and Carr, 2019), it does for barn owls, given their physiological range of ITD and the unusually broad frequency range over which their brain can encode ITD (Carr and Konishi, 1990; Köppl, 1997). Recent findings showing that this space map is also a frequency map that excludes frequency ranges carrying unreliable spatial information (Cazettes et al., 2014) and can represent sensory reliability (Cazettes et al., 2016) further illuminate the evolutionary advantage of this specialization.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants DC012949, DC007690, and NS104911. We thank Catherine Carr and Dan Tollin for feedback; and Roland Ferger and Keanu Shadron for comments on the manuscript.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Jose L. Peña at jose.pena{at}einstein.yu.edu

References

  1. ↵
    1. Ashida G,
    2. Carr CE
    (2011) Sound localization: Jeffress and beyond. Curr Opin Neurobiol 21:745–751. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2011.05.008 pmid:21646012
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Beckert MV,
    2. Pavão R,
    3. Peña JL
    (2017) Distinct correlation structure supporting a rate-code for sound localization in the owl's auditory forebrain. eNeuro 4:ENEURO.0144–17.2017. doi:10.1523/ENEURO.0144-17.2017 pmid:28674698
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Belliveau LA,
    2. Lyamzin DR,
    3. Lesica NA
    (2014) The neural representation of interaural time differences in gerbils is transformed from midbrain to cortex. J Neurosci 34:16796–16808. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2432-14.2014 pmid:25505332
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Boehnke SE,
    2. Phillips DP
    (1999) Azimuthal tuning of human perceptual channels for sound location. J Acoust Soc Am 106:1948–1955. doi:10.1121/1.428037 pmid:10530019
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Brand A,
    2. Behrend O,
    3. Marquardt T,
    4. McAlpine D,
    5. Grothe B
    (2002) Precise inhibition is essential for microsecond interaural time difference coding. Nature 417:543–547. doi:10.1038/417543a pmid:12037566
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Bremen P,
    2. Joris PX
    (2013) Axonal recordings from medial superior olive neurons obtained from the lateral lemniscus of the chinchilla (Chinchilla laniger). J Neurosci 33:17506–17518. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1518-13.2013 pmid:24174683
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    1. Briley PM,
    2. Kitterick PT,
    3. Summerfield AQ
    (2013) Evidence for opponent process analysis of sound source location in humans. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol 14:83–101. doi:10.1007/s10162-012-0356-x pmid:23090057
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Briley PM,
    2. Goman AM,
    3. Summerfield AQ
    (2016) Physiological evidence for a midline spatial channel in human auditory cortex. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol 17:331–340. doi:10.1007/s10162-016-0571-y pmid:27164943
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Brown AD,
    2. Benichoux V,
    3. Jones HG,
    4. Anbuhl KL,
    5. Tollin DJ
    (2018) Spatial variation in signal and sensory precision both constrain auditory acuity at high frequencies. Hear Res 370:65–73. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2018.10.002 pmid:30326382
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Brugge JF,
    2. Reale RA,
    3. Hind JE
    (1996) The structure of spatial receptive fields of neurons in primary auditory cortex of the cat. J Neurosci 16:4420–4437. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-14-04420.1996 pmid:8699253
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Burgess N,
    2. O'Keefe J
    (1996) Neuronal computations underlying the firing of place cells and their role in navigation. Hippocampus 6:749–762. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:6%3C749::AID-HIPO16%3E3.0.CO;2-0 pmid:9034860
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Caminiti R,
    2. Johnson PB,
    3. Galli C,
    4. Ferraina S,
    5. Burnod Y
    (1991) Making arm movements within different parts of space: the premotor and motor cortical representation of a coordinate system for reaching to visual targets. J Neurosci 11:1182–1197. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-05-01182.1991 pmid:2027042
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. ↵
    1. Carr CE,
    2. Konishi M
    (1990) A circuit for detection of interaural time differences in the brain stem of the barn owl. J Neurosci 10:3227–3246. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-10-03227.1990 pmid:2213141
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Carr CE,
    2. Shah S,
    3. McColgan T,
    4. Ashida G,
    5. Kuokkanen PT,
    6. Brill S,
    7. Kempter R,
    8. Wagner H
    (2015) Maps of interaural delay in the owl's nucleus laminaris. J Neurophysiol 114:1862–1873. doi:10.1152/jn.00644.2015 pmid:26224776
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Cazettes F,
    2. Fischer BJ,
    3. Peña JL
    (2014) Spatial cue reliability drives frequency tuning in the barn Owl's midbrain. Elife 3:e04854. doi:10.7554/eLife.04854 pmid:25531067
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Cazettes F,
    2. Fischer BJ,
    3. Peña JL
    (2016) Cue reliability represented in the shape of tuning curves in the owls sound localization system. J Neurosci 36:2101–2110. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3753-15.2016 pmid:26888922
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    1. Cazettes F,
    2. Fischer BJ,
    3. Beckert MV,
    4. Peña JL
    (2018) Emergence of an adaptive command for orienting behavior in premotor brainstem neurons of barn owls. J Neurosci 38:7270–7279. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0947-18.2018 pmid:30012694
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Churchland MM,
    2. Lisberger SG
    (2001) Shifts in the population response in the middle temporal visual area parallel perceptual and motor illusions produced by apparent motion. J Neurosci 21:9387–9402. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-23-09387.2001 pmid:11717372
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Day ML,
    2. Delgutte B
    (2013) Decoding sound source location and separation using neural population activity patterns. J Neurosci 33:15837–15847. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2034-13.2013 pmid:24089491
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    1. Day ML,
    2. Semple MN
    (2011) Frequency-dependent interaural delays in the medial superior olive: implications for interaural cochlear delays. J Neurophysiol 106:1985–1999. doi:10.1152/jn.00131.2011 pmid:21775710
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Dingle RN,
    2. Hall SE,
    3. Phillips DP
    (2010) A midline azimuthal channel in human spatial hearing. Hear Res 268:67–74. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2010.04.017 pmid:20457238
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Dosso SE,
    2. Wilmut MJ
    (2012) Maximum-likelihood and other processors for incoherent and coherent matched-field localization. J Acoust Soc Am 132:2273–2285. doi:10.1121/1.4730978 pmid:23039424
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. du Lac S,
    2. Knudsen EI
    (1990) Neural maps of head movement vector and speed in the optic tectum of the barn owl. J Neurophysiol 63:131–146. doi:10.1152/jn.1990.63.1.131 pmid:2299378
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Fischer BJ,
    2. Peña JL
    (2011) Owl's behavior and neural representation predicted by Bayesian inference. Nat Neurosci 14:1061–1066. doi:10.1038/nn.2872 pmid:21725311
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Fischer BJ,
    2. Peña JL
    (2017) Optimal nonlinear cue integration for sound localization. J Comput Neurosci 42:37–52. doi:10.1007/s10827-016-0626-4 pmid:27714569
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Fischer BJ,
    2. Christianson GB,
    3. Peña JL
    (2008) Cross-correlation in the auditory coincidence detectors of owls. J Neurosci 28:8107–8115. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1969-08.2008 pmid:18685035
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. ↵
    1. Fitzpatrick DC,
    2. Kuwada S,
    3. Batra R
    (2000) Neural sensitivity to interaural time differences: beyond the Jeffress model. J Neurosci 20:1605–1615. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-04-01605.2000 pmid:10662850
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    1. Fontaine B,
    2. Brette R
    (2011) Neural development of binaural tuning through Hebbian learning predicts frequency-dependent best delays. J Neurosci 31:11692–11696. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0237-11.2011 pmid:21832198
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Fortier PA,
    2. Kalaska JF,
    3. Smith AM
    (1989) Cerebellar neuronal activity related to whole-arm reaching movements in the monkey. J Neurophysiol 62:198–211. doi:10.1152/jn.1989.62.1.198 pmid:2754472
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Gaese BH,
    2. Johnen A
    (2000) Coding for auditory space in the superior colliculus of the rat. Eur J Neurosci 12:1739–1752. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00054.x pmid:10792451
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Georgopoulos AP,
    2. Schwartz AB,
    3. Kettner RE
    (1986) Neuronal population coding of movement direction. Science 233:1416–1419. doi:10.1126/science.3749885 pmid:3749885
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Gilbert CD,
    2. Wiesel TN
    (1990) The influence of contextual stimuli on the orientation selectivity of cells in primary visual cortex of the cat. Vision Res 30:1689–1701. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(90)90153-C pmid:2288084
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Girshick AR,
    2. Landy MS,
    3. Simoncelli EP
    (2011) Cardinal rules: visual orientation perception reflects knowledge of environmental statistics. Nat Neurosci 14:926–932. doi:10.1038/nn.2831 pmid:21642976
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Goldberg JM,
    2. Brown PB
    (1969) Responses of binaural neurons of dog superior olivary complex to dichotic tonal stimuli: some physiological mechanisms of sound localization. J Neurophysiol 32:613–636. doi:10.1152/jn.1969.32.4.613 pmid:5810617
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Gonzalez-Bellido PT,
    2. Peng H,
    3. Yang J,
    4. Georgopoulos AP,
    5. Olberg RM
    (2013) Eight pairs of descending visual neurons in the dragonfly give wing motor centers accurate population vector of prey direction. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110:696–701. doi:10.1073/pnas.1210489109 pmid:23213224
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    1. Goodman DF,
    2. Benichoux V,
    3. Brette R
    (2013) Decoding neural responses to temporal cues for sound localization. Elife 2:e01312. doi:10.7554/eLife.01312 pmid:24302571
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Groh JM
    (2001) Converting neural signals from place codes to rate codes. Biol Cybern 85:159–165. doi:10.1007/s004220100249 pmid:11561817
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Grothe B,
    2. Pecka M,
    3. McAlpine D
    (2010) Mechanisms of sound localization in mammals. Physiol Rev 90:983–1012. doi:10.1152/physrev.00026.2009 pmid:20664077
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Hancock KE,
    2. Delgutte B
    (2004) A physiologically based model of interaural time difference discrimination. J Neurosci 24:7110–7117. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0762-04.2004 pmid:15306644
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    1. Harper NS,
    2. McAlpine D
    (2004) Optimal neural population coding of an auditory spatial cue. Nature 430:682–686. doi:10.1038/nature02768 pmid:15295602
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Henning GB
    (1974) Detectability of interaural delay in high-frequency complex waveforms. J Acoust Soc Am 55:84–90. doi:10.1121/1.1928135 pmid:4815755
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Higgins NC,
    2. Storace DA,
    3. Escabí MA,
    4. Read HL
    (2010) Specialization of binaural responses in ventral auditory cortices. J Neurosci 30:14522–14532. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2561-10.2010 pmid:20980610
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    1. Jeffress LA
    (1948) A place theory of sound localization. J Comp Physiol Psychol 41:35–39. doi:10.1037/h0061495 pmid:18904764
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Jenkins WM,
    2. Masterton RB
    (1982) Sound localization: effects of unilateral lesions in central auditory system. J Neurophysiol 47:987–1016. doi:10.1152/jn.1982.47.6.987 pmid:7108581
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Joris PX,
    2. Van de Sande B,
    3. Louage DH,
    4. van der Heijden M
    (2006) Binaural and cochlear disparities. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:12917–12922. doi:10.1073/pnas.0601396103 pmid:16908859
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  46. ↵
    1. Kavanagh GL,
    2. Kelly JB
    (1987) Contribution of the auditory cortex to sound localization by the ferret (Mustela putorius). J Neurophysiol 57:1746–1766. doi:10.1152/jn.1987.57.6.1746 pmid:3598629
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Keating P,
    2. Dahmen JC,
    3. King AJ
    (2015) Complementary adaptive processes contribute to the developmental plasticity of spatial hearing. Nat Neurosci 18:185–187. doi:10.1038/nn.3914 pmid:25581359
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Kettler L,
    2. Carr CE
    (2019) Neural maps of interaural time difference in the American alligator: a stable feature in modern archosaurs. J Neurosci 39:3882–3896. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2989-18.2019 pmid:30886018
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  49. ↵
    1. King AJ
    (1993) The Wellcome Prize Lecture: a map of auditory space in the mammalian brain: neural computation and development. Exp Physiol 78:559–590. doi:10.1113/expphysiol.1993.sp003708 pmid:8240794
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. King AJ,
    2. Schnupp JW,
    3. Carlile S,
    4. Smith AL,
    5. Thompson ID
    (1996) The development of topographically-aligned maps of visual and auditory space in the superior colliculus. Prog Brain Res 112:335–350. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(08)63340-3 pmid:8979840
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Knudsen EI
    (1982) Auditory and visual maps of space in the optic tectum of the owl. J Neurosci 2:1177–1194. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.02-09-01177.1982 pmid:7119872
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  52. ↵
    1. Knudsen EI
    (1984) Auditory properties of space-tuned units in owl's optic tectum. J Neurophysiol 52:709–723. doi:10.1152/jn.1984.52.4.709 pmid:6491713
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Knudsen EI
    (2018) Neural circuits that mediate selective attention: a comparative perspective. Trends Neurosci 41:789–805. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2018.06.006 pmid:30075867
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Knudsen EI,
    2. Konishi M
    (1978) A neural map of auditory space in the owl. Science 200:795–797. doi:10.1126/science.644324 pmid:644324
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  55. ↵
    1. Konishi M
    (2003) Coding of auditory space. Annu Rev Neurosci 26:31–55. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131123 pmid:14527266
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Köppl C
    (1997) Phase locking to high frequencies in the auditory nerve and cochlear nucleus magnocellularis of the barn owl, Tyto alba. J Neurosci 17:3312–3321. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-09-03312.1997 pmid:9096164
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    1. Köppl C,
    2. Carr CE
    (2008) Maps of interaural time difference in the chicken's brainstem nucleus laminaris. Biol Cybern 98:541–559. doi:10.1007/s00422-008-0220-6 pmid:18491165
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Lee CC,
    2. Middlebrooks JC
    (2013) Specialization for sound localization in fields A1, DZ, and PAF of cat auditory cortex. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol 14:61–82. doi:10.1007/s10162-012-0357-9 pmid:23180228
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Lee C,
    2. Rohrer WH,
    3. Sparks DL
    (1988) Population coding of saccadic eye movements by neurons in the superior colliculus. Nature 332:357–360. doi:10.1038/332357a0 pmid:3352733
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Lee J,
    2. Groh JM
    (2014) Different stimuli, different spatial codes: a visual map and an auditory rate code for oculomotor space in the primate superior colliculus. PLoS One 9:e85017. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0085017 pmid:24454779
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Lingner A,
    2. Pecka M,
    3. Leibold C,
    4. Grothe B
    (2018) A novel concept for dynamic adjustment of auditory space. Sci Rep 8:8335. doi:10.1038/s41598-018-26690-0 pmid:29844516
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Lord Rayleigh MA
    (1876) On our perception of the direction of a source of sound. Proc Music Assoc 2:75–84.
    OpenUrl
  63. ↵
    1. Lüling H,
    2. Siveke I,
    3. Grothe B,
    4. Leibold C
    (2011) Frequency-invariant representation of interaural time differences in mammals. PLoS Comput Biol 7:e1002013. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002013 pmid:21445227
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Magezi DA,
    2. Krumbholz K
    (2010) Evidence for opponent-channel coding of interaural time differences in human auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol 104:1997–2007. doi:10.1152/jn.00424.2009 pmid:20702739
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Malhotra S,
    2. Stecker GC,
    3. Middlebrooks JC,
    4. Lomber SG
    (2008) Sound localization deficits during reversible deactivation of primary auditory cortex and/or the dorsal zone. J Neurophysiol 99:1628–1642. doi:10.1152/jn.01228.2007 pmid:18199813
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Masino T
    (1992) Brainstem control of orienting movements: intrinsic coordinate systems and underlying circuitry. Brain Behav Evol 40:98–111. doi:10.1159/000113906 pmid:1422810
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Masino T,
    2. Knudsen EI
    (1993) Orienting head movements resulting from electrical microstimulation of the brainstem tegmentum in the barn owl. J Neurosci 13:351–370. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-01-00351.1993 pmid:8423480
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  68. ↵
    1. Mazer JA
    (1998) How the owl resolves auditory coding ambiguity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95:10932–10937. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.18.10932 pmid:9724807
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  69. ↵
    1. McAlpine D,
    2. Grothe B
    (2003) Sound localization and delay lines: do mammals fit the model? Trends Neurosci 26:347–350. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00140-1 pmid:12850430
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. McAlpine D,
    2. Jiang D,
    3. Palmer AR
    (2001) A neural code for low-frequency sound localization in mammals. Nat Neurosci 4:396–401. doi:10.1038/86049 pmid:11276230
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. McNaughton BL,
    2. Knierim JJ,
    3. Ma W
    (1994) Vector encoding and the vestibular foundations of spatial cognition: neurophysiological and computational mechanisms. In: The cognitive neurosciences (Gazzaniga M, ed). Boston: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
  72. ↵
    1. Meredith MA,
    2. Stein BE
    (1983) Interactions among converging sensory inputs in the superior colliculus. Science 221:389–391. doi:10.1126/science.6867718 pmid:6867718
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  73. ↵
    1. Mickey BJ,
    2. Middlebrooks JC
    (2003) Representation of auditory space by cortical neurons in awake cats. J Neurosci 23:8649–8663. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-25-08649.2003 pmid:14507964
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  74. ↵
    1. Middlebrooks JC,
    2. Knudsen EI
    (1984) A neural code for auditory space in the cat's superior colliculus. J Neurosci 4:2621–2634. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-10-02621.1984 pmid:6491727
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  75. ↵
    1. Middlebrooks JC,
    2. Pettigrew JD
    (1981) Functional classes of neurons in primary auditory cortex of the cat distinguished by sensitivity to sound location. J Neurosci 1:107–120. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.01-01-00107.1981 pmid:7346555
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  76. ↵
    1. Middlebrooks JC,
    2. Clock AE,
    3. Xu L,
    4. Green DM
    (1994) A panoramic code for sound location by cortical neurons. Science 264:842–844. doi:10.1126/science.8171339 pmid:8171339
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  77. ↵
    1. Middlebrooks JC,
    2. Xu L,
    3. Eddins AC,
    4. Green DM
    (1998) Codes for sound-source location in nontonotopic auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol 80:863–881. doi:10.1152/jn.1998.80.2.863 pmid:9705474
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. Mlynarski W
    (2015) The opponent channel population code of sound location is an efficient representation of natural binaural sounds. PLoS Comput Biol 11:e1004294. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004294 pmid:25996373
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Moiseff A
    (1989) Bi-coordinate sound localization by the barn owl. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 164:637–644. doi:10.1007/BF00614506
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Moiseff A,
    2. Konishi M
    (1981) Neuronal and behavioral sensitivity to binaural time differences in the owl. J Neurosci 1:40–48. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.01-01-00040.1981 pmid:7346557
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  81. ↵
    1. Moiseff A,
    2. Konishi M
    (1983) Binaural characteristics of units in the owl's brainstem auditory pathway: precursors of restricted spatial receptive fields. J Neurosci 3:2553–2562. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.03-12-02553.1983 pmid:6655499
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  82. ↵
    1. Palanca-Castan N,
    2. Köppl C
    (2015) Change in the coding of interaural time difference along the tonotopic axis of the chicken nucleus laminaris. Front Neural Circuits 9:43. doi:10.3389/fncir.2015.00043 pmid:26347616
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  83. ↵
    1. Palmer AR,
    2. King AJ
    (1982) The representation of auditory space in the mammalian superior colliculus. Nature 299:248–249. doi:10.1038/299248a0 pmid:7110344
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  84. ↵
    1. Parise CV,
    2. Knorre K,
    3. Ernst MO
    (2014) Natural auditory scene statistics shapes human spatial hearing. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111:6104–6108. doi:10.1073/pnas.1322705111 pmid:24711409
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  85. ↵
    1. Pavao R,
    2. Sussman ES,
    3. Fischer BJ,
    4. Peña JL
    (2018) Anticipated ITD statistics built-in human sound localization. BioRxiv. Advance online publication. Retrieved January 25, 2019. doi:10.1101/303347
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  86. ↵
    1. Peña JL,
    2. Konishi M
    (2000) Cellular mechanisms for resolving phase ambiguity in the owl's inferior colliculus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:11787–11792. doi:10.1073/pnas.97.22.11787 pmid:11050210
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  87. ↵
    1. Peña JL,
    2. Konishi M
    (2001) Auditory spatial receptive fields created by multiplication. Science 292:249–252. doi:10.1126/science.1059201 pmid:11303092
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  88. ↵
    1. Peña JL,
    2. Viete S,
    3. Funabiki K,
    4. Saberi K,
    5. Konishi M
    (2001) Cochlear and neural delays for coincidence detection in owls. J Neurosci 21:9455–9459. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-23-09455.2001 pmid:11717379
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  89. ↵
    1. Phillips DP,
    2. Hall SE
    (2005) Psychophysical evidence for adaptation of central auditory processors for interaural differences in time and level. Hear Res 202:188–199. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2004.11.001 pmid:15811711
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  90. ↵
    1. Razak KA,
    2. Fuzessery ZM
    (2010) GABA shapes a systematic map of binaural sensitivity in the auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol 104:517–528. doi:10.1152/jn.00294.2010 pmid:20484524
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  91. ↵
    1. Reale RA,
    2. Brugge JF
    (1990) Auditory cortical neurons are sensitive to static and continuously changing interaural phase cues. J Neurophysiol 64:1247–1260. doi:10.1152/jn.1990.64.4.1247 pmid:2258745
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  92. ↵
    1. Reijniers J,
    2. Vanderelst D,
    3. Jin C,
    4. Carlile S,
    5. Peremans H
    (2014) An ideal-observer model of human sound localization. Biol Cybern 108:169–181. doi:10.1007/s00422-014-0588-4 pmid:24570350
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  93. ↵
    1. Remington ED,
    2. Wang X
    (2019) Neural representations of the full spatial field in auditory cortex of awake marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). Cereb Cortex 29:1199–1216. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhy025 pmid:29420692
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  94. ↵
    1. Rich D,
    2. Cazettes F,
    3. Wang Y,
    4. Peña JL,
    5. Fischer BJ
    (2015) Neural representation of probabilities for Bayesian inference. J Comp Neurosci 38:315–323. doi:10.1007/s10827-014-0545-1 pmid:25561333
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  95. ↵
    1. Salinas E,
    2. Abbott LF
    (1994) Vector reconstruction from firing rates. J Comp Neurosci 1:89–107. doi:10.1007/BF00962720 pmid:8792227
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  96. ↵
    1. Salminen NH,
    2. Tiitinen H,
    3. Yrttiaho S
    4. May PJ
    (2010) The neural code for interaural time difference in human auditory cortex. J Acoust Soc Am 127: EL60–EL65. doi:10.1121/1.3290744 pmid:20136180
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  97. ↵
    1. Schnupp JW,
    2. Carr CE
    (2009) On hearing with more than one ear: lessons from evolution. Nat Neurosci 12:692–697. doi:10.1038/nn.2325 pmid:19471267
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  98. ↵
    1. Schnupp JW,
    2. King AJ
    (1997) Coding of auditory space in the nucleus of the brachium of the inferior colliculus in the ferret. J Neurophysiol 78:2717–2731. doi:10.1152/jn.1997.78.5.2717 pmid:9356421
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  99. ↵
    1. Scott BH,
    2. Malone BJ,
    3. Semple MN
    (2009) Representation of dynamic interaural phase difference in auditory cortex of awake rhesus macaques. J Neurophysiol 101:1781–1799. doi:10.1152/jn.00678.2007 pmid:19164111
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  100. ↵
    1. Slee SJ,
    2. Young ED
    (2013) Linear processing of interaural level difference underlies spatial tuning in the nucleus of the brachium of the inferior colliculus. J Neurosci 33:3891–3904. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3437-12.2013 pmid:23447600
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  101. ↵
    1. Slee SJ,
    2. Young ED
    (2014) Alignment of sound localization cues in the nucleus of the brachium of the inferior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 111:2624–2633. doi:10.1152/jn.00885.2013 pmid:24671535
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  102. ↵
    1. Spitzer MW,
    2. Semple MN
    (1995) Neurons sensitive to interaural phase disparity in gerbil superior olive: diverse monaural and temporal response properties. J Neurophysiol 73:1668–1690. doi:10.1152/jn.1995.73.4.1668 pmid:7643174
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  103. ↵
    1. Stecker GC,
    2. Mickey BJ,
    3. Macpherson EA,
    4. Middlebrooks JC
    (2003) Spatial sensitivity in field PAF of cat auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol 89:2889–2903. doi:10.1152/jn.00980.2002 pmid:12611946
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  104. ↵
    1. Stecker GC,
    2. Harrington IA,
    3. Middlebrooks JC
    (2005) Location coding by opponent neural populations in the auditory cortex. PLoS Biol 3:e78. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0030078 pmid:15736980
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  105. ↵
    1. Steinmetz MA,
    2. Motter BC,
    3. Duffy CJ,
    4. Mountcastle VB
    (1987) Functional properties of parietal visual neurons: radial organization of directionalities within the visual field. J Neurosci 7:177–191. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.07-01-00177.1987 pmid:3806193
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  106. ↵
    1. Stern RM Jr.,
    2. Colburn HS
    (1978) Theory of binaural interaction based on auditory-nerve data: IV: A model for subjective lateral position. J Acoust Soc Am 64:127–140. doi:10.1121/1.381978 pmid:711991
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  107. ↵
    1. Stevens SS,
    2. Newman EB
    (1936) The localization of actual sources of sound. Am J Psychol 48:297–306. doi:10.2307/1415748
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  108. ↵
    1. Sullivan WE,
    2. Konishi M
    (1986) Neural map of interaural phase difference in the owl's brainstem. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 83:8400–8404. doi:10.1073/pnas.83.21.8400 pmid:3022292
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  109. ↵
    1. Takahashi T,
    2. Konishi M
    (1986) Selectivity for interaural time difference in the owl's midbrain. J Neurosci 6:3413–3422. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.06-12-03413.1986 pmid:3794779
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  110. ↵
    1. Takahashi T,
    2. Moiseff A,
    3. Konishi M
    (1984) Time and intensity cues are processed independently in the auditory system of the owl. J Neurosci 4:1781–1786. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-07-01781.1984 pmid:6737040
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  111. ↵
    1. Theunissen FE,
    2. Miller JP
    (1991) Representation of sensory information in the cricket cercal sensory system: II. Information theoretic calculation of system accuracy and optimal tuning-curve widths of four primary interneurons. J Neurophysiol 66:1690–1703. doi:10.1152/jn.1991.66.5.1690 pmid:1765802
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  112. ↵
    1. Thompson GC,
    2. Cortez AM
    (1983) The inability of squirrel monkeys to localize sound after unilateral ablation of auditory cortex. Behav Brain Res 8:211–216. doi:10.1016/0166-4328(83)90055-4 pmid:6860463
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  113. ↵
    1. Van Gisbergen JA,
    2. Van Opstal AJ,
    3. Tax AA
    (1987) Collicular ensemble coding of saccades based on vector summation. Neuroscience 21:541–555. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(87)90140-0 pmid:3614643
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  114. ↵
    1. Vigneault-MacLean BK,
    2. Hall SE,
    3. Phillips DP
    (2007) The effects of lateralized adaptors on lateral position judgements of tones within and across frequency channels. Hear Res 224:93–100. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2006.12.001 pmid:17223297
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  115. ↵
    1. Vonderschen K,
    2. Wagner H
    (2009) Tuning to interaural time difference and frequency differs between the auditory arcopallium and the external nucleus of the inferior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 101:2348–2361. doi:10.1152/jn.91196.2008 pmid:19261709
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  116. ↵
    1. Vonderschen K,
    2. Wagner H
    (2012) Transformation from a pure time delay to a mixed time and phase delay representation in the auditory forebrain pathway. J Neurosci 32:5911–5923. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5429-11.2012 pmid:22539852
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  117. ↵
    1. Wagner H,
    2. Asadollahi A,
    3. Bremen P,
    4. Endler F,
    5. Vonderschen K,
    6. von Campenhausen M
    (2007) Distribution of interaural time difference in the barn owl's inferior colliculus in the low- and high-frequency ranges. J Neurosci 27:4191–4200. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5250-06.2007 pmid:17428997
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  118. ↵
    1. Werner-Reiss U,
    2. Groh JM
    (2008) A rate code for sound azimuth in monkey auditory cortex: implications for human neuroimaging studies. J Neurosci 28:3747–3758. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5044-07.2008 pmid:18385333
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  119. ↵
    1. Wood KC,
    2. Town SM,
    3. Atilgan H,
    4. Jones GP,
    5. Bizley JK
    (2017) Acute inactivation of primary auditory cortex causes a sound localization deficit in ferrets. PLoS One 12: e0170264. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0170264 pmid:28099489
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  120. ↵
    1. Woods TM,
    2. Lopez SE,
    3. Long JH,
    4. Rahman JE,
    5. Recanzone GH
    (2006) Effects of stimulus azimuth and intensity on the single-neuron activity in the auditory cortex of the alert macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol 96:3323–3337. doi:10.1152/jn.00392.2006 pmid:16943318
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  121. ↵
    1. Wurtz RH,
    2. Sommer MA,
    3. Cavanaugh J
    (2005) Drivers from the deep: the contribution of collicular input to thalamocortical processing. Prog Brain Res 149:207–225. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(05)49015-9 pmid:16226586
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  122. ↵
    1. Yin TC,
    2. Chan JC
    (1990) Interaural time sensitivity in medial superior olive of cat. J Neurophysiol 64:465–488. doi:10.1152/jn.1990.64.2.465 pmid:2213127
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  123. ↵
    1. Zhang J,
    2. Nakamoto KT,
    3. Kitzes LM
    (2004) Binaural interaction revisited in the cat primary auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol 91:101–117. doi:10.1152/jn.00166.2003 pmid:14507982
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  124. ↵
    1. Zhou Y,
    2. Wang X
    (2012) Level dependence of spatial processing in the primate auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol 108:810–826. doi:10.1152/jn.00500.2011 pmid:22592309
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 39 (46)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 39, Issue 46
13 Nov 2019
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Synthesis of Hemispheric ITD Tuning from the Readout of a Neural Map: Commonalities of Proposed Coding Schemes in Birds and Mammals
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Synthesis of Hemispheric ITD Tuning from the Readout of a Neural Map: Commonalities of Proposed Coding Schemes in Birds and Mammals
Jose L. Peña, Fanny Cazettes, Michael V. Beckert, Brian J. Fischer
Journal of Neuroscience 13 November 2019, 39 (46) 9053-9061; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0873-19.2019

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Synthesis of Hemispheric ITD Tuning from the Readout of a Neural Map: Commonalities of Proposed Coding Schemes in Birds and Mammals
Jose L. Peña, Fanny Cazettes, Michael V. Beckert, Brian J. Fischer
Journal of Neuroscience 13 November 2019, 39 (46) 9053-9061; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0873-19.2019
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Thinking as Analogy-Making: Toward a Neural Process Account of General Intelligence
  • Close Encounters with Art of Neuroscience 14th Edition
  • New Perspective of the Persistent Gender and Diversity Gap in Nobel Prizes
Show more Viewpoints
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.