Abstract
Modifications in the sensitivity of neural elements allow the brain to adapt its functions to varying demands. Frequency-dependent short-term synaptic depression (STD) provides a dynamic gain-control mechanism enabling adaptation to different background conditions alongside enhanced sensitivity to input-driven changes in activity. In contrast, synapses displaying frequency-invariant transmission can faithfully transfer ongoing presynaptic rates enabling linear processing, deemed critical for many functions. However, rigid frequency-invariant transmission may lead to runaway dynamics and low sensitivity to changes in rate. Here, I investigated the Purkinje cell to deep cerebellar nuclei neuron synapses (PC_DCNs), which display frequency invariance, and yet, PCs maintain background activity at disparate rates, even at rest. Using protracted PC_DCN activation (120 s) to mimic background activity in cerebellar slices from mature mice of both sexes, I identified a previously unrecognized, frequency-dependent, slow STD (S-STD), adapting IPSC amplitudes in tens of seconds to minutes. However, after changes in activation rates, over a behavior-relevant second-long time window, S-STD enabled scaled linear encoding of PC rates in synaptic charge transfer and DCN spiking activity. Combined electrophysiology, optogenetics, and statistical analysis suggested that S-STD mechanism is input-specific, involving decreased ready-to-release quanta, and distinct from faster short-term plasticity (f-STP). Accordingly, an S-STD component with a scaling effect (i.e., activity-dependent release sites inactivation), extending a model explaining PC_DCN release on shorter timescales using balanced f-STP, reproduced the experimental results. Thus, these results elucidates a novel slow gain-control mechanism able to support linear transfer of behavior-driven/learned PC rates concurrently with background activity adaptation, and furthermore, provides an alternative pathway to refine PC output.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The brain can adapt to varying demands by dynamically changing the gain of its synapses; however, some tasks require ongoing linear transfer of presynaptic rates, seemingly incompatible with nonlinear gain adaptation. Here, I report a novel slow gain-control mechanism enabling scaled linear encoding of presynaptic rates over behavior-relevant time windows, and adaptation to background activity at the Purkinje to deep cerebellar nuclear neurons synapses (PC_DCNs). A previously unrecognized PC_DCNs slow and frequency-dependent short-term synaptic depression (S-STD) mediates this process. Experimental evidence and simulations suggested that scaled linear encoding emerges from the combination of S-STD slow dynamics and frequency-invariant transmission at faster timescales. These results demonstrate a mechanism reconciling rate code with background activity adaptation and suitable for flexibly tuning PCs output via background activity modulation.
- background activity
- gain modulation
- short-term memory
- short-term plasticity
- sustained activity
- synaptic transmission
Introduction
Changes in the sensitivity or gain of neural structures is a general principle that allows the brain to adapt to varying sensory, motor, emotional, or cognitive demands (Silver, 2010; Whitmire and Stanley, 2016). One mechanism supporting gain adaptation is use-dependent short-term synaptic depression (STD), where high levels of activity lead to lower synaptic strength, thereby dynamically adapting the output while enhancing the sensitivity to changes in presynaptic activity (Abbott et al., 1997; Tsodyks and Markram, 1997). The latter is because changes in synaptic strength lag behind the change in presynaptic activity, such that for short time windows, in the millisecond range for fast-STDs (Zucker and Regehr, 2002), the output becomes proportional to the new rate, before readapting. This process produces brief transient signals informative about the timing and proportion of the initial change in rate (Abbott et al., 1997). However, beyond this brief window, STD filters the synaptic output preventing faithful transfer of complete presynaptic rate profiles, which may hold important information.
Such is the case of the cerebellar cortex principal neurons, the GABAergic Purkinje cells (PCs), which display changes in rate associated to cerebellar-controlled behaviors (e.g., eye-blinks, saccades, and whisks) extending beyond the referred STD time window, considered critical for representing and controlling specific behavioral parameters (e.g., Catz et al., 2008; Herzfeld et al., 2015; Ten Brinke et al., 2017; Romano et al., 2018). Suitably, the synapses of the PCs to their main target, the deep cerebellar nuclei neurons, the cerebellar output stage (PC_DCNs), exhibit frequency-invariant transmission supporting linear encoding of PC rates (Turecek et al., 2016). Mechanistically, PC_DCN frequency invariance is mediated by a developmentally regulated synaptotagmin-7-dependent process, possibly synaptic facilitation (Turecek et al., 2017) counteracting frequency-dependent fast-STD (Pedroarena and Schwarz, 2003). Functionally, linear encoding has been widely reported in the cerebellum, including the cerebellar nuclei (Arenz et al., 2008; Herzfeld et al., 2015; S. Chen et al., 2016; Hong et al., 2016; Jelitai et al., 2016; Abbasi et al., 2017; Dugué et al., 2017; Ten Brinke et al., 2017; Payne et al., 2019; but see Halverson et al., 2015), although alternative coding strategies could (co)exist (De Zeeuw et al., 2011). Reasonably, the latter studies focused on time windows matching usual behaviors (i.e., ranging from subsecond to few seconds).
Yet, PCs discharge simple spikes almost continuously at disparate rates diverging by as much as 100 Hz, even in awake animals at rest and within the same region (Thach, 1968; De Zeeuw et al., 1995; Bryant et al., 2009; Xiao et al., 2014; Zhou et al., 2014; Laurens and Angelaki, 2017). Therefore, PC signals associated with particular behaviors consist of transitory changes in rate embedded in the background activity. PC background activity has received much less attention than the behavior-driven signals, although it is altered in some cerebellar-associated diseases (Rinaldo and Hansel, 2010; Tsai et al., 2012). Differences in intrinsic properties and ongoing PC inputs explain PC background rates diversity and variability (Ozden et al., 2012; Zhou et al., 2014; Jelitai et al., 2016). This suggests that PC background activity might be a controlled variable, potentially moldable via PC intrinsic plasticity (Belmeguenai et al., 2010; Shim et al., 2017; Grasselli et al., 2020), plastic changes of the upstream cerebellar circuits (Gao et al., 2012), and/or context-dependent changes in the ongoing activity of PCs numerous inputs. However, PC background activity represents a challenge for frequency-invariant synapses because the postsynaptic effect of behavior-driven/learned changes in PC rate could be obscured by the wide span of background rates from converging PCs (Abbott et al., 1997), as for keeping DCNs within their working-firing range.
Expanding the computational power of PC_DCNs by coexpression of different forms of short-term plasticity (STP) (Magleby, 1973; MacLeod et al., 2007; Turecek et al., 2016; Doussau et al., 2017) could solve this conundrum; however, PC_DCN-STP has not been previously explored at timescales matching background activity. This study, by closing this gap, identified a novel mechanism supporting concurrent rate code and background activity adaptation and potentially useful for tuning PC output.
Materials and Methods
The animal protocol was reviewed and approved by an independent local committee and the Regional Council of Tübingen and conducted according to the standards of German law and the Society for Neurosciences.
Cerebellar slices preparation
Cerebellar slices from C57BL/6 or L7-ChR2-eYFP (Chaumont et al., 2013) mice (either sex, P21–P32), deeply anesthetized with ketamine (150 mg/kg), were prepared using a vibratome (Leica Microsystems) and ACSF (containing the following, in mM: 125.5 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 1.3 NaH2PO4, 1 MgCl2, 26 NaHCO3, 20 glucose, 1.5 CaCl2), bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2 and, warmed at 26°C. Slices were first stored for 30 min at 36°C and afterward at room temperature in the same solution. The same solution was used for recordings. A set of experiments using male and female C57 BL/6 adult mice (P55 to P72) was conducted using similar procedures.
Patch-clamp recordings and extracellular stimulation
Whole-cell voltage-clamp, or cell-attached recordings were made from large (>20 µm) DCNs in the lateral or interpositus nuclei of cerebellar slices maintained at 36 ± 0.5°C using an Axoclamp1D-amplifier (Molecular Devices). For voltage-clamp recordings, the electrode solution contained the following (in mM): 145 CsCl, 10 K-HEPES, 0.5 EGTA, 0.4 NaGTP, 4.8 K-ATP, 2.5 MgCl2, 0.1 CaCl2, 5 QX314. The pH was adjusted to 7.3 with CsOH. In a small set of experiments (n = 5), electrodes were filled with a solution containing the following (in mM): 134 K-gluconate, 6 KCl, 10 K-HEPES, 0.1 EGTA, 0.3 NaGTP, 2 K-ATP, 10 phosphocreatine, 2 MgCl2, 5 QX-314, and cells were clamped at 0 mV. The results were similar to the ones obtained with the CsCl-based solution, and thus later pooled together for analysis. Series resistance was adjusted up to 75%, and recordings were discarded when series resistance (usually controlled before and after trials) changed >20%. For cell-attached recordings, the pipettes were filled with ACSF. Recordings were digitized (12.5 kHz), and stored using programmable software (Spike 2, CED).
To stimulate PC axons, a pair of tungsten microelectrodes (Frederick Haer) was located in the white matter surrounding the cerebellar nuclei. Biphasic current pulses (100 µs each phase or 100/50 µs, 2-100 µA, usually <25 µA) were delivered using a constant current unit (stimulus isolator, WPI) triggered by the Spike 2 software. The electrodes were oriented to minimize the stimulation current necessary to activate axons in the white matter and to reduce the possibility of intranuclear stimulation. Single stimuli for evaluating IPSCs under control conditions were delivered at 0.1 or 0.05 Hz. To simulate PC tonic background firing, PC axons were repetitively stimulated at 10, 29.5, or 67 Hz for at least 2 min, interleaving a pause of at least 10 min between trials and in variable order. To investigate the effect of irregular PC activation, Poisson-like activation patterns with mean frequencies 10, 30, or 70 Hz were delivered using Spike 2 software.
For opto-genetic (OG) stimulation of PC axons and terminals light pulses (0.5-2 ms, 470 nm) from a LED (CoolLed), triggered by the Spike 2 software were delivered through the microscope objective to cerebellar slices from L7-ChR2-eYFP mice (Chaumont et al., 2013). The location of the spot of light was adjusted before recording using the microscope diaphragm in the light path. The time course of OG and combined electrical stimulation (ES) IPSCs could be very similar, suggesting similar mechanism of neurotransmitter release. However, in some experiments (even when the spot of light was centered in the white matter), the OG IPSCs presented steps in the rising and decaying phase and were onaverage slower. This observation may reflect desynchronized activation due to variable latency for triggering spikes in different axons or terminals.
Drugs
Kynurenate (3-5 mm, Tocris Bioscience), a broad-spectrum antagonist of ionotropic glutamate receptors, was systematically applied to the ACSF, to isolate pharmacologically the response of PC synapses from the potential glutamatergic responses evoked by activation of mossy and climbing fiber collaterals in the white matter. In addition, in specific experiments exploring DCN responses, strychnine (1 μm, Sigma Millipore), a glycine receptor antagonist, and CGP 55 845 (3 μm, Tocris Bioscience), a GABAB receptor antagonist, were applied to the bath. These results were similar to those without these two blockers, and thus later pooled together for analysis.
Experimental design and statistical analysis
Programmable software (Spike 2, CED) was used to automatically calculate IPSC peak amplitude as the difference between the average baseline (measured for 300 µs before each stimulus) and the IPSC peak. For trains with interpulse intervals ≤10 ms, the baseline was calculated by fitting an exponential decay to the preceding IPSC. For analysis of repetitive IPSCs, the IPSC peak amplitude was normalized to the amplitude of corresponding control IPSCs evoked at low frequency immediately before each train of stimuli. For experiments using protracted stimulation, only data from experiments with at least two different stimulating frequencies of either regular or Poisson-like patterns were analyzed. In some cases, and apparently erratically, responses to trains of stimuli displayed after a phase of depression a transient rebound in amplitude and later a depression as usual. As it is not clear whether this is a true synaptic phenomenon, these traces were not included for the analysis of changes in IPSC amplitudes along the train.
For calculating the charge transferred (Q) during trains of IPSCs, the stimulus artifacts (which terminated before the foot of the IPSCs) were digitally removed and the difference in area between an ROI of the trace (e.g., high frequency stimulation [HFS]) and control (no stimulation) was calculated using Spike 2 software (CED). The values were normalized to the charge transferred by the corresponding control IPSC and expressed per unit of time. Because the level of depression varied in different neurons, for normalization to the steady-state values of IPSCs, I used the mean charge transferred by the corresponding IPSCs at steady state.
To investigate slow and frequency-dependent short-term synaptic depression (S-STD) input specificity, I combined ES and OG stimulation of PC axons. Using optical stimulation alone was not practical. First, contrary to the reliable activation of PC somas recorded from the same slices, evoking PC_DCN IPSCs by directing the spot of light toward terminals and axons over the recorded DCN soma or the neighboring white matter required higher intensity than for activating PC somas, but using too high light intensity could lead to transmission block. For this reason, the light intensity was kept submaximal throughout the OG experiments. Second, OG stimulation using repetitive stimuli at high frequencies resulted in stronger depression of PC_DCN IPSCs than using ES, as observed earlier, presumably due to channelrhodopsin inactivation (Najac and Raman, 2015). Third, block of transmission occurred sometimes after repeated OG activation at high or low frequency withoutevident alteration of the transmission evoked by electrical activationof PC axons, suggesting long-lasting channelrhodopsin inactivation. Therefore, to test input specificity, I assessed the effect of 120 s of repetitive ES on OG IPSCs and ES IPSCs, with intensities adjusted to evoke larger OG IPSCs. Assuming larger OG IPSCs resulted from a larger number of OG than ES activated synapses, a subgroup of synapses was activated exclusively (or almost exclusively) by the light pulses and, thus, spared during the 120 s of ES.
Analysis of changes in the inverse squared coefficient of variation (CV−2) of IPSCs was used as described previously (Pedroarena and Schwarz, 2003) to assess the presynaptic or postsynaptic locus of the STP and possible mechanisms. Briefly, assuming the mean amplitude of the IPSCs (M) can be explained by the product of the number of releasable quanta (N), the probability of each quanta to be released (p), and the mean postsynaptic response to one quantum (q), the CV−2, where
is the variance around M, estimates the variability of synaptic events (Bekkers and Stevens, 1990; Malinow and Tsien, 1990). Plots of CV−2 versus the corresponding M values (as ratio of controls) are useful to differentiate presynaptic from postsynaptic mechanisms of synaptic plasticity because the CV−2 is mostly insensitive to q. Furthermore, as CV−2, in contrast to M, is differentially affected by changes in N or p, this method provides an indication of possible presynaptic basis of the changes in M, as confirmed at PC_DCNs previously (Pedroarena and Schwarz, 2003). For illustration, Figure 5C (inset) shows the expected CV−2 versus M values as calculated from changes in N, p, or q. At least 100 IPSCs obtained under steady-state conditions (Control, 0.1 or 0.05 Hz, or after IPSC depression stabilized using 10, 29.5, or 67 Hz of background frequency [BF]) were used for this analysis.
Programmable software (Spike 2, CED; Igor, Wavemetrics; and Sigma Plot SPSS, IBM) was used for data processing and to carry statistical analysis. Data were tested for normality (using the Shapiro-Wilcoxon test) and equal variance. For populations that passed or failed the normality and equal variance tests, parametric (Student two tailed t tests, unpaired or paired according to the conditions) or nonparametric tests (Mann–Whitney rank sum test or Wilcoxon signed rank sum tests) were used, respectively. Data are presented as mean ± SEM, unless otherwise noted. Sample sizes were not predetermined but are reported together with the type of test used and the corresponding p values.
Simulations
To simulate synaptic transmission and STP at PC_DCN synapses as a starting point, I used a version of a previously published model, which has been shown to correctly predict the synaptic output to repetitive activation of PC_DCN synapses with second long trains (Turecek et al., 2016, 2017). Specifically, two pools of ready to be released vesicles (RRPs), differing in release probability, recovery rate, and presence or not of facilitation of release, were required by this model to explain the IPSCs initial fast depressing phase, the sustained release during hundreds of events, and the frequency invariance observed experimentally. As noted previously, the two different pools could correspond to two different states of a single pool (Neher and Sakaba, 2008); and in this scenario, facilitation could, at least in part, result from expedited transition to the mature state (Pan and Zucker, 2009; Miki et al., 2016). However, here for simplicity and comparison with previous results at this synapse, I adopted the parallel model. The present version is referred here as the D + F model. Briefly, the initial conditions are determined by the number of ready to be released vesicles in each pool (RRPA and RRPB), and the intrinsic probability of each vesicle class to be released (PrA, PrB), plus the level of total facilitation for the Pool B (F1 and F2). The total number of vesicles released (P) in response to a stimulus (Sp) is the linear sum of Pools A and B released vesicles (PA and PB, respectively), and it is assumed that the postsynaptic response to neurotransmitter is identical for both pools. The total number of vesicles released from each pool with each stimulus is given by the product of the corresponding RRP and Pr or Pr plus the corresponding level of facilitation (F). Therefore,
Although the precise mechanism(s) underlying synaptic facilitation (and probability of release) is not yet completely understood (Valera et al., 2012; Turecek et al., 2017; Chang et al., 2018), here following others, facilitation of release was simulated by an increase in the release probability, in particular, an increase in PrB. The total amount of facilitation after each stimulus is given by the addition of a constant amount (f) added right after each stimulus plus the remaining facilitation, which decays to the initial condition with time constant Tauf during the interval between stimuli. Fp+ is as follows:
where Int is the time interval between the present stimulus and the preceding one. f is added right after each stimulus; thus, the amount of facilitation added to PrB at the moment of the stimulus p is = (Fp+ – f).
Evidence from previous studies using mice younger than P21 suggested that a different rapid and short-lasting facilitation could occur at these synapses (Pedroarena and Schwarz, 2003). Thus, two facilitation factors (F1 and F2) were included following the same scheme, but one of them was constrained to have Tauf < 10–15 ms. The total facilitation added to PrB is the sum of these two factors. However, fits to the model did not show an important contribution of the short-lasting facilitation component under the conditions here explored.
During the interval between stimuli, the RRP of each vesicle pool recovers to the initial condition following a time constant (TauRA or TauRB). Therefore, the number of available vesicles at Stimulusp for each pool is given by the following:
Accordingly, depression in this model results from depletion of RRPs, when release exceeds the number of vesicles recovered, and therefore is larger, the shorter the interval between stimuli. The simulations and fits were conducted using IgorPro (Wavemetrics). The parameters were repeatedly adjusted by fitting the model to the normalized peak amplitude of averaged PC_DCN IPSCs evoked by stimulation trains of 100 events delivered at different frequencies (10-200 Hz). The set of parameters that best fit all conditions was used to further simulate the responses to protracted stimulation (RRPA0 = 7, RRPB0 = 25, PrA = 0.098, PrB = 0.017, TaurA = 12, TaurB = 0.5, f1 = 0.0005, TauF1 = 0.007, f2 = 0.001, Tauf2 = 0.1).
Because the D + F model, with or without facilitation, did not explain the slow depression observed with protracted stimulation, it was extended to include a slow-depression component. To this end, I simulated an activity-dependent decrease in the number of “active release sites” according to findings of this study and previous postulates (see Discussion) (Stevens and Wesseling, 1999; Neher, 2010). Because the pool of vesicles A is largely depleted after 100 events, I simulated an activity-dependent decrease in release sites of the Pool B alone. For this purpose, the new model (referred as the SD_RS model) features a number of release sites B (R_SitesB) that initially is identical to RRPB with all release sites occupied by vesicles. After use, some of these sites become unavailable to be refilled, and availability is restored with a time constant TauRSr. The remaining release sites can be either empty or filled, according to the rate of depletion of Pool B. Importantly, the processes affecting remaining release sites are not affected using this implementation. Therefore, although the mechanism of synaptotagmin-7-dependent PC_DCN frequency-invariant transmission may not be completely elucidated (C. Chen et al., 2017), the use of balanced depression and facilitation to simulate frequency invariance is justified. A previous study based on the presence of a PC_DCN tonic current associated to frequency-invariant depression evoked by repetitive PC_DCN stimulation in recordings from 10 to 20 µm DCNs carried at 31°C and obtained in slices from young mice postulated pooling and saturation of postsynaptic receptors as a possible mechanisms for frequency independence (Telgkamp et al., 2004). This idea seems unlikely at PC synapses of large DCNs from mature animals at close to physiological temperatures, as used in the present study, because application of TPMPA, a low-affinity competitive GABA-A antagonist (which relieves postsynaptic receptor saturation and pooling) does not prevent the developmentally dependent frequency invariance of PC_DCN IPSCs (Turecek et al., 2016). Moreover, under the here used conditions and type of synapses, I (and others) did not find significant tonic currents (Najac and Raman, 2015; Turecek et al., 2016).
To avoid making assumption about the molecular nature of the activity-dependent mechanism that leads to decrease in “active release sites,” and following empirical calculations, the amount of release sites inactivated per time unit (NdecRS) was calculated as follows:
where ARSB is the maximal number of release sites that can become unavailable, F_RS is a parameter determining the frequency dependence of this process. The inverse of the preceding interval was used here to calculate Freq; thus, we assume that the local level of activity determines this parameter. Alternatively, the averaged stimulating frequency over × time preceding each stimulus could be used to simulate a process depending on global level of activity; but under the conditions here tested, good fits were found using instantaneous frequency. The number of release sites inactivated per stimulus is the value of NdecRS calculated at each stimulus time point, scaled by the preceding time interval.
The unavailable release sites recover following:
Where RSrp represents the number of “active release sites” of Pool B recovered during the preceding interval, is the initial number of active release sites,
is the number of active release sites available at the precedent event, and TauRSr is the time constant of release sites recovery.
Therefore, the number of “active release sites” available at the time of each stimulus is as follows:
In the SD_RS model, R_SitesBp substitutes for RRP0 in Equation 3 for Pool B.
To estimate the new parameters corresponding to the SD_RS model, the normalized peak amplitude of averaged PC_DCN IPSCs evoked by stimulation trains of 120 s delivered at different frequencies was used to fit the model in Igor. The set of parameters that best fit all conditions was used to evaluate the response of the model to transient changes in stimulating frequency from steady state (ARSB = 0.47, F_RS = 29; TauRSr = 30). Additionally, I tested the ability of the model to explain the responses from different sets of experiments using a different stimulation paradigm (i.e., a sustained change in stimulating frequency in the same file) (Figs. 7 and 8).
Results
S-STD of PC_DCN IPSCs
To explore the response of PC_DCN synapses to sustained activity, I used cerebellar slices from juvenile (P21–P32) or adult (P55–P72) mice maintained under conditions like those in situ(i.e., temperature: 36.5 ± 0.5°C and extracellular CaCl2 concentration: 1.5 mm), as variations in these parameters and the developmental stage affect synaptic transmission and STP (Borst, 2010). Whole-cell patch-clamp or cell-attached recordings were obtained from large DCNs (>20 μm) in the lateral or the interpositus nuclei because this group of DCNs can be unambiguously identified as projecting, putative glutamatergic DCNs, while smaller DCNs may be part of different subgroups, potentially displaying different synaptic properties (Uusisaari et al., 2007; Uusisaari and Knopfel, 2008; Najac and Raman, 2015). To mimic the persistent discharge of PCs, I used protracted ES of PC axons in the surrounding white matter (≥2 min) at three different stimulating background frequencies (BF: 10, 29.5, and 67 Hz), which are within the PCs firing rate range in awake animals at rest (Thach, 1968; Zhou et al., 2014).
The responses to 2-min-long trains of stimuli confirmed an initial phase of rapid decrease in phasic IPSC peak amplitude with successive events, which slowed down and leveled after tens of events (Telgkamp and Raman, 2002; Pedroarena and Schwarz, 2003; Turecek et al., 2016) (representative traces from one neuron in Fig. 1A, and mean normalized peak amplitude as a function of time across neurons in Fig. 1B). However, the use of protracted stimulation unveiled a different slow phase of decay in IPSC peak amplitude ensuing the fast one, with time constants ranging from 18 to 47 s, most evident by analyzing the results in a compressed timescale (Fig. 1B, insets, fits using double exponential decay functions). Furthermore, the average IPSC amplitude at the end of the 2 min stimulation periods decreased with increasing stimulating BFs, suggesting divergence from the frequency-invariant phase described before at earlier stages of PC activation trains (Turecek et al., 2016) (Fig. 1B,C). Importantly, the population results reflected the results from single experiments, depicted individually in Figure 1D. Although the magnitude of depression varied among neurons, for each case, the normalized steady-state synaptic strength decreased with increasing BFs (mean values: 0.4 ± 0.02 [10 Hz], vs 0.26 ± 0.02 [29.5 Hz], n = 8, paired t test, p < 0.001; 0.26 ± 0.02 [29.5 Hz] vs 0.15 ± 0.015 [67 Hz], n = 9, paired t test, p < 0.001). Therefore, protracted activation of PC_DCNs revealed an S-STD, which at steady state appears as frequency-dependent. Because PCs are continuously active, the differences in mean IPSC amplitudes for different BFs (more than double comparing 10 vs 67 Hz) are likely functionally relevant.
Protracted PC stimulation unveils an S-STD. A, Top, Sketch of experimental configuration and stimulation paradigm. Bottom, Typical examples of IPSCs from the same neuron evoked by 2 min trains of stimuli applied to PC axons at the frequencies (BF) indicated on the left, at the beginning (left), and at the end of stimulation (right). Note the different time scales. B, Top to bottom, Mean normalized IPSC peak amplitude as a function of time for BFs 10 Hz (light blue, n = 11), 29.5 Hz (red, n = 9), and 67 Hz (dark blue, n = 9), and all three plots overlaid (symbols represent mean ± SEM) illustrate two phases of decay. Color code applies to all figures, unless otherwise stated. The double exponential decay fits in the insets: fast tau: 0.81 ± 0.04, 0.41 ± 0.012, and 0.33 ± 0.007 s and slow tau: 23 ± 1.9, 18 ± 0.5, and 47 ± 1.2 s for 10, 29.5, and 67 Hz BFs, respectively. C, Average normalized steady-state phasic IPSCs amplitude (IPSCSS) versus BF (averages calculated over the last 100 events of the data plotted in B; 0.37 ± 0.002, 0.25 ± 0.002, and 0.15 ± 0.002 for 10, 29.5, and 67 Hz BFs, respectively: 10 versus 29.5 Hz [Mann–Whitney rank sum test] and 29.5 vs 67 Hz [t test], p < 0.001 for both comparisons). D, Results from individual neurons illustrate decreasing mean IPSCSS with increasing BF (averages over 100 events). Top, IPSC amplitude for 29.5 versus 10 Hz. Bottom, IPSC amplitude for 67 versus 29.5 Hz. All points fall below the unity line. E, Typical examples from the same neuron in A of IPSCs evoked by the same ordinal stimuli but at different BFs (indicated on the left). The 10 Hz trace is the same shown in A for comparison. F, Summary of IPSC amplitude as in B, but as a function of the event number (color labeled as B). Top, Events 1-50. Bottom, Events 1-1200. G, Averages as in C, calculated over the events indicated on top of each bar plot. Top, Events 10-50: 0.49 ± 0.01, 0.47 ± 0.02, and 0.47 ± 0.03, for 10, 29.5, and 67 Hz, respectively; Wilcoxon signed rank sum test: 10 versus 29.5 Hz, p = 0.115; 29.5 versus 67 Hz, p = 0.7. Bottom, Events 1000-1100: 0.37 ± 0.002, 0.276 ± 0.002, and 0.236 ± 0.002, for 10, 29.5, and 67 Hz, respectively, t test, 10 versus 29.5 and 29.5 versus 67 Hz, p < 0.001 for both. H, Similar as in D, but for mean IPSCs amplitudes calculated over stimuli 1000-1100th for each neuron. Top, 29.5 Hz versus 10 Hz. Bottom, 67 versus 29.5 Hz. Data are mean ± SEM. **p < 0.001.
To confirm S-STD frequency dependence and to explore its buildup along the stimulating trains, I analyzed the changes in synaptic strength as a function of the event number (Fig. 1E–H). In agreement with previous studies using similar conditions as here, the IPSC amplitudes initially, over the first 10 events, depressed significantly but on average with slightly lower rates with increasing BFs (Fig. 1F, top), while later, after tens of events, the IPSCs evoked with different BFs displayed similar amplitudes (frequency-invariant phase; Fig. 1F,G, top plots) (Turecek et al., 2016). However, after hundreds of events, the IPSC amplitudes started to diverge, indicating dependence to the time interval between stimuli (Fig. 1F, bottom). The IPSCs further decayed, but at different rates for different BFs. The time constants of decay calculated after the first 100 events using single exponential fits were 216 ± 24, 612 ± 21, and 3305 ± 83 events for 10, 29.5, and 67 Hz BF, respectively. This is more than one order of magnitude slower than previously described forms of depression at PC_DCNs (Telgkamp and Raman, 2002; Pedroarena and Schwarz, 2003; Turecek et al., 2016), including a “slow” frequency-dependent depression (i.e., with time constant of 117 events for 100 Hz stimulus trains) found in smaller DCNs (15–20 µm) from younger animals (P13–P15), recorded at lower temperatures (31°C) (Telgkamp and Raman, 2002). Significant differences in average IPSC amplitudes as a function of the BFs could be detected considering events 1000-1100 or further (Fig. 1G, bottom). Moreover, the results from single experiments agreed with the population ones (Fig. 1H): in all but 1 case, the mean IPSCs amplitude decreased with increasing BFs (0.402 ± 0.02 vs 0.29 ± 0.02, n = 8, paired t test, p < 0.001 for 10 vs 29.5 Hz; and 0.29 ± 0.02 vs 0.24 ± 0.02, for 29.5 Hz vs 67 Hz, n = 9, paired t test, p = 0.003).
Similar results arose using Poisson-like trains of stimuli with mean frequencies 10 or 70 Hz (Fig. 2A), ruling out that the alternation of regular and irregular periods of firing in awake animals (Shin et al., 2007; Hong et al., 2016) could result in recovery from depression during long intervals abolishing the frequency differences in synaptic strength found here. Importantly, despite developmentally regulated PC_DCN STP (Turecek et al., 2016, 2017), I found similar late phase of slow frequency-dependent decay in IPSC amplitude along trains using slices from adult mice (P55–P72), albeit with shallower levels of depression at steady state than at younger ages (Fig. 2B). In addition, similar to previous studies using slices from mature animals (>P20), recordings from large DCNs, and close to physiological temperatures (Najac and Raman, 2015; Turecek et al., 2016) (33°C–34°C and 34°C–35°C, respectively), PC_DCN activation did not evoke prominent slow tonic currents, in contrast to recordings using smaller DCNs (15–20 µm), from younger mice (P13–P15), and lower recording temperatures (31°C) (Telgkamp and Raman, 2002). Type of cell, developmental stage, and temperature all influence PC-DCN kinetics and could explain differences in slow currents (Linnemann et al., 2004; Person and Raman, 2012; Najac and Raman, 2015; Turecek et al., 2016).
S-STD frequency dependence under irregular activation patterns and in DCNs from adult mice. A, Top, Representative traces of IPSCs evoked in the same neuron by 2 min Poisson-like stimulation at the mean frequencies indicated on the left. Bottom, Summary plots of mean IPSCs amplitudes calculated over the stimuli indicated on top of each graph as in Figure 1, but for Poisson-like stimulation at 10 Hz (n = 9) and 70 Hz (n = 7). Significance assessed using Mann–Whitney rank sum test (0.33 ± 0.002, 0.23 ± 0.003 for 10 and 70 Hz, for events 1000-1100th; and 0.33 ± 0.002, 0.15 ± 0.003 for 10 and 70 Hz, respectively, for steady-state IPSCs, p < 0.001). B, Left, Normalized average amplitude as a function of event number of PC_DCN IPSC recordings obtained from slices from P55-P72 animals (BF 10 Hz, n = 8, BF 67 Hz, n = 7). Right, Summary plots of mean IPSC amplitudes calculated from the graphs on the left, over the events indicated on top of each bar plot (1000-1100th for 10 and 67 Hz BFs: 0.45 ± 0.002 and 0.32 ± 0.002, t test, p < 0.001; average of 100 events at steady state: 0.45 ± 0.002 and 0.26 ± 0.003 for 10 and 67 Hz, respectively, t test, p < 0.001). **p < 0.001.
Together, these results strongly argue that regular or irregular sustained activity of young or adult PC_DCNs induces a previously undetected slow form of frequency-dependent STD (S-STD), requiring hundreds to thousands of events to reach steady-state level.
Because of the acknowledged difficulties in carrying large DCN recordings using slices from older animals (Uusisaari et al., 2007; Turecek et al., 2016) and the need of particularly long recordings for the present study, the rest of the experiments were performed using slices from juvenile mice (> P20) as in other studies (Turecek et al., 2016, 2017).
Sustained PC activation modulates IPSCs and DCN spike responses evoked by HFS trains
Given the typical PCs sustained activity found in awake-behaving animals (e.g., Schonewille et al., 2006), presumably S-STD continuously controls PC_DCN efficacy. This raises the question of whether and how S-STD modulates the responses to moretransient changes in PC rate associated to cerebellar controlled behaviors. To address this question experimentally, first, PC_DCN synapses were depressed to steady state using protracted stimulation at different BFs; and afterward, a brief, high frequency train of stimuli (HFS, 100 events, 180 Hz) was used to mimic behavior-driven transient signals. The HFS duration covers the length of usual discrete motor events (e.g., reaches, steps, saccades, etc.). To quantify and compare the synaptic output induced by HFSs preceded by different BFs, I measured the total charge transferred during the HFS (QHFS, see Materials and Methods), then normalized it to the charge transferred by corresponding control IPSCs (Q PCtrl) to be able to average across neurons, and expressed it per second. Analysis of changes in QHFS as a function of the preceding BF revealed a remarkable modulation (Fig. 3A). Specifically, the normalized QHFS decreased with increasing BF (no prior activity, 0 Hz: 72 ± 4.5, n = 22; BF 10 Hz: 61 ± 3.3, n = 16, 29.5 Hz: 40 ± 4.0, n = 9, and 67 Hz: 16 ± 1.9, n = 12, t test: 0 vs 10 Hz, p = 0.033, 1 tail; 10 vs 29.5 Hz; p < 0.001; 29.5 vs 67 Hz, p < 0.001). These QHFS values contrast with the expected for either, a nondepressing synapse (considering 1 the charge transfer of control IPSCs and 180 the HFS frequency: 1 × 180 = 180), or for synapses that after the first phase of depression remain stable and frequency-invariant (Fig. 1, ∼0.48 × 180 = 86). The mean QHFS for HFSs preceded by 2 min stimulation at 67 Hz was approximately one-fourth of the QHFS elicited by HFSs preceded by 10 Hz stimulation. These results strongly support the view that BFs modulates the output produced by behavior-driven PC signals.
PC background activity modulates the DCN synaptic and spiking responses to HFS trains. A, Top, Stimulation paradigm. Bottom, Mean charge transfer amplitude during HFS (100 events, 180 Hz), normalized to that of control IPSCs as a function of prior BF. B, Typical examples from two different neurons illustrating how prior BF (indicate on the left of each trace) modulates the amplitude of IPSCs evoked by the HFS (indicated by the gray box). C, Four typical examples of DCN cell-attached recordings used to investigate how BF modulates the suppression of DCN spontaneous firing rate induced by HFSs delivered after protracted stimulation at 10 Hz (left) or at 67 Hz (right). Each row corresponds to the same neuron. Vertical line indicates the occurrence of one spike. D, Scatter plot of changes in DCN firing rate induced by HFSs (HFS-Basal rate), for HFS preceded by BF 67 Hz versus HFSs preceded by BF 10 Hz. All points fall below the unity lines (diagonal), indicating stronger inhibition after BF 10 Hz. Inset, Mean values and SEM for BF 10 and 67 Hz. E, Effect of nonstationary BF stimulation on HFSs suppression of DCN rate. Top, Stimulation paradigm. Bottom, Mean HFS-induced changes in DCN firing rate as a function of the 67 Hz train duration, normalized to the responses evoked without switch to 67 Hz (top, dashed line, 100%). Bottom, Dashed line indicates the value of mean responses to HFS after 120 s at 67 Hz only. The data were fitted with a single exponential function (R2 = 0.99, n = 6). F, Same as in E, but for a switch in BF from 120 s at 67 Hz to 10 Hz (R2 = 0.94, n = 6). **p < 0.001, *p < 0.05.
To further probe the latter idea, I used cell-attached recordings of spontaneously firing DCNs and similar stimulating protocols as in Figure 3A, to investigate whether the changes in synaptic output translated to changes in DCN spiking activity (Fig. 3C,D). Large mature DCNs recorded in vitro at physiological temperatures fired spontaneously at high frequencies (mean firing rate 81.9 ± 3.5 Hz, range 48–121 Hz, n = 12), which are within the DCN firing rate range found in behaving mice at rest (e.g., Ten Brinke et al., 2017; Sarnaik and Raman, 2018). Furthermore, under cell-attached recordings, the intracellular content is not dialyzed, reflecting the conditions found in the intact animal/neuron. In a number of experiments, I used the same number of preceding stimuli (7000 stimuli at 10 or 67 Hz) to rule out that the number of stimuli (and not the stimulating frequency) determined the changes in synaptic strength. The results were similar to those using 120 s of sustained stimulation indicating that at steady state BF determined S-STD. Thus, the results were pooled together for statistical analysis. Individual and summary results show that HFSs preceded by sustained stimulation at 10 Hz completely suppressed or sharply decreased DCN firing rate, while HFSs delivered after 67 Hz only induced a moderate effect (Fig. 3C,D) (−67.7 ± 7.4 and −26.5 ± 6.6 Hz, respectively, paired t test, p < 0.001, n = 12). These results strongly suggest that the modulation induced by S-STD not only has an impact on the synaptic output but on DCN firing rate, and thus possibly on the cerebellar output in behaving animals.
Having found that S-STD modulates DCN responses to HFSs, I next explored the time course of this effect. This question is relevant because (1) it could provide information on the existence and duration of a time window for linear encoding; and (2) it could inform on the consequences of changes in PC background activity in different timescales. To explore the time course, I leveraged the above-described differential modulation of HFS spiking responses accordingto preceding BFs (Fig. 3C,D). Usingcell-attached recordings, as above, I interposed between the 2 min blocks of stimulation at 10 or 67 Hz and the HFSs, a switch in the stimulating frequency (to 67 or 10 Hz, respectively), for variable periods of time (ΔT, shown 1, 10, 50, or 100 s; Fig. 3E,F, top). The change in DCN firing rate for the 10 to 67 Hz switches (normalized to the response to HFSs preceded by 120 s at 10 Hz without switch to 67 Hz) decreased with increasing ΔT at 67 Hz, with a time constant of 22 s. The change was undetectable for 67 Hz periods of ≤1 s and, on average, only after 100 s the suppression reached the same level than after 2 min of stimulation at 67 Hz. For the switch from 67 to 10 Hz, the time constant of recovery was 14 s (i.e., the recovery from deeper depression seemed faster than the buildup), but within a similar time range, and it took also similar periods to reach a new steady-state level. Again, no changes were detected for periods of ≤1 s (Fig. 3F). The bidirectional amplitude modulation established in the course of tens of seconds highlight the time scale and flexibility of S-STD and consequent HFS response modulation. Moreover, the slow time course (from tens of seconds to minutes) contrasts with the rapid adaptation (in the millisecond range) of other forms of depression expressed earlier during the train (Telgkamp and Raman, 2002), strongly suggesting that S-STD is based on a different mechanism. The minor differences in buildup and recovery time course of S-STD could depend on differences in the activation or deactivation rates of faster synaptic forms of plasticity and/or S-STD itself.
Importantly, these results revealed a time window up to few seconds during which changes in BF did not significantly alter the response to subsequent HFS; in other words, the PC_DCN synaptic gain seemed unchanged over this period. Remarkably, this is the range of durations of common motor events controlled by cerebellum. If S-STD guaranteed stable synaptic gain during this time window for all functionally relevant PC rates, then S-STD could support linear encoding of behavior-related PC signals while still being able to adapt synaptic strength to the background activity.
Linear input/output transfer function for 0.5-s-long PC test trains with gain dependent on PC BF
To test the above-mentioned idea, I used 500 ms test trains of stimuli, with frequencies ranging between 10 and 300 Hz to match the usual PC signals related to common movements, preceded by protracted PC activation with different BFs (Fig. 4A, top). To estimate the synaptic output evoked by the test trains, I measured the charge transfer amplitude as in Figure 3A, B (Q test, normalized to Q PCtrl, and expressed per time unit; for further details, see Materials and Methods), and plotted it against the corresponding test train frequency for each explored BF (Fig. 4A). Indeed, in favor of the idea that S-STD provides a time window for linear encoding, the Q test values were proportional to the corresponding test train frequency, such that the relationship between these two parameters was approximately linear over the physiological range of PC rates (10-200 Hz), with slopes or gains decreasing for increasing BFs. In contrast, the transfer function corresponding to test trains applied from rest displayed a saturating type of curve characteristic of depressing synapses (0 Hz, black empty circles). The dashed unity line indicates the expected values for a nondepressing/nonfacilitating synapse. These results strongly support the view that S-STD provides a time window of hundreds of milliseconds to seconds over which PC rates can be linearly encoded in the synaptic output.
Linear PC_DCN transfer function of subsecond changes in PC rate with gain dependent on preceding BF. A, Top, Stimulation paradigm to investigate changes in PC_DCN input/output gain: 0.5 s test trains of stimuli at different frequencies (from 10 to 260 Hz) delivered after 120 s of stimulation at different BFs (0, 10, 29.5, or 67 Hz). Bottom, Summary of measured PC_DCN synaptic charge transfer during the test trains (Q TEST), normalized to the charge transfer of corresponding control IPSCs (Q PCtrl), expressed per time unit, as a function of the test frequency. The plots corresponding to test trains applied from rest (0 Hz), or after 120 s at 10, 29.5, or 67 Hz are labeled on the right of each curve (color code as before). Note the almost linear relationship between 10 and 180 Hz and the different slopes for different BFs, indicative of different gains (gain of linear fits from 10 to 200 Hz: 0.34 ± 0.002, 0.23 ± 0.001, and 0.085 ± 0.004 for 10, 29.5, and 67 Hz, respectively). Dashed black line (unity line) indicates the expected values for nondepressing/nonfacilitating PSCs. B, Plots of changes in DCN firing rate (difference to steady state rate) as a function of the test train frequency from experiments using the same stimulation paradigm as in A, and DCN cell-attached recordings. Top, Two typical examples exhibit approximately linear relationships (lines correspond to linear fits to the data). Left, bottom, One unit exhibited nonlinear relationship after 10 Hz BF (points linked by straight lines). Right, bottom, Average changes in DCN firing rate from neurons with linear relationship (n = 6, lines correspond to linear fits; gain: −0.34 ± 0.005 and −0.22 ± 0.012 Hz/Hz and R2 = 0.999 and 0.997 for 10 and 67 Hz BF, respectively). C, Scatter plots of gains (estimated by the slopes of linear fits as in B) for BF 67 Hz versus their corresponding values for BF 10 Hz (n = 6) for single neurons (Wilcoxon sign rank test, p = 0.031, n = 6; diagonal is the unity line). D, Same data from A, but normalized to the charge transferred by the corresponding IPSCs at steady state (Q PSS). The responses to test stimuli applied from rest, 0 Hz, in empty black. The plots corresponding to different BFs overlap around the unity line indicating that the synaptic strength at steady state determines the BF-dependent gains (i.e., the slopes shown in A). Linear fits for the test frequency range 10-200 Hz (for clarity not shown). For BF 10 Hz: slope 1.0 ± 0.04, R2 = 0.99; BF 29.5 Hz: slope 1.1 ± 0.06, R2 = 0.99; BF 67 Hz: slope 0.9 ± 0.03, R2 = 0.99. Dashed line indicates unity line. Inset, same plot as in A overlaid to colored dashed lines indicating the result of multiplying the corresponding mean Q Pss by the test train frequency, for each BF. E, Average charge transfer amplitude for IPSCs at steady state normalized to control IPSCs (Q Pss) as a function of the BF (n = 25). Curve indicates the fit with a rational function (n = 25, R2 = 0.999). Inset, Averaged measured charge transfer amplitude at steady state per time unit, normalized to control IPSCs (Q ss) versus the BF (note different scale). F, Same as in D, but for the first 100 ms of the responses to the 500 ms test trains show gain > 1.
To assess the functional relevance of these results, a different series of experiments using DCN cell-attached recordings tested whether similar stimulation patterns as in Figure 4A induced equivalent changes in DCN firing rates (Fig. 4B,C). Although synchronous PC inputs are expected to be less effective than desynchronized ones in changing DCN firing rates (Sarnaik and Raman, 2018), 6 of 7 recorded DCNs (average basal rates preceding stimulation 83.7 ± 4.8 Hz, range 60–121 Hz, n = 7) displayed linear decreases in firing rates with increasing test frequencies (for examples, see Fig. 4B, top). Moreover, the gain of these relationships, that is, the decrease in DCN firing rate per increase in PC test frequency was in each case higher for test trains preceded by 10 Hz BF compared with those applied after 67 Hz (Fig. 4A, bottom right, C). Thus, these results strongly suggest that S-STD is able to support scaled linear encoding of PC rates in the DCN spike output, over behavior-relevant time windows. These results do not rule out that under different conditions (i.e., responsive state of DCNs, presence of other inputs, or other PC input patterns), DCNs could respond with other spike outputs, a matter for further studies but indicate that DCN spike output suppression can faithfully reflect PCs inhibitory drive, as suggested previously (Pedroarena, 2010).
The finding that the gain of PC_DCN transfer function depends on the preceding BF opens two types of questions: (1) about the origin and (2) about the consequences of BF-dependent gains. Regarding the first, different gains could derive simply from the differences in synaptic strength caused by S-STD. Alternatively or additionally, if S-STD involved modifications in fast forms of STP (e.g., decreased facilitation), different balance of fast facilitation/depression in response to the test trains could contribute to the different gains. To elucidate this issue, I renormalized the data in Figure 4A to the charge transferred by the corresponding IPSCs at steady state (Q Pss; Fig. 4D). After this transformation, over the range of 10-200 Hz, the gain of the curves corresponding to different BFs collapsed, coinciding with the unity line, consistent with the notion that the different gains were exclusively dependent on the preceding level of synaptic strength. This is further illustrated in the inset, where the experimentally obtained Q test values (plotted as in Fig. 4A) are similar to the values calculated by multiplying the average Q Pss for different BFs (see later), and the test train frequencies (dashed lines, color code as before). Together with recent reports of linear transfer function of PC_DCN synapses after tens of events, when S-STD is not yet expressed (Turecek et al., 2016, 2017), this outcome strongly suggests that S-STD expression did not interfere with the mechanism(s) responsible for linear encoding, which could be the equilibrated fast facilitation and depression.
Regarding the question of the consequences of BF-dependent gain for PC-DCN function, it is important to note, first, that because the synaptic gain at steady state determines the gain for the test train periods, the gains are the same when considering changes from steady state and, thus, effective under the typical incessant PC activity. Inspection of the results in Figure 4A reveals one possible consequence of BF-dependent gain. A jump to 180 Hz from BF 10 Hz resulted in a change in synaptic output of 57 charge units, in contrast to the 9.1 units produced from BF 67 Hz. Thus, changes in background rate could represent a way for tuning the responses of different PCs to stereotyped signals (e.g., complex spikes). Further inspection of the same results illustrates another possible consequence. It has been previously shown that for many fast depressing synapses, changes in gain determine a Weber-Fechner effect (i.e., independently of the BF), equal fractional changes in frequency (ΔF/BF) produce equal absolute outputs, despite the difference in absolute presynaptic rate change (Abbott et al., 1997). Figure 4A shows that here, similarly, a change from BF 10–29.5 Hz (∼20 Hz, 200% of change) produced a change in output of 6.2 charge units, which is close to the 9.1 units resulting from changing BF 67 to 180 Hz (113 Hz, but ∼170% of change). For many fast depressing synapses, synaptic strength at steady state becomes proportional to 1/BF with increasing BFs (Abbott et al., 1997; Tsodyks and Markram, 1997), such that the response to a change in frequency becomes transiently proportional to ΔF/BF, explaining the Weber-Fechner effect. For PC_DCNs, the charge transfer produced by single IPSCs at steady state (Q Pss) plotted as a function of the BF shows Q Pss decreasing nonlinearly in proportion to 1/BF with increasing BFs (Fig. 4E, black line: a rational function fit). This finding suggests S-STD, too, could support a Weber-Fechner effect, but importantly, for time windows in the timescale of usual movements. Indeed, calculated from the data in Figure 4A, the changes in synaptic charge transfer as a function of ΔF/BF were similar for BFs > 10 Hz (6.69 ± 0.05 and 5.69 ± 0.26 charge units, for BF 29.5 and 67 Hz, respectively). Thus, S-STD could support similar impact on target DCNs from PCs with different BFs.
A different consequence of the BF-dependent gain is that, as indicated in the inset of Figure 4E, the charge transferred at steady state per time unit (Q SS) increases nonlinearly withthe BF (compare with Fig. 4A), signaling low sensitivity of PC_DCNs to BFs. This property could be useful to prevent silencing of DCNs with increasing BFs and in general to maintain DCNs within their working range.
Overall, these results indicate the following: (1) the BF-dependent gain modulation reduces the sensitivity to steady PC rates; (2) PCs independently of their BF would produce equal outputs to equal fractional changes in rate; and (3) changes in PC BF could be used to tune the response to stereotyped signals (e.g., complex spikes).
Finally, the observation that changes in PC stimulation frequency often produced relative transient facilitation of IPSCs(e.g., Fig. 3B) (Pedroarena and Schwarz, 2003; Turecek et al., 2016, 2017), and that some cerebellar signals are short-lasting, raised the question of whether the linear transfer holds for signals shorter than the test trains used here. I analyzed the charge transferred during the first 100 ms of the test trains when relative facilitation is predominant (Fig. 4F; Q, test normalized to Q Pss as in Fig. 4D). The transfer function for the 100 ms periods was approximately linear within the 10–200 Hz range but with slopes slightly >1, suggesting linear and preferential transfer of brief signals or the onset of longer ones. In addition, for the higher test frequencies (260 Hz), the plots deviated from linearity depending on the preceding BF: after 10 or 29.5 Hz, the responses were often supralinear, although variable (note the high SEM). The variability is in line with the idea that similar type of synapses differ mainly by the degree of facilitation (Fekete et al., 2019), but other mechanisms cannot be ruled out and should be addressed in further studies. These results unveil a new role of facilitation at PC_DCN synapses in promoting the transfer of short signals, in addition to its possible role in linear encoding.
Together, these findings strongly suggest that, in the timescales of usual motor behaviors, S-STD enables scaled linear encoding of PC rates in both the synaptic output and the DCN rates while producing adaptation at longer timescales.
Evidence that S-STD mechanism is input-specific, presynaptic, and independent of fast forms of plasticity
To understand the basis of the apparently opposing S-STD effects at different timescales, first, I set out to explore the S-STD mechanism. S-STD input specificity was investigated by combining OG and ES in slices from mice expressing channelrhodopsin in PCs under the L7 promoter (L7-ChR2-eYFP; for further details, see Materials and Methods) (Chaumont et al., 2013). I adjusted the stimulation intensities to obtain OG IPSCs larger than the ES IPSCs under control conditions and assumed this reflects a larger number of OG activated synapses. If depression was input-specific, tonic ES should depress transmission at ES-activated synapses but spare those exclusively (or mainly) activated by light. Indeed, after 2 min of repetitive ES at 67 Hz, the depression of OG IPSCs was significantly less than for ES IPSCs, supporting the idea that depression is input-specific (Fig. 5A,B). The much lower depression of OG IPSCs suggested that the OG and ES stimuli were largely nonoverlapping. This is likely, since OG activation was submaximal (see Materials and Methods). The possibility that OG activation modified synaptic function(e.g., by inducing prolonged depolarization of terminals and branches or calcium influx promoting alternative release pathways) could not be completely ruled out. However, in one experiment where the degree of occlusion of OG and ES IPSCs was monitored, the degree of depression of the OG IPSC coincided with the expected depression of the overlapping component (not shown), supporting the idea that the results reflectedS-STD input specificity.
Exploration of S-STD mechanism. A, Combined OG and ES to explore input specificity. Top, Experimental paradigm. Bottom, Representative examples of OG IPSCs and ES IPSCs before (control, black) and after (test, red) 2 min ES at 67 Hz. B, Plot showing significant differences in average OG and ES IPSC amplitudes after 2 min ES at 67 Hz (normalized to the corresponding controls: IPSCs induced after 2 min without ES; 0.81 ± 0.078 and 0.21 ± 0.03 for OG and ES IPSCs, respectively, paired t test, p < 0.001, n = 8) suggests input specificity. C, Plot of CV−2 versus their corresponding mean amplitudes (M) for IPSCs depressed to steady state steady using 120 s stimulation at 10, 29.5, or 67 Hz, normalized to the values of their corresponding control IPSCs (n = 8). Inset, Expected CV−2 versus M values for changes in the parameters N (red line), p (green), or q (blue) of a binomial release model. D, Same as in C, but for IPSCs depressed using BF 29.5 or 67 Hz normalized the corresponding BF 10 Hz values (n = 7). Dashed line indicates unity line. Fit of data using a linear function (R2 = 0.78, slope 1.07 ± 0.13, n = 7; data not shown). E, Left, Plot of mean IPSC peak amplitude normalized to control IPSCs as a function of the HFS event number for HFSs (100 events, 180 Hz) applied from rest (black, n = 17) or after 120 s of stimulation at 10 Hz (light blue, n = 11), 29.5 Hz (red, n = 9), and 67 Hz (dark blue, n = 9). Continuous lines indicate exponential fits to the curves. Note the faster decay for the HFS applied from rest but similar slower values for the other curves, suggesting similar probability of release after the first events. Right, Plot of IPSC peak amplitudes normalized to the value of the first IPSC of the HFS, illustrates relative facilitation during the first events for BFs 10 and 30 Hz, but similar decay rate and percentage of decay for different BFs afterward. F, Left, Same as in E (left), but for HFSs applied after 50 events at 10 or 67 Hz (color code as in E). Note the relative facilitation of the first events for BF 10 Hz, but similar degree of depression at the end of the HFS for 10 and 67 Hz BF. Continuous lines indicate exponential fits. The data correspond to a different set of neurons than those shown in E. Right, Same data as on the left, but normalized to the corresponding first IPSC of the HFS. Bottom, Plots represent superimposed mean IPSC amplitude normalized to the first HFS event for HFSs delivered after 120 s of stimulation (red circles) or 50 events (empty circles) after BF 10 Hz (left) or 67 Hz (right) as a function of the HFS event number. The similarity in the IPSC amplitude time course after 120 s or 50 events suggests that S-STD did not alter the relative proportion of fast synaptic facilitation and depression evoked by the HFSs. **p < 0.001.
Another important conclusion drawn from the small changes induced by 120 s ES in OG IPSCs compared with ES IPSCs is that changes in chloride gradient is unlikely the cause for S-STD (as OG IPSCs recorded from the same DCNs would have been expected to change if this were the case).
To gain further insight into the basis of S-STD, I investigated changes in the inverse squared CV−2 of IPSCs, an index of synaptic variability useful to clarify the mechanism underlying changes in synaptic efficacy (Bekkers and Stevens, 1990; Malinow and Tsien, 1990) (for details, see Materials and Methods). Briefly, specific changes in CV−2 versus the respective changes in M are expected from changes in each one of the factors assumed to explain the mean IPSC amplitude (M = N.p.q), that is, the number of presynaptic releasable vesicles (N), the probability of each one to be released (p), and the postsynaptic response to one vesicle (q) (for guidance, see Fig. 5C, inset). Figure 5C illustrates how the CV−2 of PC_DCNs IPSCs depressed to steady state (normalized to control IPSCs) decreased more than the corresponding means. This result is compatible (1) with a presynaptic locus because changes in q would have caused no change in CV−2; and (2) with a reduction in p, because changes in N would have caused proportional changes in the CV−2 and M (see inset). Next, to explore the specific basis of S-STD, I compared IPSCs depressed with steady state using different BFs (Fig. 5D). The CV−2 of IPSCs evoked by BF 29.5 or 67 Hz normalized to the values corresponding to BF 10 Hz decreased proportionally to their means, a result consistent with a presynaptic mechanism and particularly with a change in the number of available quanta (N). Overall, this analysis suggests that repetitive stimulation from rest induces first a decrease in the probability of release and that S-STD depends on a presynaptic mechanism, specifically, a decrease in the available quanta (N). Moreover, the present results are incompatible with S-STD being dependent on changes in q, for example, due to changes in the postsynaptic chloride gradient by sustained stimulation or in the amount of NT contained in each quantum (Fig. 5C, inset, blue line).
To verify the CV−2 analysis outcome, an alternative estimation of the release probability was obtained. The rate with which the peak amplitude of synaptic responses decay during a train is in part dependent on the release probability, as larger use of the available quanta would deplete faster the remaining pool (Buchs and Senn, 2002). Because S-STD adjusts slowly (Fig. 3), one would expect persistent changes in the release probability if this were the basis for S-STD. Thus, I analyzed the rate of decay of peak amplitudes of IPSCs evoked by HFSs (180 Hz) applied from rest or after different periods of protracted PC stimulation. For HFSs delivered after 120 s at different BFs, the IPSC peak amplitudes decayed to different steady-state levels depending of the preceding BF (Fig. 5E), in agreement with the idea that S-STD adjust slowly to new levels (see later for details). The rate of decay for HFSs preceded by 120 s stimulation at different BFs was similar but slower than for HFSs applied from rest (Fig. 5E; exponential fits to the data, tau: 18.6 ± 0.53, 27.9 ± 3.1, 28.7 ± 3.6, and 27.7 ± 5.1 for rest [n = 34], and BFs 10 [n = 11], 29.5 [n = 10], and 67 [n = 9] Hz, respectively). This is consistent with a decrease in release probability at the beginning of the train but not between different S-STD levels of depression. Moreover, normalizing the amplitudes to the first IPSC of the HFS more clearly illustrates the similarity in decay rate and proportion of decay for HFSs preceded by different BFs (Fig. 5E, right). In contrast, in a different set of experiments using HFSs applied after 50 events at 10 or 67 Hz, the IPSC peak amplitude decayed to similar steady-state levels (Fig. 5F; IPSC amplitude averaged over the last 30 events, normalized to control IPSC: 0.247 ± 0.079 and 0.23 ± 0.07 for 10 or 67 Hz, respectively, paired t test, n = 5, p = 0.162). These results demonstrate that the synaptic strength can be rapidly adjusted to new levels before S_STD expression (Telgkamp and Raman, 2002) (compare with Fig. 5E, mean IPSC peak amplitude over the last HFS 30 events after 2 min stimulation: 0.185 ± 0.25 vs 0.067 ± 0.09 for 10 or 67 Hz BF, respectively, n = 11 and n = 9, Mann–Whitney rank sum test, p = 0.001). The rates of decay for HFS after 50 events at different BFs were similar (tau: 27.8 ± 3 and 31.2 ± 5.1 events for BF 10 [n = 5] and 67 [n = 5] Hz, respectively), and similar to rates of decay for HFS delivered after 120 s of stimulation (Fig. 5E), but slower than the rate of decay from rest. This is congruent with the main decrease in probability of release occurring during the first 50 events. Finally, the bottom plots illustrate how the time course of IPSC amplitudes normalized to the first HFS event is similar after 50 events or 120 s of stimulation, although different for BF 10 or 67 Hz (left and right, respectively, i.e., higher relative facilitation at the beginning of HFS delivered from BF 10 Hz than from 67 Hz). These results suggest that the proportions of fast facilitation and depression recruited by changes in rate differ depending on the BF (10 or 67 Hz), but importantly, this proportion is not altered by S-STD expression. This supports the view that the S-STD mechanism is distinct and behaves independently from the processes underlying faster forms of plasticity.
Together, these results strongly suggest that S-STD is input-specific, involves a presynaptic mechanism, probably a change in the number of available quanta (N), and its mechanism does not interfere with the balance of faster forms of synaptic plasticity.
A model with an independent slow-depression component explains well the experimental results
Computer simulations were used to further gain insight into how different synaptic processes could interact to explain the present results. As a starting point, I tested a version of previously published models with two different pools of ready-to-be-released vesicles (RRPs) (Trommershauser et al., 2003), which successfully explained PC_DCN responses to trains of hundreds of stimuli (Turecek et al., 2016). Briefly, this model requires two different RRPs (RRPA with high probability of release (Pr) and slow recovery time constant (TauR); and RRPB with low Pr, fast TauR, and facilitation) to explain PC_DCNs initial fast phase of depression, the substantial output with continuous activation, and the phase of frequency invariance (Turecek et al., 2016) (for further details, see Materials and Methods; Fig. 6, scheme). This model (hereafter D + F model; Fig. 6) explained well the present PC_DCN responses to the first 100 stimuli of trains with frequencies 10, 29.5, and 67 Hz (Fig. 6, same data as in Fig. 1), including frequency invariance phases and approximately linear encoding explained by the counteracting fast facilitation and depression of release, confirming previous results (Turecek et al., 2016).
Experimental and simulated responses of a two-pool and facilitation model of PC_DCNs to different BFs. A, Top, D + F model scheme. Synapses contain two different pools of vesicles A and B, which differ in release probability (Pr), time constant of recovery (TauR), and presence of facilitation. Bottom, The predicted output of a two-pool and facilitation model (D + F model) explains the experimental responses to the first 100 events of stimulation trains at 10, 29.5, and 67 Hz (same data as in Fig. 1). B, Detail of the model responses to the first 8 events of the trains at 10 and 67 Hz using a semilogarithmic plot to illustrate slower decaying rates with higher stimulating frequencies. C, Left and right plots (for 10 and 67 Hz stimulation rates, respectively) represent, from top to bottom, the data (circles) and model output (continuous lines), the predicted IPSC amplitudes for Pools A and B (PA, PB, red and blue), and the size of the ready to be released A and B pools (RRPA, RRPB, red and blue, respectively). D, The relationship of the predicted synaptic output at steady state (estimated by the product of the predicted IPSC amplitude and the stimulation frequency) versus the stimulation frequency is almost linear. E, Summary of results predicted by the D + F model at steady state as a function of the stimulation frequency: the predicted output of the Pool B (PB, blue circles), the total probability of release of the Pool B normalized to Event 1 (PrB + facilitation, black circles), and the change in RRPB (yellow circles), normalized to Event 1. RRPA0 = 7, RRPB0 = 25, Pr A = 0.098, Pr B = 0.017, TauR A = 12, TauR B = 0.5, f1 = 0.0005, Tauf1 = 0.007, f2 = 0.001, Tauf2 = 0.1.
However, the D + F model could not explain the further decay in IPSC amplitude with protracted stimulation (Fig. 7A), even when facilitation was omitted. The mechanism(s) of slow depression is debated, but it has been recently proposed that, under intense or sustained synaptic activity, the most sensitive bottleneck for the availability of vesicles for release is the number of release sites that after undergoing exocytosis are cleared and prepared to receive new vesicles or quanta (Neher, 2010). Considering the latter idea, the frequency dependence of S-STD, the evidence that S-STD depends on decrease in available quanta, and that is independent from fast forms of plasticity, I implemented a slow-depression component simulated by an activity-dependent decrease in active release sites of Pool B (R_SitesB), recovering with fixed time constant (Fig. 7B) (see Materials and Methods). The properties of the release sites remaining active are unchanged by this process. Inclusion of this component extended the D + F model. The extended model (hereafter, SD_RS model) was able to explain the changes in IPSC amplitude observed during 120-s-long stimulation trains (Fig. 7B). Importantly, this model explained as well the results from a different set of experiments using a switch in the protracted stimulation frequency within the same trial (i.e., from 10 to 67 Hz) (Fig. 7C, details in Fig. 8). The experimental results in Figure 7C confirmed those obtained using cell-attached recordings (Fig. 3E,F), further demonstrating that S-STD adjusts slowly to new levels, requiring several tens of seconds to reach a new steady state. In the model, the slow changes in the number of “active release sites” after the switch correlated with the slow time course of depression (R_Sites B, Fig. 8D, inset). Note the similarity in the time course of the amplitudes of the experimental and predicted responses: first, a relative facilitation of IPSCs amplitude follows the switch, recovers, and afterward slowly readapts up to a new (lower) steady-state level.
A two-pool and facilitation model extended by a slow-depression component (SD) explains experimental S-STD and scaled linear encoding of responses to 0.5 s steps of changes in rate. A, Predictions from a two-pool and facilitation model (D + F model) (yellow line, same fitting parameters as in Fig. 6) fail to explain S-STD (experimental data: blue and violet circles represent 10 and 67 Hz, respectively; same data as in Fig. 1). B, Instead, both, early and late experimental responses were explained by a model featuring a slow-depression (SD) component simulating an activity-dependent decrease in number of active release sites of Pool B (ready to be refilled after use and filled), schematized on top. Bottom, Data and model output (SD-RS model: dark blue line; see Materials and Methods, data as in A). In lower rows from top to bottom (Pool A in red and Pool B in blue): the predicted IPSC amplitudes of Pools A and B (PA, PB), the size of (filled) ready-to-be-released A and B pools (RRPA, RRPB), and the number of active release sites of Pool B (R_SitesB). C, Experimental (light blue) and SD-RS model predictions (dark blue line) to a sustained change in stimulation frequency (120 s at 10 Hz followed by 50 s at 67 Hz, indicated by the top scheme). Right, Detail in expanded time scale, around the switch time. Data from a different set of neurons than A and B (n = 6); the model estimated parameters are in Figure 8. D, Left, Summary of predicted steady-state normalized values of RRPB (NRRPB, filled squares) and PB (open circles) by the D + F (yellow) and SD_RS (dark blue) models and of the slow-depression component (red triangles, R_SitesB of the RS-SD model, normalized to the value at P0) as a function of the BF from experiments as in A and B. Right, a multiplicative effect of the SD component explains the differences between the models. Light blue curves indicate the product of the D + F curves (yellow) times the corresponding fractional R_SitesB (shown on the left plot in red). Dark blue traces (SD_RS model predictions) are occluded by the light blue curves. E1, Model predictions to steps of change in stimulating frequency (proxy for the test trains in Fig. 4). Top, Stimulation paradigm. Bottom, Examples of SD_RS model responses. Red dashed line indicates beginning of the step. E1, Plots of the integral of predicted IPSCs (proxy for charge transfer) as a function of the step frequency, for different BFs (labeled on the right). The D + F and SD_RS model outputs correspond to the left and right plots, respectively. E3, Multiplicative effect of SD: predicted input/output functions by the D + F (yellow) and SD-RS (dark blue) models as in E2, for 10 and 67 Hz BF (left and right plots, respectively). Light blue traces were obtained by dividing the output of the SD-RS model (dark blue) by the corresponding fraction of R_SitesB available before the frequency step. (SD-RS: RRPA0 = 7, RRPB0 = 25, PrA=0.098, PrB = 0.017, TauRA = 12, TauRB = 0,5, f1 = 0.0005, Tauf1 = 0.007, f2 = 0.001, Tauf2 = 0.1, ARSB = 0.47, F_RS = 29, TauRSr = 30, apply to all panels except Fig. 6C, see details in main text).
The comparison of steady-state values of the normalized RRPB size (NRRPB, diamonds) and the output of Pool B (PB, circles) predicted by the SD_RS (blue) and the D + F (yellow) models, which differ only by the presence or absence of the slow-depression component, highlights the effect of the latter (Fig. 7D, left). The plot also illustrates the frequency-dependent change in the slow-depression component represented by the fractional size of active release sites of Pool B (R_SitesBss normalized to the value at P0, NR_SitesB, red triangles). The scaling effect of S-STD (Fig. 4A) suggested a multiplicative relationship of the slow-depression component. The here implemented inactivation of sites does not interfere with the properties of the remaining active ones, predicting a pure scaling effect. Indeed, the product of the NRRPB or the PB values of the D + F model, times the slow-depression component (NR_SitesBss), returned the RRPB and PB values of the SD_RS model, respectively (Fig. 7D, right, respective products shown in light blue), graphically illustrating the multiplicative operation of the slow-depression component for steady-state conditions. Noteworthy, due to the minimal contribution of Pool A to the total output at steady state, similar results emerged from the analysis of the total output (PA + PB) (Fig. 8E). Therefore, the extended model explained both the changes in amplitude and the time course of these changes during protracted stimulation and after a switch in BF.
Predicted responses of the SD-RS model. A, SD-RS model predictions to synapse activation at 10 Hz (120 s) followed by 67 Hz (50 s) PA and PB (red and blue, respectively, as in Fig. 7). B, RRPA and RRPB (red and blue, respectively, as in Fig. 7). C, Total facilitation for Pool B. D, Number of release sites active (R_SitesB). Inset, In expanded scale, the values around the switch time (red arrow). RRPA0 = 7, RRPB0 = 25, PrA = 0.098, PrB = 0.01, TauRA = 12, TauRB = 1.55, f1 = 0.0005, Tauf1 = 0.007, f2 = 0.0024, Tauf2 = 0.1, ARSB = 0.74, F_RS = 29, TauRSr = 30. E, Plot represents the predictions of the total synapse output (PoolA+PoolB) at steady state (Pss) by the D + F and the SD_RS models (yellow and dark blue circles, respectively) as a function of BF. Red triangles represent the normalized values of the number of active release sites of the RS_SD model (R_SitesB). Light blue (almost occluding the dark blue trace) represents the result of multiplying the Pss values of the D + F model by the normalized R_SitesB of the SD_RS model.
Next, I checked whether this model accounted as well for the BF-dependent changes in gain for the responses to test trains(e.g., Fig. 4A). The predictions of both models to steps of changes in activation rate (duration: 0.5 s) induced after sustained activation, as applied experimentally to PC_DCN synapses (Fig. 7E1), were investigated. As expected, the integral of the predicted output over the step duration (proxy for the charge transfer amplitude) by the D + F model increased almost linearly as a function of the step frequency up to 200 Hz; however, in contrast to the experimental results, the slopes or gains for different BFs were almost coincident (Fig. 7E2, left vs Fig. 4A). Instead, the predictions by the SD_RS model (Fig. 7E2, right) showed similar linear increases in output but clearly distinct slopes for different BFs, indicating that the model captures the changes in gain mediated by the different BFs. Moreover, dividing the values obtained with the SD_RS model by the normalized number of release sites present at steady state before the step returned the values of the D + F model (Fig. 7E3), indicating that the multiplicative effect of this slow-depression component of Pool B was sufficient to explain the effect. Furthermore, the number of active release sites changed slowly (Fig. 8D), explaining that the number of sites available just before the step determines the gain during the post-ceding subsecond-to-few seconds periods of the steps in frequency. The fast STP components (given by the total facilitation and RRP depletion) led to opposing effects during the step (Fig. 8), contributing to the linear encoding during this time window.
Summarizing, a model of synaptic transmission, including a slow-depression component with a multiplicative relationship to the simulated fast plasticity components, explains well the experimental data. Moreover, these results demonstrate that an activity-dependent change in release sites is a plausible molecular mechanism for slow depression at PC_DCN synapses and consequent gain control.
Discussion
This study examined how cerebellar PCs, which are continuously active, can faithfully transfer behavior-driven signals embedded in the incessant background activity and this, without driving the postsynaptic DCNs out of range. By using protracted PC_DCN activation, I identified a previously unrecognized frequency-dependent S-STD, which requires tens of seconds or thousands of events to reach or readjust to new steady-state levels, adapting PC_DCNs output to the background activity. Yet, PC stimulation paradigms mimicking behavior-related signals embedded in the background activity revealed a subsecond to second-long time window during which the evoked charge transfer or changes in DCN firing rate were proportional to the PC rates, with gain dependent on preceding background activity. Thus, S-STD supports a novel slow-gain control mechanism enabling faithful linear encoding of PC rates in the timescale of common movements (e.g., reaches, steps) and adaptation to background activity at longer timescales. Experimental evidence and simulations suggested the scaled linear encoding emerged from the combined S-STD slow dynamics and frequency-invariant gain at faster time scales. Mechanistically, a presynaptic mechanism based on decreased ready-to-release vesicles, distinct from faster plasticity forms, plausibly, a reduction in the numberof active release sites, could explain S-STD.
The use of protracted PC_DCN activation has been key in the identification of S-STD, which was a robust finding, revealed using regular and irregular activation patterns, in preparations from young or adult animals, and under conditions approximating those found in situ (i.e., temperature, calcium concentration). Furthermore, the consequences of S-STD were readily assessable measuring synaptic charge transfer or DCN changes in firing rate, confirming its functional significance. Since protracted synaptic activation tests are rarely used, the prevalence of slow depression at other synapses is uncertain. Evidence for S-STD was first recognized at the neuromuscular junction (Del Castillo and Katz, 1954; Elmqvist and Quastel, 1965) and subsequently in hippocampal cell cultures, hippocampal slices, and the auditory pathway (Liu and Tsien, 1995; Stevens and Wesseling, 1999; Garcia-Perez et al., 2008; Hennig et al., 2008; Krächan et al., 2017). However, functional S-STD expression requires sustained activation, a condition clearly met at PC_DCNs (Sato et al., 1992; Norris et al., 2004; Schonewille et al., 2006; Cao et al., 2012; Hong et al., 2016), but apparently not at hippocampal synapses (Klyachko and Stevens, 2006). Still, as sustained activity is central to several brain processes, the relevance of the present findings might bear relevance to other brain functions.
Several lines of evidence argue that S-STD is distinct from all forms of depression previously described at PC_DCNs. First, S-STD is more than one order of magnitude slower than previously reported forms of depression at these synapses (Telgkamp and Raman, 2002; Pedroarena and Schwarz, 2003; Najac and Raman, 2015; Turecek et al., 2016), requiring tens of seconds to minutes, instead of milliseconds to seconds, to reach steady state or to adjust to new levels after changes in rate (Figs. 1, 3, 5, and 7). Second, the relative proportion of fast facilitation/depression evoked by test trains is similar before and after S-STD induction (Fig. 5F). Moreover, S-STD enables linear encoding of PC rates, presumed to result from balanced fast facilitation and depression (Turecek et al., 2016) (Fig. 4). Finally, simulations using an S-STD component multiplicatively related to fast facilitation/depression ones explain the experimental responses to diverse stimulation paradigms, further supporting the idea that S-STD is different from faster forms of PC_DCN plasticity.
The mechanism of S-STD is debated, with proposed mechanisms including depletion of a reserve pool (Elmqvist and Quastel, 1965; Richards et al., 2003), “fatigue” in the replenishment of the readily releasable pool of vesicles (Gabriel et al., 2011), and decreased release probability (Hennig et al., 2008). The latter mechanism seems unlikely for PC_DCNs given the analysis of depression rates and CV−2 (Fig. 5) (but see Faber and Korn, 1991). The same results speak against a decreased quantal size, for example, by incomplete vesicle refilling (Bennett et al., 1976) or postsynaptic change in the chloride gradient. Furthermore, GABAB-dependent slow gain control (Magnusson et al., 2008) is unlikely the cause for PC_DCN S-STD as their respective time courses differ, and S-STD modulation persists under GABAB antagonists, in line with previous results (Pedroarena and Schwarz, 2003). Recently, the availability of “receptive” release sites has been proposed to be the principal bottleneck for the maintenance of intense sustained synaptic transmission (Neher, 2010), suggested by observations of rapid reduced synaptic release under unbalanced exocytosis and endocytosis, faster than expected from a failure in vesicle recycling (Kawasaki et al., 2000; Hosoi et al., 2009). The exact mechanism limiting release site availability is not clear (for review, see Byczkowicz et al., 2018) but could explain S-STD. Indeed, changes in release sites was proposed earlier to explain a slow phase of recovery from intense activity (Stevens and Wesseling, 1999). However, S-STD must not be universally obligatory (Mayer et al., 2014), as other synaptic processes or properties (Kalkstein and Magleby, 2004; Kandaswamy et al., 2010; Byczkowicz et al., 2018; Xue et al., 2018) could mitigate the release site limitation. In the present study, I implemented “release site availability” as a component of a previously published synaptic transmission model explaining PC_DCN responses on shorter time scales (Turecek et al., 2016). The success of this extended model in predicting experimental S-STD and scaled linear transmission strongly favored “release site availability” as PC_DCN S-STD mechanism. The change in “release site availability” sites does not (necessarily) influence the properties of the remaining sites and thus is compatible with present evidence that S-STD expression does not interfere with faster plasticity processes. Here, the rate at which single vesicle-depleted release sites refill was kept constant, although activity-dependent refill (Pan and Zucker, 2009) could explain supralinear responses evoked by brief high frequency PC stimulation (Fig. 4F). The total refilling rate varied here only due to changes in the total number of “accepting release sites.” Overall, this study supports a decrease in “accepting release sites” as a plausible mechanism for S-STD and (together with fast frequency-invariant synaptic transmission) the derived slow-gain control mechanism described here. Nonetheless, the molecular identity behind the S-STD phenomenon could correspond to an equivalent process, and recent advances warrant further studies (Doussau et al., 2017; Milovanovic et al., 2018; Patzke et al., 2019; Vaden et al., 2019).
What functional consequences arise from PC_DCN-S-STD? First, the mere presence of S-STD and resulting shallow relationship between the background-stimulation frequency (BF) and steady-state output (Fig. 5C, inset) suggests that S-STD deemphasizes the effect of PC background activity in behaving animals. Thus, S-STD could partly explain previous findings of little sensitivity of DCN firing responses to sustained changes PC_BF (Belmeguenai et al., 2010). The decreased sensitivity to steady PC rates could be useful for maintaining the inhibitory PC output within the working firing range of DCNs, avoiding transmission failure (Pedroarena, 2010).
Second, S-STD offers a period of stable gain supporting scaled linear encoding of PC rates in the PC_DCNs synaptic output over behavior-relevant time windows and in the firing rates of large DCNs. Because DCN rate is less affected by synchronized than desynchronized PC inputs (Sarnaik and Raman, 2018), the present results suggest that rate code could be robust under more physiological desynchronized conditions. The findings are in line with in vivo OG manipulation studies showing inverse variations in PC and DCN rates (Heiney et al., 2014; Ozcan et al., 2020). Moreover, the fact that S-STD guarantees similar outputs from PCs with widely varying background activity favors linear encoding of changes in rate at the PC population level (Fig. 4) (Abbott et al., 1997). Noteworthy, although PC_DCN linear encoding was proposed as advantageous for cerebellar computations, the period of stable gain provided by S-STD could also support other previously proposed PC_DCN encoding strategies (Shidara et al., 1993; Walter and Khodakhah, 2009; De Zeeuw et al., 2011; Heck et al., 2013): for example, transferring the timing of synchronized (complex or simple) spikes, or firing pauses (Welsh et al., 1995; Gauck and Jaeger, 2000; Shin et al., 2007; Hoebeek et al., 2010; Person and Raman, 2012; Hong et al., 2016; Tang et al., 2019; Ozcan et al., 2020), or inducing binaryor graded signal inversion (Llinás and Mühlethaler, 1988; Pedroarena, 2010; Boehme et al., 2011; Ten Brinke et al., 2017). In this line, findings of preferential transfer of brief PC signals (Fig. 4F) suggest that PC_DCNs could discriminate signal length or timing, promoting the expression of temporal codes.
Finally, and intriguingly, the S-STD mediated changes in gain without changes in the linear transfer function for stimuli mimicking behavior-driven/learned PC signals is reminiscent, at synaptic level, of the gain modulation of neuronal population responses (Salinas and Thier, 2000), a computation proposed to account for nonlinear combination of signals (Salinas, 2004). PC sustained firing and PC behavior-related signals converge at single synapses, but each may represent different information and, consequently, change independently. Long-term or transient modifications of PC background activity could modulate PC_DCN gain for stereotyped signals, such as complex spikes (see Fig. 4A,B), providing an alternative and flexible mechanism to adapt PC output at the last cerebellar stage (Titley et al., 2017), revealing a novel role for PC background activity in controlling cerebellar output.
Footnotes
The author declares no competing financial interests.
I thank Cornelius Schwarz for comments on a version of this manuscript and support for this research, alongside that from the Cognitive Neurology Department; Ute Grosshennig and Ursula Pascht for technical assistance; all members of the Cognitive Neurology Department for comments and helpful discussions; as well as the editors and the anonymous reviewers for helpful comments and suggestions.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Christine M. Pedroarena at christine.pedroarena{at}uni-tuebingen.de