Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Journal Club

The Neural Code of Working Memory Maintenance

Peng Yuan (袁鹏)
Journal of Neuroscience 11 December 2019, 39 (50) 9883-9884; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1606-19.2019
Peng Yuan (袁鹏)
CNC program, Stanford University, Palo Alto, California 94304
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Peng Yuan (袁鹏)
  • Article
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

The pursuit of the neural mechanism of working memory has been a fundamental topic in neuroscience. The mental function of maintaining information for immediate use is a universal experience, but measuring the specific neural signature of working memory during task has been challenging. To temporally separate the memory signal from signals related to stimulus perception, early researchers introduced a delay between stimulus presentation and behavioral response. Pioneering work in the 1970s using such a design in an oculomotor task demonstrated that individual frontal lobe neurons remain active during the delay period only when the stimulus was shown in a specific location (Goldman and Rosvold, 1970; Fuster and Alexander, 1971; Kubota and Niki, 1971). This work propelled a persistent-firing model of working memory, in which the remembered items are encoded by the activity of a subset of neurons, which is sustained by reciprocal excitatory connections among them (Compte et al., 2000).

Although extensive experimental evidence supports the hypothesis that persistent firing during delay periods is a task-related neural signature, the experimental design described above does not completely rule out the possibility that this signal contains representations of motor planning, response-rule selection, or reward expectation. For example, in the oculomotor task, the subject indicated the stimulus location with a saccade in the same direction, so the observed persistent activity could represent either the stimulus location or the planned eye movement. One strategy to separate these mental processes is to use a “response cue” during the delay period to specify a response rule, such as indicating whether the subject should saccade in the same or opposite direction of the stimulus. In such a design, the subject cannot form a specific motor plan before receiving the response cue; thus, neural activity before response-cue presentation should be a cleaner representation of memory maintenance, whereas signal after the response cue may be related to planning a motor response. Several studies have used such a behavioral design in combination with unbiased brain activity monitoring techniques to search for the neural signature of working memory maintenance (Rowe et al., 2000; Rowe and Passingham, 2001). These studies revealed two consistent findings: (1) the initial activity triggered by the stimulus rapidly decays; and (2) persistent activity emerges after the response cue. These data suggest that the persistent firing represents memory utilization and/or motor planning, but not memory maintenance.

Quentin et al. (2019) provide additional support for this interpretation. The authors measured brain-wide neural activity using MEG while human subjects completed a visually guided memory task, with a response cue in the middle of the delay period. Instead of examining bulk activity levels, the study used multivoxel pattern analysis and constructed logistic and ridge regression decoders to analyze the neural activity during each epoch of the task. Consistent with previous findings, a persistent and stable neural pattern emerged only after the appearance of the response cue. Because response rule and stimulus identity were randomly assigned in each trial, the authors were also able to demonstrate that only the response rule, not the stimulus identity, was decodable from this persistent neural pattern (Quentin et al., 2019, their Fig. 2). Importantly, this persistent neural pattern did not exist when subjects' responses did not depend on the response cue (Quentin et al., 2019, their Figs. 4, 5), ruling out that this persistent neural pattern represents the visual memory of the response cue itself. Furthermore, the authors determined that the persistent neural pattern originates from occipital and frontal lobes at the α and theta frequency bands (Quentin et al., 2019, their Figs. 2, 3). While these findings cannot rule out the possibility that the same group of neurons that show heightened persistent activity after the response cue exhibits a low level of persistent activity that escapes detection or that before the response cue the memory is held at a subcortical nucleus, such as thalamus (Bolkan et al., 2017; Guo et al., 2017), which cannot be picked up by MEG, a more parsimonious interpretation is that the persistent activity in the frontal area, classically considered to be the memory maintenance signal, is indeed encoding a signal for memory-guided response selection.

Where, then, is the neural signal for memory maintenance? As discussed above, a neural signature of memory maintenance might be a representation of the stimulus during the delay period, but before the response cue occurs. However, Quentin et al. (2019) showed that the decoding accuracy of the stimulus decays rapidly to chance level in the delay period, suggesting that either the memory is maintained in activities that cannot be resolved by MEG or that the maintenance of memory does not require persistent neural activity. These two hypotheses imply two different cellular models of working memory: a dynamic coding model in which serial neural loops maintain the information without heightened activity (Druckmann and Chklovskii, 2012; Rajan et al., 2016) and a silent activity model in which the memory is encoded by the short-term plasticity changes that require no activity to maintain (Mongillo et al., 2008). Previous work with fMRI showed that the visual stimulus can be decoded tens of seconds after its presentation (Harrison and Tong, 2009), favoring the dynamic coding model. However, this is not conclusive because fMRI looks at blood vessel dynamics, which can be much slower than the neural change, and the study did not use a response cue to separate stimulus maintenance from response selection. These two models make two different predictions regarding the neural activity at the moment of memory utilization. While the dynamic coding model suggests no change in the stimulus decodability between memory maintenance and recall, the silent activity model predicts that item-specific activity emerges when the network is reactivated during recall. Quentin et al. (2019) showed that the stimulus identities can again be decoded from the neural data after the response cue, consistent with the silent-activity model. However, the decoding performance the authors observed was relatively poor. This may be partially due to the fact that recall-related activation can be a transient signal whose appearance may be variable in time across trials and/or subjects, so the averaged decoding result may not accurately reflect the existence of item-specific information. Interestingly, the recall-related activity involved the general occipital lobe, overlapping with the brain regions activated by the stimulus. This observation is consistent with previous work showing that the brain regions that perceive certain features may be responsible for memorization (Ungerleider, 1995). Again, this phenomenon is in line with the silent-activity model, in which a subset of neurons activated by the stimulus maintains this information.

Finally, Quentin et al. (2019) showed that the delay-period stimulus-decoding algorithm before and after response cue was different. The authors concluded that the neural representation of item-specific information is transformed after the initial stage, and that the representation of the same information in two epochs of the task were different. However, considering the fact that MEG signal primarily reflects current flow within neurites, not neuronal firing, the dendritic and axonal signal from the same neural ensemble may be registered as different wavelets in MEG recordings. In this case, a shift in decoders may represent a utilization of different signal sources that may or may not be related to the same neural ensemble. Hence, the authors' conclusion that different subsets of neurons are engaged in visual perception and memory maintenance remains speculative.

In conclusion, Quentin et al. (2019) provide important evidence that frontal lobe persistent activity represents a neural process of memory utilization, not memory maintenance. The results are consistent with a silent-activity model for memory maintenance, yet the evidence is not conclusive. In theory, the silent-activity model of working memory dictates that the information is held among transient changes in the synaptic weights, rather than neuronal firing. This idea is very similar to the concept of long-term memory storage by structural changes of synaptic connections (Queenan et al., 2017). If true, this similarity between short- and long-term memory may be a common scheme in the neural circuits of memory, simplifying the neural processing of memories with different time scales. Future studies using modern imaging or electrophysiological recording techniques and causal manipulations in animals may provide direct evidence regarding the mechanism of working memory.

Footnotes

  • Editor's Note: These short reviews of recent JNeurosci articles, written exclusively by students or postdoctoral fellows, summarize the important findings of the paper and provide additional insight and commentary. If the authors of the highlighted article have written a response to the Journal Club, the response can be found by viewing the Journal Club at www.jneurosci.org. For more information on the format, review process, and purpose of Journal Club articles, please see http://www.jneurosci.org/content/jneurosci-journal-club.

  • The author declares no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Peng Yuan at pyuan25{at}stanford.edu

References

  1. ↵
    1. Bolkan SS,
    2. Stujenske JM,
    3. Parnaudeau S,
    4. Spellman TJ,
    5. Rauffenbart C,
    6. Abbas AI,
    7. Harris AZ,
    8. Gordon JA,
    9. Kellendonk C
    (2017) Thalamic projections sustain prefrontal activity during working memory maintenance. Nat Neurosci 20:987–996. doi:10.1038/nn.4568 pmid:28481349
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Compte A,
    2. Brunel N,
    3. Goldman-Rakic PS,
    4. Wang XJ
    (2000) Synaptic mechanisms and network dynamics underlying spatial working memory in a cortical network model. Cereb Cortex 10:910–923. doi:10.1093/cercor/10.9.910 pmid:10982751
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Druckmann S,
    2. Chklovskii DB
    (2012) Neuronal circuits underlying persistent representations despite time varying activity. Curr Biol 22:2095–2103. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.058 pmid:23084992
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Fuster JM,
    2. Alexander GE
    (1971) Neuron activity related to short-term memory. Science 173:652–654. doi:10.1126/science.173.3997.652 pmid:4998337
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Goldman PS,
    2. Rosvold HE
    (1970) Localization of function within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the rhesus monkey. Exp Neurol 27:291–304. doi:10.1016/0014-4886(70)90222-0 pmid:4987453
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Guo ZV,
    2. Inagaki HK,
    3. Daie K,
    4. Druckmann S,
    5. Gerfen CR,
    6. Svoboda K
    (2017) Maintenance of persistent activity in a frontal thalamocortical loop. Nature 545:181–186. doi:10.1038/nature22324 pmid:28467817
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Harrison SA,
    2. Tong F
    (2009) Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas. Nature 458:632–635. doi:10.1038/nature07832 pmid:19225460
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Kubota K,
    2. Niki H
    (1971) Prefrontal cortical unit activity and delayed alternation performance in monkeys. J Neurophysiol 34:337–347. doi:10.1152/jn.1971.34.3.337 pmid:4997822
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Mongillo G,
    2. Barak O,
    3. Tsodyks M
    (2008) Synaptic theory of working memory. Science 319:1543–1546. doi:10.1126/science.1150769 pmid:18339943
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Queenan BN,
    2. Ryan TJ,
    3. Gazzaniga MS,
    4. Gallistel CR
    (2017) On the research of time past: the hunt for the substrate of memory. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1396:108–125. doi:10.1111/nyas.13348 pmid:28548457
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Quentin R,
    2. King JR,
    3. Sallard E,
    4. Fishman N,
    5. Thompson R,
    6. Buch ER,
    7. Cohen LG
    (2019) Differential brain mechanisms of selection and maintenance of information during working memory. J Neurosci 39:3728–3740. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2764-18.2019 pmid:30833510
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. Rajan K,
    2. Harvey CD,
    3. Tank DW
    (2016) Recurrent network models of sequence generation and memory. Neuron 90:128–142. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2016.02.009 pmid:26971945
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Rowe JB,
    2. Passingham RE
    (2001) Working memory for location and time: activity in prefrontal area 46 relates to selection rather than maintenance in memory. Neuroimage 14:77–86. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0784 pmid:11525340
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Rowe JB,
    2. Toni I,
    3. Josephs O,
    4. Frackowiak RS,
    5. Passingham RE
    (2000) The prefrontal cortex: response selection or maintenance within working memory? Science 288:1656–1660. doi:10.1126/science.288.5471.1656 pmid:10834847
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Ungerleider LG
    (1995) Functional brain imaging studies of cortical mechanisms for memory. Science 270:769–775. doi:10.1126/science.270.5237.769 pmid:7481764
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 39 (50)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 39, Issue 50
11 Dec 2019
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
The Neural Code of Working Memory Maintenance
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
The Neural Code of Working Memory Maintenance
Peng Yuan (袁鹏)
Journal of Neuroscience 11 December 2019, 39 (50) 9883-9884; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1606-19.2019

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
The Neural Code of Working Memory Maintenance
Peng Yuan (袁鹏)
Journal of Neuroscience 11 December 2019, 39 (50) 9883-9884; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1606-19.2019
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • The Oscillatory Nature of Movement Initiation
  • Adult-Born Granule Cells Contribute to Dentate Gyrus Circuit Reorganization after Traumatic Brain Injury
  • Lateral Preoptic Hypothalamus: A Window to Understanding Insomnia
Show more Journal Club
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.