Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Systems/Circuits

Dendritic Spikes Expand the Range of Well Tolerated Population Noise Structures

Alon Poleg-Polsky
Journal of Neuroscience 13 November 2019, 39 (46) 9173-9184; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0638-19.2019
Alon Poleg-Polsky
Department of Physiology and Biophysics, University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colorado 80045
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Alon Poleg-Polsky
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The brain operates surprisingly well despite the noisy nature of individual neurons. The central mechanism for noise mitigation in the nervous system is thought to involve averaging over multiple noise-corrupted inputs. Subsequently, there has been considerable interest in identifying noise structures that can be integrated linearly in a way that preserves reliable signal encoding. By analyzing realistic synaptic integration in biophysically accurate neuronal models, I report a complementary denoising approach that is mediated by focal dendritic spikes. Dendritic spikes might seem to be unlikely candidates for noise reduction due to their miniscule integration compartments and poor averaging abilities. Nonetheless, the extra thresholding step introduced by dendritic spike generation increases neuronal tolerance for a broad category of noise structures, some of which cannot be resolved well with averaging. This property of active dendrites compensates for compartment size constraints and expands the repertoire of conditions that can be processed by neuronal populations.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Noise, or random variability, is a prominent feature of the neuronal code and poses a fundamental challenge for information processing. To reconcile the surprisingly accurate output of the brain with the inherent noisiness of biological systems, previous work examined signal integration in idealized neurons. The notion that emerged from this body of work is that accurate signal representation relies largely on input averaging in neuronal dendrites. In contrast to the prevailing view, I show that denoising in simulated neurons with realistic morphology and biophysical properties follows a different strategy: dendritic spikes act as classifiers that assist in extracting information from a variety of noise structures that have been considered before to be particularly disruptive for reliable brain function.

  • active dendrite
  • NMDA spike
  • noise
  • noise correlation
  • simulation
  • synaptic integration

Introduction

Neurons generate variable responses to the repetitive presentation of the same stimulus (Faisal et al., 2008). This variability, or noise, can corrupt the neuronal representation of the signal and limit the amount of usable information in neuronal populations (Kohn et al., 2016). Numerous models have examined the function of neural networks in noisy conditions (Faisal et al., 2008; Franke et al., 2016; Kohn et al., 2016; Zylberberg et al., 2016). In virtually all studies, neurons were formulated to integrate their synaptic inputs linearly (McCulloch and Pitts, 1990). Linear integration is more likely for inputs that are distributed and activated in a sparse manner (Jia et al., 2010). In this regime, synaptic inputs are largely independent of each other and summate at the cell body.

However, most biological neurons are not linear integrators of synaptic inputs. Instead, dendritic voltage-dependent channels can dramatically affect synaptic processing. For example, numerous studies have demonstrated that dendrites can generate spikes in response to synaptic stimulation (Spencer and Kandel, 1961; Schiller et al., 2000; London and Häusser, 2005; Branco and Häusser, 2010; Major et al., 2013). The basic nonlinearity that results from dendritic spikes can underlie dynamic dendritic computational compartments, in which each dendritic unit integrates its inputs linearly and passes them through a nonlinear all-or-none filter (Mel, 1993; Poirazi et al., 2003; Polsky et al., 2004; London and Häusser, 2005; Major et al., 2013). Sufficiently strong activation of spatiotemporally coordinated synaptic inputs produces a regenerative dendritic spike that amplifies the synaptic drive to the dendrite and potentially boosts representation of salient input patterns (Lavzin et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2013; Palmer et al., 2014; Hawkins and Ahmad, 2016; Schmidt-Hieber et al., 2017).

Importantly, the size of the dendritic integration compartment for a dendritic spike can be surprisingly small; in many cell types, activation of only a handful of synaptic inputs is enough for generation of a dendritic spike (Major et al., 2008; Polsky et al., 2009; Branco and Häusser, 2011; Schmidt-Hieber et al., 2017). Although the concept of dendritic computational units is attractive, their small size may challenge their ability to accomplish reliable computations in the presence of a noisy synaptic drive.

Classically, the central neuronal denoising element has been assumed to be averaging over a large number of synaptic inputs (Fig. 1B). A rich theory emerged that divided noise structures between amenability and resistance to denoising by linear integration. However, a limitation of signal processing with dendritic spikes is that their integration compartments do not offer significant protection against high-variance noise. For this reason, trial-to-trial fluctuations that can be effectively negated by integration over multiple synaptic inputs in linear dendrites are likely to have a significant impact on the reliability of computations in active dendrites.

Here, I propose a distinct, and complementary role for dendritic subunits in information processing in the presence of noise. My theory is based on the observation that nonlinear dendritic spike generation enhances neuronal abilities to filter out noise based on a decision cutoff (Fig. 1C). To critically evaluate this concept, I first provide the conceptual rationale for how these denoising approaches apply to distinct input dynamics. Next, to demonstrate this effect in biophysically-realistic conditions, I test the prediction that active dendrites have unique processing capabilities in multi-compartmental models. My results demonstrate that dendritic units have an exceptional capability to mitigate noise structures that are paradoxically resistant to averaging, such as stimuli misclassification. Thus, active dendrites can compute in the presence of noise, and even surprisingly surpass the denoising capabilities of linear dendrites for a wide range of noise statistics. These outcomes hold for independent inputs, as well as for correlated presynaptic population dynamics. Taken together, the processing abilities of dendritic spikes challenge the noise tolerance rules formulated for linearly integrating neurons and extend the dynamic range of biological brains.

Materials and Methods

Computer simulations

Computer simulations were conducted on multicompartmental models using the NEURON 7.4 simulation platform. The modeled cell was subdivided into 483 compartments, with a maximum length of 19 μm. The area of the soma was 1350 μm2, and the total dendritic length was 3320 μm2. The resting membrane potential was set to −60 mV, the membrane resistance was set to 10,000 Ω·cm2, the axial resistance was 100 Ω·cm, and the membrane capacitance was set to 1 μF/μm2. Fast sodium channels (reversal potential = 50 mV, gNa = 200 mS/cm2), and delayed rectifier and slow non-inactivating potassium channels (reversal potential = −87 mV, gKdr = 30 mS/cm2, gKs = 1 mS/cm2) were used to allow for spike generation and adaptation, respectively (Lavzin et al., 2012).

Synapses.

Excitatory postsynaptic synapses contained AMPA receptors (AMPARs) and NMDA receptors (NMDARs). AMPAR currents had an instantaneous rise time and a decay time of 1.5 ms. NMDAR currents had a rise time of 2 ms and a decay time of 100 ms. The synaptic current reversed at zero, and the unitary AMPAR/NMDAR conductance was 1 nS. The NMDAR conductance voltage dependence was modeled as follows: gNMDA = Embedded Image where Vm is the local membrane potential. When a linear synaptic integration was modeled, synapses were distributed over the entire dendritic tree and NMDAR conductance was typically set to zero to prevent spurious dendritic spike generation by randomly clustered synapses.

Presynaptic stimulation, directional simulations:

The angle of preferred direction for each cell in the presynaptic population was chosen from a normal distribution with a mean at 180° and standard deviation (SD, σ) of 45°. The postsynaptic cell was assumed to prefer inputs from cells aligned with its preferred direction (PD) = 180°; toward this goal, the amplitude of each presynaptic cell was multiplied by e−iθi2/452, where θi is the difference (in degrees) between the PD angle of the presynaptic and the postsynaptic cell i. The direction selectivity index (DSI) was calculated as DSI = Embedded Image. The DSI of the presynaptic population was set to 0.33.

The tuning profile of presynaptic cells was set according to the following generalized normal distribution: Ri = Ai · Embedded Image, where Ri is the response from presynaptic cell i, α is the angle of stimulus direction, ω is the width of the presynaptic tuning profile (90°), and Ai is the response amplitude factor drawn at random for each presynaptic cell i before simulation run from a normal distribution with a mean of 4 and a SD of 4. Gaussian noise was implemented by changing the amplitude of each presynaptic cell individually by a value drawn at random for each trial from a normal distribution with the mean at 0 and variance equal to the noise-free presynaptic amplitude response. Directional classification errors were modeled by drawing a value from a normal distribution with a mean of 0 and SD of 45 for each presynaptic cell; this value was then added to the PD of each presynaptic cell and the tuning profile recalculated (Poleg-Polsky et al., 2018). This procedure effectively shifted the entire tuning profile of each presynaptic cell by a random value independently across trials.

Noise correlations.

Presynaptic population activity correlations in a trial were modeled iteratively over all 360 possible directions, in 20° advancements. In each iteration, a noise value was drawn as above for the corresponding direction angle (θj). The effect on the presynaptic responses in cell i was computed as a function of the distance of the cell's PD from that angle: Ai = e(PDi−θj)2/452. Next, a random value was drawn from a normal distribution whose variance was set by the noise level. Last, the amplitude/PD of the tuning profile of each cell was then changed by Ai multiplied by this random value for the “magnitude”/“directional” noises, respectively. This procedure guaranteed coordinated response fluctuations in cells with similar tuning profiles (Moreno-Bote et al., 2014; Pitkow et al., 2015; Kohn et al., 2016; Zylberberg et al., 2017).

To convert between presynaptic response amplitude and firing rate, the amplitude of each presynaptic cell was rounded to the nearest integer. This value indicated the number of presynaptic spikes from that cell. The temporal pattern was set by NEURON′s negexp function, with a mean interval between spikes of 20 ms. In addition to stimulus-mediated presynaptic activity, all presynaptic cells had a random firing rate of 4 Hz to emulate synaptic activation observed in the cortex. Where simulated, background synapses were activated with a random firing pattern; their average firing rate was 10 Hz.

Binary discrimination.

In these simulations, the presynaptic population was modeled as in the direction selectivity case. Only preferred and null (180° and 0°, respectively) directions were probed. Classification errors were introduced as in the simple model (Fig. 1) by randomly flipping signal identity in ∼15% of the presynaptic cells.

Analysis.

The number of stimulus-evoked spikes for each presynaptic cell was measured within a 100-ms-long period that commenced at stimulus presentation (500 ms after simulation initialization). The Fano factor (FF) was computed as the ratio between the variance = σ2 = Embedded Image and the mean (μ) firing rates across cells within the 100 ms period. The skewness was computed as Embedded Image, where n is the number of cells. All statistical analysis was performed in IgorPro version 7 (Wavemetrics).

The presence of dendritic spikes was measured as prolonged (>30 ms) depolarization above −30 mV in the activated dendritic location. Mutual information was measured as the entropy of signals across all stimulated directions minus the average entropy of individual directions. Unless noted otherwise, error bars indicate SD.

The code used to generate these data can be accessed from https://senselab.med.yale.edu/ModelDB/showmodel.cshtml?model=259732.

Analysis of experimental data

Neuronal spiking responses to presentation of drifting gratings were downloaded from the Collaborative Research in Computational Neuroscience Data Sharing (CRCNS) database (Scholl et al., 2013b).

Custom-made MATLAB (The MathWorks) script was used to extract firing rates for each of the 72 neurons in the database for repeated trials and stimulus conditions. The baseline firing rate for each trial (computed from 250 ms preceding and following stimulus presentation) was subtracted from the firing rates during stimulus presentation. Cells were included in the analysis if the responses had an observable directional (DSI > 0.2) but not orientation-selective components (Scholl et al., 2013a). Ten cells met these criteria. The PDs of the analyzed cells were determined by a vector sum of the responses to drifting grating in all 12 stimulus directions. FF was computed from firing rates recorded for repeated PD stimulus presentations. The angles of the vector sum for each stimulation round (12 directions) were recorded, and the standard deviation of the resulting population was used to measure PD angle dispersion (SDθPD).

Artificial tuning curves.

Artificial tuning curves were generated independently for each examined cell to estimate the effect of Gaussian noise on PD angle dispersion. Simulated responses were drawn from a normal distribution in which the mean was chosen to match the average response of the cell at the appropriate stimulus direction. The variance of the artificial distribution was set to replicate the FF of the recorded cell. PD angle dispersion of the artificial tuning curves was analyzed similarly to the observed data, from a similar number of stimulus presentation trials. The distributions of 10,000 artificial SDθPD values were compared against the calculated SDθPD from neuronal responses. A statistical significant difference was determined by the portion of artificial SDθPD below the observed PD angle dispersion.

Results

Noise sensitivity in linear versus active dendrites

To illustrate the dependence of synaptic integration on compartment size, consider a binary discrimination task where we assume that a neuron is presented with two stimuli such that the preferred stimulus (to which the neuron should respond) is represented by 1 arbitrary unit (AU) firing rate of the presynaptic population and a null (distractor) stimulus by 0 AU (Fig. 1A). We also assume that the trial-to-trial presynaptic input intensity follows a Gaussian (normal) distribution with a variance of one, giving a FF (variance/mean) of 1 for the preferred input, a realistic level of noise in the brain (Smith et al., 2013; Zhu et al., 2015; Zylberberg et al., 2016). We now ask how synaptic integration affects the ability of the postsynaptic cell to differentiate between the two stimuli in the presence of noise.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Distinct denoising operations in linear and active dendrites. A, Representative spike trains (left) in a simulated neuronal population that respond to a stimulus with normally distributed firing rates (right). The mean firing rates (gray) correspond to noise-free responses. Data represent simulation results that resemble neuronal firing statistics recorded in the brain (Churchland et al., 2010). B, Linear integration of a normally distributed population activity decreases the trial-to-trial variability of the postsynaptic cell. C, The effectiveness of denoising by synaptic averaging depends on the size of the integration compartment. In active dendrites, dendritic spikes generation is typically governed by a small number of synaptic inputs, which limits the ability of the dendritic spike to reduce the negative impact of noise ('thresholding mistakes', black). D–F, As in A–C but for a simple asymmetric noise distribution of population responses, where preferred stimulation is signaled with a single spike (DeWeese et al., 2003). Some presynaptic cells produce “classification errors” if they spike to the null or do not produce a response to the preferred stimulations (D). E, Linear synaptic integration of an asymmetric presynaptic population shifts the mean output away from the noise-free population activity, diminishing the difference between stimuli representations (“suboptimal summation”). F, Spikes form a decision threshold that can help increase the separation between postsynaptic stimuli representations, compensating for the imperfect linear integration of asymmetric noises.

The precision of the encoding is diminished by the overlap in the distributions of presynaptic signals (Fig. 1A, right). The intersection between the signals is undesired because it reduces the ability of an ideal observer to discriminate between the stimuli. Therefore, the goal of a denoising algorithm is to reduce this overlap between population activity distributions, with the optimal outcome of nonoverlapping stimuli representations. For such normal presynaptic population activity distribution, an acceptable denoising approach is a linear integration over the inputs. The trial-to-trial output of this algorithm also follows Gaussian distribution, which has the same mean as the original input but a narrower width (reduced from the original variance proportionally to the square root of the number of samples; Fig. 1B,C). Given a sufficient sample size, linear integration reliably distinguishes between the inputs; in the example shown in Figure 1B, a linear integrator operating on 200 inputs produces a nearly perfect separation between preferred and null stimuli representations.

In contrast to the whole-cell integrator, Gaussian noise structure degrades the reliability of signal processing in active dendrites with a realistic integration compartment size of 10 inputs (Fig. 1C). The averaging capacity of such dendritic subunits is not enough to eliminate the overlap between dendritic signal representations—resulting in erroneous amplification of the null stimulus or a lack of dendritic spiking in response to the preferred drive (Fig. 1C). Unreliable synaptic integration in active dendrites in the presence of a high-variance input distribution reveals a potential computational weakness of dendritic subunits (Fig. 1C).

While dendritic processing is challenged by noise-corrupted signals in this example, nonlinear dendritic computations do not necessarily require low-noise conditions. Instead, active dendrites may be particularly sensitive to high-variance noises but perform well in the presence of other noise modalities. Recent empirical and theoretical studies have shown that stochastic Gaussian noise is not the only, and likely not even the most prevalent noise structure in the brain (Beck et al., 2012; Babadi and Sompolinsky, 2014; Pitkow et al., 2015; Gollnick et al., 2016; Kohn et al., 2016; Inagaki et al., 2019). Active dendrites may perform better when exposed to these alternative noise modalities.

Dendritic spikes enhance signal estimation from asymmetric presynaptic distributions

While Gaussian distribution has been traditionally used to describe the trial-to-trial variability in the brain, most physiological responses to sensory stimulation actually follow asymmetric distributions (Buzsáki and Mizuseki, 2014). Population noise can take many forms, as I will show below. An important consequence of the asymmetric population variations is that the arithmetic mean no longer accurately describes noise-free input activity. This mathematical property reduces the denoising effectiveness of linear postsynaptic decoders because the (averaged) postsynaptic responses in the presence of noise diverge from the noise-free postsynaptic activity.

Simple examples of asymmetric response distributions to stimulation can be found in the primary auditory cortex that is stimulated with pure tones (DeWeese et al., 2003) or active touch encoding in the somatosensory cortex (Hires et al., 2015). Neurons in these cortices often respond with a single spike to their preferred stimulus and remain mostly silent otherwise (null trials; Fig. 1D). The behavior with respect to stimulation is almost binary and resembles processing in synthetic digital systems. A typical communication error in computers is a (mis)classification error, also known as a bit encoding error, which manifests as a mismatch between the transmitted and the received signals (Shannon, 1948). Similar classification errors are present in the brain; some neurons do not fire action potentials during the presentation of the preferred stimulus, and a nonzero rate of background activity implies that some cells will fire during the null trials (Fig. 1D, left; DeWeese et al., 2003). The resulting distributions of firing responses are skewed in the opposite direction for the preferred and null responses (Fig. 1D, right). Although most cells respond with a correct firing intensity to the stimulus in a given trial, the minority of cells that make classification errors pull the mean population activity toward the opposite response. The effect on the activity in the postsynaptic cell is significant, as the convergence of stimuli representations increases linearly with the percentage of the misclassifying cells (Fig. 1E).

In contrast to the Gaussian noise, pooling across multiple presynaptic cells does not reduce the negative impact of classification errors (Fig. 1E) indicating that linear integration is not the optimal strategy to address misclassification. Instead, this noise structure necessitates a denoising mechanism that is based on a decision threshold, such as the threshold formed by regenerative dendritic spikes (Fig. 1F; Hires et al., 2015). These theoretical considerations suggest that the function of dendritic units is impacted by classification errors to a lesser degree than the computations in linear dendrites, perhaps enhancing neuronal ability to reliably process this noise modality.

Distinct noise tolerance of linear versus active dendrites revealed in a multi-compartmental model of a cortical cell

Studying the conceptually distinct sensitivities to Gaussian noise and classification errors of linear and active dendrites requires making observations of the input-output signal transformation in the postsynaptic cell. Although recent advances in high-resolution imaging techniques and electrophysiological recordings will likely be able to accurately describe the dynamics of the inputs to a cell at a single synapse resolution and the resulting postsynaptic output, such data are not yet available. For this reason, I took a modeling approach: I simulated signal processing in a detailed multicompartmental NEURON model of a layer 2/3 cortical pyramidal cell in the visual cortex (see the Materials and Methods). This cell type was chosen because its basic biophysical properties and physiological function are both well understood (Larkum et al., 2007; Branco et al., 2010; Petersen and Crochet, 2013; Smith et al., 2013; Palmer et al., 2014; Ujfalussy et al., 2018).

To approximate a linear synaptic integration, I distributed AMPAR-containing synapses randomly over the entire dendritic tree of the postsynaptic cell (Fig. 2B). To model synaptic integration in computational compartments formed by dendritic spikes, synaptic inputs containing both AMPARs and NMDARs were clustered on a single branch (Fig. 2G). NMDARs, which help to mediate glutamatergic transmission in many neurons, can trigger NMDA spikes in otherwise relatively passive dendrites (Polsky et al., 2004; Gordon et al., 2006; Major et al., 2008; Antic et al., 2010; Branco and Häusser, 2011; Chalifoux and Carter, 2011; Katona et al., 2011; Lavzin et al., 2012; Major et al., 2013; Palmer et al., 2014; Kumar et al., 2018).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Distinct noise tolerance rules for linear versus active dendrites in a reconstructed cortical layer 2/3 pyramidal cell. A, Example presynaptic population dynamics with Gaussian noise (A1, FF = 1) and classification errors (A2, error rate = 15%). The mean population responses to the preferred/null stimuli were four and two action potentials, respectively. The baseline firing rates (4 Hz) were similar across the simulated inputs. Top, Distribution of population firing rates. Bottom, FF and the skewness calculated from the presynaptic distribution. B–F, Synaptic integration in linear dendrites simulated by 200 randomly distributed AMPAR synapses (stimulated branches marked in red, B). C, Top, Example presynaptic spike trains in a single trial. Dotted line marks the time of stimulus onset. Bottom, Postsynaptic voltages at the soma (marked with an arrow in B) in 10 representative trials in the presence of a Gaussian noise (green) or classification errors (blue). D, ROC curve computed by plotting the normalized number of postsynaptic spikes for the preferred versus the null stimuli presentations in 1000 simulation trials. Color coding as in A; black, noise-free simulation. E, Mutual information calculated from the postsynaptic output. ***p < 0.001, n = 1000 trials, ANOVA followed by Tukey's test. F, As in E as a function of the number of presynaptic inputs, shades indicate standard deviation. G–K, As for B–F for dendritic computational subunits engaged by activation of 10 clustered AMPAR/NMDAR synapses on the dendrite marked in red (G), which sampled a random subset of the inputs (C, gray). Arrows in G mark the dendritic and somatic recording locations.

Similar input statistics were presented in both integration schemes to ensure reliable comparison of noise tolerance rules in linear and active dendrites. The identity of the stimulus was encoded by the firing rates of the presynaptic population (Fig. 2 and Materials and Methods). In the absence of noise, presynaptic cells spiked with twice as many action potentials in response to the preferred stimulus versus the null stimulus (on average producing 4 and 2 spikes respectively). For a Gaussian population activity, the number of spikes for each presynaptic cell was drawn independently from a normal distribution with a variance = mean (Fig. 2A1). To model classification errors, 15% of the presynaptic cells were chosen at random to fire with the intensity of the opposite signal (Fig. 2A2). In addition to stimulus-evoked firing, all presynaptic cells had a baseline firing rate of 4 Hz.

To characterize postsynaptic noise sensitivity, I used receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis to compute the discrimination ability of a linear observer in the presence of noise. The normalized area under the ROC curve can vary between 0.5 (no discrimination) and 1 (perfect performance). In the nominally noise-free condition, the area under the ROC curve measured from the somatic firing of the model with linear dendrites was 0.9 ± 0.05 (the area under the curve did not reach 1 due to small levels of trial-to-trial variability mediated by random arrive times of presynaptic spike trains). The discrimination was not affected by the introduction of the Gaussian noise (area under ROC = 0.92 ± 0.06; Fig. 2D, green). As predicted by the conceptual model, this noise structure had a pronounced effect on synaptic integration in active dendrites; the area under the ROC curve diminished from 0.99 ± 0.01 to 0.78 ± 0.07; Fig. 2I, green). Similar results were observed when the information content of the postsynaptic output was measured with a mutual information metric. Mutual information is a useful indicator of the uncertainty between stimulus encoding and the output of a cell. The information is typically measured in bits, which specify the reduction in the number of yes/no questions needed to identify the stimulus after observing the outcome. The peak information in a binary discrimination task is 1 bit per trial, and a random input conveys zero bits. Gaussian noise did not change the information rate in linear dendrites (Fig. 2E, black vs green), but dramatically reduced the performance of active dendrites (0.37 ± 0.09 bits vs 0.99 ± 0.01 bits in control, Fig. 2J; p < 0.001, n = 1000, ANOVA followed with Tukey's test).

Next, I measured the effect of classification errors on integration in both linear and active dendrites. The area under the ROC curve and the mutual information calculated from the postsynaptic firing were significantly degraded by classification errors in linear dendrites (Fig. 2D,E, blue) despite signal transformation by somatic threshold nonlinearity (Fig. 2C). In contrast to the pernicious effect of classification errors on linear integration, this noise had a relatively mild effect on synaptic integration in active dendrites. Both the area under the ROC curve (0.92 ± 0.05; Fig. 2I, blue) and the postsynaptic mutual information (0.75 ± 0.08; Fig. 2J, blue) were significantly higher compared with the corresponding metrics in the presence of Gaussian noise (p < 0.001, n = 1000, ANOVA followed with Tukey's test).

These results, observed for a wide range of stimulation intensities (Fig. 2F,K), and imposed noise levels (Fig. 3) echo the enhanced tolerance of dendritic compartments to misclassification errors proposed by the conceptual model (Fig. 1).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Performance in linear and active dendrites as a function of noise levels. Information rate per trial (mean ± SD), measured from the number of somatic spikes as a function of the imposed Gaussian noise intensity (green) or classification error rate (blue). A total of 1000 simulation runs were conducted for each noise level for both noise modalities. Other model parameters were as in Figure 2.

Dendritic computations in the presence of background synaptic activity

In the simulations presented so far, the postsynaptic cell was stimulated with signal-bearing synapses only. However, cortical cells in vivo are exposed to a seemingly random background synaptic bombardment (London et al., 2010; Petersen, 2017). Variable membrane potentials mediated by the background drive can directly affect dendritic spike generation (Major et al., 2008; Lavzin et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2013). Can dendritic computations remain reliable in these challenging conditions?

To assess the sensitivity of dendritic processing to a more realistic synaptic environment, I added a signal-independent synaptic background. This background activity consisted of excitatory and inhibitory synapses distributed randomly over the dendritic tree (Fig. 4A and Materials and Methods). Activation of these inputs depolarized the cell, increased the trial-to-trial variability of the resting membrane potential and promoted background postsynaptic firing (Fig. 4B) (Fellous et al., 2003). Yet, even at high levels of background activity, the postsynaptic cell was able detect, and signal the occurrence of the preferred stimulus (Fig. 4B,C). As before, computations in active dendrites were robust to classification errors, while linear dendrites were more informative during Gaussian noise (Fig. 4C).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Active dendrites retain their performance in the presence of background activity. A, Schematic position of the signal-mediating AMPAR/NMDAR synapses located on a single branch as in Figure 2, activated over a background of a randomly distributed 300 excitatory and 60 inhibitory inputs. B, Top, Example spiking activity in signal bearing (black) and background (“bias,” gray) inputs. The dotted line indicates the time of stimulus presentation. Bottom, Representative responses of the postsynaptic cell in 10 trials. Dendritic and somatic recording locations are marked by arrows (A). C, Calculated mutual information as a function of the postsynaptic depolarization inflicted by background activity in models with active (top) or linear (bottom) dendrites. Black indicates noise-free simulation. Shades indicate standard deviation. n = 1000 repeats for each stimulus/condition. Note that the background activity does not significantly diminish the degree of information extracted by the postsynaptic cell over most bias levels.

Distinct noise tolerance of linear versus active dendrites in the computation of direction selectivity

Can the effects of noise on postsynaptic computations be generalized to more sophisticated computational tasks than a binary discrimination? To explore this question, I examined how noise affects direction selectivity (DS), a classic neurocomputation that is commonly used to analyze the impact of presynaptic signal variability on neuronal responses (Cafaro and Rieke, 2010; Franke et al., 2016; Kohn et al., 2016; Zylberberg et al., 2016). Dendritic computational subunits participate in the computation of direction selectivity (Oesch et al., 2005; Lavzin et al., 2012; Sivyer and Williams, 2013; Smith et al., 2013; Trenholm et al., 2014; Wilson et al., 2016); therefore, DS represents a relevant task to compare between synaptic integration in linear versus active dendrites.

I began probing directional information processing in the presence of noise by assigning directional tuning to presynaptic neurons (Fig. 5A, black). The amplitude and the direction of the preferred response varied between presynaptic cells (Materials and Methods). The Gaussian noise was modeled as before (Fig. 5A1, green). To account for multiple directions of stimulation, I implemented direction misclassification (directional errors) by shifting the directional preference of presynaptic cells (Figs. 5A2, blue): in each simulation trial, I rotated the entire directional tuning curve for each presynaptic cell by a random value taken from a normal distribution with a standard deviation of 45° (Poleg-Polsky et al., 2018). This manipulation mirrors the observed shifts of the peak of the tuning curves following adaptation in the cortex (Felsen et al., 2002; Benucci et al., 2013; Jeyabalaratnam et al., 2013; Jin et al., 2019) and other brain regions (Rivlin-Etzion et al., 2012).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Distinct noise sensitivity rules in linear versus active dendrites in the computation of direction selectivity. A, Illustration of directional noise structures used in the model. A1, Example tuning profile (firing rate vs direction of the stimulus) of a single presynaptic cell in the presence of a Gaussian noise, implemented as fluctuations along the vertical axis. Shades of green indicate the response probability in the presence of noise (black indicates the noise-free condition). A2, Directional tuning in the presence of classification errors. The noise was introduced as shifts in the directional profile along the bottom (direction) axis. Bottom, example construction of the presynaptic response from labile directional tuning (solid blue) in eight individual trials. Arrows mark the probed direction. B–D, Synaptic integration of directional information in linear dendrites. B, Polar plots of the number of postsynaptic action potentials versus the stimulated direction in the presence of population noises. Lines indicate the vector sum of responses in a stimulation round composed of eight directions. Insets, representative presynaptic spike trains in a single trial. Dotted line marks the time of stimulus onset. C, D, DSI (C) and mutual information (D) transmitted by the somatic firing of the postsynaptic cell in a stimulation round. E–G, As in B–D but for active dendrites. Dendritic directional tuning in E was computed from the peak potentials in the active dendrite. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, n = 1000 rounds, ANOVA followed by Tukey's test. Error bars indicate standard deviation.

Next, I recorded dendritic potentials and somatic spikes in the presence of Gaussian noise and directional errors (Fig. 4B–G) and analyzed the sharpness of the postsynaptic directional tuning with direction selectivity index Embedded Image and with mutual information, calculated from the somatic firing for 8 directions of stimulation. The DSI output in the linear dendrites model was only marginally better in the noise-free case than the DSI of the input (0.39 ± 0.32 vs 0.33; Fig. 5C). Direction selectivity in the presence of Gaussian noise was higher than in control (DSI = 0.51 ± 0.37), but reduced to 0.25 ± 0.23 when directional errors were introduced (Fig. 5C; p < 0.001 between the three simulated conditions, n = 1000, ANOVA followed by Tukey's test). Similar trends were observed in the information content of the postsynaptic firing response (Fig. 5D).

An important impact of active dendritic processing was on the DSI levels and the information content of the postsynaptic cell, both of which increase well above the linear model (DSI = 0.84 ± 0.22 and 1.2 ± 0.02 bits/trial respectively, Fig. 5F,G, noise-free condition, black). Similar to the binary discrimination task, the reliability of the computation was significantly impacted in the presence of Gaussian noise (Fig. 5E–G, green), but less so by directional errors (Fig. 5E–G, blue). These results indicate that the different noise tolerance rules for linear versus active dendrites are apparent in salient computations involving graded signals.

Experimental evidence for a non-Gaussian directional noise

My goal in the previous section was to determine whether input statistics influence the processing of neuronal information in active and passive dendrites. Comparison of response properties between synaptic integration scenarios required a well defined statistical description of the noise in the system. Given this constraint, I chose to characterize information processing with a particular implementation of a directional noise since the effectiveness of synaptic integration in active dendrites in the presence of this noise model should generalize to other non-Gaussian noise classes. Although the implemented structure of the non-Gaussian noise is likely to be present in real cells given the reported variability of directional responses (Felsen et al., 2002; Rivlin-Etzion et al., 2012; Benucci et al., 2013; Jeyabalaratnam et al., 2013; Jin et al., 2019), direct evidence for a non-Gaussian directional noise is lacking.

In the following analysis, I set out to examine the presence of non-Gaussian noise in DS cells. For this purpose, I examined a published database of electrophysiological recordings from mouse lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) relay neurons during visual stimulation with drifting grating (Scholl et al., 2013a). LGN provides the main visual input to the primary visual cortex and therefore is an appropriate brain region to study population dynamics that contribute to cortical processing.

We have previously developed an algorithm to test for the presence of directional noise (Poleg-Polsky et al., 2018). According to this model, neuronal responses are determined by a combination of Gaussian and non-Gaussian noise sources. The greatest possible extent of the Gaussian noise can be estimated from response variance and applied to simulate artificial directional tuning profiles. Comparison between the artificial distribution to the observed data can be used to reveal statistical properties that are not captured by Gaussian noise alone, indicating the presence of a non-Gaussian noise structure.

Figure 6 ranks the 10 identified DS neurons from the dataset (cells with DSI > 0.2; Materials and Methods) according to their response variability to repetitive stimuli presentations. The calculated FF imposes a cap on the level of Gaussian noise in each of these cells. Next, for each cell, I computed the vector sum of responses for each round of directional stimulation to obtain the angle (θ) of the preferred direction (Fig. 6A,B, left). I then quantified the dispersion of preferred angles between stimulation rounds by taking the standard deviation of the resulting angle distribution (SDθPD; Fig. 6C). To determine if Gaussian noise can fully explain preferred angle dispersion, I simulated artificial tuning profiles for each neuron in the dataset. The simulated firing rates were sampled from Gaussian distributions in which the mean corresponded to the average recorded directional tuning curve and the variance was computed to replicate the observed FF (Fig. 6A,B, right). SDθPD was measured from the same number of stimulation rounds as in the experiment (typically five or six). A total of 10,000 SDθPD values were computed for each cell and compared with the observed dispersion.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Experimental evidence for non-Gaussian directional noise. A, B, Left, Example experimentally recorded tuning profiles of two DS LGN cells from (Scholl et al., 2013). Top, Polar plots of firing rates and the calculated vector sums calculated from a round of responses to 12 stimulus directions. The variability of the preferred direction angles between stimulation rounds is captured by the standard deviation of vector sum angles (σθPD). Bottom, Tuning profile of the cells (mean ± SD) calculated from the same data. Right, Example simulated directional tuning curves, computed with Gaussian noise only. To generate these tuning profiles, artificial responses were drawn from a normal distribution. For each stimulus direction, the mean of the distribution matched the experimentally recorded average-firing rate for that particular direction; the variance was set to match the observed FF. C, Summary of tuning variability in all 10 DS LGN cells (defined as cells with DSI > 0.2, bottom) present in the dataset as a function of their response variability. Filled circles indicate the experimentally recorded preferred angle dispersion; open circles indicate the extent of the σθPD variability predicted by 10,000 simulations calculated as in A and B. Experimentally recorded preferred angle dispersion was significantly higher than predicted by pure Gaussian noise in three of four high-SNR (FF < 1) cells. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, bootstrap analysis. Error bars indicate standard deviation.

For all cells with a high FF (FF > 1), and thus a low signal-to-noise ratio (SNR), experimentally observed preferred direction variability fell within the range predicted by the Gaussian noise (Fig. 6B,C). However, three out of four neurons with high-SNR responses exhibited directional variability that could not be accounted for by Gaussian noise alone, demonstrating a presence of an additional, non-Gaussian source of variability that impacts the directional tuning in these low-variance cells.

Impact of input correlations on noise tolerance of active dendrites

Despite the importance of the effect of independent (uncorrelated) fluctuations in the presynaptic population, the discussion of the effect of noise on active dendrites is incomplete without considering coordinated presynaptic activity. Therefore, here I focused on correlations in the shared response variability to repeated presentation of the same stimulus, often referred to as noise correlations, which are different from signal correlations that indicate preference for similar stimuli (Cohen and Kohn, 2011; Kohn et al., 2016).

The impact of correlated population code on information detection in linear dendrites is well understood (Moreno-Bote et al., 2014; Pitkow et al., 2015; Franke et al., 2016; Kohn et al., 2016; Zylberberg et al., 2016; Zavitz et al., 2019). The black line in Figure 7A illustrates an example trajectory of a pair of direction-selective neurons, for diverse stimulus parameters in the absence of noise. This trajectory can be expanded into a multidimensional signal space for the entire population (Fig. 7B). A noise that pushes the population response away from this signal space is considered to be relatively benign, whereas noises that spread the responses along the signal space are thought to limit the precision with which the signal can be decoded (Fig. 7A,B). Thus, noise structures that mimic the signal being transmitted should have a disproportionally higher negative impact on the fidelity of information encoding by the postsynaptic cell.

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Signal-space noise correlations enhance information capacity of active dendrites. Population noise can be described by a vector in the n-dimensional space where each dimension corresponds to the activity of a single presynaptic cell. The trial-to-trial correlation structure between neuronal responses determines the amount of information that can be represented by the population. A, Illustrated neural response space with two neurons with different directional tuning curves (inset). Black indicates the noise-free activity plotted for all possible stimulus angles; green indicates a “magnitude” noise structure that shifts the trial-to-trial response variability along a vector that points in the orthogonal direction from the population curve. Magnitude noise was shown to be well tolerated in linear dendrites. Blue indicates “directional” noise correlations, which translate the responses along a tangent to the curve, are known to diminish the information that can be extracted with linear integration (Moreno-Bote et al., 2014; Zylberberg et al., 2016). B, Similar to A but for n presynaptic cells. Directional correlations tend to vary the population activity along the signal space that mostly lies within the shell of a hypersphere (Zylberberg et al., 2016). C, Postsynaptic DSI (top) and mutual information (bottom) versus imposed noise intensity in linear (left) and active (right) dendrites. Presynaptic tuning curves and correlations statistics remained similar across trials. Unlike linear dendrites, processing with dendritic spikes produced better DSI values and carried more information about the stimulus in the presence of directional correlations. Shades indicate standard error of the mean.

Given the difference in noise tolerance between linear versus active dendrites, is it possible that noise correlations have different effects on the decoding abilities of dendritic subunits? To address this question, I modeled correlated activity in the presynaptic population. The geometric representation of the signals in the multi-dimensional space helped to design correlated noise structures. The signal space, or the activity of the presynaptic population, is largely restricted to the shell of a hypersphere, where each axis represents the mean activation of a single presynaptic cell (Fig. 7B; Zylberberg et al., 2016). “Directional” correlations arise from similar directional errors in a subpopulation of the inputs and follow the curvature of the signal space (Fig. 7A,B, blue). Fluctuations that are orthogonal to the signal space point in the radial direction (Fig. 5A,B, green; Zylberberg et al., 2016). I simulated these “magnitude” correlations by picking similar response values from a Gaussian distribution across neurons with similar directional preference (Moreno-Bote et al., 2014; Zylberberg et al., 2016). This algorithm provides an intuitive explanation for how the formulation of uncorrelated directional and Gaussian noises can be expanded to include correlations. Based on the differential effects of uncorrelated noise structures, it is likely that the function of active dendrites will be better maintained in the presence of directional correlations. To examine this prediction, I varied the intensity of the noises by changing the mean size of the noise vector - either as a function of the direction of the stimulus (directional noise) or the amplitude of the Gaussian noise distribution (magnitude noise; Fig. 7C). Consistent with the findings of previous studies (Pitkow et al., 2015; Franke et al., 2016; Zylberberg et al., 2016), the DSI and the information calculated from the firing output of the linearly integrating postsynaptic cell were both consistently higher for magnitude noise correlations (Fig. 7C, left). Importantly, this relationship was reversed for active dendrites, where shifts in population activity along the signal space were integrated better than the orthogonal, magnitude correlations across much of the tested noise range (Fig. 7C, right). This result supports the conclusion that differences in synaptic integration between linear versus active dendrites found in independent population activity apply to correlated noise structures.

Discussion

In this study, I examine how synaptic integration in active dendrites affects the robustness of neuronal computations in noisy environments. By analyzing simulated postsynaptic responses to different empirically characterized population activity patterns, I describe for the first time that neuronal computations with dendritic processing units do not follow the noise tolerance rules established in conventional population models. Simple conceptual models and biophysically realistic simulations demonstrate differences between linear dendrites versus active dendrites, both in terms of denoising strategies, as well as robustness for discrimination of dissimilar noise structures (Fig. 8).

Figure 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 8.

Summary of the main findings. A, Traditionally, synaptic integration is considered to be linear and robust to stochastic changes in presynaptic input amplitudes (inset, green). B, C, Real neurons often integrate their inputs nonlinearly in dendritic computational subunits. These subunits were assumed to have a similar noise profile to linear integrators (B). Here, I report a distinct noise tolerance of active dendrites. Although susceptible to high-variance noise, dendritic compartments excel in the presence of binary classification errors, labile directional tuning, and noise correlations that limit the information that can be extracted with linear dendrites (C). Dendritic spikes can thus help to expand the repertoire of computationally benign noise structures.

Dendritic spikes are important for cortical processing

In mammals, various neuronal types possess the necessary dendritic machinery for focal dendritic spike generation (Manita et al., 2017). Dendritic spikes are potent mediators of somatic firing (Oesch et al., 2005; Grienberger et al., 2014; Palmer et al., 2014; Schmidt-Hieber et al., 2017). They contribute to neuronal computations in vivo and correlate with perception (Lavzin et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2013; Takahashi et al., 2016; Wilson et al., 2016; Manita et al., 2017; Schmidt-Hieber et al., 2017). The calcium influx associated with a dendritic spike is an important mechanism for induction of synaptic plasticity (Gambino et al., 2014), linking dendritic regenerativity with learning (Kaifosh and Losonczy, 2016; Schiess et al., 2016) and memory formation (Sheffield and Dombeck, 2015; Guerguiev et al., 2017).

I have shown that computations in active dendrites consistently outperform linear signal integration (Figs. 2, 3, 4, 5, 7). This finding resonates with the progressive enhancement of metrics for direction and orientation selectivity in the cortex that occurs in the presence of dendritic spikes. In mice, thalamic input to V1 is weakly tuned (Sun et al., 2016) similar to the presynaptic drive used in current simulations (Fig. 5). The increase in cortical visual selectivity between processing layers is most pronounced in neurons that exhibit dendritic nonlinear input-output transformation (Lavzin et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2013; Ujfalussy et al., 2015; Wilson et al., 2016; but see Jia et al., 2010). Suppression of dendritic spikes in the cortex by hyperpolarization or intracellular perfusion of NMDAR blockers results in decreased subthreshold and suprathreshold selectivity, confirming that dendritic amplification mechanisms are crucial for synaptic integration (Lavzin et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2013). These multiple lines of investigation strongly suggest that reliable dendritic spike generation is essential for brain function.

Currently, the consensus is that by combining neural responses, the brain can average out the impact of noise and increase the SNR. Linear input integration is especially useful in limiting stochastic fluctuations in synaptic signals, implying that a Gaussian noise source is relatively benign for signal decoding, but only when the sampled presynaptic population is large. As I have shown here, due to their relatively small integrative compartment sizes, dendritic spikes could be particularly sensitive to variability at the SNR levels that are common in the brain (Magee, 2000; Smith et al., 2013; Franke et al., 2016; Zylberberg et al., 2016; Zavitz et al., 2019).

Evidence for a non-Gaussian noise in the brain

Although we intuitively assume that noise fluctuations follow a Gaussian distribution, noise representation in the brain is a fundamental open question in neuroscience. I have taken the question of “what is noise?” one step further. A broad class of neuronal computations can be formulated as discrimination problems, where the network is tasked with sorting the input into distinct categories. Beck et al. (2012) provided compelling theoretical evidence to suggest that misclassification noise is practically unavoidable in complex nervous systems due to suboptimal inference (Beck et al., 2012). Misclassifications plague artificial neural networks; our conscious experience suggests that classification errors are common in biological brains. Instead of treating classification errors as computational failures, I propose to consider them as a noise source that decreases the accuracy of the computation.

Experimental results provide examples of classification errors. Cortical responses to sensory stimulation can be binary, composed of a single or no action potentials, as originally described by DeWeese et al. in the primary auditory cortex of the rat (DeWeese et al., 2003) and more recently seen in the mouse barrel cortex (Hires et al., 2015; Ranjbar-Slamloo and Arabzadeh, 2017). Neurons in these areas fire for most, but not all, repeated stimulation trials and often have a nonzero background firing rate in the absence of stimulation (DeWeese et al., 2003; Tao et al., 2016). Classification errors are harder to distinguish from the normally distributed fluctuations for stronger firing responses (DeWeese et al., 2003; Tao et al., 2016) and more complex stimulation paradigms (Fig. 6; Poleg-Polsky et al., 2018). The presence of classification errors and other classes of non-Gaussian noises can be estimated from response statistics that cannot be replicated with the observed levels of Gaussian noise (Fig. 6; Poleg-Polsky et al., 2018). Additional useful criterion that can discriminate between different noise structures is symmetry. By definition, a Gaussian process elicits responses that are normally (symmetrically) distributed around the noise-free mean (Figs. 1, 2; Poleg-Polsky et al., 2018). Thus, trial-to-trial response variability that is not symmetric is likely to arise from a different source. A possible example of a complex, non-stochastic signal variability in the brain was demonstrated by Gollnick et al. (Gollnick et al., 2016). In this study, wide-field voltage sensitive dye imaging from a sensory cortex revealed a probabilistic population encoding of whisker deflection intensity where the sensory input affected the likelihood of observing a response, but not the mean response intensity. With the caveat of interpreting neuronal firing from voltage imaging of population activity, the magnitude of whisker deflection could not be estimated correctly with a linearly operating ideal observer, in line with my findings that linear decoders of presynaptic neuronal population activity fail with a non-symmetric input profile.

Despite the high trial-to-trial SNR levels in the presence of classification errors, linear mechanisms cannot average away this noise, regardless of the size of the presynaptic population (Figs. 1, 2). The situation is different with dendritic computations because nonlinear synaptic integration utilizes spike threshold to increase the reliability of presynaptic input classification. In Figures 1, 2, 3, and 4, I showed how this process works for a binary decision process; the probability of dendritic spike initiation encodes the information in the case of a continuous variable (Fig. 5). These results are consistent with, and expand on, previous theoretical observations that dendritic computations are reliable with high-SNR manipulations (Rhodes, 2008; Wu and Mel, 2009; Hawkins and Ahmad, 2016).

Synaptic integration of correlated noise structures

Studies of neuronal population dynamics have primarily focused on network computations emerging from interactions between highly simplified, linearly integrating neurons (Seung and Sompolinsky, 1993; Zohary et al., 1994; Vogels et al., 2011; but see Zylberberg et al., 2017). These investigations revealed that coordinated neuronal activity has a substantial impact on information encoding. Arguably, one of the most interesting findings is that correlations that change the variability of neuronal responses along the direction parallel to the multi-dimensional signal space distort the population activity in a way that cannot be averaged out, limiting the accuracy of stimulus decoding. This correlation structure is known as differential (Moreno-Bote et al., 2014), choice (Pitkow et al., 2015) or directional noise (Zylberberg et al., 2016). To understand why these correlations are information limiting, consider estimation of stimulus direction from responses perturbed by this noise structure. The activity of the population shifts from the noise-free position on the signal space toward another point on (or close to) that space, which by definition represents a noise-free response to a different stimulus direction. The estimation of an ideal linear decoder will deviate from the true stimulus, regardless of the number of presynaptic inputs (Kohn et al., 2016). Thus, similar to uncorrelated noise structures, unreliable encoding of stimulus identity degrades the performance of a linear integrator. Because differential/directional correlations are prevalent in the brain, neural coding has been suggested to be inherently suboptimal (Pitkow et al., 2015).

My results support the opposite conclusion: directional correlations are a relatively benign noise structure for signal processing in small dendritic integration compartments. The quality of information transfer by a small number of correlated inputs is already mostly independent of the structure of the noise, at least for a linear decoder (Pitkow et al., 2015). As in the uncorrelated directional noise condition, the dendritic spike threshold provides a decision cutoff that allows the postsynaptic cell to categorize the stimulus. As a result, active dendrites do not directly help in addressing the question of what was the direction of the stimulus. Rather, they signal the probability that the stimulus was in the preferred, null or intermediate directions, which is likely to be more behaviorally-relevant information.

To conclude, my analysis of a binary discrimination task and both correlated and uncorrelated directional noises drew strong conclusions about information processing in active dendrites. Computations performed with dendritic spikes are necessary to enhance salient stimuli features, but the conventional wisdom is that synaptic noise should have a significant negative impact on the reliability of dendritic calculations. In contrast to this view, I present theoretical arguments in favor of the idea that dendritic computational compartments have robust tolerance to noise. I argue that active dendrites provide neurons with an optimal strategy to process noise structures that differ from the “benign” noise types established for linear neurons. These conclusions characterize a key component of cortical signal processing and highlight the importance of studying the impact of noise in the context of postsynaptic integration, because signal transformation in dendrites can fundamentally change our understanding of how brains process sensory information.

Footnotes

  • I thank John Gaynes, Gautam Awatramani, Jeffrey Diamond, and Joel Zylberberg for helpful comments on this manuscript.

  • The author declares no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Alon Poleg-Polsky at alon.poleg-polsky{at}ucdenver.edu

References

  1. ↵
    1. Antic SD,
    2. Zhou WL,
    3. Moore AR,
    4. Short SM,
    5. Ikonomu KD
    (2010) The decade of the dendritic NMDA spike. J Neurosci Res 88:2991–3001. doi:10.1002/jnr.22444 pmid:20544831
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Babadi B,
    2. Sompolinsky H
    (2014) Sparseness and expansion in sensory representations. Neuron 83:1213–1226. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.035 pmid:25155954
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Beck JM,
    2. Ma WJ,
    3. Pitkow X,
    4. Latham PE,
    5. Pouget A
    (2012) Not noisy, just wrong: the role of suboptimal inference in behavioral variability. Neuron 74:30–39. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.016 pmid:22500627
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Benucci A,
    2. Saleem AB,
    3. Carandini M
    (2013) Adaptation maintains population homeostasis in primary visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 16:724–729. doi:10.1038/nn.3382 pmid:23603708
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Branco T,
    2. Häusser M
    (2010) The single dendritic branch as a fundamental functional unit in the nervous system. Curr Opin Neurobiol 20:494–502. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2010.07.009 pmid:20800473
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Branco T,
    2. Häusser M
    (2011) Synaptic integration gradients in single cortical pyramidal cell dendrites. Neuron 69:885–892. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.006 pmid:21382549
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Branco T,
    2. Clark BA,
    3. Häusser M
    (2010) Dendritic discrimination of temporal input sequences in cortical neurons. Science 329:1671–1675. doi:10.1126/science.1189664 pmid:20705816
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Buzsáki G,
    2. Mizuseki K
    (2014) The log-dynamic brain: how skewed distributions affect network operations. Nat Rev Neurosci 15:264–278. doi:10.1038/nrn3687 pmid:24569488
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Cafaro J,
    2. Rieke F
    (2010) Noise correlations improve response fidelity and stimulus encoding. Nature 468:964–967. doi:10.1038/nature09570 pmid:21131948
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Chalifoux JR,
    2. Carter AG
    (2011) Glutamate spillover promotes the generation of NMDA spikes. J Neurosci 31:16435–16446. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2777-11.2011 pmid:22072693
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Churchland MM,
    2. Yu BM,
    3. Cunningham JP,
    4. Sugrue LP,
    5. Cohen MR,
    6. Corrado GS,
    7. Newsome WT,
    8. Clark AM,
    9. Hosseini P,
    10. Scott BB,
    11. Bradley DC,
    12. Smith MA,
    13. Kohn A,
    14. Movshon JA,
    15. Armstrong KM,
    16. Moore T,
    17. Chang SW,
    18. Snyder LH,
    19. Lisberger SG,
    20. Priebe NJ, et al
    . (2010) Stimulus onset quenches neural variability: a widespread cortical phenomenon. Nat Neurosci 13:369–378. doi:10.1038/nn.2501 pmid:20173745
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Cohen MR,
    2. Kohn A
    (2011) Measuring and interpreting neuronal correlations. Nat Neurosci 14:811–819. doi:10.1038/nn.2842 pmid:21709677
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. DeWeese MR,
    2. Wehr M,
    3. Zador AM
    (2003) Binary spiking in auditory cortex. J Neurosci 23:7940–7949. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-21-07940.2003 pmid:12944525
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Faisal AA,
    2. Selen LP,
    3. Wolpert DM
    (2008) Noise in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:292–303. doi:10.1038/nrn2258 pmid:18319728
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Fellous JM,
    2. Rudolph M,
    3. Destexhe A,
    4. Sejnowski TJ
    (2003) Synaptic background noise controls the input/output characteristics of single cells in an in vitro model of in vivo activity. Neuroscience 122:811–829. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.08.027 pmid:14622924
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Felsen G,
    2. Shen YS,
    3. Yao H,
    4. Spor G,
    5. Li C,
    6. Dan Y
    (2002) Dynamic modification of cortical orientation tuning mediated by recurrent connections. Neuron 36:945–954. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01011-5 pmid:12467597
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Franke F,
    2. Fiscella M,
    3. Sevelev M,
    4. Roska B,
    5. Hierlemann A,
    6. da Silveira RA
    (2016) Structures of neural correlation and how they favor coding. Neuron 89:409–422. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.037 pmid:26796692
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Gambino F,
    2. Pagès S,
    3. Kehayas V,
    4. Baptista D,
    5. Tatti R,
    6. Carleton A,
    7. Holtmaat A
    (2014) Sensory-evoked LTP driven by dendritic plateau potentials in vivo. Nature 515:116–119. doi:10.1038/nature13664 pmid:25174710
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Gollnick CA,
    2. Millard DC,
    3. Ortiz AD,
    4. Bellamkonda RV,
    5. Stanley GB
    (2016) Response reliability observed with voltage-sensitive dye imaging of cortical layer 2/3: the probability of activation hypothesis. J Neurophysiol 115:2456–2469. doi:10.1152/jn.00547.2015 pmid:26864758
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Gordon U,
    2. Polsky A,
    3. Schiller J
    (2006) Plasticity compartments in basal dendrites of neocortical pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci 26:12717–12726. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3502-06.2006 pmid:17151275
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Grienberger C,
    2. Chen X,
    3. Konnerth A
    (2014) NMDA receptor-dependent multidendrite Ca(2+) spikes required for hippocampal burst firing in vivo. Neuron 81:1274–1281. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.014 pmid:24560703
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Guerguiev J,
    2. Lillicrap TP,
    3. Richards BA
    (2017) Towards deep learning with segregated dendrites. Elife 6:e22901. doi:10.7554/eLife.22901 pmid:29205151
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Hawkins J,
    2. Ahmad S
    (2016) Why neurons have thousands of synapses, a theory of sequence memory in neocortex. Front Neural Circuits 10:23. doi:10.3389/fncir.2016.00023 pmid:27065813
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Hires SA,
    2. Gutnisky DA,
    3. Yu J,
    4. O'Connor DH,
    5. Svoboda K
    (2015) Low-noise encoding of active touch by layer 4 in the somatosensory cortex. Elife 4. doi:10.7554/eLife.06619 pmid:26245232
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Inagaki HK,
    2. Fontolan L,
    3. Romani S,
    4. Svoboda K
    (2019) Discrete attractor dynamics underlies persistent activity in the frontal cortex. Nature 566:212–217. doi:10.1038/s41586-019-0919-7 pmid:30728503
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Jeyabalaratnam J,
    2. Bharmauria V,
    3. Bachatene L,
    4. Cattan S,
    5. Angers A,
    6. Molotchnikoff S
    (2013) Adaptation shifts preferred orientation of tuning curve in the mouse visual cortex. PLoS One 8:e64294. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0064294 pmid:23717586
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Jia H,
    2. Rochefort NL,
    3. Chen X,
    4. Konnerth A
    (2010) Dendritic organization of sensory input to cortical neurons in vivo. Nature 464:1307–1312. doi:10.1038/nature08947 pmid:20428163
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Jin M,
    2. Beck JM,
    3. Glickfeld LL
    (2019) Neuronal adaptation reveals a suboptimal decoding of orientation tuned populations in the mouse visual cortex. J Neurosci 39:3867–3881. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3172-18.2019 pmid:30833509
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Kaifosh P,
    2. Losonczy A
    (2016) Mnemonic functions for nonlinear dendritic integration in hippocampal pyramidal circuits. Neuron 90:622–634. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.019 pmid:27146266
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Katona G,
    2. Kaszás A,
    3. Turi GF,
    4. Hájos N,
    5. Tamás G,
    6. Vizi ES,
    7. Rózsa B
    (2011) Roller coaster scanning reveals spontaneous triggering of dendritic spikes in CA1 interneurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:2148–2153. doi:10.1073/pnas.1009270108 pmid:21224413
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    1. Kohn A,
    2. Coen-Cagli R,
    3. Kanitscheider I,
    4. Pouget A
    (2016) Correlations and neuronal population information. Annu Rev Neurosci 39:237–256. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-070815-013851 pmid:27145916
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Kumar A,
    2. Schiff O,
    3. Barkai E,
    4. Mel BW,
    5. Poleg-Polsky A,
    6. Schiller J
    (2018) NMDA spikes mediate amplification of inputs in the rat piriform cortex. Elife 7:e38446. doi:10.7554/eLife.38446 pmid:30575520
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Larkum ME,
    2. Waters J,
    3. Sakmann B,
    4. Helmchen F
    (2007) Dendritic spikes in apical dendrites of neocortical layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci 27:8999–9008. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1717-07.2007 pmid:17715337
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    1. Lavzin M,
    2. Rapoport S,
    3. Polsky A,
    4. Garion L,
    5. Schiller J
    (2012) Nonlinear dendritic processing determines angular tuning of barrel cortex neurons in vivo. Nature 490:397–401. doi:10.1038/nature11451 pmid:22940864
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. London M,
    2. Häusser M
    (2005) Dendritic computation. Annu Rev Neurosci 28:503–532. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135703 pmid:16033324
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. London M,
    2. Roth A,
    3. Beeren L,
    4. Häusser M,
    5. Latham PE
    (2010) Sensitivity to perturbations in vivo implies high noise and suggests rate coding in cortex. Nature 466:123–127. doi:10.1038/nature09086 pmid:20596024
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Magee JC
    (2000) Dendritic integration of excitatory synaptic input. Nat Rev Neurosci 1:181–190. doi:10.1038/35044552 pmid:11257906
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Major G,
    2. Polsky A,
    3. Denk W,
    4. Schiller J,
    5. Tank DW
    (2008) Spatiotemporally graded NMDA spike/plateau potentials in basal dendrites of neocortical pyramidal neurons. J Neurophysiol 99:2584–2601. doi:10.1152/jn.00011.2008 pmid:18337370
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Major G,
    2. Larkum ME,
    3. Schiller J
    (2013) Active properties of neocortical pyramidal neuron dendrites. Annu Rev Neurosci 36:1–24. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150343 pmid:23841837
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Manita S,
    2. Miyakawa H,
    3. Kitamura K,
    4. Murayama M
    (2017) Dendritic spikes in sensory perception. Front Cell Neurosci 11:29. doi:10.3389/fncel.2017.00029 pmid:28261060
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. McCulloch WS,
    2. Pitts W
    (1990) A logical calculus of the ideas immanent in nervous activity. 1943. Bull Math Biol 52:99–115; discussion 73–97. pmid:2185863
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Mel BW
    (1993) Synaptic integration in an excitable dendritic tree. J Neurophysiol 70:1086–1101. doi:10.1152/jn.1993.70.3.1086 pmid:8229160
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Moreno-Bote R,
    2. Beck J,
    3. Kanitscheider I,
    4. Pitkow X,
    5. Latham P,
    6. Pouget A
    (2014) Information-limiting correlations. Nat Neurosci 17:1410–1417. doi:10.1038/nn.3807 pmid:25195105
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Oesch N,
    2. Euler T,
    3. Taylor WR
    (2005) Direction-selective dendritic action potentials in rabbit retina. Neuron 47:739–750. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.06.036 pmid:16129402
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Palmer LM,
    2. Shai AS,
    3. Reeve JE,
    4. Anderson HL,
    5. Paulsen O,
    6. Larkum ME
    (2014) NMDA spikes enhance action potential generation during sensory input. Nat Neurosci 17:383–390. doi:10.1038/nn.3646 pmid:24487231
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Petersen CC,
    2. Crochet S
    (2013) Synaptic computation and sensory processing in neocortical layer 2/3. Neuron 78:28–48. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.020 pmid:23583106
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Petersen CCH
    (2017) Whole-cell recording of neuronal membrane potential during behavior. Neuron 95:1266–1281. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.049 pmid:28910617
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Pitkow X,
    2. Liu S,
    3. Angelaki DE,
    4. DeAngelis GC,
    5. Pouget A
    (2015) How can single sensory neurons predict behavior? Neuron 87:411–423. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.033 pmid:26182422
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Poirazi P,
    2. Brannon T,
    3. Mel BW
    (2003) Pyramidal neuron as two-layer neural network. Neuron 37:989–999. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00149-1 pmid:12670427
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Poleg-Polsky A,
    2. Ding H,
    3. Diamond JS
    (2018) Functional compartmentalization within starburst amacrine cell dendrites in the retina. Cell Rep 22:2898–2908. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2018.02.064 pmid:29539419
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Polsky A,
    2. Mel BW,
    3. Schiller J
    (2004) Computational subunits in thin dendrites of pyramidal cells. Nat Neurosci 7:621–627. doi:10.1038/nn1253 pmid:15156147
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Polsky A,
    2. Mel B,
    3. Schiller J
    (2009) Encoding and decoding bursts by NMDA spikes in basal dendrites of layer 5 pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci 29:11891–11903. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5250-08.2009 pmid:19776275
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  53. ↵
    1. Ranjbar-Slamloo Y,
    2. Arabzadeh E
    (2017) High-velocity stimulation evokes “dense” population response in layer 2/3 vibrissal cortex. J Neurophysiol 117:1218–1228. doi:10.1152/jn.00815.2016 pmid:28003414
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Rhodes PA
    (2008) Recoding patterns of sensory input: higher-order features and the function of nonlinear dendritic trees. Neural Comput 20:2000–2036. doi:10.1162/neco.2008.04-07-511 pmid:18336083
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Rivlin-Etzion M,
    2. Wei W,
    3. Feller MB
    (2012) Visual stimulation reverses the directional preference of direction-selective retinal ganglion cells. Neuron 76:518–525. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.041 pmid:23141064
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Schiess M,
    2. Urbanczik R,
    3. Senn W
    (2016) Somato-dendritic synaptic plasticity and error-backpropagation in active dendrites. PLoS Comput Biol 12:e1004638. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004638 pmid:26841235
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Schiller J,
    2. Major G,
    3. Koester HJ,
    4. Schiller Y
    (2000) NMDA spikes in basal dendrites of cortical pyramidal neurons. Nature 404:285–289. doi:10.1038/35005094 pmid:10749211
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Schmidt-Hieber C,
    2. Toleikyte G,
    3. Aitchison L,
    4. Roth A,
    5. Clark BA,
    6. Branco T,
    7. Häusser M
    (2017) Active dendritic integration as a mechanism for robust and precise grid cell firing. Nat Neurosci 20:1114–1121. doi:10.1038/nn.4582 pmid:28628104
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Scholl B,
    2. Tan AY,
    3. Corey J,
    4. Priebe NJ
    (2013a) Emergence of orientation selectivity in the mammalian visual pathway. J Neurosci 33:10616–10624. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0404-13.2013 pmid:23804085
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  60. ↵
    1. Scholl B,
    2. Tan AYY,
    3. Corey J,
    4. Priebe NJ
    (2013b) Spiking responses of LGN neurons in the mouse to drifting gratings. Available at http://dx.doi.org/10.6080/K05Q4T1N.
  61. ↵
    1. Seung HS,
    2. Sompolinsky H
    (1993) Simple models for reading neuronal population codes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 90:10749–10753. doi:10.1073/pnas.90.22.10749 pmid:8248166
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  62. ↵
    1. Shannon CE
    (1948) A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical Journal 27:379–423. doi:10.1002/j.1538-7305.1948.tb01338.x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  63. ↵
    1. Sheffield ME,
    2. Dombeck DA
    (2015) Calcium transient prevalence across the dendritic arbour predicts place field properties. Nature 517:200–204. doi:10.1038/nature13871 pmid:25363782
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Sivyer B,
    2. Williams SR
    (2013) Direction selectivity is computed by active dendritic integration in retinal ganglion cells. Nat Neurosci 16:1848–1856. doi:10.1038/nn.3565 pmid:24162650
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Smith SL,
    2. Smith IT,
    3. Branco T,
    4. Häusser M
    (2013) Dendritic spikes enhance stimulus selectivity in cortical neurons in vivo. Nature 503:115–120. doi:10.1038/nature12600 pmid:24162850
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Spencer WA,
    2. Kandel ER
    (1961) Electrophysiology of hippocampal neurons: IV. Fast prepotentials. J Neurophysiol 24:272–285. doi:10.1152/jn.1961.24.3.272 pmid:25286477
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Sun W,
    2. Tan Z,
    3. Mensh BD,
    4. Ji N
    (2016) Thalamus provides layer 4 of primary visual cortex with orientation- and direction-tuned inputs. Nat Neurosci 19:308–315. doi:10.1038/nn.4196 pmid:26691829
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Takahashi N,
    2. Oertner TG,
    3. Hegemann P,
    4. Larkum ME
    (2016) Active cortical dendrites modulate perception. Science 354:1587–1590. doi:10.1126/science.aah6066 pmid:28008068
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  69. ↵
    1. Tao C,
    2. Zhang G,
    3. Zhou C,
    4. Wang L,
    5. Yan S,
    6. Zhang LI,
    7. Zhou Y,
    8. Xiong Y
    (2016) Synaptic basis for the generation of response variation in auditory cortex. Sci Rep 6:31024. doi:10.1038/srep31024 pmid:27484928
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Trenholm S,
    2. McLaughlin AJ,
    3. Schwab DJ,
    4. Turner MH,
    5. Smith RG,
    6. Rieke F,
    7. Awatramani GB
    (2014) Nonlinear dendritic integration of electrical and chemical synaptic inputs drives fine-scale correlations. Nat Neurosci 17:1759–1766. doi:10.1038/nn.3851 pmid:25344631
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Ujfalussy BB,
    2. Makara JK,
    3. Branco T,
    4. Lengyel M
    (2015) Dendritic nonlinearities are tuned for efficient spike-based computations in cortical circuits. Elife 4:e10056. doi:10.7554/eLife.10056 pmid:26705334
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Ujfalussy BB,
    2. Makara JK,
    3. Lengyel M,
    4. Branco T
    (2018) Global and multiplexed dendritic computations under in vivo-like conditions. Neuron 100:579–592.e5. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2018.08.032 pmid:30408443
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Vogels TP,
    2. Sprekeler H,
    3. Zenke F,
    4. Clopath C,
    5. Gerstner W
    (2011) Inhibitory plasticity balances excitation and inhibition in sensory pathways and memory networks. Science 334:1569–1573. doi:10.1126/science.1211095 pmid:22075724
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  74. ↵
    1. Wilson DE,
    2. Whitney DE,
    3. Scholl B,
    4. Fitzpatrick D
    (2016) Orientation selectivity and the functional clustering of synaptic inputs in primary visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 19:1003–1009. doi:10.1038/nn.4323 pmid:27294510
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Wu XE,
    2. Mel BW
    (2009) Capacity-enhancing synaptic learning rules in a medial temporal lobe online learning model. Neuron 62:31–41. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.02.021 pmid:19376065
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Zavitz E,
    2. Yu HH,
    3. Rosa MGP,
    4. Price NSC
    (2019) Correlated variability in the neurons with the strongest tuning improves direction coding. Cereb Cortex 29:615–626. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhx344 pmid:29300838
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Zhu Y,
    2. Qiao W,
    3. Liu K,
    4. Zhong H,
    5. Yao H
    (2015) Control of response reliability by parvalbumin-expressing interneurons in visual cortex. Nat Commun 6:6802. doi:10.1038/ncomms7802 pmid:25869033
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. Zohary E,
    2. Shadlen MN,
    3. Newsome WT
    (1994) Correlated neuronal discharge rate and its implications for psychophysical performance. Nature 370:140–143. doi:10.1038/370140a0 pmid:8022482
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Zylberberg J,
    2. Cafaro J,
    3. Turner MH,
    4. Shea-Brown E,
    5. Rieke F
    (2016) Direction-selective circuits shape noise to ensure a precise population code. Neuron 89:369–383. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.019 pmid:26796691
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Zylberberg J,
    2. Pouget A,
    3. Latham PE,
    4. Shea-Brown E
    (2017) Robust information propagation through noisy neural circuits. PLoS Comput Biol 13:e1005497. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005497 pmid:28419098
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 39 (46)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 39, Issue 46
13 Nov 2019
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Dendritic Spikes Expand the Range of Well Tolerated Population Noise Structures
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Dendritic Spikes Expand the Range of Well Tolerated Population Noise Structures
Alon Poleg-Polsky
Journal of Neuroscience 13 November 2019, 39 (46) 9173-9184; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0638-19.2019

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Dendritic Spikes Expand the Range of Well Tolerated Population Noise Structures
Alon Poleg-Polsky
Journal of Neuroscience 13 November 2019, 39 (46) 9173-9184; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0638-19.2019
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • active dendrite
  • NMDA spike
  • noise
  • noise correlation
  • simulation
  • synaptic integration

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • p140Cap regulates the composition and localization of the NMDAR complex in synaptic lipid rafts
  • Crossed corticostriatal projections in the macaque brain
  • Hypothalamic Control of Forelimb Motor Adaptation
Show more Research Articles

Systems/Circuits

  • Crossed corticostriatal projections in the macaque brain
  • Induction of Activity-Dependent Plasticity at Auditory Nerve Synapses
  • Hypothalamic Control of Forelimb Motor Adaptation
Show more Systems/Circuits
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2022 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.