Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Journal Club

Information-Limiting Correlations in Neural Populations: The Devil Is in the Details

Reebal Rafeh and Geetika Gupta
Journal of Neuroscience 7 October 2020, 40 (41) 7782-7784; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0917-20.2020
Reebal Rafeh
1Department of Psychology, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 5C2, Canada
2Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 5C1, Canada
3Brain and Mind Institute, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 5B7, Canada
4Robarts Research Institute, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 5K8, Canada
5Neuroscience Graduate Program, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Reebal Rafeh
Geetika Gupta
1Department of Psychology, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 5C2, Canada
3Brain and Mind Institute, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 5B7, Canada
4Robarts Research Institute, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 5K8, Canada
5Neuroscience Graduate Program, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Neurons are noisy. Neuronal activity varies in response to repetitions of the same stimulus. Neuronal noise is ubiquitous in the brain and is often correlated among neighboring neurons, which means that stimulus-independent fluctuations in neuronal activity can affect entire populations of cells (Schmitz and Duncan, 2018). One might therefore reasonably expect that correlated neuronal noise poses a major challenge for the capacity of the brain to process and store information. How can one brain region reliably interpret a stimulus that was just presented from the noisy output of another region? Yet the brain somehow accomplishes this feat since our perceptions and actions are remarkably stable.

The influence of neuronal noise on population coding has become a focus of intense debate in computational neuroscience over recent decades. Some researchers have found evidence that correlated noise fundamentally constrains the amount of information that can be carried by neuronal population activity (Cohen and Maunsell, 2009; Mitchell et al., 2009; Verhoef and Maunsell, 2017). By contrast, earlier work has shown that correlated noise is inconsequential to the amount of information carried by the population (Averbeck and Lee, 2003, 2006). In a recent report in The Journal of Neuroscience, Bartolo et al. (2020) conducted a series of electrophysiology experiments using multielectrode array recordings in macaques to reconcile these two seemingly conflicting lines of evidence.

Bartolo et al. (2020) recognized a major source of methodological variability in prior monkey electrophysiology experiments examining neuronal noise, namely the total number of neurons recorded in each animal. Using simulations of neuronal activity, previous work had yielded predictions of how neuronal noise impacts information processing at increasing population sizes (Zohary et al., 1994; Abbott and Dayan, 1999). Based on these predictions, Bartolo et al. (2020) hypothesized that the impact of neuronal noise on population coding becomes more prevalent with increasing population size. If the influence of neuronal noise on population coding scales with the size of the recorded population, this might reconcile why different studies report a different influence of noise correlations on information.

To test their hypothesis, the authors leveraged high-density multielectrode arrays to simultaneously record from hundreds of neurons in two macaque monkeys. They placed the arrays over prefrontal regions known to be involved in the generation of eye movements. Neuronal activity was recorded from these arrays while the monkeys performed a simple task in which they were cued to perform a leftward or rightward saccade.

To estimate the amount of information in the recorded neuronal populations, Bartolo et al. (2020) examined distributions of population activity corresponding to either of the two saccade directions. The greater the overlap between the two response distributions, the less the information carried by the population for distinguishing the two conditions. With this measure of information in hand, Bartolo et al. (2020) next examined how correlated noise influences population coding as the recorded population grows in size.

Bartolo et al. (2020) found that correlated noise limits the amount of information carried by the population. However, this effect became apparent only when the population size surpassed ∼350 neurons. Increasing the population size further, up to 700 neurons, revealed still larger effects of noise correlation on population coding. Thus, Bartolo et al. (2020) confirmed their hypothesis that the effect of noise on information encoded by a neuronal population increases with population size. They also provide a lower bound on the population size needed for detecting information-limiting correlations in prefrontal cortex during a simple saccade task. However, their results leave open how noise correlations arise, what their potential neurobiological function is, and what mechanisms the brain is endowed with for altering the dynamics of correlated noise in population coding. Below, we discuss each of these three issues through the lens of recent theoretical and empirical neuroscience research.

We first consider the issue of how noise correlations arise from population activity. To do so, we use the metaphor of a choir for our neuronal population, where each singer's voice is the spiking output of a neuron, and the driving stimulus is the choir conductor. What tune a singer sings (the spiking output of the neuron) is determined by both the input from the conductor (the stimulus) and their harmonization with neighboring singers' voices (the neuronal population). Now consider that the choir is composed of groups of neighboring singers, with each group specializing in a different pitch: the sopranos, altos, tenors, and basses. So too, cortical populations are composed of ensembles of neurons, with each ensemble preferring different stimulus features: a specific orientation, color, or eye movement. For a high note in the song, the conductor signals the sopranos, whose voices then synchronize and dominate the other groups in the choir. Similarly, an ensemble of neurons strongly activated by their preferred stimulus will synchronize and dominate other ensembles of neurons. They dominate not only because they are strongly excited by the stimulus, but also because they inhibit neighboring neurons with similar preferences (Verhoef and Maunsell, 2017). Thus, the outputs of the neighboring neurons are the joint result of excitatory stimulus inputs and inhibitory synchronized fluctuations originating from the dominant population. These fluctuations are correlated noise.

The noisy correlated fluctuations appear to represent an important mechanism by which neurons maintain accurate coding of information—they are not an accident but rather an important component of neurobiological function. Specifically, shared noise allows neurons to maintain an excitatory–inhibitory balance (Reynolds and Heeger, 2009; Carandini and Heeger, 2012; Verhoef and Maunsell, 2017). Consider a neuronal ensemble maximally excited by its preferred stimulus, leading to the injection of shared noise into neighboring similarly tuned ensembles. These noisy fluctuations reduce the amount of information carried by other “competing” ensembles and, thus, inhibit their contribution to the population code. In this way, populations can dynamically code stimuli through continuous perturbation of this excitatory–inhibitory balance (Schmitz and Duncan, 2018).

This coding scheme works well in most cases, but it also leaves the brain open to mistakes. Consider a noisy fluctuation received by multiple similarly tuned neuronal ensembles at the same time. This wave of noise could momentarily “wiggle” the distribution of outputs in an entire population such that it mimics a stimulus change, leading downstream populations to misinterpret what stimulus actually occurred. Recent theoretical and empirical work has demonstrated that this type of effect does indeed occur, though it accounts for a relatively small proportion of shared noise in the cortical population activity (Moreno-Bote et al., 2014; van Bergen et al., 2015; Nogueira et al., 2020). These forms of noise correlation are thus termed information-limiting noise because they distort the stimulus information carried by the population. It is this form of information-limiting noise that Bartolo et al. (2020) were interested in isolating. Their findings demonstrate that the detectable effect of information-limiting noise scales with the number of neurons recorded.

The information-limiting effects of correlated noise only become apparent at larger population sizes because the correlated noise then starts to dominate the population response (Zohary et al., 1994; Shadlen and Newsome, 1998; Averbeck et al., 2006). Let us consider a population consisting of five neurons versus a population consisting of a hundred neurons. Five neurons can make 20 pairs while a hundred neurons can make 9900 pairs. The number of possible pairs thus grows exponentially with population size. Shared fluctuations because of correlated noise between neuron pairs contribute to the fluctuations of the population response. Therefore, the relative contribution of correlated noise to the population response grows exponentially with population size. Now consider adding a neuron to an existing large population. The unique contribution of the neuron will be dwarfed by the correlated noise. Correlated noise thus places an upper bound on the stimulus information that can be gained by including additional neurons (Zohary et al., 1994; Shadlen and Newsome, 1998; Averbeck et al., 2006). The upper bound is lower and reached more quickly with increasing strengths of correlated noise (Zohary et al., 1994). Because electrophysiology studies of monkey visual cortex often report analyses on similarly tuned populations of neurons (Mitchell et al., 2009; Smith and Sommer, 2013; Ruff and Cohen, 2014), these studies likely captured the effects of information-limiting correlations despite recording from smaller populations.

If noise correlations represent an important mechanism by which neuronal ensembles compete for dominance in the population code, and can also limit the amount of information carried in populations of neurons, the brain must be endowed with mechanisms for adjusting noise correlations. Over the past decade, multielectrode monkey electrophysiology research has shown that attention represents one such mechanism (Cohen and Maunsell, 2009; Mitchell et al., 2009; Rabinowitz et al., 2015; Verhoef and Maunsell, 2017). Attention is critical for biasing competition among similarly tuned neuronal ensembles to enable fine-grained discrimination of behaviorally relevant stimuli. This biasing mechanism appears to be accomplished in part by reducing shared inhibitory inputs arriving in the dominant (preferred) ensemble. Another mechanism the brain may use to deal with correlated noise is to read out information from neuronal populations that are diversely tuned (Ecker et al., 2011; Tremblay et al., 2015; Leavitt et al., 2017). If the sampled population has sufficiently heterogeneous tuning functions, information processing remains unaffected by correlated noise (Ecker et al., 2011).

Bartolo et al. (2020) demonstrated that population size plays a crucial role in our ability to detect information-limiting noise correlations in population activity. Their study further underscores the multiple theoretical and methodological considerations needed to systematize this body of research: neuronal population size, tuning similarity, and stimulus competition. The devil is in the details. Each of these variables can dramatically influence the relationship of noise correlation to information coding in population activity. Future electrophysiology work with more extensive coverage of the brain in multiple different patches of cortex and a broader array of stimuli and tasks will likely lead to further advances in our understanding of the functional basis of noise correlations in cognition and potentially will pave the way for translating these discoveries to humans.

Footnotes

  • Editor's Note: These short reviews of recent JNeurosci articles, written exclusively by students or postdoctoral fellows, summarize the important findings of the paper and provide additional insight and commentary. If the authors of the highlighted article have written a response to the Journal Club, the response can be found by viewing the Journal Club at www.jneurosci.org. For more information on the format, review process, and purpose of Journal Club articles, please see http://jneurosci.org/content/jneurosci-journal-club.

  • We thank Drs. Marieke Mur and Taylor Schmitz for comments and feedback in the preparation of this manuscript.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Reebal Rafeh rrafeh{at}uwo.ca

References

  1. ↵
    1. Abbott LF,
    2. Dayan P
    (1999) The effect of correlated variability on the accuracy of a population code. Neural Comput 11:91–101. doi:10.1162/089976699300016827 pmid:9950724
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Averbeck BB,
    2. Lee D
    (2003) Neural noise and movement-related codes in the macaque supplementary motor area. J Neurosci 23:7630–7641. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-20-07630.2003 pmid:12930802
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    1. Averbeck BB,
    2. Lee D
    (2006) Effects of noise correlations on information encoding and decoding. J Neurophysiol 95:3633–3644. doi:10.1152/jn.00919.2005 pmid:16554512
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Averbeck BB,
    2. Latham PE,
    3. Pouget A
    (2006) Neural correlations, population coding and computation. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:358–366. doi:10.1038/nrn1888 pmid:16760916
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Bartolo R,
    2. Saunders RC,
    3. Mitz AR,
    4. Averbeck BB
    (2020) Information limiting correlations in large neural populations. J Neurosci 40:1668–1678. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2072-19.2019 pmid:31941667
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Carandini M,
    2. Heeger DJ
    (2012) Normalization as a canonical neural computation. Nature Re-views Neuroscience 13:51–62.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Cohen MR,
    2. Maunsell JH
    (2009) Attention improves performance primarily by reducing interneuronal correlations. Nat Neurosci 12:1594–1600. doi:10.1038/nn.2439 pmid:19915566
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Ecker A,
    2. Berens P,
    3. Tolias A,
    4. Bethge M
    (2011) The effect of noise correlations in populations of diversely tuned neurons. J Neurosci 31:14272–14283. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2539-11.2011 pmid:21976512
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Leavitt ML,
    2. Mendoza-Halliday D,
    3. Martinez-Trujillo JC
    (2017) Sustained activity encoding working memories: not fully distributed. Trends Neurosci 40:328–346. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2017.04.004 pmid:28515011
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Mitchell JF,
    2. Sundberg KA,
    3. Reynolds JH
    (2009) Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in macaque area V4. Neuron 63:879–888. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.013
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Moreno-Bote R,
    2. Beck J,
    3. Kanitscheider I,
    4. Pitkow X,
    5. Latham P,
    6. Pouget A
    (2014) Information-limiting correlations. Nat Neurosci 17:1410–1417. doi:10.1038/nn.3807 pmid:25195105
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Nogueira R,
    2. Peltier NE,
    3. Anzai A,
    4. DeAngelis GC,
    5. Martínez-Trujillo J,
    6. Moreno-Bote R
    (2020) The effects of population tuning and trial-by-trial variability on information encoding and behavior. J Neurosci 40:1066–1083. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0859-19.2019 pmid:31754013
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. ↵
    1. Rabinowitz NC,
    2. Goris RL,
    3. Cohen M,
    4. Simoncelli EP
    (2015) Attention stabilizes the shared gain of V4 populations. Elife 4:e08998. doi:10.7554/eLife.08998 pmid:26523390
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Reynolds JH,
    2. Heeger DJ
    (2009) The normalization model of attention. Neuron 61:168–185.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Ruff DA,
    2. Cohen MR
    (2014) Attention can either increase or decrease spike count correlations in visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 17:1591–1597. doi:10.1038/nn.3835 pmid:25306550
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Schmitz TW,
    2. Duncan J
    (2018) Normalization and the cholinergic microcircuit: a unified basis for attention. Trends Cogn Sci 22:422–437. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2018.02.011 pmid:29576464
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Shadlen MN,
    2. Newsome WT
    (1998) The variable discharge of cortical neurons: implications for connectivity, computation, and information coding. J Neurosci 18:3870–3896. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-10-03870.1998 pmid:9570816
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Smith MA,
    2. Sommer MA
    (2013) Spatial and temporal scales of neuronal correlation in visual area V4. J Neurosci 33:5422–5432. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4782-12.2013 pmid:23516307
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Tremblay S,
    2. Pieper F,
    3. Sachs A,
    4. Martinez-Trujillo J
    (2015) Attentional filtering of visual information by neuronal ensembles in the primate lateral prefrontal cortex. Neuron 85:202–215. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.11.021 pmid:25500502
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. van Bergen RS,
    2. Ma WJ,
    3. Pratte MS,
    4. Jehee JF
    (2015) Sensory uncertainty decoded from visual cortex predicts behavior. Nat Neurosci 18:1728–1730. doi:10.1038/nn.4150 pmid:26502262
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Verhoef BE,
    2. Maunsell JH
    (2017) Attention-related changes in correlated neuronal activity arise from normalization mechanisms. Nat Neurosci 20:969–977. doi:10.1038/nn.4572 pmid:28553943
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Zohary E,
    2. Shadlen MN,
    3. Newsome WT
    (1994) Correlated neuronal discharge rate and its implications for psychophysical performance. Nature 370:140–143. doi:10.1038/370140a0 pmid:8022482
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 40 (41)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 40, Issue 41
7 Oct 2020
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Information-Limiting Correlations in Neural Populations: The Devil Is in the Details
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Information-Limiting Correlations in Neural Populations: The Devil Is in the Details
Reebal Rafeh, Geetika Gupta
Journal of Neuroscience 7 October 2020, 40 (41) 7782-7784; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0917-20.2020

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Information-Limiting Correlations in Neural Populations: The Devil Is in the Details
Reebal Rafeh, Geetika Gupta
Journal of Neuroscience 7 October 2020, 40 (41) 7782-7784; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0917-20.2020
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Parabrachial Projections to PAG-RVM Axis May Promote Placebo Hypoalgesia and Nocebo Hyperalgesia
  • Differential Dopamine Dynamics in Adolescents and Adults
  • Complementing Neuroregeneration: Deciphering the Role of Neuro-Immune Interactions in CNS Repair
Show more Journal Club
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2022 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.