Abstract
Peripheral sources of individual variation in taste intensity perception have been well described. The existence of a central source has been proposed but remains unexplored. Here we used functional magnetic resonance imaging in healthy human participants (20 women, 8 men) to evaluate the hypothesis that the amygdala exerts an inhibitory influence that affects the “gain” of the gustatory system during tasting. Consistent with the existence of a central gain mechanism (CGM), we found that central amygdala response was correlated with mean intensity ratings across multiple tastants. In addition, psychophysiological and dynamic causal modeling analyses revealed that the connection strength between inhibitory outputs from amygdala to medial dorsal and ventral posterior medial thalamus predicted individual differences in responsiveness to taste stimulation. These results imply that inhibitory inputs from the amygdala to the thalamus act as a CGM that influences taste intensity perception.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Whether central circuits contribute to individual variation in taste intensity perception is unknown. Here we used functional magnetic resonance imaging in healthy human participants to identify an amygdala–thalamic circuit where network dynamics and connectivity strengths during tasting predict individual variation in taste intensity ratings. This finding implies that individual differences in taste intensity perception do not arise solely from variation in peripheral gustatory factors.
Introduction
Taste acts as a gatekeeper to our internal milieu, with taste quality tuned to identify potentially nutritive or harmful substances (Breslin, 2013) and taste intensity reflecting concentration, and hence quantity, of these substances. Notably, there are considerable individual differences in taste intensity perception that investigators have endeavored to link with food preferences, diet, and risk for obesity and alcohol consumption (Duffy and Bartoshuk, 2000; Pepino and Mennella, 2007; Hayes et al., 2011; Allen et al., 2014; Thibodeau et al., 2017). Investigations into the source of this variance have ocused primarily on possible peripheral mechanisms, such as allelic variation in the TAS2R38 bitter taste receptor gene or the number of taste papillae, but the evidence for such mechanisms in humans is not strong (for review, see Feeney and Hayes, 2014). Less well explored is the possible influence of central mechanisms. However, there is evidence that such a mechanism may exist. Studies of the phenomenon of thermal taste, in which some individuals perceive taste sensations from temperature alone (Cruz and Green, 2000), has shown that the ability to perceive thermal taste is significantly correlated with ratings of the perceived intensity of chemical tastes and the sweet odorant vanillin (Green and George, 2004; Green et al., 2005; Lim et al., 2008). Importantly, the same studies also showed that the individuals who perceived thermal taste did not rate oral somatosensory sensations of creaminess, spiciness, and astringency or thermal sensations produced on the lip and hand any higher than individuals who did not perceive thermal taste. The specificity of these correlations to taste and olfactory stimuli rules out a cognitive bias or differences in scale usage as significant factors in the individual differences. Furthermore, because taste, olfaction, and somesthetic stimulation (temperature, creaminess, and chemical irritation) are all transduced by different peripheral receptors and cranial nerves, these data suggest that the correlations are driven primarily by a central neural mechanism that is common to taste and smell but not to somesthesis (Green and George, 2004; Green et al., 2005).
The neurobiology of such a “central gain mechanism” (CGM) is unexplored. However, early evidence of taste perception in patients who had undergone surgical resection of the anterior medial temporal lobe for the treatment of pharmacologically intractable epilepsy suggests amygdala involvement. Specifically, resections involving the amygdala, particularly from the right hemisphere, result in reduced sour taste detection thresholds (i.e., increased sensitivity; Small et al., 1997) and stronger ratings of the perceived intensity of suprathreshold taste stimuli (Small et al., 2001a,b). These paradoxical observations have been interpreted as evidence for an inhibitory influence from the amygdala to either the gustatory cortex in the insula, or to earlier taste relays (Small et al., 2001b). Blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) responses in the central nucleus are sensitive to modulation of intensity perception (Small et al., 2003), and there are reciprocal connections between the amygdala and the insular taste cortex (Wang et al., 2018), the gustatory thalamus [ventral posterior medial nucleus (VPM); Krettek and Price, 1977], and brainstem [rostral nucleus of the solitary tract (NST); Norgren, 1976] relays. In addition, the amygdala has previously been described as conferring gain during directed attention by biasing signals to modulate early sensory cortex, assigning greater “weights” to emotionally relevant or salient sensory events (Vuilleumier, 2015).
The current study was designed to test the hypothesis that individual variation in taste intensity perception is associated with inhibitory projections from the amygdala to gustatory regions. Our approach was to assess BOLD response to sweet, sour, salty, and bitter tastants in 28 healthy participants using regression, psychophysiological measurement, and dynamic causal modeling (DCM) analyses to determine whether (1) amygdala response to the tastants covaried with the mean intensity rating across tastants; (2) the responsive region of the amygdala was functionally coupled to gustatory regions including brainstem, thalamus, and insula; and (3) using Bayesian model estimation, whether inhibitory influences from the amygdala to gustatory regions (i.e., effective connectivity; Friston et al., 2011) characterize gustatory network dynamics associated with intensity perception.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Thirty right-handed, nonsmoking participants [21 women, 9 men; mean age, 27.53 ± 4.99 years; age range, 18–38 years; mean body mass index (BMI), 21.67 ± 2.88 kg/m2; BMI range, 16.08–26.93 kg/m2], with a mean Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971) score of 90.17 ± 11.41, participated in the current study. They were recruited through advertisements around Yale University and the city of New Haven. The Yale School of Medicine Human Investigation Committee approved the informed consent form, which was subsequently obtained from all study participants. All participants reported having no known taste, smell, neurologic, psychiatric, or other pathologic disorders. Two (of the original 30) participants were excluded because one participant's average taste intensity rating was <2 SDs from the sample mean and another participant's head movements during scanning exceeded a predetermined limit of 1 mm of movement in any direction. The remaining 28 participants (20 women and 8 men) were a mean age of 27.14 ± 4.75 years with an age range of 18–37 years, and had a mean BMI of 21.58 ± 2.96 kg/m2 with a range of 16.08–26.93 kg/m2.
Taste stimuli and delivery
The taste stimuli consisted of a sweet [0.32 m sucrose (Su), Sigma-Aldrich], sour (5.6 × 10−3 m citric acid (CA)], salty [0.14 m sodium chloride (NaCl)], bitter [3.2 × 10−5 m quinine sulfate (Qu)], and a “tasteless” solution designed to mimic the ionic components of saliva (O'Doherty et al., 2001). We used the tasteless solution as the control stimulus because water activates taste cortex (Frey and Petrides, 1999; Zald and Pardo, 2000) and is sometimes reported to have a taste (Bartoshuk et al., 1964). The tasteless solution was mixed to the following five different concentrations: 18.75 mm KCl/1.875 mm NaHCO3 in distilled water, and its 75%, 50%, 25%, and 12.5% dilutions. Liquids were delivered to the participants through a portable gustometer system as 0.5 ml of solution infused into the mouth over 4 s. Detailed description of the gustometer system can be found in previous publications (Veldhuizen et al., 2007, 2011b).
Experimental procedures
Participants took part in one training and taste psychophysics session, and one fMRI scanning session. Sessions were conducted on separate days.
Training and taste psychophysics session.
This session served to select an appropriate tasteless solution for each participant; assess reliable intensity ratings of four tastants representing sweet, sour, salty, and bitter qualities; familiarize participants with the fMRI task' and identify those who found it uncomfortable to perform the task in a simulated fMRI environment. Participants were instructed to refrain from eating or drinking anything other than water for at least an hour before the session. Upon arrival, they received a detailed explanation of the study. After participants read and signed the consent form, anthropometric measurements were taken. Body weight was measured to the nearest 0.1 kg using a calibrated Seca mBCA 514 medical Body Composition Analyzer (Itin Scale Company) with light clothes on only, and height was measured to the nearest 0.1 cm using a calibrated Seca Digital stationary stadiometer (Itin Scale Company). Body mass index (BMI) was calculated as weight (in kilograms) divided by the squared height (in meters) of the participant (BMI = kg/m2). Participants were then presented with the five versions of the tasteless solution and selected the one that “tastes most like nothing” in a sequential pairwise choice procedure.
Next, training on how to use the general labeled magnitude scale (gLMS) was provided. The gLMS is an experimentally derived scale validated for use in describing the breadth of intensities an individual can experience in daily life, from sights and sounds to tastes and pains (Green et al., 1993, 1996; Bartoshuk et al., 2004). The scale consists of a vertical line bounded by the labels “no sensation” at the bottom and “strongest imaginable sensation” at the top, with the intensity labels “barely detectable,” “weak,” “moderate,” “strong,” and “very strong” spaced quasi-logarithmically along the scale (Green et al., 1993). We then asked participants to use the gLMS, which was presented via computer, to rate the intensity of imagined stimuli, such as the sweetness of milk and the bitterness of fresh spring water as well as actual pure taste stimuli and taste stimuli in mixtures.
Following gLMS training, participants were brought to the fMRI simulator and outfitted with the taste delivery system. Participants lay supine within the simulated bore and were asked to rate the intensity of their feelings of hunger, fullness, thirst, anxiety, and need to urinate. Next, participants used the gLMS to rate the perceived intensity of the four taste stimuli and the tasteless solution they had selected. Each taste stimulus and tasteless solution was delivered eight times, resulting in 40 trials in total. The order of stimulus delivery was randomized across participants. In each trial (Fig. 1A), the participant was instructed to swallow immediately after receiving each liquid and then rate the intensity of the specific taste quality of that tastant (sweetness, saltiness, sourness, bitterness; Fig. 1B). When they received the tasteless solution, they were asked to rate overall intensity, since there is no specific taste quality associated with this stimulus. Participants then made a second set of internal states. Liking was measured using a labeled hedonic scale (Lim et al., 2008) consisting of a vertical line scale with the labels “most disliked sensation imaginable” at the lower anchor point, “most liked sensation imaginable” at the upper anchor point, “neutral” in the middle, and “(dis)like slightly,” “(dis)like moderately,” “(dis)like very much,” and “(dis)like extremely” spaced quasi-logarithmically along the scale. Familiarity and wanting were rated using two different visual analog scales. For wanting, “How much do you want to eat more of this?” (“I would never want to eat this” on the left anchor, “Neutral” in the middle, “I would want to eat this more than anything” on the right anchor); for familiarity, “How familiar is this taste?” (“Not familiar at all” on the left anchor, “Neutral” in the middle, “Very familiar” on the right anchor). All of the ratings were conducted in the bore of the simulator using a mouse on a computer monitor viewed via backprojection on a headcoil-mounted mirror. Finally, participants underwent a simulation of a single scanner run, described in detail below.
fMRI scanning session.
The fMRI scanning session took place on a separate day after the training and taste intensity rating session. Participants provided internal state ratings as well as perceptual ratings of the stimuli before and after scanning. Four taste runs were completed during this session. Each run was 9 min and 40 s long, and consisted of the uncued delivery of five different types of stimuli in eight blocks. Blocks had four, six, or eight repeats of the same stimulus. Each run consisted of one block for each of the four taste qualities [throughout this article referred to as taste(Su), taste(CA), taste(NaCl), and taste(Qu)] and four tasteless blocks. A tasteless block was coded as the tasteless control for the tastant that, within a given run, had the same number of repeats in a block [referred to as tasteless(Su), tasteless(CA), tasteless(NaCl), and tasteless(Qu)]. In a block, a repeat of a stimulus presentation consisted of an “on” period of 3 s, during which we presented 0.5 ml of liquid, which was followed by a 6 s interval during which the subject is instructed to swallow (Fig. 1C). Following each block, there is a 1 ml rinse with water followed by a 10 s rest. The participants were instructed to swallow after receiving each liquid and to keep their eyes closed during the entire run. The order of four taste stimuli blocks and four tasteless blocks within each run was counterbalanced, and the order of the runs was counterbalanced across participants. The number of repeats across taste blocks in this protocol was designed for collapsing across taste qualities and to provide balanced presentation of taste and tasteless blocks. Block design was chosen to improve power within participants and is favored for DCM analyses (Daunizeau et al., 2011).
fMRI scanner.
Images were acquired on a Siemens TIM Trio 3T MRI Scanner equipped with a 32-channel head coil. Echoplanar imaging (EPI) was used to measure the BOLD signal as an indication of cerebral brain activation. A susceptibility-weighted echoplanar method with multiband acceleration factor 4 and GRAPPA (generalized, autocalibrating, partially parallel acquisitions) factor 2 was used to image the regional distribution of the BOLD signal with the following parameters: TR, 1000 ms; TE, 30 ms; flip angle, 60°; FOV, 220 mm; matrix, 64 × 64; slice thickness, 2.5 mm; number of slices, 60. Slices were acquired in an interleaved mode, to reduce the cross talk of the slice selection pulse, and in coronal orientation, to cover the medulla oblongata of the brainstem, where the nucleus of the solitary tract is located. In total, 2376 volumes (594 volumes per run) were acquired. At the beginning of each functional run, the MR signal was allowed to equilibrate over two scans (“dummy images”) for a total of 2 s, which were then excluded from analysis. Anatomical scans were acquired with T1-weighted MPRAGE (magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo sequence; TR/TE, 1900/2.52 ms; flip angle, 9°; FOV, 256; matrix, 256 × 256; slice thickness, 1 mm; number of slices, 176). EPI-based field maps were also acquired to correct for susceptibility-induced geometric distortions.
Overview of task design. A, Chronological events and their duration in a trial during the taste psychophysics session. Each trial commenced with a taste delivery of.5 ml over 2 s. The participant was instructed to swallow immediately after receiving each liquid and then rate the intensity of taste. The intensity scale disappeared from the screen after ∼7 s or as soon as the rating was submitted by clicking a mouse button, whichever occurred first. Then a water rinse of 1 ml was presented over 4 s, followed by a rest period of 10 s before the next trial started, except after the tasteless stimulus, which was immediately followed by the next trial. B, The gLMS that was used for measuring perceived taste intensity. The specific taste quality to be rated (e.g., “sweetness” for sucrose and “saltiness” for sodium chloride) was indicated above the scale. When a participant received the tasteless solution the label was “overall intensity,” since no specific taste quality is associated with this stimulus. Participants used a scanner-compatible handheld trackball mouse to make ratings. The horizontal red cursor tracks up and down the scale with vertical mouse position, and clicking the left mouse button submitted the rating. A new gLMS then appeared on the screen to rate the next taste stimulus. C, Chronological events during a run in the fMRI session. Each block consisted of four, six, of eight repeats of a taste stimulus. Each repeat consists of a taste stimulus presentation over 2 s (0.5 ml), followed by a 7 s interval for swallowing. At the end of each block, a water rinse is presented over 4 s (1 ml), followed by a 15 s rest period before the start of the next block. Tasteless blocks are identical except that there is no water rinse before the 15 s rest period. Each run contained four taste blocks (one for each taste quality) and four tasteless blocks. No intensity ratings were made during the fMRI run.
Data analysis
Behavior.
Taste intensity ratings made on the gLMS for each trial and each participant were first log10 transformed before statistical analysis. Eight intensity ratings for each of the tastants and the tasteless solution were then averaged within each participant, respectively. We ran a one-way repeated-measures ANOVA with GraphPad Prism version 7.01 for Windows to test for differences in average perceived intensity differences between the different stimuli. We performed paired t tests with Bonferroni correction for 15 possible multiple comparisons. For each pair of different taste solutions (i.e., sweet–sour, sweet–salty sweet–bitter, sour–salty, sour–bitter, and salty–bitter) we then calculated the Pearson product-moment correlation of log-mean intensity ratings across participants. We performed one-tailed significance tests due to the strong a priori prediction of positive correlations between the intensities of the taste solutions and the absence of any rationale for a negative correlation. We then corrected for multiple comparisons using a Bonferroni correction for a total of six tests, leading to an α of 0.0167. We averaged across the taste solutions that showed significant correlations in intensity to calculate a grand average score for each participant's taste intensity ratings. This included sweet, sour, and salty tastants, and excluded quinine as a tastant (see Taste psychophysics in the Results section). We used principal component analysis in R studio to confirm the existence of more than a single component in the variance across participants. We calculated a grand taste intensity score for each participant by averaging ratings for sweet, sour, and salty tastants, referred to hereafter as the mean log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl) to be used in subsequent data analyses. Data visualizations were created with GraphPad Prism (scatterplots) and ggplot2 package for RStudio.
fMRI analysis.
Data were analyzed on Linux workstations using MATLAB R2011a (MathWorks) and SPM12 (Wellcome Trust Center for Neuroimaging, London, UK). All functional images from each participant were corrected for geometric distortions caused by susceptibility-induced field inhomogeneities. A combined approach was used that corrects for both static distortions and changes in these distortions attributable to head motion (Andersson et al., 2001; Hutton et al., 2002). The static distortions were calculated for each participant by acquiring a field map and processing it using the FieldMap toolbox implemented in SPM12 (Hutton et al., 2004). The images were then realigned, unwarped, and coregistered to the participant's own anatomic T1 image. The anatomic T1 image was processed using a unified segmentation procedure combining segmentation, bias correction, and spatial normalization (Ashburner and Friston, 2005); the same normalization parameters were then used to normalize the functional images. Then, all functional images were detrended using a method for removing any linear components matching the global signal at each voxel (Macey et al., 2004). Finally, functional images were smoothed with a 6 mm FWHM isotropic Gaussian kernel.
For the time-series analysis on all participants, a high-pass filter (300s) was included in the filtering matrix (adjusted from the convention in SPM12 to reflect the maximal period between two blocks of tasteless) to remove low-frequency noise and slow drifts in the signal. Condition-specific effects at each voxel were estimated using the general linear model (Friston et al., 1995; Worsley and Friston, 1995). The response to events was modeled using a canonical hemodynamic response function included in SPM12. The temporal derivative of the hemodynamic response function was also included as part of the basis set to account for up to 1 s shifts in the timing of the events (Henson et al., 2002). There were two events of interest: “taste” and “tasteless.” For taste, taste blocks taste(Su), taste(CA), and taste(NaCl) were collapsed into a single event “taste(μ Su CA NaCl),” referring to the mean response to these tastants, parallel to the averaging of the ratings in the analysis for psychophysics. The corresponding tasteless blocks were also collapsed into a single event, “tasteless.” Rinses, taste(Qu), and tasteless(Qu) blocks were each modeled as a nuisance effect. The Artifact Detection Tools (ART) toolbox (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/artifact_detect/) for MATLAB was used to detect global mean and motion outliers in the functional data. Motion parameters were included as regressors in the design matrix at the single-subject level. In addition, image volumes in which the z-normalized global brain activation exceeded 3 SDs from the mean of the run or showed 1 mm of composite (linear plus rotational) movement were flagged as outliers and deweighted during SPM estimation. We additionally calculated a composite movement estimate for each participant, frame-to-frame displacement. Frame-to-frame displacement was calculated by taking the Euclidean distance between the center of gravity of an image and the next image (Satterthwaite et al., 2013). We summed frame-to-frame displacement for all images within a run and subsequently averaged them across runs.
To assess group-level main effects of versus Tasteless independent of differences in individual sensitivity, the parameter estimate images for each participant were entered into a one-sample t test using individual contrasts of taste(μ Su CA NaCl)-tasteless. We also included all individual grand average scores of taste intensity ratings and composite movement estimates. To test the hypothesis that response in the amygdala would correlate with overall subjective taste intensity perception, we regressed individual grand average scores of taste intensity against BOLD response to taste(μ Su CA NaCl)-tasteless across all 28 participants using a small volume correction (sphere with 6 mm radius, containing 123 voxels) defined from a region of amygdala that we have previously shown codes taste intensity perception (at coordinate [(–) 24 −6 −21]) [30]. The t map threshold was set at puncorrected < 0.001 and a cluster size at a minimum of 5 contiguous voxels. Unpredicted clusters were considered significant at p < 0.05, false discovery rate corrected across the whole brain.
Psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis (Friston et al., 1997) was used to identify functional connectivity between the area of the amygdala sensitive to taste intensity perception and earlier gustatory relays (insular cortex, thalamus, and brainstem) using small volume corrections (sphere with 6 mm radius, containing 123 voxels) defined by centroids from two prior meta-analyses (at coordinates: [(−)8 −26 8, and (−)10 −14 −8]; Veldhuizen et al., 2011a; Yeung et al., 2017). We used two meta-analyses because the first only reported thalamic responses in the mediodorsal (md) thalamus, and we wished to include the gustatory thalamus. Peaks were corrected for multiple comparison across the voxels within the small volumes and then corrected for the number of regions included (insula, thalamus, and brainstem). Regions that showed a main effect of taste(μ Su CA NaCl)-tasteless at the group level or a relation with individual grand average scores were used as seed regions. The first eigenvariate of the time-series data were extracted from a 6 mm sphere with a centroid on the coordinate of the amygdala cluster. The eigenvariate was then deconvolved (Gitelman et al., 2003), multiplied with the psychological variable (taste(μ Su CA NaCl)-tasteless), and reconvolved with a hemodynamic response function to form the PPI term. For each participant, we computed new parameter estimate images with the PPI as a regressor of interest and the time-series eigenvariate and psychological variable as nuisance regressors. In order to examine which areas display increased or decreased connectivity with the seed regions associated with intensity perception, we then regressed individual grand average scores of taste intensity ratings against PPI parameter estimate images across all 28 participants.
DCM was used to characterize associations between individual sensitivity to taste and neural circuitry in a network of regions responding to taste(μ Su CA NaCl)-tasteless. We selected the seed and target regions that resulted from using the central amygdala PPI analysis. We specified a single fully connected and driven DCM between these regions. The DCM specified driving inputs (taste and tasteless regressors) into the network at all five regions. The DCM also specified a complete connectivity matrix for intrinsic, steady-state, bidirectional connections among the five regions. No modulatory effects on the connections between regions were specified, as they are not obligatory (Friston et al., 2003) and the design included no within-participant variables qualifying for modulation in the DCM. We first estimated the full DCM for each individual participant. We then used the second-level parametric empirical Bayes framework (Friston et al., 2016) pruning approach to reduce the model to a configuration of parameters to best explain the data observed in the regions. We used the pruning approach separately for the driving inputs and connectivity matrix (Zeidman et al., 2019a). We created a second-level general linear model with a design matrix that includes a constant factor, a regressor-of-interest for the individual grand average scores of taste intensity, and composite movement estimates as a nuisance variable. This results in a (Bayesian) hierarchical model, with estimates for connection strengths (positive or negative) between regions and their uncertainty (i.e., posterior probability; Zeidman et al., 2019b). There are two sets of estimates, as follows: (1) for a model that describes the average neural circuitry across participants, independent of individual sensitivity to taste; and (2) for a model that describes the neural circuitry associated with individual grand average scores of taste intensity. We display any parameters that showed a probability of >0.90. We used all connectivity parameters to predict individual grand average scores of taste intensity, using a leave-one-out cross-validation (LOOCV) framework. In this regression procedure, a model is fitted to data from all but one participant and a prediction of the score is made for that participant, which is repeated in turn for all participants. This results in a Pearson's correlation between predicted and empirical intensity scores. We used an α of 0.05 to determine significance for this correlation.
Data visualizations were created with GraphPad Prism (GraphPad Software) for scatterplots, ggplot2 for boxplots (R-studio), Mango by Research Imaging Institute, UTHSCSA (for SPMs overlaid on brain sections of the BigBrain (Amunts et al., 2013), and Illustrator for DCM models (Adobe).
Data availability
Raw fMRI data are available upon request. Contact dana.small{at}yale.edu.
Statistical maps of the human brain are available on NeuroVault repository: https://identifiers.org/neurovault.collection:7635.
Results
Taste psychophysics
We first set out to determine whether prior findings of high cross-correlations between taste intensity ratings were replicated (Green and George, 2004; Lim et al., 2008). The four taste solutions were all rated as stronger in intensity than tasteless (Fig. 2; main effect of taste stimulus: F(1,135) = 69.14, p < 0.001; sweet-tasteless: t(27) = 9.83, p < 0.001; sour-tasteless: t(27) = 8.94, p < 0.001; salty-tasteless: r = t(27) = 9.94, p < 0.001; bitter-tasteless: t(27) = 8.51, p < 0.001), while the taste solutions were not rated significantly different in intensity from each other (all corrected p values >0.826). Pearson correlation analysis showed that intensity ratings of the four taste qualities positively correlated with each other (sweet-sour, r = 0.776; sweet-salty, r = 0.855; sweet-bitter, r = 0.373; sour-salty, r = 0.807; sour-bitter, r = 0.387; and salty-bitter, r = 0.417; Fig. 2); however, the correlation between quinine and the other taste solutions did not survive correction for multiple comparisons. The latter result is consistent with significant individual differences that have been found even among the sensitivities to different bitter compounds (Yokomukai et al., 1993; Delwiche et al., 2001), which has been proposed to result from differences in receptor expression among the 25 bitter taste receptors in humans (Meyerhof et al., 2010). Given these differences, a lower correlation between bitter and other tastes is not unexpected. A principal component analysis confirmed the existence of two components underlying the variance across participants, the first component accounted for 72% of the variance and received loadings from sweet, sour, and salty intensity, and the second component accounted for 19% of the variance and received a loading from quinine. Note also that the tasteless solution showed no significant correlation with any of the four prototypical tastes (sweet-tasteless, r = 0.061; sour-tasteless, r = −0.109; salty-tasteless, r = 0.119; bitter-tasteless, r = 0.041). Thus, we calculated grand intensity scores for each participant's taste intensity ratings by averaging ratings for sweet, sour, and salty tastants, referred to hereafter as the mean log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl). The Pearson correlation matrix between grand intensity scores and the intensity scores of the four taste solutions are shown in Figure 2, including all r values (effect size) and p values (corrected for multiple comparisons).
Taste intensity ratings. Below the diagonal: scatterplots of log-transformed intensity ratings on the gLMS for all tastant pairs and each tastant with the average of the three significantly correlated tastants (from top to bottom: citric acid, sodium chloride, quinine, and mean log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl); from left to right sucrose, citric acid, sodium chloride, quinine). Each datapoint represents one participant. Above the diagonal, The boxplots indicate central tendencies and spread of the log-transformed intensity rating (y-axis on far right), as follows: average (represented by a cross symbol), median (middle bar in box), first and third quartiles (lower and upper hinge), 1.5 × the interquartile range (top and bottom whiskers) and outlying points (separate dots outside the whiskers). We overlaid individual data points (one for each participant) on the boxplots with within-participant SEM of the average of eight ratings they gave for each tastant represented as horizontal error bars. From left to right, the boxplots represent the following: sucrose, citric acid, sodium chloride, quinine, and mean log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl). For interpretation, gLMS label placement is given on the y-axis on the far right.
fMRI data
Main effect of taste versus tasteless
Consistent with previous studies, a main effect of taste(μ Su NA NaCl)-tasteless was observed in bilateral md and anterior-ventral insula, and extending into overlying operculum, bilateral pre- and post-central gyrus, left amygdala, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, and left putamen (Fig. 3, Table 1).
Significant clusters of BOLD response to taste(μ Su CA NaCl)-tasteless
Neural response to taste(μ Su CA NaCl)-tasteless in insula, pre- and post-central gyrus, amygdala, and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; key: r, right; l, left; MI, middle insula; VI, ventral insula; PG, pre- and post-central gyrus; AM, amygdala; ACC, anterior cingulate cortex). Sections (slice location indicated in MNI coordinates) show canonical anatomic template with SPM T-map overlaid, thresholded at puncorrected < 0.005, and a minimum of five contiguous voxels. The color gradient scale depicts suprathreshold t values. Boxplot graphs show median (center line), mean (diamond), first and third quartiles (lower and upper hinge), 1.5 × the interquartile range (top and bottom whiskers), and outlying points (separate dots outside the whiskers). We plotted parameter estimate of the peak voxels within a significant cluster for the average of taste(μ Su CA NaCl)-tasteless. We overlaid individual data points on the boxplots and connected the dots of an individual participant between the taste and tasteless bars to make it easier to inspect the difference within a single participant.
Regression of BOLD response with mean log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl)
To identify regions sensitive to overall gustatory system sensitivity, we used the mean log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl) as the independent variable in a regression analysis with the BOLD response to taste(μ Su CA NaCl)-tasteless. As predicted, the BOLD response in the left central amygdala was significantly associated with mean log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl). We also observed significant associations with response in the cuneus (Fig. 4, Table 2). Notably, response in insular cortex did not correlate with the mean intensity rating. We also note that neither swallowing estimates (r(22) = −0.051, p = 0.819; see Extended Data Materials) nor composite movement estimates (r(27) = −0.124, p = 0.531) correlate with the mean intensity ratings.
Significant clusters of correlation between log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl) and BOLD response to taste(μ Su CA NaCl)-tasteless
Neural response to taste(μ Su CA NaCl)-tasteless correlated with mean log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl) in A, left amygdala (AM), and B, bilateral cuneus. Brain slices are as described in Figure 3. Scatterplots show each participant's values, with mean log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl) on the x-axis and parameter estimate on the y-axis for the peak voxel within a significant cluster. Regression line with regression coefficient and 95% confidence interval bands are overlaid for illustrative purposes. Positive associations are shown in blue, and negative associations are shown in red.
Functional connectivity of the CGM
Next, to identify regions of the brain where functional connectivity with the amygdala is influenced by taste sensitivity, we first performed a PPI analysis (Friston et al., 1997) and then regressed the responses of areas displaying significant functional connectivity with the mean log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl). The presence of functional connectivity indicates that the connected regions display temporal correlation beyond that accounted for by the task and suggests that the regions form a functional network. The subsequent regression then determines which of these network associations are influenced by taste sensitivity. This analysis revealed amygdala functional connectivity with the md and ventral posterior medial/pulvinar (vpm/pul) thalamus (Fig. 5A, Table 3). We observed unpredicted functional connectivity in the mid-temporal gyrus (Table 3). No other significant effects were observed.
Significant clusters of correlation between log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl) and psychophysical interactions using peaks from correlation between log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl) (see Table 2)
Connectivity correlated with mean log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl). A, Connectivity parameter estimates from PPI analysis between seed region in left amygdala (am) and regions in thalamus (md and vpm/pul) that correlated with log mean intensityμ(Su CA NaCl). Brain sections are as described in Figure 3, and scatterplots are as described in Figure 4. B, Connectivity Bayesian estimates in the DCM of the amygdala and thalamus areas. The gradient of arrows indicates the estimates for connection strengths (positive in orange or negative in blue) between regions for any connections with a posterior probability of p > 0.95. The scatterplot shows each participant's values from the multivariate LOOCV regression, with mean log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl) on the x-axis and connectivity parameter estimate of the multivariate linear DCM model on the y-axis. The regression line with regression coefficient and 95% confidence interval bands is overlaid for illustrative purposes. Asterisks in the model indicate the connection that survived univariate LOOCV regression.
Effective connectivity of the CGM
Whereas PPI identifies correlations between regions, DCM can be used to establish effective connectivity, enabling inferences to be made about the influence that one neuronal system exerts over another (Friston et al., 2011). Based on the PPI and our hypothesized CGM, we predicted the existence of inhibitory modulatory influences from the amygdala to the thalamic nuclei, where the strength of the connection is associated with taste sensitivity. We therefore specified a DCM with five regions (left amygdala, bilateral md/thalamus, and bilateral vpm/pul) to examine the ensemble of connectivity parameters in relation to log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl) and subjected this to Bayesian model estimation. The architecture of the selected model conformed to our prediction, as follows: inhibitory connections were identified from the amygdala to the four thalamic peaks (Fig. 5B). We also identified a positive bidirectional connection between right md and right vpm/pul. When we entered the connectivity parameters into a multivariate LOOCV regression, we found a significant association between predicted and observed mean log intensityμ(Su CA NaCl (r(27) = 0.37, p = 0.027; Fig. 5B). To determine which of these connections contributed most to this predictive model, we used a univariate LOOCV approach, which revealed that the connections from amygdala to left vpm/pul and right md survived, as well as the positive connection from right vpm/pul to right md.
Discussion
Significant individual differences exist in taste intensity perception. Some of these differences can be explained by peripheral mechanisms, such as genetic variation in taste receptor expression and biology. However, as noted in the Introduction, while peripheral mechanisms can play a significant role in individual differences in the perceived intensity of individual taste qualities or even specific tastants (e.g., PROP), they are not able to account for overall differences in gustatory system sensitivity, such as the high within-subject cross-correlations among intensity ratings of different taste qualities (Green and George, 2004; Green et al., 2005; Lim et al., 2008). These observations are suggestive of a central mechanism that affects the responsiveness to all taste stimulation. Here we used fMRI and taste psychophysics to identify a region of amygdala where the response to a series of tastants is correlated with overall gustatory system sensitivity and drives inhibitory outputs to the gustatory and limbic nuclei of the thalamus. We further show that the dynamics and strength of these inhibitory connections are able to predict individual variation in taste intensity ratings.
Our primary hypothesis for a CGM focused on the amygdala for several reasons. First, patients with amygdala removal for the treatment of epilepsy show enhanced taste, but not visual, intensity ratings (Small et al., 1997, 2001a,b). This enhanced perception suggests the existence of an inhibitory influence from the amygdala to the gustatory network, which when removed releases the target regions from inhibition to enhance the taste signal. Second, our prediction aligns with the known anatomy and physiology of the amygdala. The amygdala can be broadly subdivided into the basolateral complex, which receives sensory inputs, and a central medial division that produces the primary efferent information. Central nucleus efferent projections tend to be inhibitory and play an important role in regulating innate emotional responses and their associated autonomic outflow via connections to brainstem nuclei and md thalamus (Aggleton and Mishkin, 1984; Swanson and Petrovich, 1998; Barbas, 2000; Fadok et al., 2018). Accordingly, and in line with our prediction, we observed a correlation between the response to taste in the central medial nucleus of the amygdala and the mean log intensity ratings to sweet, sour, and salty tastants. Further, our connectivity analyses revealed inhibitory influences from this region to the md and gustatory (i.e., VPM) thalamic nuclei.
In primates, including humans, taste information ascends from the periphery to the rostral NST (Beckstead and Norgren, 1979). In rodents, after an obligatory synapse in the parabrachial pons, the gustatory pathway bifurcates at this point with one projection targeting the amygdala and hypothalamus and a second projection targeting the VPM gustatory thalamus, which subsequently projects to insular taste cortex (Norgren and Leonard, 1971; Norgren, 1976, 1984, 1990; Kosar et al., 1986). In primates, there is no bifurcation and only a single projection from NST to VPM and finally insular taste cortex (Beckstead et al., 1980; Pritchard et al., 1986, 2000). Given this significant interspecies difference, we formed our prediction from the anatomic work in nonhuman primates and reasoned that the target for the inhibitory signal from the amygdala could be NST, thalamus, or insula (and not pons). However, the results from the PPI, which evaluated functional connectivity between the amygdala and the rest of the voxels in the brain, identified selective bilateral connectivity with two thalamic nuclei, highlighting these nuclei as the primary targets for inhibitory modulation.
The md thalamus is not part of the canonical taste pathway. However, it does receive gustatory and olfactory input (Price and Slotnick, 1983; Shi and Cassell, 1998; Cavada et al., 2000), and, although amygdala–thalamic connectivity is understudied, there is some evidence for a direct projection from the central nucleus of the amygdala to the md thalamus (Aggleton and Mishkin, 1984). By contrast, reciprocal connections do not appear to exist (Aggleton et al., 1980), which is consistent with the lack of reciprocal connections revealed here in the DCM analysis. While activation of the md thalamus is frequently found in gustatory fMRI studies, little is understood about its role in gustation. Work in nonhuman primates shows that it projects to the posterior orbitofrontal cortex, which is a site of convergence for taste and smell information (De Araujo et al., 2003; Small et al., 2004) and is proposed to form a tripartite circuit to convey information about emotional (and presumably physiological) significance to afferent sensory information (Timbie and Barbas, 2015). Accordingly, in rodents c-Fos expression is greater for familiar tastes and taste odor mixtures, suggesting sensitivity to flavor nutrient conditioning and therefore physiological significance (Fredericksen et al., 2019).
The second thalamic activation was further posterior and closely corresponded to the thalamic region previously associated with taste intensity perception (Small et al., 2003; Yeung et al., 2016). However, the activation extended into pulvinar, which is of relevance because it is often coactivated with amygdala in response to salient but nongustatory stimuli (Morris et al., 1997, 1999). Importantly, the entire human thalamus is only ∼30 × 20 × 20 mm, with the VPM occupying less than one-tenth of this volume (Herrero et al., 2002). Given our 2 mm3 voxel size and 6 mm smoothing kernel, the taste thalamus, which is the parvicellular division of the VPM, is beyond the limits of our spatial resolution. It is therefore likely that the cluster of response we have observed overlaps the taste nucleus. The VPM is the target of gustatory afferents and projects to the insular taste cortex. However, no known direct connections exist between central medial amygdala and VPM (Kaskan et al., 2019). How the inhibitory influences from the central medial amygdala reach the VPM thalamus to regulate perception is therefore unknown and an important topic for future work. A related issue is whether this circuit operates independently from the known basolateral amygdala influence on taste cortex, which has been shown to enhance detection of expected taste stimuli in rodents (Samuelsen et al., 2012). Here we observed no associations between overall intensity perception and basolateral amygdala response or connectivity. Another consideration is the well known role the amygdala plays in orienting to salient stimuli (Cunningham and Brosch, 2012), even when presentation is implicit (Morris et al., 1999). Since the primary function of taste sensation is to guide feeding decisions, amygdala “gating” of taste information could be part of a larger system devoted to the identification of salient stimuli and attentional allocation.
However, whereas amygdala lesions impair taste recognition (Small et al., 1997) similarly to the effects observed with other salient stimuli, such as fearful faces (Adolphs et al., 1994; Calder, 1996), opposing effects are seen with sensitivity. Specifically, sensitivity to taste is enhanced while sensitivity to visual and auditory stimuli that should evoke fear is severely blunted (Adolphs et al., 1994; Calder, 1996). Interestingly, taste identification is likely encoded in parallel with taste detection (Wallroth and Ohla, 2018), with evidence that taste intensity is computed at the perceptual (Veldhuizen et al., 2010) and neural (Katz et al., 2002) levels before identity and hedonic significance. These observations suggest that the CGM may operate on the earliest stages of gustatory processing. Accordingly, the CGM may function to modulate taste and olfactory stimulation to increase the saliency of significant flavor stimuli in the service of selective attention or in the course of associative learning. Indeed, CGMs have been proposed and studied as a mechanism of selective attention in vision and hearing (Connor et al., 1996; Hillyard et al., 1998; Kauramäki et al., 2007; Womelsdorf and Fries, 2007; Jacobs et al., 2012). In this view, individual differences in the perceived intensity of tastes and odors would be attributable in part to differences in the baseline “gain” of such a system. The alternative explanation is that the amygdala response reflects a downstream effect of an earlier modulatory mechanism. However, while the correlational nature of fMRI data means that it is not possible to rule this possibility out, it is unlikely to account for our findings. First, a peripheral mechanism cannot account for the across-tastant and across-modal (olfactory) nature of the effect (Green and George, 2004; Green et al., 2005) and, second, neither can the insular cortex response, which reflects the first cortical relay, because it was not correlated with mean intensity ratings.
Relatedly, there is evidence for opposing responses in gustatory and visual streams of processing, especially regarding allocation of attentional resources. For example, breaches of taste expectation that occur when an individual expects to sense one tastant (e.g., sweet) but receives another (e.g., tasteless) are associated with enhanced response in gustatory, attentional, and error signaling regions coupled with decreased response in visual cortex (Veldhuizen et al., 2011b). Likewise, here we observed positive functional connectivity between the amygdala and the thalamic targets and negative functional connectivity between the amygdala and visual cortex. This suggests that the amygdala may play a role in coordinating the allocation of resources to visual (or distal) versus gustatory (or proximal/visceral) sensory systems, depending on the behavioral context or state of the organism.
We scanned with a multiband sequence to increase spatial coverage and spatial and temporal resolution. Slice leakage artifacts may result from multiband sequences and lead to false positive results (Todd et al., 2016). Here we used a combination of acceleration parameters that are at an intermediate level of signal-to-noise ratio and have an intermediate probability of obtaining a false-positive result. This was aimed to strike a good balance between higher signal-to-noise ratio and avoiding false positive results.
Finally, we note that although we excluded bitter taste from our analyses, we do not suggest that bitter tastants are unaffected by the CGM. Rather, it is likely that central factors are harder to detect because of the significant genetic variation in bitter taste receptors (Kim et al., 2004; Drayna, 2005), causing bitter taste perception to be more strongly influenced by peripheral mechanisms (Meyerhof et al., 2010; Hayes et al., 2015).
Here we identify a region of amygdala where the response to a series of tastants is correlated with overall gustatory system responsiveness and drives inhibitory outputs to the gustatory and limbic nuclei of the thalamus. Since the dynamics and strength of these inhibitory connections are able to predict individual variation in taste intensity ratings, we suggest that this circuit acts to create gain in the gustatory system to regulate taste intensity perception, and that the magnitudeof the gain contributes to individual differences in taste perception.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Training Grant T32-NS-041228 (M.C.F.); the Yale Medical School Fellowship (M.C.F.); and National Institutes of Health Grant R01-DC-006706 (D.M.S.).
- Correspondence should be addressed to Maria G. Veldhuizen at margaveldhuizen{at}gmail.com or Dana M. Small at dana.small{at}yale.edu