Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Dissociation of Medial Frontal β-Bursts and Executive Control

Steven P. Errington, Geoffrey F. Woodman and Jeffrey D. Schall
Journal of Neuroscience 25 November 2020, 40 (48) 9272-9282; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2072-20.2020
Steven P. Errington
Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt Vision Research Center, Center for Integrative and Cognitive Neuroscience, Vanderbilt Brain Institute, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee 37240
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Geoffrey F. Woodman
Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt Vision Research Center, Center for Integrative and Cognitive Neuroscience, Vanderbilt Brain Institute, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee 37240
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jeffrey D. Schall
Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt Vision Research Center, Center for Integrative and Cognitive Neuroscience, Vanderbilt Brain Institute, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee 37240
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Jeffrey D. Schall
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Tables
  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Experimental procedures. A, Saccade-countermanding task. Monkeys initiated trials by fixating on a central point. After a variable time, the center of the fixation point was extinguished. A peripheral target was presented simultaneously at one of two possible locations. On no-stop-signal trials monkeys were required to shift gaze to the target, whereupon after 600 ± 0 ms a high-pitched auditory feedback tone was delivered, and 600-ms later fluid reward was provided. On stop-signal trials (∼40% of trials), after the target appeared the center of the fixation point was re-illuminated after a variable SSD, which instructed the monkey to cancel the saccade in which case the same high-pitched tone was presented after a 1500 ± 0 ms hold time followed, after 600 ± 0 ms by fluid reward. SSD was adjusted such that monkeys successfully canceled the saccade in ∼50% of trials. In the remaining trials, monkeys made non-canceled errors which were followed after 600 ± 0 ms by a low-pitched tone, and no reward was delivered. Monkeys could not initiate trials earlier after errors. B, Countermanding behavior. Top left, Cumulative distribution function of response latencies on no-stop (green) and non-canceled (yellow) trials. Response latencies on non-canceled trials were faster than those on no-stop trials. Top right, Inhibition function plotting the probability of responding across SSDs. Weibull functions were fitted to data from each session. The mean of these Weibull functions across sessions and the corresponding 95% CI is plotted for each monkey (monkey Eu: purple; X: blue). Bottom left, Distribution of mean SSRTs across sessions. Bottom right, Distribution of the proportion of trigger failures across sessions. C, LFP processing. EEG was recorded with leads placed on the cranial surface over the medial frontal cortex at the location analogous to FCz in humans. The EEG lead was located over the supplementary eye field (SEF) and pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA). For each session, raw data were extracted. After bandpass filtering between 15 and 29 Hz, this signal was epoched from −1000 to 2500 ms relative to target presentation, saccade initiation, and stop-signal presentation. D, β-Burst processing. The epoched signal for each trial was convolved with a complex Morlet wavelet. Time-frequency power estimates were extracted by calculating the squared magnitude of the complex wavelet-convolved data. Individual β-bursts were defined as local maxima in the trial-by-trial band time-frequency power matrix, for which the power exceeded a threshold of six times the median power of the entire time-frequency power matrix for the electrode. An example burst is shown in the time-frequency plot at the bottom. E, Examples of β band time frequency in 12 randomly selected trials, aligned on SSD (solid red), with the corresponding SSRT (dashed red). These plots are indistinguishable from counterparts derived from human data.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    β-Bursts during stopping. A, Boxplots showing the incidence of β-bursts observed during the STOP process interval (from scheduled SSD to SSRT) during no-stop (green), non-canceled (orange), canceled (red), and during an equivalent period of time before target presentation (gray). β-Bursts are observed in ∼10–15% of trials and are slightly but significantly more commonly observed when saccades are inhibited. B, Raster plot of β-bursts aligned on a pretarget baseline interval (left), stop signal (middle), and saccade initiation (right) across all sessions. Each tick-mark shows the time of peak β-amplitudes satisfying inclusion criteria on each trial. Rasters are shown for non-canceled, no-stop, and canceled trials. The rough equivalence of β-burst frequency across types of trials is evident, as is the elevation of β-burst rate at the end of non-canceled error trials. C, β-Burst density function derived from raster plots. β-Burst peak times were convolved with a Gaussian function. During the stopping period, β-bursts were slightly but significantly more common on canceled trials (red line) than on no-stop (green) or non-canceled trials (yellow). D, Comparing time course of β-burst (top) and single neuron discharges (bottom) on canceled and latency-matched no stop signal trials for a single session. At no time did the incidence of β-bursts on single session differentiate between movement initiation and inhibition. In contrast, as demonstrated previously, the discharge rate of an example FEF movement neuron sampled in one session shows a clear separation between trial types occurring before the STOP process concludes. The neuron was recorded from another monkey in a separate study performing a choice countermanding task (Middlebrooks et al., 2020) and is provided as an example to demonstrate the mechanistic differences between the signals.

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    No relationship between β-bursts and response inhibition. A, Primary ordinate, average probability for a representative session of inhibiting on stop signal trials as a function of stop signal delay (orange line). Secondary ordinate, average probability of β-bursts determined with amplitudes exceeding the median threshold by 2 (light blue), 6 (blue), and 10 (dark blue) times in that session. The neurometric function derived from the probability of β-bursts does not correspond to the probability of inhibiting as a function of stop signal delay. B, Boxplots of the sum of squared differences between probabilities of canceling and of β-bursts across sessions determined with amplitudes exceeding from one to 10 times the median threshold. Open circles indicate outlier values. From generous to severe measurement thresholds, the incidence of β-bursts did not account for the probability of cancellation with stop signal delay.

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Relationship between β-bursts and performance monitoring. A, β-Bursts during error and correct responses. Top, Boxplot showing significantly greater incidence of β-bursts observed 100–300 ms following non-canceled (orange) compared with correct saccades (green). Middle, Raster plot aligned on saccade. Each tick-mark shows the time of one β-burst on a non-canceled (yellow) and no-stop (green) trials. Bottom, β-Burst density function. Following a saccade, the incidence of bursts on both error and correct trials decreases. This is followed by a pronounced increase in β-burst frequency ∼150 ms after noncancelled error saccades. B, Boxplots of RT adaptation following non-canceled errors and successful cancellations across sessions for Eu (purple) and X (cyan). Values greater than one represent slowing. As observed previously, both monkeys tend to delay responses somewhat after errors and more after successful stopping. C, β-Bursts are unrelated to RT adaptation. The incidence of β-bursts observed after errors did not vary as a function of the post-error RT adaptation index across sessions for Eu (purple) or X (cyan). Non-significant regression lines (±95% CI) include 0 slope.

Tables

  • Figures
    • View popup
    Table 1.

    Stop-signal task performance (mean ± SEM) for both monkeys across all sessions

    Monkey Eu (n = 12 sessions)Monkey X (n = 17 sessions)
    No-stop RT (ms)313.4 ± 1.6263.0 ± 1.0
    Non-canceled RT (ms)259.4 ± 2.0229.6 ± 1.0
    SSRTmean (ms)112.4 ± 6.4114.3 ± 1.8
    SSRTstd (ms)30.5 ± 1.830.9 ± 1.0
    p (trigger failures)0.069 ± 0.0100.032 ± 0.003
    • View popup
    Table 2.

    Percentage of trials (mean ± SEM) with β-bursts during a baseline and stopping period, for all trial types

    No-stopNon-canceledCanceled
    Baseline11.6 ± 0.6%11.7 ± 0.7%11.2 ± 0.7%
    Stopping period11.8 ± 0.8%10.5 ± 0.7%13.1 ± 0.8%
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 40 (48)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 40, Issue 48
25 Nov 2020
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Dissociation of Medial Frontal β-Bursts and Executive Control
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Dissociation of Medial Frontal β-Bursts and Executive Control
Steven P. Errington, Geoffrey F. Woodman, Jeffrey D. Schall
Journal of Neuroscience 25 November 2020, 40 (48) 9272-9282; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2072-20.2020

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Dissociation of Medial Frontal β-Bursts and Executive Control
Steven P. Errington, Geoffrey F. Woodman, Jeffrey D. Schall
Journal of Neuroscience 25 November 2020, 40 (48) 9272-9282; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2072-20.2020
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • countermanding
  • EEG
  • error monitoring
  • response inhibition
  • Stop signal
  • stopping

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • PVN–NAc Shell–VP Circuit OT and OTR Neurons Regulate Pair Bonding via D2R and D1R
  • Continuous Diffusion-Detected Neuroplasticity during Motor Learning
  • Gestational Chlorpyrifos Exposure Imparts Lasting Alterations to the Rat Somatosensory Cortex
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Zooming In and Out: Selective Attention Modulates Color Signals in Early Visual Cortex for Narrow and Broad Ranges of Task-Relevant Features
  • The Amygdala Regulates Social Motivation for Selective Vocal Imitation in Zebra Finches
  • Continuous Diffusion-Detected Neuroplasticity during Motor Learning
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.