Abstract
Age-related cognitive impairments are associated with differentially expressed genes (DEGs) linked to defined neural systems; however, studies examining multiple regions of the hippocampus fail to find links between behavior and transcription in the dentate gyrus (DG). We hypothesized that use of a task requiring intact DG function would emphasize molecular signals in the DG associated with a decline in performance. We used a water maze beacon discrimination task to characterize young and middle-age male F344 rats, followed by a spatial reference memory probe trial test. Middle-age rats showed increased variability in discriminating two identical beacons. Use of an allocentric strategy and formation of a spatial reference memory were not different between age groups; however, older animals compensated for impaired beacon discrimination through greater reliance on spatial reference memory. mRNA sequencing of hippocampal subregions indicated DEGs in the DG of middle-age rats, linked to synaptic function and neurogenesis, correlated with beacon discrimination performance, suggesting that senescence of the DG underlies the impairment. Few genes correlated with spatial memory across age groups, with a greater number in region CA1. Age-related CA1 DEGs, correlated with spatial memory, were linked to regulation of neural activity. These results indicate that the beacon task is sensitive to impairment in middle age, and distinct gene profiles are observed in neural circuits that underlie beacon discrimination performance and allocentric memory. The use of different strategies in older animals and associated transcriptional profiles could provide an animal model for examining cognitive reserve and neural compensation of aging.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Hippocampal subregions are thought to differentially contribute to memory. We took advantage of age-related variability in performance on a water maze beacon task and next-generation sequencing to test the hypothesis that aging of the dentate gyrus is linked to impaired beacon discrimination and compensatory use of allocentric memory. The dentate gyrus expressed synaptic function and neurogenesis genes correlated with beacon discrimination in middle-age animals. Spatial reference memory was associated with CA1 transcriptional correlates linked to regulation of neural activity and use of an allocentric strategy. This is the first study examining transcriptomes of multiple hippocampal subregions to link age-related impairments associated with discrimination of feature overlap and alternate response strategies to gene expression in specific hippocampal subregions.
Introduction
Multiple brain regions exhibit similar transcriptional changes related to aging (e.g., immune response and lysosomal processes genes). In contrast, region-specific transcription is associated with performance of cognitive tasks that depend on the corresponding brain region (Prolla, 2002; Haberman et al., 2011; Masser et al., 2014; Ianov et al., 2016, 2017). For studies that examine the three main hippocampal subregions, impaired spatial memory is associated with robust transcriptional differences in regions CA1 and CA3 but limited differences in transcription within the dentate gyrus (DG) (Haberman et al., 2011; Masser et al., 2014; Ianov et al., 2017). The negligible correlation of DG transcription with cognitive impairment is somewhat surprising, given marked DG changes that occur with age, including decreased neurogenesis (Toda and Gage, 2018) and altered synaptic function (Gray and Barnes, 2015). This may be because studies exploring the relationship of transcription to cognitive impairment have not emphasized cognitive processes that depend on the DG (Ianov et al., 2017).
Experimental evidence supports a role for the DG in minimizing interference between overlapping features, such as those occurring in the same location or with perceptually similar objects. Animals sustaining a DG lesion exhibit impaired performance in a cheeseboard task, as demonstrated by lower accuracy to correctly identify a target object placed in close proximity to a lure object (Gilbert et al., 2001). In mice, localized NMDA receptor knock-out impairs the ability to distinguish two similar contexts or two identical beacons on a water maze task (McHugh et al., 2007; Bannerman et al., 2012; Taylor et al., 2013).
An age-related decline in the ability to distinguish objects as feature overlap increases or discriminate between the locations of two adjacent identical stimuli, often referred to a pattern separation, has been well documented in both humans (Bakker et al., 2008; Toner et al., 2009; Stark et al., 2010, 2013, 2015; Yassa et al., 2011; Holden et al., 2012; Ly et al., 2013; Reagh et al., 2014, 2018; Baker et al., 2016; Clark et al., 2017; Huffman and Stark, 2017) and animal models (Burke et al., 2010, 2011; Creer et al., 2010; Gracian et al., 2013; Wu et al., 2015; Johnson et al., 2016, 2017; Cès et al., 2018). Human functional imaging studies implicate the DG/CA3 complex in the age-related decline in ability to discriminate objects as feature overlap increases (Yassa et al., 2011; Bakker et al., 2012; Reagh et al., 2018).
Impaired ability to discriminate objects as feature overlap increases, associated with age, DG damage, or altered DG neurogenesis, can result in the use of different response strategies to maximize reward. Aging canines exhibit difficulties when discriminating between identical objects that are placed closer together but are still able to reach behavioral criteria (Snigdha et al., 2017). The ability to reach behavioral criteria was related to how quickly the animals acquired a new egocentric strategy to compensate for the impairment. In the case of DG lesions, rats were impaired in the ability to discriminate between adjacent locations signaled by two identical stimuli; however, rats were eventually able to acquire a spatial reference memory for the correct location (Gilbert et al., 2001; Morris et al., 2012). We hypothesized that impaired ability to distinguish two identical beacons on a water maze task will be measurable in middle-age rats compared with young, and that worse performance at middle-age will be associated with differential expression of genes (DEGs) within the DG. For middle-age animals, impairment was associated with age-related DEGs in the DG associated with synaptic function and neurogenesis. In addition, use of spatial reference memory in older animals, which were impaired for beacon discrimination, was associated with CA1 genes previously linked to superior spatial memory in aged animals and the use of an allocentric strategy.
Materials and Methods
Animals
Young (5 months, n = 22) and middle-age (12 months, n = 30) male Fisher 344 rats were obtained from the National Institute on Aging via the University of Florida Animal Care and Service facility. All rats were pair housed and maintained on a 12:12 h light cycle with ad libitum food and water access. All experiments were conducted in accordance with guidelines described by the US Public Health Service Policy on Humane Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the University of Florida Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Because of sex differences in behavioral pattern separation ability (Yagi et al., 2016) and variability in neurogenesis, transcription, and synaptic plasticity during the course of the rat estrus cycle (Crispino et al., 1999; Foster, 2005; DiCarlo et al., 2017), the current study focused only on male rats.
Apparatus
Behavioral experiments (Fig. 1A) were conducted in a circular black pool ∼169 cm in diameter. Water was maintained at 27°C-29°C and a total depth of ∼20 cm. The pool was located in a room with uniform black surroundings. Spatial cues were only present during spatial testing. The circular escape platform (∼17.8 cm diameter) was covered with a gray plastic mesh to facilitate gripping and was maintained 1 cm below the surface of the water. Beacons were comprised of white polystyrene foam balls ∼8 cm in diameter. The beacon above the escape platform was positioned on a black metal rod emerging from the center of the platform. The decoy beacon was positioned at a height equal to that of the beacon over the platform on an identically colored black rod that extended directly to the floor of the pool (Fig. 1B).
Cue discrimination training
Animals were allowed to habituate to the colonies of the McKnight Brain Institute for 7 d before beginning behavioral experiments. First, rats underwent a series of cue trials designed to train them to associate an escape platform with an overlying beacon (Foster, 2012). Rats were placed in the pool to swim freely for 30 s, allowing for habituation to the pool. Rats were then subjected to 5 blocks of three trials each, with intertrial intervals of 20 s and interblock intervals of ∼15 min. In each trial, rats were placed in one of four equally spaced start positions (N, S, E, and W) and allowed up to 60 s to swim to an escape platform with an overlying beacon. Rats were gently guided to the platform if they were unable to find the platform within 60 s. Rats remained on the platform for ∼20 s between trials. The positions of platforms and start locations were different for every trial within a block and were randomly determined in advance of the experiment. Between blocks, animals were returned to a holding cage with a warming fan placed near the cage.
Spatial discrimination testing
Forty-eight hours following cue testing (Fig. 1), rats were behaviorally characterized on a modified version of the beacon discrimination task as previously described (Bannerman et al., 2012). During the 5 d of discrimination testing, rats were placed in different positions within the perimeter of the pool containing two beacons: one with an underlying platform and the other in a diametrically opposite position of the pool with no underlying platform. Animals were able to navigate to the correct beacon using visual spatial cues placed on the walls around the pool. Each day of testing was comprised of two blocks of four trials with each trial starting point occurring in a pseudo-random sequence. Starting points occurred in one of six locations: two equidistant between the two beacons, two adjacent to the beacon overlying a platform (S+), and two adjacent to the decoy beacon with no underlying platform. The S+ and S– start positions were 45° offset from the equidistant points relative to the center of the pool (Fig. 1B). A cohort of young (n = 10) and middle-age (n = 14) rats were first tested on the task with beacons separated by 73 cm. A second cohort of young (n = 12) and middle-age (n = 16) rats were tested with beacons separated by 45 cm to allow assessments of increased task difficulty due to greater stimulus proximity.
One day after the last beacon discrimination test, animals were placed at a single equidistant start point in the pool and allowed to swim freely for 60 s with both beacons and the platform removed, but with all spatial cues unchanged, to determine whether animals had formed a spatial reference memory of the platform location (Fig. 1).
Statistical analysis of behavioral data
Statistics were computed using SPSS Statistics, version 26.0 (IBM). Data describing performance by day, start point, or block (in the case of cue trials) were analyzed by mixed ANOVA with within-subjects factors of day or start position and with between-subjects factors of age or beacon separation where applicable. Data from the first day of behavioral testing were excluded from analysis to correct for the time required to train rats to adapt to a second beacon. All post hoc tests were conducted with independent samples t tests with Bonferroni correction. For measures of spatial reference memory, a discrimination index (DI) score was calculated for the probe trial using the formula: (time in goal zone – time in decoy zone)/(time in goal zone + time in decoy zone). The goal and decoy zones were defined as the circular areas within an 18 cm radius of the center of the goal and decoy zones, respectively. For instances in which Mauchly's test of sphericity was significant, Greenhouse-Geisser estimates of sphericity were used to correct degrees of freedom. Significant statistical effects and correlations were defined as those with p < 0.05.
Tissue collection
Four to five days following the conclusion of spatial testing, rats were run on an abridged version of the discrimination task in which the positions of the beacon and platform were shifted 90° from their position during beacon discrimination testing (Fig. 1). For these trials, each rat was placed at each equidistant start point (relative to the new beacon positions) twice for a total of four trials. Spatial cues for these tests remained in the positions used in initial beacon discrimination testing. These trials were done to stimulate hippocampal transcription of genes relevant to performance of the behavioral task. Approximately 1 h after concluding the fourth trial, rats were briefly anesthetized with isoflurane (Patterson Veterinary) and swiftly decapitated. The brains were then rapidly removed, and the hippocampi were dissected according to previously described methods (Lein et al., 2004; Zeier et al., 2011). The hippocampal regions DG, CA1, and CA3 were separated, placed in plastic tubes, and flash-frozen in liquid nitrogen. All samples were stored at −80°C until processed. Precision of the dissection technique was verified by comparison of genes highly expressed in each tissue of the present study to genes expected to be highly expressed in each tissue based on data from the Allen Brain Atlas (Lein et al., 2007) (Extended Data Figs. 1-1, 1-2, and 1-3).
Extended Data Figure 1-1
Comparison of DG gene expression in current study to selection of genes previously identified to be enriched in the DG. Graphs of normalized counts include data from both young and middle-age animals of the current study. Images below figures represent the histologic distribution of the gene in the corresponding figure as reported in the Allen Brain Atlas (mouse). Selected genes demonstrated at least a threefold higher expression in the versus CA1 and CA3 in the Allen Brain Atlas. Download Figure 1-1, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 1-2
Comparison of CA1 gene expression in current study to selection of genes previously identified to be enriched in the CA1. Graphs of normalized counts include data from both young and middle-age animals of the current study. Images below figures represent the histologic distribution of the gene in the corresponding figure as reported in the Allen Brain Atlas (mouse). Selected genes demonstrated at least a threefold higher expression in versus the DG and CA3 in the Allen Brain Atlas. Download Figure 1-2, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 1-3
Comparison of CA3 gene expression in current study to selection of genes previously identified to be enriched in the CA3. Graphs of normalized counts include data from both young and middle-age animals of the current study. Images below figures represent the histologic distribution of the gene in the corresponding figure as reported in the Allen Brain Atlas (mouse). Selected genes demonstrated at least a threefold higher expression in versus the DG and CA1 in the Allen Brain Atlas. Download Figure 1-3, EPS file.
RNA isolation
RNA isolation was performed according to methods previously described (Ianov et al., 2016; Barter et al., 2019) from subregions of a hippocampus from a single hemisphere of each animal. Briefly, RNA was isolated using the RNeasy Lipid Tissue Mini kit (QIAGEN, catalog #74804). Total RNA was treated with DNase using the RNase-Free DNase Set (QIAGEN, catalog #79254). Concentration of total RNA was assessed using the NanoDrop 2000 spectrophotometer (Thermo Fisher Scientific, catalog #ND-2000), and the RNA integrity number was quantified using the Agilent 2200 Tapestation system with High Sensitivity RNA Screen Tape (Agilent Technologies). Mean RNA integrity number across all samples was 9.05 (SEM ± 0.02) with values ranging from 8.2-9.3. Poly-A-selection for mRNA was performed using 250 ng of isolated total RNA in the Dynabeads mRNA DIRECT Micro kit (Thermo Fisher Scientific, catalog #61021). External RNA Controls Consortium (ERCC) RNA Spike-In Control Mixes were included in all isolated mRNA samples (Thermo Fisher Scientific, catalog #4456740).
RNA sequencing
Library preparation was performed using the Ion Total RNA-seq Kit version 2 (Thermo Fisher Scientific, catalog #4475936). Ion Xpress barcodes (Thermo Fisher Scientific, catalog #4475485) were included with libraries for multiplex sequencing. In summary, isolated mRNA was fragmented with RNase III then ligated to the Ion Adapter Mix version 2. RNA samples were then reverse-transcribed. cDNA from each biological replicate was uniquely barcoded and amplified with 16 PCR cycles. Library concentration from each sample was quantified using the Qubit dsDNA HS Assay (Thermo Fisher Scientific, catalog #Q32851). DNA fragment size distribution was subsequently evaluated using High Sensitivity D1000 ScreenTape in the 2200 Tapestation system (Agilent Technologies).
Templates were prepared using the Ion Chef system (Thermo Fisher Scientific, catalog #4484177), and sequencing was performed using the Ion Proton system (Thermo Fisher Scientific, catalog #4476610) or Ion GeneStudio S5 system (Thermo Fisher Scientific, catalog #A38194). A principle component analysis performed in the Partek Flow servers (Partek), comparing counts normalized by the trimmed mean of M-values method of all samples run on the Ion Proton System (n = 45) with all samples run on the Ion GeneStudio S5 system (n = 39), indicated no systematic difference between the two sequencing devices (Extended Data Fig. 1-4). Extended Data Fig. 1-5 lists the platform used to sequence each individual sample. Two-tailed independent samples t tests demonstrated no significant difference between the two sequencing platforms between DI scores (p = 0.79) or average equidistant errors (p = 0.13). Two-sided Fisher's exact tests demonstrated no significant difference between platforms for age (p = 0.38) or tissue type (p = 0.76).
Extended Data Figure 1-4
Principle component analysis of all samples, separated by RNA sequencing apparatus, tissue, age and cognitive performance. Across figures, clear separation is evident for DG relative to other regions. (A) Substantial overlap is present between samples performed on the Ion Proton and Ion GeneStudio platforms. (B) High performers and low performers were categorized as middle-age animals performing in the top and bottom half of beacon discrimination scores among all middle-age animals. More errors equated to worse beacon discrimination performance. Within the DG, young animals and middle-age low performing animals exhibit relatively tight and separate clusters. The middle-age high performing animals exhibit a relatively tight cluster that is closer to young than middle-age low performing animals. Download Figure 1-4, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 1-5
Complete list of samples sequenced on each sequencing apparatus. A total of 45 samples were sequenced using the Ion Proton platform, and 39 were sequenced on the Ion GeneStudio S5 platform. Within samples from young animals and middle-age animals respectively, 17 and 28 samples were sequenced using the Ion Proton platform; and 19 and 20 samples were sequenced using the Ion GeneStudio. For the DG, CA1 and CA3 respectively, the Ion Proton platform was used for 16, 13, and 16 samples, and the Ion GeneStudio platform was used for 12, 15, and 12 samples. Overall approximately half of all samples were sequenced using each platform, and approximately half of samples of each tissue type and half of samples from each age group were sequenced on each platform. Download Figure 1-5, XLS file.
RNA sequencing analysis
ERCC analysis was performed with the ERCC analysis plugin in Torrent Suite Software version 5.10.1 (Thermo Fisher Scientific). All spiked samples had an R2 > 0.9. Ion Torrent and Ion GeneStudio-derived reads were then aligned to the rattus norvegicus genome (rn6) using STAR 2.5.3a in the Partek Flow servers (Partek). Gene read counts were generated from Binary Alignment Map files and annotated to the genome using Ensembl (version 92; Partek). Mean number of reads across all samples was (in millions) 32.18 (± 0.37 SEM), ranging from 26.01 to 40.78. Reads from FASTQ files produced by the Ion Torrent System were trimmed from both ends until all samples' collective average Phred quality score was > 25. Reads < 25 bp were also excluded. This yielded an average read length of 144.4 (± 1.5 SEM) bp and average count of 31.56 million reads (± 0.38 SEM) per sample.
Gene filtering was performed according to our previously published work (Blalock et al., 2003; Aenlle et al., 2009; Aenlle and Foster, 2010; Zeier et al., 2011; Ianov et al., 2016, 2017; Barter et al., 2019). Briefly, gene lists were filtered to exclude genes with an average count of ≤5 (averaged across all samples included in a comparison). Normalization and pairwise differential expression analysis, including calculation of false discovery rate (FDR), were conducted using the DESeq2 package (version 3.5) for comparisons by age. A statistical filter was then applied with a threshold of significance for DESeq2 pairwise analyses set at p < 0.025 (age p value). This filtered list of genes was used for gene enrichment clustering analysis for age effects and examining gene clusters for age-related genes that correlated with behavioral measures in middle-age animals (see below).
To assess the relationship of age-related differences in gene expression and cognitive performance, Spearman correlations were calculated between normalized counts of gene expression for middle-age animals within each brain region and either the average number of errors per trial when beginning trials from the equidistant start points across all days of testing or the DI score measured during the probe task. The criterion for a significant correlation, limited to subjects in the middle-age group, was set at p < 0.05, consistent with our previous work (Blalock et al., 2003). An FDR was calculated for genes that passed both age and correlation thresholds. Genes were ranked by the combined p value (p value age × p value for behavioral correlation), and the FDR for each rank (qi) was calculated by the formula qi = (p value agei × p value for behavioral correlationi) × total genes examined/i). The Spearman correlation was used, as it has been previously validated for differential correlation analysis in sequencing data (Siska and Kechris, 2017), and there was no assumption within the current study of a normal distribution for either behavioral variable assessed. As genes whose expression is altered with age are of primary interest in this study, only genes that met the significance threshold for both age and correlation were reported. Multiple regression analyses were performed in R (R Core Team, 2019).
Confidence in the significance of individual genes is low due to false positives associated with multiple comparisons across all genes. Gene enrichment analysis was performed under the assumption that changes in biological processes with age or cognition would result in a shift in the expression of clusters of genes related to the biological process (Ianov et al., 2016). Gene enrichment and functional annotation clustering analysis was conducted using the National Institutes of Health database for annotation, visualization, and integrated discovery (DAVID, version 6.8) (Huang et al., 2009) using Gene ontology (GO) for biological process, cellular compartment, and molecular function in the “Direct” and “FAT” categories, in addition to Kyoto Encyclopedia of Genes and Genomes (KEGG) pathways. Benjamini FDR was set at p < 0.05 as a threshold for cluster selection. For all functional annotation results, we reported significant GO terms among cellular compartment, molecular function, and biological process categories for each annotation cluster. Significant KEGG pathways were also reported. Separate functional cluster analyses were performed for each brain region (DG, CA1, and CA3) for genes filtered for age alone or age and behavioral measures. Analyses were further split by direction of fold change (in the case of age effects) or correlation (in the case of cognitive measures). All counts of filtered genes, including those used in DESeq2 and correlation analyses, include only genes indexed in DAVID. FASTQ files derived from RNA sequencing were submitted to the National Center for Biotechnology Information's Gene Expression Omnibus under accession number GSE140420.
Results
Behavior
Cue discrimination training
Figure 1 illustrates the timeline of behavioral testing. Consistent with previous work (Guidi et al., 2014), repeated-measures ANOVA indicated an effect of training (F(4,192) = 17.83, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.27) and an effect of age (F(1,48) = 23.81, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.33) in the absence of an effect of cohort or any interactions (Fig. 2A). Post hoc ANOVAs for each age group indicated that both groups decreased their escape swim distance with training (p < 0.001 in each case). All animals were able to locate the escape platform within 1 min by the end of cue training. The absence of a cohort effect justified further comparisons within cohorts tested at different beacon separations.
Spatial discrimination water maze beacon task experimental setup. A, Experimental timeline. B, For beacon discrimination testing, animals were placed at one of six starting positions: two located near the platform beacon (S+, red arrows), two located equidistant between the decoy and platform beacons (Equid, black arrows), and two located near the decoy beacon (S–, blue arrows). Spatial cues, platform positions, and beacon positions were unchanged through all days of beacon discrimination testing.
Middle-age animals are impaired relative to young in beacon discrimination, but not spatial reference memory. A, Young (blue symbols) and middle-age animals (red symbols) showed improvement in escape swim distance during cue training, despite impaired performance of middle-age animals compared with young. B, Average number of errors made per trial (Average Errors) across all days of testing and all trial start positions. Average errors decreased with training, with increased separation (circles represent 45 cm; diamonds represent 73 cm), and for young relative to middle-age animals. C, Average errors were influenced by start location such that errors increased as the distance from the start location and goal increased (S– > equidistant > S+) and was higher in animals tested at 45 cm beacon separation compared with 73 cm for trials beginning at the equidistant or S– start positions. D, DI scores were higher for animals trained at 73 cm beacon separation compared with 45 cm separation, and no age difference in DI score was observed for either the 45 or 73 cm separation group. E, Relationship of beacon discrimination performance (average errors from the equidistant starting position for the 45 cm separation) and spatial reference memory (DI score). Within middle-age animals, a significant correlation was noted between DI score and average number of errors made in the beacon discrimination task with trials beginning at the equidistant start point (Pearson's r = 0.72, p = 0.002). Better reference memory was thus associated with worse beacon discrimination ability. Dotted lines indicate the threshold for chance-level performance. Error bars indicate SEM. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001.
Beacon discrimination task
A repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted for the average number of times animals visited the incorrect beacon per trial (i.e., average errors), with between-subjects factors of age and beacon separation and within-subjects factors of start position and day of training. This analysis indicated main effects of training across days (F(2.6,122.4) = 8.00, p < 0.0001, η2 = 0.14), due to decreased errors with training, an effect of age (F(1,48) = 24.12, p < 0.0001, η2 = 0.33), due to more errors by older animals, and an effect of beacon separation (F(1,48) = 47.10, p < 0.0001, η2 = 0.50), due to increased errors among animals tested at 45 cm beacon separation. An interaction was observed for days of training × age (F(2.6,122.4) = 3.38, p = 0.03, η2 = 0.066), due mainly to better performance by younger animals on days 3 and 4 (Fig. 2B). The results indicate that older animals exhibit poorer performance than young, regardless of beacon separation, that both age groups increased accuracy over the course of training, and that discrimination became more difficult as the beacons exhibited less spatial separation.
A main effect was observed for start position (F(1.5,71.2) = 51.02, p < 0.0001, η2 = 0.52), as well as an interaction of start location and beacon separation (F(1.5,71.2) = 8.91, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.16) (Fig. 2C). The number of errors increased as the distance between the escape platform and the start location increased S– > equidistant > S+, indicating that the animals were likely to initially choose the beacon closest to the start location. Finally, post hoc tests for effects of beacon separation at each start position indicated an increase in errors at the 45 cm separation relative to the 73 cm separation for trials starting from the S– (p < 0.001) or equidistant point (p < 0.01).
Post hoc tests to localize age effects at each separation and start position indicated that middle-age animals made more errors than young when beginning at the equidistant start position, whether tested at 45 cm (p < 0.01) or 73 cm (p < 0.001) separation, and when beginning trials at the S+ position at 73 cm separation (p = 0.02). Together, the results distinguish the equidistant start location as the most appropriate for detecting effects of age and beacon separation on performance. This start position also provides the most unbiased visual presentation of the two beacons, making data from this position the most ideal for assessing beacon discrimination performance.
Behavioral differences in middle-age animals could reflect nonspecific motivational or sensorimotor deficits. For example, age-related differences in the cue task are largely due to thigmotaxis, related to the initial stress of the water maze, and differences decrease with exposure to the pool (Foster, 2012; Guidi et al., 2015). To determine whether the impairment in beacon discrimination was due to motivation or sensorimotor function, Pearson's correlations were conducted, within each age group for each beacon separation, between the average number of errors made by each animal at the equidistant starting point and the distance to escape for the last block of cue discrimination training. The results did not reveal a relationship between performance on the cue discrimination task and beacon discrimination performance for either age group at either beacon separation (young, 45 cm, r = 0.16, p = 0.63; middle-age, 45 cm, r = 0.21, p = 0.43; young, 73 cm, r = 0.28, p = 0.44; middle-age, 73 cm, r = 0.41, p = 0.15). The results indicate that beacon discrimination performance is not likely due to nonspecific sensorimotor deficits or motivational differences. Finally, for animals trained in the 45 cm separation task, the number of equidistant errors increased following the shift in the beacons, relative to the last day of training (F(1,26) = 11.25, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.3) in the absence of an age effect (p = 0.41), indicating that both age groups had learned to discriminate the beacon/location (Extended Data Fig. 2-1).
Extended Data Figure 2-1
Average errors made at the equidistant start position by day, including in the 90° shift task. Only young (blue symbols) and middle-age animals (red symbols) animals tested at 45 cm beacon separation were tested in a 90° shift task. Error bars represent standard error of the mean. Download Figure 2-1, EPS file.
Spatial reference memory assessment
One day after the last beacon discrimination test, a single probe trial was conducted with the beacons and the platform removed to determine whether animals had formed a spatial reference memory of the platform location. An examination of DI scores for the reference memory probe trial indicated an effect of beacon separation (F(1,48) = 15.14, p < 0.0005, η2 = 0.24), with higher DI scores for the larger beacon separation, in the absence of an age effect (Fig. 2D). In order to examine the relationship between beacon separation performance and the formation of a spatial reference memory, the DI scores for each age group were correlated with the average number of errors for the equidistant starting location for each beacon separation. The average number of errors for the equidistant starting position in the 45 cm beacon separation task was positively correlated with the DI score for middle-age animals, such that animals that exhibited more errors discriminating the beacons were more likely to use a reference memory (r2 = 0.52, p < 0.005; Fig. 2E). The distribution of DI scores for young animals also suggests individual variability in the use of an allocentric strategy. However, in contrast to older animals, the selection of an allocentric strategy was not associated with beacon discrimination performance. A similar analysis for the 73 cm beacon separation did not indicate a relationship between beacon discrimination and spatial reference memory for either age group (data not shown). The results suggest that, for the larger interbeacon distance, animals were more likely to rely on a spatial reference memory strategy. In contrast, for the smaller interbeacon distance, middle-age animals that were impaired for beacon discrimination were more likely to use a spatial reference memory strategy, and young animals performed well on the beacon discrimination task regardless of the strategy used.
Sequencing
Age-related transcriptomic changes
In order to examine the transcriptional profile associated with age and beacon discrimination performance, young and middle-age animals behaviorally characterized on the 45 cm task were examined using next-generation sequencing for gene expression in the three regions of the hippocampus. A total of 2593 genes were upregulated and 2821 were downregulated within the DG in middle-age animals compared with young (p < 0.025; FDR < 0.063). CA1 and CA3 exhibited relatively fewer differentially expressed genes with age compared with the DG, with CA1 showing 2149 genes upregulated and 2226 downregulated genes (p < 0.025; FDR < 0.077), and CA3 showing 1952 genes upregulated and 1847 downregulated with age (p < 0.025; FDR < 0.091) (Fig. 3A,B). Extended Data Fig. 4-1 provides a full list of genes showing significant differential expression with age as well as corresponding FDR-adjusted p values derived from DESeq2.
Age is associated with differential regulation of genes within the DG, CA1, and CA3. A, By middle-age, the DG was found to show the greatest number of DEGs relative to young rats. B, The number of DEGs in common among different brain regions. Arrowheads indicate the direction of differential gene expression. All gene counts represent genes that passed our statistical filter for age and were indexed in DAVID.
Extended Data Figure 4-1
Complete list of genes showing significant differential expression with age in all tissues studied. DESeq2 derived p-values, FDR-adjusted p-values, fold change, and Log2 fold change are provided for each gene as they occurred in each tissue. Download Figure 4-1, XLS file.
Functional enrichment analysis with DAVID identified a number of genes clusters altered similarly across all hippocampal subregions with age (Fig. 4A–C, Extended Data Fig. 4-2). Upregulated gene clusters in all brain regions included extracellular exosome (GO:0070062), RNA splicing (GO:0008380), translational initiation (GO:0006413), response to endoplasmic reticulum stress (GO:0034976), cellular component biogenesis (GO:0044085), and chromosome organization (GO:0051276). Clusters of downregulated genes within all brain regions included glutamatergicsynapse (KEGG rno04724), axon part (GO:0033267), postsynapse (GO:0098794), presynapse (GO:0098793), neuron part (GO:0097458), synapse maturation (GO:0060074), cell death (GO:0008219), regulation of catabolic process (GO:0009894), CNS development (GO:0007417), transcription from RNA polymerase II promoter (GO:0006366), and regulation of neuronal synaptic plasticity (GO:0048168).
Functional annotation clustering shows substantial differential gene expression by middle-age compared with young animals in the DG (A), CA1 (B), and CA3 (C). Graphs represent the top GO and KEGG terms of the DEGs of middle-age rats relative to young. The complete list of significant GO and KEGG terms for each tissue is provided in Extended data Figure 4-3. Gene categories showing common differential regulation in all hippocampal subregions with age are provided in Extended data Figure 4-2.
Extended Data Figure 4-2
List of functional annotations clusters altered in common among all hippocampal tissues with age. Annotation clusters of genes either up- or downregulated in the same direction across the DG, CA1, and CA3 are shown from data listed in Figure 4-3. Download Figure 4-2, XLS file.
Extended Data Figure 4-3
Complete list of significantly up- or downregulated GO terms and KEGG pathways associated with age for each hippocampal tissue. For each age-related functional annotation cluster, the Table includes a corresponding list of gene names, direction of fold change for the genes, biological concept, and Benjamini adj p-value. Data are extended from those in Figure 4. Download Figure 4-3, XLS file.
Within the DG, clusters with the lowest p values for genes upregulated with age included cellular localization (GO:0051641), RNA metabolic process (GO:0016070), spliceosomal complex (GO:0005681), and centrosome (GO:0005813) (Fig. 4A). The most significant clusters of downregulated genes in the DG included nervous system development (GO:0007399), regulation of signaling (GO:0023051), neuron part (GO:0097458), synapse (GO:0045202), glutamatergic synapse (KEGG rno04724), and protein modification process (GO:0036211) (Fig. 4A, Extended Data Fig. 4-3). These results are consistent with studies describing a reduction in neurogenesis and synapse number in the DG with age. Extended Data Figs. 4-4, 4-5, and 4-6 provide examples of the distribution of a selection of genes from the DG, CA1, and CA3, respectively, which were differentially regulated with age, and associated with significant GO terms.
Extended Data Figure 4-4
Selection of genes showing significant differential expression with age in the DG. Data in the y-axis reflect the z-distributions of normalized counts for the respective gene. Download Figure 4-4, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 4-5
Selection of genes showing significant differential expression with age in CA1. Data in the y-axis reflect the z-distributions of normalized counts for the respective gene. Download Figure 4-5, EPS file.
Extended Data Figure 4-6
Selection of genes showing significant differential expression with age in CA3. Data in the y-axis reflect the z-distributions of normalized counts for the respective gene. Download Figure 4-6, EPS file.
Cognition-related transcriptomic changes
As noted above, the average errors for the equidistant start location (equidistant average errors) was the most sensitive andconceptually appropriate for detecting beacon discrimination deficits in middle-age animals. Because of the lack of variability in equidistant average errors for young animals, analysis of the relationship between gene expression and discrimination behavior on the 45 cm beacon separation task was limited to middle-age animals. Spearman's correlations were used to examine the relationship of equidistant average errors and expression of genes that differed across age groups. From the list of DEGs that were different with age in the DG, 329 genes were positively correlated with equidistant errors and 539 genes were negatively correlated with equidistant errors. CA1 expressed 69 and 77 genes positively and negatively correlated with errors, respectively, that were also significantly altered with age. Finally, CA3 showed 51 and 76 genes that were altered with age and exhibited a positive and negative correlation with errors, respectively (Fig. 5A). Cluster enrichment analysis indicated that DG genes, which were differentially expressed with age and negatively correlated with errors (i.e., increasing expression with better performance), included synapse part (GO:0044456), synaptic signaling (GO:0099536), nervous system development (GO:0007399), and neurogenesis (GO:0022008) (Fig. 5B). Unabridged lists of these clusters are provided in Extended Data Fig. 5-1, and a full list of individual genes with significant correlations to errors, along with FDR-adjusted p values is provided in Extended Data Fig. 5-2. Figure 6 provides representative plots of DG synaptic or neurogenesis genes that were significantly different with age and negatively correlated with beacon discrimination errors.
Beacon discrimination performance is correlated with differential regulation of genes primarily within the DG. A, Among all hippocampal subregions tested, the DG showed the greatest number of genes correlating with beacon discrimination performance on the 45 cm beacon separation task within middle-age animals. B, The DG showed the greatest number of gene clusters correlating with beacon discrimination performance, including downregulation of genes related to the synapse (GO:0044456) and neurogenesis (GO:0022008) with worse beacon discrimination performance. C, CA3 featured downregulation of genes relating to the myelin sheath (GO:0043209) with worse beacon discrimination performance. All values represent genes, found to pass our statistical filters for age and correlation with beacon discrimination performance within middle-age rats tested at 45 cm beacon separation only, which were also indexed in DAVID.
Examples of DG genes that are differentially expressed with age and significantly correlated with equidistant average errors in middle-age animals. Each gene depicted shows decreased expression in animals with worse beacon discrimination performance. For plots of age differences, data in the y axes reflect the z distributions of normalized gene counts within both young and middle-age animals. For plots depicting correlations for middle-age animals, the y axes reflect the z distributions of normalized gene counts for middle-age animals only. In correlation figures, the x axes represent the z distributions of average equidistant errors within middle-age animals only. Each gene depicted shows decreased expression in animals with worse beacon discrimination performance. A, B, Genes from the synapse part (GO:0044456). C–F, Genes associated with neurogenesis (GO:0022008).
Extended Data Figure 5-1
Complete list of significant GO terms and KEGG pathways among genes that were different with age and correlated with equidistant average errors in middle-age animals. Annotation clusters are provided for genes with either a positive or negative correlation with equidistant average errors. Data are extended from those in Figure 5(B, C), with each cluster including a corresponding list of gene names, direction of correlation for the genes, associated biological concept, and Benjamini adj p-value. Download Figure 5-1, XLS file.
Extended Data Figure 5-2
Correlations of gene expression to errors in the beacon discrimination task. Genes listed showed both significant differential regulation by age, and a significant correlation with errors within middle-age animals. Data are provided for age-related p-values, Spearman correlations and associated p-values; the product of p-values for age and correlation; q-values, and adjusted p-values derived from q-values for each gene. Download Figure 5-2, XLS file.
The use of an allocentric strategy, as measured by the probe trial DI scores, exhibited large variability regardless of age and was not different across age groups. To examine the possibility that the DI score was associated with similar gene expression across age groups, we performed multiple regression analysis to control for age. We filtered the genes such that they exhibited an overall significance (p < 0.05) for the multiple regression, a significant (p < 0.05) correlation with the DI scores, and were not different by age (p > 0.05). CA1 exhibited the most genes positively (73) and negatively (74) correlated with the DI score, followed by the DG (46 positively and 53 negatively correlated), then CA3 (38 positively and 35 negatively correlated). Examination of enrichment clustering was null for each region. However, several of the CA1 genes that negatively (Wdr5, Med20, Sel1l3, Igfbp6, Thsd7b, Recql5, Olfm2, Atp2b4, Fxyd6) or positively (Ppp3ca, Cyp1b1, Ythdc1, Arhgef37, Cdo1, Tac1, Grm8) correlated with the DI scores have previously been reported to decrease or increase expression, respectively, in CA1 of aged animals with intact spatial reference memory, compared with older memory impaired animals (Rowe et al., 2007; Masser et al., 2014; Ianov et al., 2017).
To determine whether the DI score, a measure of the use of an allocentric strategy, was linked to CA1 genes that change with age, we used the list of genes that were different with age to examine the relationship between gene expression and DI scores in middle-age animals. For CA1, 54 and 77 genes that were different with age exhibited a positive or negative correlation with the DI scores, respectively, in middle-age animals. Gene clustering indicated no enrichment of positively correlated genes, and negatively correlated genes exhibited clusters of neuronal genes, including genes for voltage-gated cation channel activity (GO:0022843). Interestingly, Fos, which has previously been reported to decrease in regions CA1 of animals that preferentially use an allocentric strategy (Fouquet et al., 2013; Yagi et al., 2016), was negatively correlated with DI scores. Figure 7 depicts representative plots of individuals genes altered with age and correlated with DI score within CA1. A full list of genes that were differentially expressed with age and exhibited a significant correlation to DI score within CA1, including FDR-adjusted p values, is provided in Extended Data Fig. 7-1. The results are consistent with previous work indicating that successful formation of a spatial reference memory does not result in the same pattern of gene expression in young and older animals, and suggest that some gene changes represent compensation to maintain function of CA1.
Examples of CA1 genes that are differentially expressed with age and significantly correlated with DI score in middle-age animals. For plots of age differences, data in the y axes reflect the z distributions of normalized gene counts within both young and middle-age animals. For plots depicting correlations for middle-age animals, the y axes reflect the z distributions of normalized gene counts for middle-age animals only. In correlation figures, the x axes represent the z distributions of DI scores within middle-age animals only. Animals with better reference memory performance, reflected by greater DI score, showed increased expression of Syt6 (A), Mr1 (B), and Cyb5d2 (C); and decreased expression of Fos (D), Hcn4 (E), and Kcnj14 (F). A full list of genes showing a significant correlation to DI score is provided in Extended data Figure 7-1.
Extended Data Figure 7-1
Correlations of gene expression to probe trial DI score. Genes listed showed both significant differential regulation by age, and a significant correlation with DI score within middle-age animals. Data are provided for age-related p-values, Spearman correlations and associated p-values; the product of p-values for age and correlation; q-values, and adjusted p-values derived from q-values for each gene. Download Figure 7-1, XLS file.
Discussion
For studies examining multiple hippocampal subregions, little correlation is observed between DG gene expression and hippocampal-dependent spatial memory. Rather, an age-related decline in spatial memory is associated with transcription in regions CA1 and CA3 (Haberman et al., 2011; Masser et al., 2014; Ianov et al., 2017). The lack of DG transcriptional correlates may be due to the use of tasks that do not emphasize cognitive processes that depend on the DG (Ianov et al., 2017). Impaired DG function is thought to contribute to an age-related decline in the ability to perform pattern separation tasks, observed as an impairment in the ability to distinguish objects as feature overlap increases or discriminate between the locations of two identical stimuli as the distance between objects is diminished, in humans (Bakker et al., 2008; Yassa et al., 2011; Holden et al., 2012; Reagh et al., 2018) and animal models (Creer et al., 2010; Burke et al., 2011; Gracian et al., 2013; Gray and Barnes, 2015; Wu et al., 2015; Johnson et al., 2016, 2017; Cès et al., 2018).
Similar to aging, DG lesions, decreased neurogenesis, or disruption of DG synaptic function is associated with impairment on pattern separation tasks (Gilbert et al., 2001; McHugh et al., 2007; Hunsaker et al., 2008; Clelland et al., 2009; Bannerman et al., 2012; Morris et al., 2012; Taylor et al., 2013). In contrast, the role of the DG and neurogenesis in spatial reference memory is more nuanced (Xavier et al., 1999; Bizon et al., 2004; McHugh et al., 2007; Epp and Galea, 2009; Bannerman et al., 2012; Morris et al., 2012; Taylor et al., 2013; Barha et al., 2015; Coradazzi et al., 2016).
Animals can use a variety of cognitive strategies, associated with different brain networks, to solve tasks. Moreover, different cognitive strategies and associated brain networks may be used to compensate for deficits associated with senescence or pathology of a particular network (Steffener and Stern, 2012). Animals with impaired ability to distinguish objects as feature overlap increases or discriminate between the locations of two identical stimuli as the distance between objects is diminished can use cue-response or allocentric strategies to locate goals (Costa et al., 2005; Morris et al., 2012; Bannerman et al., 2014). For example, aged or lesioned animals, impaired in the ability to distinguish similar objects, shift egocentric strategies and exhibit response bias as feature overlap increases (Snigdha et al., 2017; Burke et al., 2018). In addition, animals can use an allocentric strategyto compensate for impaired ability to discriminate betweenadjacent locations (Gilbert et al., 2001; Morris et al., 2012; Bannerman et al., 2014). For S+ and S– start positions, animals typically choose the closest beacon, indicating an egocentric or cue-response strategy. For the equidistant start locations, errors increased as the distance between the cues decreased, and errors increased with age, confirming that deficits emerge by middle age (Huxter et al., 2012; Stark et al., 2013). Probe trials indicated that, when the beacons were far apart, animals exhibited an allocentric strategy and formation of a spatial reference memory. For the 45 cm beacon separation, probe trial data indicated that middle-age animals with increased errors were more likely to use an allocentric strategy, concentrating their search on the spatial location. In contrast, animals with fewer equidistant errors exhibited search behavior that was not focused on the spatial location, indicating that they relied on cue-response behavior to discriminate the beacons.
Differences in gene expression suggest that impaired beacon discrimination results from senescence within the task-related network, specifically the DG. Although most studies focus on young and aged animals, previous research indicates large changes in expression in the hippocampus and in the DG between young adult and middle-age (Blalock et al., 2003; Ianov et al., 2017). Furthermore, the DG exhibits several neurobiological changes by middle-age (Dieguez and Barea-Rodriguez, 2004; Twarkowski and Manahan-Vaughan, 2016; McGuiness et al., 2017). In particular, neurogenesis declines in middle-age (Seki and Arai, 1995; Lichtenwalner et al., 2001; Bizon and Gallagher, 2003; Bondolfi et al., 2004; Driscoll et al., 2006; Kronenberg et al., 2006; McGuiness et al., 2017). The current study supports the idea that the number of genes correlated with behavior is increased by examining brain networks that contribute to the specific behavior, including medial prefrontal cortex genes and impaired executive function (Ianov et al., 2016), and region CA1 genes and impaired spatial memory (Masser et al., 2014; Ianov et al., 2017). Thus, the gene differences likely represent neurobiological processes that are changing with age and contribute to impaired function of the circuit.
In agreement with previous studies (Burger et al., 2008; Masser et al., 2014; Ianov et al., 2016), we observed upregulation of immune (Apobec1, B2m, Cd4b, Cd74, Fcer1g, Fcgr2b, Lrrc17, Lyv, Msh2, RT1-Da, RT1-Db1, RT1-Ba, Serpinbp, Syk) and lysosomal genes (Cd74, Ctss, Myo5a, Pon2, Rab14, RT1-Da, RT1-Db1, RT1-Ba) in the DG of older animals. Importantly, immune response genes did not correlate with beacon discrimination performance. While cognitive impairment may be related to physiological changes associated with neuroinflammation, the results are consistent with the observation that immune response genes do not predict cognitive impairment (Haberman et al., 2011; Masser et al., 2014; Ianov et al., 2017).
Beacon discrimination impairment was associated with a decrease in DG synaptic genes, consistent with age-related changes in DG afferents and synaptic plasticity (Gray and Barnes, 2015) and studies indicating that impaired DG synptic function contributes to impaired pattern separation behavior (McHugh et al., 2007; Bannerman et al., 2014; Kannangara et al., 2015). The decrease in synaptic genes is also consistent with decreased neurogenesis (Chatzi et al., 2016). Aging is characterized by delayed maturation of new DG neurons and a decline in total new neurons (Bizon et al., 2004; Speisman et al., 2013; McGuiness et al., 2017; Toda and Gage, 2018). In turn, decreased neurogenesis has been linked to impaired pattern separation behavior (Clelland et al., 2009; Aimone et al., 2011; Sahay et al., 2011; Toda and Gage, 2018).
The role of neurogenesis in spatial memory is unclear (Shors et al., 2002), with several studies indicating no relationship, or decreased neurogenesis associated with increased use of an allocentric strategy (Bizon et al., 2004; Epp and Galea, 2009; Barha et al., 2015; Coradazzi et al., 2016). For the 45 cm beacon separation, both age groups exibited similar variability in the DI score for the probe trial. When both age groups were examined together, few genes were correlated with the DI score, consistent with work indicating that spatial memory behavior does not result in the same pattern of gene expression in young and older animals (Burger, 2010). Interestingly, within a multiple regression model controlling for age, CA1 exhibited the most genes whose expression correlated to DI score, including several that have previously been reported to differentiate age-impaired and aged-unimpaired for spatial memory, including genes for regulating neural activity. These cognition-related genes may represent an attempt by CA1 neurons to compensate for senescent changes (Ianov et al., 2017; Foster, 2019). Interestingly, Fos was negatively correlated with DI scores for middle-age animals (i.e., decreased expression in animals that used an allocentric strategy). The results support work demonstrating reduced c-fos expression in region CA1 of animals that preferentially use an allocentric strategy (Fouquet et al., 2013; Yagi et al., 2016). Finally, it is likely that genes and gene clusters were missed due to the stringency of analysis. Other studies have observed differences when comparing age-impaired or age-unimpaired with each other, regardless of whether the genes were different with age (Haberman et al., 2011; Masser et al., 2014; Ianov et al., 2017).
Together, these results indicate the beacon task is sensitiveto age-related cognitive impairment as early as middle-age. Different response strategies used to solve the task were related to distinct profiles of gene expression in different hippocampal subfields. The use of different strategies and associated transcriptional profiles may represent examples of cognitive reserve and neural compensation (Stern et al., 2019). Cognitive reserve has been described as an ability to flexibly and efficiently use the best strategies to perform a task, particularly in the face of brain aging or pathology. The variability in beacon discrimination errors associated with strategy selection aligns with human studies that indicate impairment in a specific cognitive domain limits the strategy repertoire, contributing to variance in performance (Hodzik and Lemaire, 2011; Barulli et al., 2013). In turn, impairment within a cognitive domain may result from pathology or senescence within the task-related network. Imaging studies in humans implicate the DG/CA3 network in the age-related decline in object pattern separation (Reagh et al., 2018). The current study indicates that disruption of the network for efficient beacon discrimination involves altered molecular signals for neurogenesis and synaptic function in the DG, and the shift to an allocentric strategy may involve neural compensation in other hippocampal regions. Future studies should examine gene expression in relation to treatments that regulate neurogenesis and synaptic plasticity in the DG and the ability to distinguish objects as feature overlap increases or discriminate between the locations of two adjacent identical stimuli (Bekinschtein et al., 2011).
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institute of Aging Grants AG037984, AG036800, AG049711, AG052258, and P30AG028740; and the Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Research Foundation. We thank Sebastian Conde, Nick Sarantos, Sophia Eikenberry, and Valentina Lavieri-Sosa for assistance in behavioral testing; and Dr. Brittney Yegla for assistance in data analysis and behavioral testing.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Thomas C. Foster at Foster1{at}ufl.edu