Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Systems/Circuits

Aversive Training Induces Both Presynaptic and Postsynaptic Suppression in Drosophila

Xiaofan Zhang, Nathaniel C. Noyes, Jianzhi Zeng, Yulong Li and Ronald L. Davis
Journal of Neuroscience 13 November 2019, 39 (46) 9164-9172; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1420-19.2019
Xiaofan Zhang
1Department of Neuroscience, The Scripps Research Institute Florida, Jupiter, Florida 33458, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Nathaniel C. Noyes
1Department of Neuroscience, The Scripps Research Institute Florida, Jupiter, Florida 33458, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jianzhi Zeng
2State Key Laboratory of Membrane Biology, Peking University School of Life Sciences, PKU-IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Peking-Tsinghua Center for Life Sciences, Chinese Institute for Brain Research, Beijing 100871, China
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Yulong Li
2State Key Laboratory of Membrane Biology, Peking University School of Life Sciences, PKU-IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Peking-Tsinghua Center for Life Sciences, Chinese Institute for Brain Research, Beijing 100871, China
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Yulong Li
Ronald L. Davis
1Department of Neuroscience, The Scripps Research Institute Florida, Jupiter, Florida 33458, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The α′β′ subtype of Drosophila mushroom body neurons (MBn) is required for memory acquisition, consolidation and early memory retrieval after aversive olfactory conditioning. However, in vivo functional imaging studies have failed to detect an early forming memory trace in these neurons as reflected by an enhanced G-CaMP signal in response to presentation of the learned odor. Moreover, whether cellular memory traces form early after conditioning in the mushroom body output neurons (MBOn) downstream of the α′β′ MBn remains unknown. Here, we show that aversive olfactory conditioning suppresses the calcium responses to the learned odor in both α′3 and α′2 axon segments of α′β′ MBn and in the dendrites of α′3 MBOn immediately after conditioning using female flies. Notably, the cellular memory traces in both α′3 MBn and α′3 MBOn are short-lived and persist for <30 min. The suppressed response in α′3 MBn is accompanied by a reduction of acetylcholine (ACh) release, suggesting that the memory trace in postsynaptic α′3 MBOn may simply reflect the suppression in presynaptic α′3 MBn. Furthermore, we show that the α′3 MBn memory trace does not occur from the inhibition of GABAergic neurons via GABAA receptor activation. Because activation of the α′3 MBOn drives approach behavior of adult flies, our results demonstrate that aversive conditioning promotes avoidance behavior through suppression of the α′3 MBn–MBOn circuit.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Drosophila learn to avoid an odor if that odor is repeatedly paired with electric shock. Mushroom body neurons (MBns) are known to be major cell types that mediate this form of aversive conditioning. Here we show that aversive conditioning causes a reduced response to the conditioned odor in an axon branch of one subtype of the MBn for no more than 30 min after conditioning, and in the dendrites of postsynaptic, MB output neurons (MBOns). Because experimenter-induced activation of the MBOn induces approach behavior by the fly, our data support a model that aversive learning promotes avoidance by suppressing the MBn–MBOn synapses that normally promote attraction.

  • Drosophila
  • mushroom body
  • olfactory conditioning
  • suppression

Introduction

Animals learn to avoid a neutral stimulus that is repeatedly coupled with an unpleasant one. This type of learning, aversive associative learning, induces cellular memory traces in engram cells in the brain and changes the representation of the neutral stimulus (Davis, 2011; Tonegawa et al., 2015). In Drosophila, several memory traces detected with the calcium indicator G-CaMP have been observed in the mushroom body (MB), a brain region critical for olfactory learning and memory (Davis, 1993; Yu et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2008; Davis, 2011; Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2013). These memory traces are detectable across discrete time periods extending from 30 min to several days after training. However, memory traces that form immediately in the MB after conditioning have not been detected with in vivo Ca2+ imaging.

The MB is composed of ∼2000 intrinsic neurons in each hemisphere that integrates olfactory cues received from antennal lobe projection neurons with aversive or rewarding stimuli from two clusters (PPL1, PAM) of dopamine neurons (DAns) (Schwaerzel et al., 2003; Claridge-Chang et al., 2009; Mao and Davis, 2009; Aso et al., 2012; Burke et al., 2012; Liu et al., 2012). MBn are classified into three major subtypes: α′β′, αβ, and γ neurons, based on their birth order and projection patterns of their axons in the brain (Crittenden et al., 1998; Lee et al., 1999). The axons of α′β′ and αβ MBn bifurcate and project within the vertical α′/α lobe and horizontal β′/β lobe neuropil, whereas the axons of γ neurons project only within the horizontal γ lobe neuropil. Although each of these MBn subtypes contributes to aversive olfactory memory, they do so at different times after conditioning (Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2013), with synaptic transmission from the α′β′ and γ MBn required for robust expression of early and intermediate-term memory (immediate to 3 h) and synaptic transmission from the αβ MBn having a more pronounced role for memory expression after 3 h. Importantly, although the α′β′ MBn are required for memory acquisition, consolidation and early memory retrieval (Krashes et al., 2007; Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2013), no immediate memory trace in α′β′ MBns has been detected using in vivo Ca2+ imaging (Wang et al., 2008).

Five different types of MB output neurons (MBOns) tile the α′β′ lobe with their dendritic trees into five discrete compartments, matching the tiling by axon terminals from presynaptic DAns (Mao and Davis, 2009; Aso et al., 2014a). Several of these MBOns are required for aversive memory or appetitive memory expression, and intermediate-term memory traces (∼1–2 h after conditioning) have been detected in some of these neurons (Séjourné et al., 2011; Owald et al., 2015). However, early memory traces have not been documented in these MBOns, and the relationship between such putative traces and those in the presynaptic MBn is unexplored. Connectome studies revealed that DAns make direct connection with MBOns (Eichler et al., 2017; Takemura et al., 2017), opening the possibility that MBOns form traces independently of the MBn.

Here, we show that a cellular memory trace forms immediately after conditioning in the MBn axons occupying the α′3 compartment and in the downstream α′3 MBOn. Functional Ca2+ imaging reveals that aversive conditioning suppresses subsequent responses to the learned odor in both the presynaptic α′3 compartment and the postsynaptic α′3 MBOn across a similar time period, suggestive of a causal relationship. In vivo ACh imaging revealed that the suppressed Ca2+ responses are accompanied by reduced ACh release in the α′3 compartment, supporting the model that the α′3 MBOn memory trace occurs from suppressed presynaptic activity. We also show that the conditioning-induced suppression in the α′3 compartment does not occur from increased inhibition through the Resistance to dieldrin (Rdl) GABAA receptor, indicating that mechanisms other than Rdl receptor activation are responsible for the suppression of activity.

Materials and Methods

Fly husbandry.

Fly stocks were cultured on standard food at room temperature. Crosses were kept at 25°C with 70% relative humidity and a 12 h light, 12 h dark cycle except for the shibirets experiments in which flies were raised on standard food at 23°C until training and testing (see below for details). Fly lines used in this study include w1118 (BDSC, 3605), c305a-gal4 (Krashes et al., 2007), MB027B split gal4 (Aso et al., 2014b), 20XUAS-IVS-GCaMP6f (Chen et al., 2013), 20XUAS-shibirets (Pfeiffer et al., 2012), UAS-mCD::GFP (BDSC, 32197), UAS-Rdli8-10 (Liu et al., 2007), and UAS-GACh4.4 (Jing et al., 2018).

Behavioral experiments.

We used 2- to 6-d-old flies of mixed gender for behavior experiments. Standard aversive olfactory associative conditioning was performed as described previously (Beck et al., 2000). Approximately 50–60 flies were equilibrated in a room dimly lit with red light and with ∼70% humidity for >30 min in fresh food vials. Then they were loaded into a training tube where they received the following stimuli in sequence: 30 s air, 1 min of an odor (CS+) paired with 12 electric shock pulses at 90 V (1.25 s each pulse; 12 pulses, 30 V in Fig. 6B), 30 s of air, 1 min of a second odor (CS−) without shocks, 30 s air. We used 4-methylcyclohexanol (MCH) and 3-octanol (OCT) as odors for conditioning. To measure 3 min memory, flies were transferred into a T-maze, where they were allowed to choose between two arms containing the two odors for 2 min. The performance index (PI) was calculated as (number of flies choosing the correct arm) − (number of flies choosing the incorrect arm)/(total number of flies). A PI = 1 means that all flies choose the correct arm, and a PI = 0 means that the flies choose equally between the two arms. To measure memory at a later time, flies were transferred back to food vial until testing. For shibirets experiments, flies were trained in 23°C, then they were transferred to 30°C (for shibirets activation) or 23°C (Ctrl) immediately after training until testing of 15 min memory at that temperature.

In vivo imaging.

We used a customized chamber for in vivo training and imaging (see Fig. 1B) similar to that used in a previous report (Berry et al., 2018). Briefly, a single female fly was aspirated into a 200 μl pipette tip cut to allow only the head to be exposed. Females are used only because of their larger size. The proboscis of the fly was secured in the retracted position with myristic acid to reduce the movement of the brain during imaging. The fly was then located in a narrow slot cut from the lid of a 5 cm Petri dish. A small piece of stainless steel foil with a hole in it was glued to a 5 cm Petri dish and covered the slot and the fly to expose the head. The head was glued to the foil with UV glue; the antenna underneath the foil remained free of glue. A small optical window was cut in the head cuticle to expose the brain and the head was then covered with fresh saline containing the following (in mm): 124 NaCl, 3 KCl, 20 MOPS, 1.5 CaCl2, 4 MgCl2·6H2O, 5 NaHCO3, 1 NaH2PO4·H2O, 10 trehalose, 7 sucrose, and 10 glucose, pH 7.2. Then, the Petri dish with the fly was attached to a base platform with magnets. To deliver electric shock pulses to the fly, a custom shock platform made with shock grids used in standard aversive training was secured to the base platform so that the fly legs were in contact but with sufficient room so that the fly could temporarily break the contact.

The fly was positioned such that the α′ lobe neuropil was in the vertical line of the objective lens to facilitate distinguishing the α′3 and α′2 compartments. In this position, the α′3 compartment appears first as a crescent-shaped object with successive Z steps in the ventral direction. Moving ventrally, a donut-shaped ring comes into focus. We defined the α′3 region as essentially half the distance between the dorsal tip of the α′ lobe neuropil and the ring. The ring disappears with deeper imaging, being replaced by a triangular region of fluorescence. We defined the α′2 compartment for imaging purposes as the triangular region. Further down, the fluorescence region becomes circular and then eventually merges at the junction area with the β′ lobe. These definitions are consistent with our immunostaining results showing the MBn α′3/α′3 MBOn compartment with MB027B split gal4 (see Fig. 4) and the MB compartments illustrated in the literature (Aso et al., 2014b).

To deliver odors (MCH and OCT) to the fly, 100 ml/min air stream was diverted from flowing through a 20 ml glass vial containing 10 ml of mineral oil to flow through a 20 ml glass vial containing 10 ml of 1 × 10−3 dilution of odorant in mineral oil. This air stream was then blended into a 1000 ml/min fresh air stream before reaching the fly through a ∼3 mm glass pipette positioned ∼1 cm from the fly's antenna. After dissection, the flies were allowed to rest for 3 min under the microscope before odor exposure. For GCaMP6f and GACh imaging, 5 s of each odor was delivered to the flies with a 30 s interstimulus interval to confirm that the fly was alive and responding. Then, each fly was presented with 2 pulses of 5 s odor A (MCH or OCT) and 2 pulses of 5 s odor B (OCT or MCH) (“Pre” response) in alternating order. Each odor stimulus was separated by 30 s of fresh air. In the paired training protocol, flies were trained using a schedule identical to the protocol described for behavior (above) starting 5 min after the Pre odor exposure. For the unpaired training protocol, 1 min of electric shock pulses was presented 2 min after the Pre odor exposure (see Fig. 1C). Following training, the flies were presented with another set of 2 pulses of 5 s odor A and 2 pulses of 5 s odor B (“Post”) identical to the “Pre” stimulation.

A Leica TCS SP8 confocal microscope with a 488 nm argon laser and 25× water-immersion objective was used for imaging. Imaging began 1 min before the first 5 s odor exposure and ended 30 s after last 5 s odor exposure for both Pre and Post recording. Images were collected with a HyD detector (495–545 nm) at 2 Hz at a resolution of 256 × 256 pixels. The baseline fluorescence F0 was calculated as the average fluorescence across the 5 s before each odor exposure. Odor responses were calculated as the average fluorescence (normalized to F0 for each frame) within 5 s odor presentation. Responses to the two pulses of the same odor in Pre and Post tests were then averaged as the Pre or Post response for each fly.

Immunostaining.

For the immunostaining of GFP driven by c305a-gal4 and MB027B-gal4 (see Figs. 1A, 4A), whole brains were isolated and processed as described previously (Jenett et al., 2012). The primary antibodies used were rabbit anti-GFP (1:1000, Thermo Fisher Scientific, catalog #A11122) and mouse anti-nc82 (1:50, DSHB, RRID:AB_2314866). The secondary antibodies used were goat anti-rabbit IgG conjugated to Alexa Fluor 488 (1:800, Thermo Fisher Scientific, catalog #A11008) and goat anti-mouse IgG conjugated to Alexa Fluor 633 (1:500, Thermo Fisher Scientific, catalog #A21052). Images were collected using a 10× objective with a Leica TCS SP8 confocal microscope with 488 and 633 nm laser excitation. The step size was 1 μm and images were collected at a resolution of 515 × 512 pixels.

Experimental design and statistical analysis.

For behavioral experiments, a mixture of both male and female flies was used. For in vivo imaging, only female flies were used because of their larger size. Statistical analyses were performed using Prism 5 (GraphPad). All tests were two tailed and confidence levels were set at α = 0.05. Nonparametric tests were used for imaging data, whereas parametric tests were used for olfactory memory scores (PI) as the values are normally distributed (Walkinshaw et al., 2015). Sample sizes and statistical tests used for each experiment are listed in the figure legends.

Results

Aversive olfactory conditioning transiently suppresses responses to the learned odor in the axons of α′β′ MBn

Because prior behavioral studies demonstrated that the output of α′β′ MBn is required during acquisition, consolidation and retrieval of memories at early times after conditioning, we searched for memory traces in the axons of these neurons to identify the plasticity that might underlie this requirement (Krashes et al., 2007; Wang et al., 2008; Tan et al., 2010; Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2013). There are five compartments in α′β′ lobe neuropil (Fig. 1A), defined by the connections made with presynaptic modulatory DAn axons and the dendritic trees of postsynaptic MBOns (Mao and Davis, 2009; Aso et al., 2014a,b). We first focused on the α′3 region. To detect possible changes in odor responses in the α′3 compartment due to associative conditioning, we used in vivo functional imaging, training the flies under the confocal microscope and recording odor responses before (Pre) and after (Post) training (Fig. 1B,C). We used a paired training protocol consisting of 12 pulses electric shock at 90 V (US) presented simultaneously with a 1 min presentation of an odor (CS+) followed by a second unpaired odor (CS−) with a 30 s interodor interval. To control for nonassociative effects, we used a protocol in which the CS+ and US were explicitly unpaired (Fig. 1C). We validated our paired and unpaired training protocols using behavioral assays. The paired training protocol induced robust memory for the CS+ at both 3 min and 1 h after training, whereas, the unpaired training protocol produced no memory at either time point (Fig. 1D).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Pairing of odor and electric shock induces suppression to CS+ odors in the α′3 compartment. A, Left, Schematic diagram of MB showing α′β′ lobe neuropil and its five compartments from a frontal perspective. The line across α′3 indicates the plane that was selected for imaging. Middle: morphology of MB α′β′ lobe neuropil visualized using c305a-gal4 > mCD8::GFP. Right, Representative in vivo image of GCaMP6f expression in the α′3 region driven by c305a-gal4 (scale bar, 10 μm; brightness and contrast were adjusted for better visualization). B, Schematic diagram of the in vivo training setup. The fly head is glued to a thin metal plate. There is a small hole in the plate through which the head cuticle is dissected and the brain can be imaged. The electric shock pulses are delivered to the fly through an electric grid contacting the fly's legs. Odor is delivered to the fly via glass pipette (∼3 mm diameter) whose tip is close to the antenna of the fly (arrows indicate odor flow direction). C, Paired and unpaired training protocol. In paired training, 1 min of 12 pulses electric shock and odor A (CS+) were presented simultaneously followed by odor B (CS−) that was unpaired with shock. In unpaired training, 1 min of electric shock pulses was presented 3 min before odor A onset. Before training, two pulses of each odor were presented to the fly in alternating fashion for 5 s each with a 30 s interstimulus interval (Pre). After training, an identical set of odor pulses were presented to the fly (Post). D, Behavior performance of flies receiving paired and unpaired training protocols. Paired training induced robust memory performance at both 3 min and 1 h after training. However, unpaired training induced no memory at 3 min or 1 h after training. Mean ± SEM, ***p < 0.0001, ns, not significant, p ≥ 0.4976, one sample t test against a theoretical mean of “0,” n = 6. E, Pseudocolored peak responses of α′3 axon segments to CS+ and CS− before (Pre) and 3 min after (Post) paired training. F, Left top, Time course of GCaMP6f response in α′3 to CS+ (MCH) during 5 s odor presentation before (Pre, blue) and 3 min after (Post, red) paired or unpaired training. Traces show the average response (±SEM) across all flies tested. Bottom, Mean odor-evoked Pre and Post responses during the 5 s odor presentation. Right, Responses to the CS− (OCT). Data are shown as mean ± SEM. ***p = 0.0003; ns, not significant, p > 0.9999; repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests, n = 8. G, Left top, Time course of GCaMP6f response in α′3 to OCT (CS+) during a 5 s odor presentation before (Pre, blue) and 3 min after (Post, red) paired or unpaired training. Traces show the average response (±SEM) across all flies tested. Bottom, Mean odor-evoked Pre and Post responses during 5 s odor presentation. Right, Responses to the CS− (MCH). Data are shown as mean ± SEM. **p = 0.0022; ns, not significant, p ≥ 0.4230; repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests, n = 8. H, Top, Mean odor-evoked Pre and Post responses during 5 s odor presentation to MCH (CS+) with 6 pulses of 30 V electric shock training protocol. Bottom, Responses to the CS− (OCT). Mean ± SEM, **p = 0.0077; ns, not significant, p ≥ 0.6267; repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests, n = 6.

Before conditioning, the MBn axons in the α′3 compartment showed robust Ca2+ responses to the two odors used as CS+ or CS−, 4-methylcyclohexanol (MCH) and 3-octanol (OCT), detected with the reporter GCaMP6f expressed using the α′β′ MBn driver, c305a-gal4 (Fig. 1A,E). When we paired MCH (CS+) with 12 pulses electric shock followed by OCT without shock (CS−), we surprisingly observed a strong suppression of the Ca2+ response in α′3 compartment to MCH but not OCT at 3 min after pairing (Fig. 1E,F). This suppression was pairing specific since the suppression was not observed in unpaired group (Fig. 1F). In addition, the suppression was not odor specific since we observed a significant suppression using OCT as CS+ and MCH as CS− (Fig. 1G). A weaker training protocol with 6 pulses of 30 V electric shock also induced significant suppression to CS+ in α′3 compartment (Fig. 1H).

We then measured the duration of the suppressed response. For this, we imaged the responses in the α′3 compartment at 15, 30, and 45 min after paired and unpaired conditioning and compared them with preconditioning responses. We found that the suppression in the α′3 compartment persisted to 15 min but was absent at 30 and 45 min after conditioning (Fig. 2A, MCH as CS+). We observed the same time course using OCT as CS+ (Fig. 2B). We note that the posttraining responses tended to increase with time when compared with pretraining responses for both odors and in both paired and unpaired groups (Fig. 2A,B). The source of this drift is unknown, but the critical measure is the difference between the paired and unpaired conditions. Together, these results demonstrate that aversive olfactory conditioning induces a very early cellular memory trace in the α′3 compartment that is registered as a suppressed response specifically to the trained odor. This memory trace is transient and persists for <30 min.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Conditioning-induced suppression to CS+ in the α′3 compartment persists <30 min. A, Change of odor response in α′3 axon segments between Post and Pre odor stimuli at 3, 15, 30, and 45 min after paired or unpaired training with MCH as CS+ and OCT as CS−. The change of odor response within each fly was calculated as (Post-Pre)/Pre. The paired training-induced suppression of the CS+ persisted for at least 15 min and became nonsignificant at 30 and 45 min. No significant difference was observed in CS− between paired and unpaired groups at any time point. Data are shown as mean ± SEM. ***p = 0.0002, **p = 0.0047; ns, not significant, p ≥ 0.2345; Mann–Whitney U test, n = 8. B, Change of odor response in the α′3 compartment between Post and Pre at 3, 15, 30, and 45 min after paired or unpaired training with OCT as CS+ and MCH as CS−. The paired training-induced suppression of the CS+ persisted for at least 15 min and became nonsignificant at 30 and 45 min. No significant difference was observed in CS− between paired and unpaired groups at any time point. Data are shown as mean ± SEM. ***p = 0.0006, *p = 0.0379; ns, not significant, p ≥ 0.1304; Mann–Whitney U test, n = 8.

The α′β′ MBn, α′2 neuropil compartment also shows conditioning-induced suppression in CS+ odor responses

We next investigated whether the conditioning-induced suppression is specific to the α′3 compartment or whether it generalizes to the vertical axons of the α′β′ MBn. Like the axonal segments in the α′3 compartment, those in the α′2 compartment region (Fig. 3A) are also innervated by DAn of the PPL1 cluster (Mao and Davis, 2009; Aso et al., 2014a). In addition, activation of α′3 MBOn and α′2 MBOn both drive approach behavior, shown by the fly's preference to the light used to stimulate these neurons with CsChrimson (Aso et al., 2014a). Indeed, we observed a significant suppression to the CS+ in the α′2 compartment at 3 min using the paired but not the unpaired conditioning protocol (Fig. 3B,C, OCT as CS+). In addition, the suppression generalized to the other odor when used as CS+ (Fig. 3D). The α′1 compartment may also show a similar memory trace, but this region was difficult to resolve. The combined results reveal that olfactory aversive conditioning suppresses Ca2+ responses in the axon segments of the α′β′ MBn residing in the α′3 and α′2 compartments.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Pairing of odor and electric shock induces suppression to CS+ odors in the α′2 compartment. A, Left, Schematic diagram of MB showing α′β′ lobe neuropil and its five compartments. The line across α′2 indicates the plane that was selected for imaging. Middle: morphology of MB α′β′ lobe visualized using c305a-gal4 > mCD8::GFP. Right, Representative in vivo image of GCaMP6f expression in the α′2 region driven by c305a-gal4 (scale bar, 10 μm; brightness and contrast were adjusted for better visualization). B, Pseudocolored peak responses of α′2 to CS+ and CS− before (Pre) and 3 min after (Post) paired training. C, Left top, Time course of GCaMP6f response in α′2 to CS+ (OCT) during 5 s odor presentation before (Pre, blue) and 3 min after (Post, red) paired or unpaired training. Traces show the average response (±SEM) across all flies tested. Bottom, Mean odor-evoked Pre and Post responses during 5 s odor presentation. Right, Responses to the CS− (MCH). Data are shown as mean ± SEM. **p = 0.0085; ns, not significant, p ≥ 0.3129; repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests, n = 9. D, Left top, Time course of GCaMP6f response in α′2 to MCH (CS+) during a 5 s odor presentation before (Pre, blue) and 3 min after (Post, red) paired or unpaired training. Traces show the average response (±SEM) across all flies tested. Bottom, Mean odor-evoked Pre and Post responses during 5 s odor presentation. Right, Responses to the CS− (OCT). Data are shown as mean ± SEM. *p = 0.0192; ns, not significant, p > 0.9999; repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests, n = 7.

Aversive olfactory conditioning transiently suppresses responses to the learned odor in the dendrites of α′3 MBOn

Because the dendrites of the α′3 MBOn innervate the α′3 compartment (Fig. 4A), we wondered whether the suppressed responses of the axonal segments of α′β′ MBn in the α′3 compartment would be transmitted to the α′3 MBOn. This was the simplest model, assuming direct innervation and transfer of information from MBn to MBOn. However, given the complexity of this neuropil, which contains the processes of α′β′ MBn, PPL1 DAn, α′3 MBOn, octopaminergic/GABAergic anterior paired lateral (APL) neurons (Liu and Davis, 2009; Wu et al., 2013) and serotonergic/GABAergic dorsal paired medial (DPM) neurons (Lee et al., 2011; Haynes et al., 2015), any signal from α′β′ MBn may easily be modulated in some way.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Pairing of odor and electric shock induces suppression in α′3 MBOn. A, Morphology of postsynaptic MBOn that innervates α′3 compartment visualized using MB027B split gal4 > mCD8::GFP. The line across α′3 MBOn indicates the plane that was selected for imaging. B, Left, Mean odor-evoked GCaMP6f responses in α′3 MBOn to MCH (CS+) during 5 s odor presentation before (Pre, blue) and 3 min after (Post, red) paired or unpaired training with 12 pulses of 90 V electric shock. Right, Responses to OCT (CS−). Data are shown as mean ± SEM. ***p = 0.0008, **p ≤ 0.0021; repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests, n = 7. C, Pseudocolored peak responses of α′3 MBOn to CS+ (MCH) and CS− (OCT) before (Pre) and 3 min after (Post) paired training with 6 pulses of 30 V electric shock. D, Left top, Time course of GCaMP6f response in α′3 MBOn to CS+ (MCH) during 5 s odor presentation before (Pre, blue) and 3 min after (Post, red) paired or unpaired training with 6 pulses of 30 V electric shock. Traces show the average response (±SEM) across all flies tested. Bottom, Mean odor-evoked Pre and Post responses during the 5 s odor presentation. Right, Responses to the CS− (OCT). Data are shown as mean ± SEM. ***p = 0.0009, *p ≤ 0.0119; ns, not significant, p = 0.2951; repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests, n = 6. E, Left, Mean odor-evoked GCaMP6f responses in α′3 MBOn to OCT (CS+) during 5 s odor presentation before (Pre, blue) and 3 min after (Post, red) paired or unpaired training with 6 pulses of 30 V electric shock. Right, Responses to MCH (CS−). Data are shown as mean ± SEM. **p = 0.0029; ns, not significant, p ≥ 0.1236; repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests, n = 8. F, Change of odor response in α′3 MBOn between Post and Pre at 3, 15, and 30 min after paired or unpaired training (CS+:MCH, CS−:OCT). The paired training-induced suppression of CS+ persisted for at least 15 min and became nonsignificant at 30 min. No significant difference was observed in CS− between paired and unpaired groups at any time point. Data are shown as mean ± SEM. **p = 0.0087, *p = 0.0411; ns, not significant, p ≥ 0.3095; Mann–Whitney U test, n = 6. G, Mean odor-evoked GCaMP6f response at 30 min after training in α′3 MBOn during 5 s odor presentation with 12 pulses of 90 V electric shock (CS+:MCH, CS−:OCT). Data are shown as mean ± SEM. ns, not significant, p ≥ 0.1372; repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests, n = 7. H, Blocking synaptic output of α′3 MBOn immediately after training through testing impaired 15 min memory. Flies were trained at 23°C, transferred to 30°C immediately after training for 15 min, and tested at 30°C. Data are shown as mean ± SEM. *p = 0.0186; ns, not significant, p > 0.9999; two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests, n = 6.

We expressed GCaMP6f specifically in the α′3 MBOn with the split gal4 MB027B (Fig. 4A) and monitored responses before and after conditioning. The standard pairing protocol of 12 pulses, 90 V of electric shock along with 1 min of CS+ (MCH) induced a strong suppression of α′3 MBOn responses to both the CS+ and CS− at 3 min after conditioning (Fig. 4B). This suppression was also observed with both odors in the unpaired groups. We wondered whether this strong, CS+/CS− and paired/unpaired-independent suppression might mask an associative conditioning-induced memory trace in α′3 MBOn. To explore this possibility, we used weaker training protocols with reduced shock voltage and fewer pulses to minimize the hypothetical nonspecific suppression.

We found that nonspecific suppression was weak or undetectable using six shock pulses of 30 V, and that pairing and CS+ (MCH) odor-specific suppression was revealed (Fig. 4C,D). We did observe a slight but significant decrease in the responses to OCT (CS−) in both the paired and unpaired groups (Fig. 4D), however, there was no significant difference in the magnitude of the decrease between the two groups (Fig. 4F, 3 min time point). Again, the training-induced suppression in α′3 MBOn was specific to the CS+ odor, since we observed the same result when we used OCT as CS+ and MCH as CS− (Fig. 4E). Together, these results show that olfactory aversive conditioning also suppresses the CS+ odor response properties of the postsynaptic α′3 MBOn immediately after conditioning.

Given that DAn and other neuron types innervate the α′3 compartment, we considered the possibility that training may induce distinct memory traces in presynaptic α′3 MBn axons and postsynaptic α′3 MBOn dendrites. To address this possibility, we studied the duration of α′3 MBOn suppression to the conditioned odor and compared it with the time course for suppression in presynaptic α′3 axons. Using the 6 pulses, 30 V conditioning schedule, we found that the suppression in the α′3 MBOn dendrites persisted for at least 15 but not 30 min after conditioning using MCH as CS+ (Fig. 4F). There was no significant difference between paired and unpaired group responses to the CS− at either time point (Fig. 4F). We worried that a US consisting of 6 pulses at 30 V may be too weak to induce suppression lasting at least 30 min after conditioning, so we performed an experiment using 12 pulses of 90 V as the US. Using this stronger conditioning paradigm, we failed to observe significant suppression in the α′3 MBOn dendrites at 30 min after conditioning (Fig. 4G). We conclude that the suppressed response to the conditioned odor in the dendrites of the α′3 MBOn follows the same time course as that observed in the MBn axonal segments in the α′3 compartment.

Output of the α′3 MBOn is required for normal memory performance early after conditioning

Although prior studies revealed that α′3 MBOn activation drives approach behavior (Aso et al., 2014a), the requirement of these neurons in the early phases of aversive memory expression remains unknown. The memory trace described here and its time course suggests that the α′3 MBOn may be required for the formation of behavioral memory and/or its retrieval at early times after conditioning. We reasoned that if α′3 MBOn output is disrupted during memory retrieval, then the training-induced difference between the CS+ and CS− evoked α′3 MBOn output will be eliminated, which will result in an impairment of memory expression.

To probe this issue, we expressed the temperature sensitive shibirets transgene (shits) in α′3 MBOn with MB027B-gal4 driver. When we blocked α′3 MBOn output immediately after aversive training by elevating the temperature for 15 min before and during testing, we found a significant impairment in memory expression (Fig. 4H). These results indicate that the synaptic output of α′3 MBOn is required for the expression of early memory, and further suggest that the short-lived cellular memory trace which forms in the α′3 MBn axon segments and/or the α′3 MBOn, contributes to behavioral memory expression early after conditioning.

Conditioning-induced suppression of calcium responses in the α′3 MBn axon segment is accompanied by reduced ACh release

The results presented above indicate that olfactory aversive conditioning suppresses the Ca2+ responses to CS+ odor in both the presynaptic α′3 MBn axon segments and in the postsynaptic α′3 MBOn dendrites. The similar time courses for the suppression in both the presynaptic and postsynaptic elements (Figs. 2, 4F) is most consistent with the possibility that the postsynaptic memory trace is a reflection of the presynaptic memory trace. This model predicts that the reduced Ca2+ responses to the conditioned odor observed in the presynaptic α′3 MBn axon segments translates into reduced neurotransmitter release and a corresponding reduction of Ca2+ responses in the postsynaptic dendrites. To probe this possibility, we measured the release of ACh in α′3 compartment as detected by the dendrites of α′3 MBOn after aversive conditioning.

We expressed an ACh sensor GACh (Jing et al., 2018) in α′3 MBOn using MB027B-gal4 and imaged the postsynaptic α′3 MBOn before and after conditioning with 6 pulses of 30 V electric shock (Fig. 5A). We found that pairing odor (MCH) with electric shock significantly suppressed the GACh signal using the paired but not the unpaired protocol when measured at 3 min after conditioning (Fig. 5B). This suggests that paired conditioning leads to reduced ACh release in response to the CS+ odor in α′3 compartment. No suppression of GACh response to CS+ was observed at 30 min after conditioning (Fig. 5C). Because the time window of reduced ACh release matches the time window of α′3 MBn/α′3 MBOn suppressed Ca2+ responses, we conclude that the suppressed Ca2+ signal of the α′3 MBOn dendrites likely occurs by reduced ACh release from the α′3 MBn axon segments.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Pairing of odor and electric shock reduces ACh release in the α′3 compartment. A, Morphology of postsynaptic MBOn that innervates α′3 compartment visualized using MB027B split gal4 > mCD8::GFP. The line across α′3 MBOn indicates the plane that was selected for imaging. B, Left top, Time course of GACh (ACh sensor) response in the dendrites of α′3 MBOn to CS+ (MCH) during 5 s odor presentation before (Pre, blue) and 3 min after (Post, red) paired or unpaired training with 6 pulses of 30 V electric shock. Traces show the average response (±SEM) across all flies tested. Bottom, Mean odor-evoked Pre and Post responses during the 5 s odor presentation. Right, Responses to the CS− (OCT). Data are shown as mean ± SEM. *p = 0.0457; ns, not significant, p > 0.9999 for MCH in unpaired group, p = 0.3003 for OCT in paired group and p = 0.0856 for OCT in unpaired group; repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests, n = 6. C, Mean odor-evoked GACh response at 30 min after training in the dendrites of α′3 MBOn during 5 s odor presentation with 6 pulses of 30 V electric shock (CS+:MCH, CS−:OCT). Data are shown as mean ± SEM. ns, not significant, p ≥ 0.6660; repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests, n = 6.

Suppression in α′3 MBn axon segments is not due to increased GABAergic inhibition through the Rdl GABAA receptor

Many different cellular or network models can explain the reduced ACh release by the α′β′ MBn due to conditioning. One of the more attractive models envisions an increased inhibitory tone on the MBn due to conditioning. Part of the attraction of this inhibitory model stems from the broad innervation of the MB neuropil by the APL and DPM GABAergic neurons. To probe this model, we reduced GABAergic input by knocking down the Rdl (GABAA) receptor in α′β′ neurons using RNAi (Liu and Davis, 2009). When Rdl was reduced in α′β′ neurons, we found that 3 min memory was enhanced using 12 pulses, 30 V as the US (Fig. 6B), consistent with previous reports (Liu et al., 2007; Liu and Davis, 2009). We failed to see this enhancement using 12 pulses, 90 V as US (Fig. 6A), probably due to a ceiling effect. We then measured the short-term cellular memory trace that forms in the α′β′ MBn with and without reduced Rdl expression. When the receptor expression was reduced using RNAi knockdown, we failed to observe any impairment of the α′3 compartment memory trace with GCaMP6f (Fig. 6C), indicating that memory trace formation is independent of Rdl-mediated GABAergic inhibition. Instead, the suppression to the CS+ odor in the knocked down group was perhaps more robust compared with the control (59% vs 44% reduction; cf. Fig. 6C,D). This slight increase in suppression might underlie the enhanced 3 min memory when Rdl is reduced in α′β′ neurons. These results argue against a model for a substantial role of increased inhibition to explain the suppression to CS+ odors due to conditioning.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Suppression in α′3 is not induced by GABAergic inhibition through Rdl GABAA receptor. A, No change in 3 min memory with 12 pulses of 90 V electric shock when Rdl (GABAA receptor) was knocked down in α′β′ MBn. Data are shown as mean ± SEM. ns, Not significant; one-way ANOVA with Tukey's post hoc tests, n = 6. B, There was significant enhancement of 3 min memory expression with 12 pulses of 30 V electric shock when Rdl was knocked down in α′β′ MBn. Data are shown as mean ± SEM. *p ≤ 0.0391; one-way ANOVA with Tukey's post hoc tests, n = 6. C, Knocking down Rdl did not impair the odor suppression to the CS+ in the α′3 compartment. Left top, Time course of GCaMP6f response in the α′3 compartment to the CS+ (MCH) during a 5 s odor presentation before (Pre, blue) and 3 min after (Post, red) paired or unpaired training. Traces show the average response (±SEM) across all flies tested. Bottom, Mean odor-evoked Pre and Post responses during 5 s odor presentation. Right, Responses to the CS− (OCT). Data are shown as mean ± SEM. ***p = 0.0009; ns, not significant, p ≥ 0.1515; repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests, n = 8. D, Control for Rdl knockdown in α′β′ MBn. Left top, Time course of GCaMP6f response in the α′3 compartment to the CS+ (MCH) during 5 s odor presentation before (Pre, blue) and 3 min after (Post, red) paired or unpaired training. Traces show the average response (±SEM) across all flies tested. Bottom, Mean odor-evoked Pre and Post responses during 5 s odor presentation. Right, Responses to the CS− (OCT). Data are shown as mean ± SEM. ***p = 0.0006; ns, not significant, p ≥ 0.5049; repeated-measures two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc tests, n = 6.

Discussion

Here, we provide evidence for the existence of immediate cellular memory traces that form in at least two adjacent segments of the axons in the vertical lobe neuropil of the α′β′ MBn and at least one (α′3 MBOn) of the corresponding output neurons. These memory traces, detected as decreased Ca2+ responses to the CS+ odor immediately after conditioning when compared with preconditioning responses, and persisting for <30 min before the response properties return to the naive state, are consistent with the fact that α′β′ MBn are required for memory acquisition, consolidation and early memory retrieval (Krashes et al., 2007; Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2013). Several other previously characterized early memory traces due to odor conditioning provide an interesting background to these newly discovered traces (Davis, 2011). The neurites of the DPM neurons innervating the vertical MB lobe neuropil exhibit an increased Ca2+ response to the learned odor from ∼30–70 min after conditioning (Yu et al., 2005; Cervantes-Sandoval and Davis, 2012). A memory trace forms in the antennal lobe, registered as the recruitment of new projection neuron activity in response to the learned odor, that lasts <10 min after conditioning (Yu et al., 2004). The activity of GABAergic APL neurons that synapse in the vertical lobe neuropil of the MBn is suppressed for a period of a few minutes after conditioning (Liu and Davis, 2009). Further, in vivo functional imaging of the α′β′ MBn axons revealed an early memory trace displayed as increased Ca2+ influx by 30 min after conditioning that persists for at least 1 h (Wang et al., 2008; Tan et al., 2010; Cervantes-Sandoval and Davis, 2012). The action of these five memory traces, together along with other unknown traces, may provide the cellular modifications required for behavioral performance gains to be made across the first hour after conditioning. Memory traces in compartments other than α′3 and/or their MBOns may underlie the requirement of α′β′ MBn for memory retrieval beyond the first hour (Séjourné et al., 2011; Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2013; Owald et al., 2015).

However, the developmental trajectory of the memory traces forming in the α′β′ MBn lobe is of additional interest. As indicated above, a cellular memory trace forms in these neurons by 30 min after conditioning that is manifested as an increased Ca2+ response to the conditioned odor (Wang et al., 2008; Tan et al., 2010). The data presented here show that the α′β′ MBn axons become suppressed across the first ∼15 min after conditioning. The combined studies thus indicate that the CS+ odor response properties in the α′β′ MBn axons are initially suppressed after conditioning but then become enhanced at later times. The time courses for the two cellular memory traces do not match exactly (0–15 min for the suppression and ∼30–60 min for the increase) given our current data showing no detectable increase at 30 or 45 min, but this is easily explained by variation in the strength of conditioning or minor technical differences between the two studies. Thus, the most parsimonious conclusion is that the vertical axon compartments of the α′β′ MBn initially exhibit a suppressed response to the CS+ followed by an increased response with the transition from suppression to enhancement occurring somewhere between ∼30–45 min after conditioning. How this evolution in response properties from negative to positive with time translates into behavioral memory expression remains unclear.

The suppressed responses to the CS+ odor were found in both the axon segments of the α′β′ MBn and the dendrites of α′3 MBOn. Given that activation of α′3 MBOn drives approach behavior (Aso et al., 2014a), our results are consistent with the model that aversive conditioning promotes avoidance through suppressing the MBn–MBOn circuits that signal positive valence, at least across the time that the MBOn responses are suppressed. Notably, the memory traces in α′3 MBn and α′3 MBOn persisted for the similar time, raising the question of whether the suppressed responses form independently or whether the α′3 MBOn memory trace simply reflects the presynaptic one. Our data support the model in which the suppressed α′β′ MBn responses are simply transmitted to the MBOn from reduced synaptic activity: the suppressed Ca2+ response in α′3 MBn axon compartment is correlated with reduced ACh release and the suppressed response in the α′3 MBOn dendrites. Our behavioral data (Fig. 4H) suggest that if the early cellular memory traces that form in the α′3 MBn–MBOn circuit cannot be readout precisely, the expression of behavioral memory early after conditioning becomes impaired. However, we cannot exclude the possibility that memory traces can be formed independently in α′3 MBOn.

We formulated the hypothesis that the immediate suppression in α′3 MBn axons after aversive conditioning might be due to enhanced GABAergic input to the α′3 compartment in an effort to delineate the underlying mechanism. However, we failed to detect any impairment of the immediate suppression in α′3 axonal compartment when we knocked down Rdl GABAA receptor in α′β′ neurons. Thus, our data argue against attributing the suppression in the α′3 compartment to GABAergic inhibition through GABAA receptor.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (Grant 1R35NS097224 to R.L.D.). We thank the Fly Light Research Team at Janelia Research for providing gal4 lines and the 20XUAS-shibirets line.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Ronald L. Davis at rdavis{at}scripps.edu

References

  1. ↵
    1. Aso Y,
    2. Sitaraman D,
    3. Ichinose T,
    4. Kaun KR,
    5. Vogt K,
    6. Belliart-Guérin G,
    7. Plaçais PY,
    8. Robie AA,
    9. Yamagata N,
    10. Schnaitmann C,
    11. Rowell WJ,
    12. Johnston RM,
    13. Ngo TT,
    14. Chen N,
    15. Korff W,
    16. Nitabach MN,
    17. Heberlein U,
    18. Preat T,
    19. Branson KM,
    20. Tanimoto H, et al
    . (2014a) Mushroom body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-based action selection in Drosophila. Elife 3:e04580. doi:10.7554/eLife.04580 pmid:25535794
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Aso Y,
    2. Hattori D,
    3. Yu Y,
    4. Johnston RM,
    5. Iyer NA,
    6. Ngo TT,
    7. Dionne H,
    8. Abbott LF,
    9. Axel R,
    10. Tanimoto H,
    11. Rubin GM
    (2014b) The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learning. Elife 3:e04577. doi:10.7554/eLife.04577 pmid:25535793
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Aso Y,
    2. Siwanowicz I,
    3. Bräcker L,
    4. Ito K,
    5. Kitamoto T,
    6. Tanimoto H
    (2010) Specific dopaminergic neurons for the formation of labile aversive memory. Curr Biol 20:1445–1451. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.048 pmid:20637624
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Beck CD,
    2. Schroeder B,
    3. Davis RL
    (2000) Learning performance of normal and mutant Drosophila after repeated conditioning trials with discrete stimuli. J Neurosci 20:2944–2953. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-08-02944.2000 pmid:10751447
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Berry JA,
    2. Phan A,
    3. Davis RL
    (2018) Dopamine neurons mediate learning and forgetting through bidirectional modulation of a memory trace. Cell Rep 25:651–662.e5. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2018.09.051 pmid:30332645
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Burke CJ,
    2. Huetteroth W,
    3. Owald D,
    4. Perisse E,
    5. Krashes MJ,
    6. Das G,
    7. Gohl D,
    8. Silies M,
    9. Certel S,
    10. Waddell S
    (2012) Layered reward signalling through octopamine and dopamine in Drosophila. Nature 492:433–437. doi:10.1038/nature11614 pmid:23103875
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Cervantes-Sandoval I,
    2. Davis RL
    (2012) Distinct traces for appetitive versus aversive olfactory memories in DPM neurons of Drosophila. Curr Biol 22:1247–1252. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.009 pmid:22658595
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Cervantes-Sandoval I,
    2. Martin-Peña A,
    3. Berry JA,
    4. Davis RL
    (2013) System-like consolidation of olfactory memories in Drosophila. J Neurosci 33:9846–9854. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0451-13.2013 pmid:23739981
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Chen TW,
    2. Wardill TJ,
    3. Sun Y,
    4. Pulver SR,
    5. Renninger SL,
    6. Baohan A,
    7. Schreiter ER,
    8. Kerr RA,
    9. Orger MB,
    10. Jayaraman V,
    11. Looger LL,
    12. Svoboda K,
    13. Kim DS
    (2013) Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature 499:295–300. doi:10.1038/nature12354 pmid:23868258
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Claridge-Chang A,
    2. Roorda RD,
    3. Vrontou E,
    4. Sjulson L,
    5. Li H,
    6. Hirsh J,
    7. Miesenböck G
    (2009) Writing memories with light-addressable reinforcement circuitry. Cell 139:405–415. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2009.08.034 pmid:19837039
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Crittenden JR,
    2. Skoulakis EM,
    3. Han KA,
    4. Kalderon D,
    5. Davis RL
    (1998) Tripartite mushroom body architecture revealed by antigenic markers. Learn Mem 5:38–51. pmid:10454371
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Davis RL
    (1993) Mushroom bodies and Drosophila learning. Neuron 11:1–14. doi:10.1016/0896-6273(93)90266-T pmid:8338661
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Davis RL
    (2011) Traces of Drosophila memory. Neuron 70:8–19. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.012 pmid:21482352
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Eichler K,
    2. Li F,
    3. Litwin-Kumar A,
    4. Park Y,
    5. Andrade I,
    6. Schneider-Mizell CM,
    7. Saumweber T,
    8. Huser A,
    9. Eschbach C,
    10. Gerber B,
    11. Fetter RD,
    12. Truman JW,
    13. Priebe CE,
    14. Abbott LF,
    15. Thum AS,
    16. Zlatic M,
    17. Cardona A
    (2017) The complete connectome of a learning and memory centre in an insect brain. Nature 548:175–182. doi:10.1038/nature23455 pmid:28796202
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Haynes PR,
    2. Christmann BL,
    3. Griffith LC
    (2015) A single pair of neurons links sleep to memory consolidation in Drosophila melanogaster. Elife 4:e03868. doi:10.7554/eLife.03868 pmid:25564731
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Jenett A,
    2. Rubin GM,
    3. Ngo TT,
    4. Shepherd D,
    5. Murphy C,
    6. Dionne H,
    7. Pfeiffer BD,
    8. Cavallaro A,
    9. Hall D,
    10. Jeter J,
    11. Iyer N,
    12. Fetter D,
    13. Hausenfluck JH,
    14. Peng H,
    15. Trautman ET,
    16. Svirskas RR,
    17. Myers EW,
    18. Iwinski ZR,
    19. Aso Y,
    20. DePasquale GM, et al
    . (2012) A GAL4-driver line resource for Drosophila neurobiology. Cell Rep 2:991–1001. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2012.09.011 pmid:23063364
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Jing M,
    2. Zhang P,
    3. Wang G,
    4. Feng J,
    5. Mesik L,
    6. Zeng J,
    7. Jiang H,
    8. Wang S,
    9. Looby JC,
    10. Guagliardo NA,
    11. Langma LW,
    12. Lu J,
    13. Zuo Y,
    14. Talmage DA,
    15. Role LW,
    16. Barrett PQ,
    17. Zhang LI,
    18. Luo M,
    19. Song Y,
    20. Zhu JJ, et al
    . (2018) A genetically encoded fluorescent acetylcholine indicator for in vitro and in vivo studies. Nat Biotechnol 36:726–737. doi:10.1038/nbt.4184 pmid:29985477
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Krashes MJ,
    2. Keene AC,
    3. Leung B,
    4. Armstrong JD,
    5. Waddell S
    (2007) Sequential use of mushroom body neuron subsets during Drosophila odor memory processing. Neuron 53:103–115. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.021 pmid:17196534
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Lee PT,
    2. Lin HW,
    3. Chang YH,
    4. Fu TF,
    5. Dubnau J,
    6. Hirsh J,
    7. Lee T,
    8. Chiang AS
    (2011) Serotonin-mushroom body circuit modulating the formation of anesthesia-resistant memory in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:13794–13799. doi:10.1073/pnas.1019483108 pmid:21808003
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Lee T,
    2. Lee A,
    3. Luo L
    (1999) Development of the Drosophila mushroom bodies: sequential generation of three distinct types of neurons from a neuroblast. Development 126:4065–4076. pmid:10457015
    OpenUrlAbstract
  20. ↵
    1. Liu C,
    2. Plaçais PY,
    3. Yamagata N,
    4. Pfeiffer BD,
    5. Aso Y,
    6. Friedrich AB,
    7. Siwanowicz I,
    8. Rubin GM,
    9. Preat T,
    10. Tanimoto H
    (2012) A subset of dopamine neurons signals reward for odour memory in Drosophila. Nature 488:512–516. doi:10.1038/nature11304 pmid:22810589
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Liu X,
    2. Davis RL
    (2009) The GABAergic anterior paired lateral neuron suppresses and is suppressed by olfactory learning. Nat Neurosci 12:53–59. doi:10.1038/nn.2235 pmid:19043409
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Liu X,
    2. Krause WC,
    3. Davis RL
    (2007) GABAA receptor RDL inhibits Drosophila olfactory associative learning. Neuron 56:1090–1102. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.036 pmid:18093529
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Mao Z,
    2. Davis RL
    (2009) Eight different types of dopaminergic neurons innervate the Drosophila mushroom body neuropil: anatomical and physiological heterogeneity. Front Neural Circuits 3:5. doi:10.3389/neuro.04.005.2009 pmid:19597562
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Owald D,
    2. Felsenberg J,
    3. Talbot CB,
    4. Das G,
    5. Perisse E,
    6. Huetteroth W,
    7. Waddell S
    (2015) Activity of defined mushroom body output neurons underlies learned olfactory behavior in Drosophila. Neuron 86:417–427. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.025 pmid:25864636
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Pfeiffer BD,
    2. Truman JW,
    3. Rubin GM
    (2012) Using translational enhancers to increase transgene expression in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:6626–6631. doi:10.1073/pnas.1204520109 pmid:22493255
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    1. Schwaerzel M,
    2. Monastirioti M,
    3. Scholz H,
    4. Friggi-Grelin F,
    5. Birman S,
    6. Heisenberg M
    (2003) Dopamine and octopamine differentiate between aversive and appetitive olfactory memories in Drosophila. J Neurosci 23:10495–10502. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-33-10495.2003 pmid:14627633
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. ↵
    1. Séjourné J,
    2. Plaçais PY,
    3. Aso Y,
    4. Siwanowicz I,
    5. Trannoy S,
    6. Thoma V,
    7. Tedjakumala SR,
    8. Rubin GM,
    9. Tchénio P,
    10. Ito K,
    11. Isabel G,
    12. Tanimoto H,
    13. Preat T
    (2011) Mushroom body efferent neurons responsible for aversive olfactory memory retrieval in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci 14:903–910. doi:10.1038/nn.2846 pmid:21685917
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Takemura S,
    2. Aso Y,
    3. Hige T,
    4. Wong A,
    5. Lu Z,
    6. Xu CS,
    7. Rivlin PK,
    8. Hess H,
    9. Zhao T,
    10. Parag T,
    11. Berg S,
    12. Huang G,
    13. Katz W,
    14. Olbris DJ,
    15. Plaza S,
    16. Umayam L,
    17. Aniceto R,
    18. Chang LA,
    19. Lauchie S,
    20. Ogundeyi O, et al
    . (2017) A connectome of a learning and memory center in the adult Drosophila brain. Elife 6:e26975. doi:10.7554/eLife.26975 pmid:28718765
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Tan Y,
    2. Yu D,
    3. Pletting J,
    4. Davis RL
    (2010) Gilgamesh is required for rutabaga-independent olfactory learning in Drosophila. Neuron 67:810–820. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.020 pmid:20826312
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Tonegawa S,
    2. Liu X,
    3. Ramirez S,
    4. Redondo R
    (2015) Memory engram cells have come of age. Neuron 87:918–931. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.08.002 pmid:26335640
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Walkinshaw E,
    2. Gai Y,
    3. Farkas C,
    4. Richter D,
    5. Nicholas E,
    6. Keleman K,
    7. Davis RL
    (2015) Identification of genes that promote or inhibit olfactory memory formation in Drosophila. Genetics 199:1173–1182. doi:10.1534/genetics.114.173575 pmid:25644700
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Wang Y,
    2. Mamiya A,
    3. Chiang AS,
    4. Zhong Y
    (2008) Imaging of an early memory trace in the Drosophila mushroom body. J Neurosci 28:4368–4376. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2958-07.2008 pmid:18434515
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    1. Wu CL,
    2. Shih MF,
    3. Lee PT,
    4. Chiang AS
    (2013) An octopamine-mushroom body circuit modulates the formation of anesthesia-resistant memory in Drosophila. Curr Biol 23:2346–2354. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.09.056 pmid:24239122
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Yu D,
    2. Ponomarev A,
    3. Davis RL
    (2004) Altered representation of the spatial code for odors after olfactory classical conditioning: memory trace formation by synaptic recruitment. Neuron 42:437–449. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00217-X pmid:15134640
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Yu D,
    2. Keene AC,
    3. Srivatsan A,
    4. Waddell S,
    5. Davis RL
    (2005) Drosophila DPM neurons form a delayed and branch-specific memory trace after olfactory classical conditioning. Cell 123:945–957. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2005.09.037 pmid:16325586
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Yu D,
    2. Akalal DB,
    3. Davis RL
    (2006) Drosophila α/β mushroom body neurons form a branch-specific long-term cellular memory trace after spaced olfactory conditioning. Neuron 52:845–855. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.030 pmid:17145505
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 39 (46)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 39, Issue 46
13 Nov 2019
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Aversive Training Induces Both Presynaptic and Postsynaptic Suppression in Drosophila
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Aversive Training Induces Both Presynaptic and Postsynaptic Suppression in Drosophila
Xiaofan Zhang, Nathaniel C. Noyes, Jianzhi Zeng, Yulong Li, Ronald L. Davis
Journal of Neuroscience 13 November 2019, 39 (46) 9164-9172; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1420-19.2019

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Aversive Training Induces Both Presynaptic and Postsynaptic Suppression in Drosophila
Xiaofan Zhang, Nathaniel C. Noyes, Jianzhi Zeng, Yulong Li, Ronald L. Davis
Journal of Neuroscience 13 November 2019, 39 (46) 9164-9172; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1420-19.2019
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • Drosophila
  • mushroom body
  • olfactory conditioning
  • suppression

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • The human centromedial amygdala contributes to negative prediction error signaling during appetitive and aversive Pavlovian gustatory learning
  • Perceptual Difficulty Regulates Attentional Gain Modulations in Human Visual Cortex
  • Concurrent- and after-effects of medial temporal lobe stimulation on directed information flow to and from prefrontal and parietal cortices during memory formation
Show more Research Articles

Systems/Circuits

  • Dorsolateral Striatum is a Bottleneck for Responding to Task-Relevant Stimuli in a Learned Whisker Detection Task in Mice
  • The Basolateral Amygdala Sends a Mixed (GABAergic and Glutamatergic) Projection to the Mediodorsal Thalamic Nucleus
  • A Short Route for Reach Planning between Human V6A and the Motor Cortex
Show more Systems/Circuits
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.