Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Converging Evidence for Differential Specialization and Plasticity of Language Systems

Kshipra Gurunandan, Jaione Arnaez-Telleria, Manuel Carreiras and Pedro M. Paz-Alonso
Journal of Neuroscience 9 December 2020, 40 (50) 9715-9724; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0851-20.2020
Kshipra Gurunandan
1BCBL. Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, 20009 Donostia-San Sebastian, Spain
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Kshipra Gurunandan
Jaione Arnaez-Telleria
1BCBL. Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, 20009 Donostia-San Sebastian, Spain
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Manuel Carreiras
1BCBL. Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, 20009 Donostia-San Sebastian, Spain
2Basque Foundation for Science, 48013 Bilbao, Spain
3Department of Basque Language and Communication, University of the Basque Country, 48015 Bilbao, Spain
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Manuel Carreiras
Pedro M. Paz-Alonso
1BCBL. Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, 20009 Donostia-San Sebastian, Spain
2Basque Foundation for Science, 48013 Bilbao, Spain
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Pedro M. Paz-Alonso
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Functional specialization and plasticity are fundamental organizing principles of the brain. Since the mid-1800s, certain cognitive functions have been known to be lateralized, but the provenance and flexibility of hemispheric specialization remain open questions. Language is a uniquely human phenomenon that requires a delicate balance between neural specialization and plasticity, and language learning offers the perfect window to study these principles in the human brain. In the current study, we conducted two separate functional MRI experiments with language learners (male and female), one cross-sectional and one longitudinal, involving distinct populations and languages, and examined hemispheric lateralization and learning-dependent plasticity of the following three language systems: reading, speech comprehension, and verbal production. A multipronged analytic approach revealed a highly consistent pattern of results across the two experiments, showing (1) that in both native and non-native languages, while language production was left lateralized, lateralization for language comprehension was highly variable across individuals; and (2) that with increasing non-native language proficiency, reading and speech comprehension displayed substantial changes in hemispheric dominance, with languages tending to lateralize to opposite hemispheres, while production showed negligible change and remained left lateralized. These convergent results shed light on the long-standing debate of neural organization of language by establishing robust principles of lateralization and plasticity of the main language systems. Findings further suggest involvement of the sensorimotor systems in language lateralization and its plasticity.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The human brain exhibits a remarkable ability to support a vast variety of languages that may be acquired at different points in the life span. Language is a complex construct involving linguistic as well as visual, auditory, and motor processes. Using functional MRI, we examined hemispheric specialization and learning-dependent plasticity of three language systems—reading, speech comprehension, and verbal production—in cross-sectional and longitudinal experiments in language learners. A multipronged analytic approach revealed converging evidence for striking differences in hemispheric specialization and plasticity among the language systems. The results have major theoretical and practical implications for our understanding of fundamental principles of neural organization of language, language testing and recovery in patients, and language learning in healthy populations.

  • bilingualism
  • comprehension
  • hemispheric specialization
  • language learning
  • laterality
  • production

Introduction

Functional specialization in the brain is a well established principle of neural organization, but studies of atypical development suggest dramatic potential for neural plasticity (Payne and Lomber, 2001; Bavelier and Neville, 2002). While the capacity for neural reorganization decreases with age, it does not disappear completely, and adult neural plasticity is essential for learning and maintaining new information or behaviors (Kleim and Jones, 2008). The human propensity for language requires a delicate balance between neural specialization and capacity for reorganization, making language learning the ideal candidate for the examination of specialization and plasticity in the human brain.

Language typically activates a fronto-temporo-parietal network (Skeide and Friederici, 2016; Hagoort, 2019) and has long been thought to be predominantly left lateralized (Broca, 1863; Dax, 1863). However, the right hemisphere appears to be capable of taking over or supporting language function if needed, as seen in cases of language recovery after left hemisphere damage (Papanicolaou et al., 1987; Boatman et al., 1999; Duffau et al., 2002, 2003; Hope et al., 2017) and in language learning (Vingerhoets et al., 2003; Park et al., 2012). It is thus unclear whether the left hemisphere is indeed specialized for language, as is broadly accepted, with the right hemisphere playing at best a supporting role (Vigneau et al., 2011), or whether hemispheric dominance is more variable across individuals, as suggested by the larger than expected prevalence of language deficits following right hemisphere brain surgery (Vilasboas et al., 2017).

Language is a complex construct involving multilevel representations that can be processed visually (reading) or auditorily (listening), or by motor production (speaking/writing), and cumulative evidence points to these functions lateralizing differently. Auditory language has been found to be bilateral in infants (Dehaene-Lambertz et al., 2002; Perani et al., 2011), with no increase in lateralization from childhood to adulthood (Lidzba et al., 2011), increasing left lateralization (Ahmad et al., 2003), or increasing right hemisphere involvement (Booth et al., 2000); and a meta-analysis of auditory comprehension studies suggested that any left lateralization from childhood to adulthood increases more slightly and gradually than previously thought (Enge et al., 2020). On the other hand, there is little evidence to suggest that language production is anything but left lateralized (Gaillard et al., 2003; Szaflarski et al., 2006; Lidzba et al., 2011).

Language learning is known to change the pattern of neural activation for language. Studies comparing bilinguals and monolinguals consistently find differences in activation between them, with bilinguals typically exhibiting greater right hemispheric involvement in comprehension tasks (Kovelman et al., 2008; Horowitz-Kraus et al., 2015). However, it is uncertain whether this increased right hemispheric involvement merely modulates the magnitude of left lateralization or whether it is significant enough to constitute a change in hemispheric dominance. Further, are differences in lateralization between monolinguals and bilinguals because of developmental differences or is hemispheric dominance in fact plastic even into adulthood? Few neuroimaging studies have looked into ecologically valid adult language learning, but findings indicate that language learning in adults involves structural changes in cortical thickness and connectivity that could indeed support shifts in lateralization (Mårtensson et al., 2012; Schlegel et al., 2012; Xiang et al., 2015), suggesting that lateralization, at least for comprehension, may be susceptible to learning-dependent changes.

We conducted two fMRI experiments, one cross-sectional and one longitudinal, with immersed late language learners, and examined lateralization of reading, speech comprehension, and verbal production in their native (L1) and non-native (Ln) languages, and how this changed with increasing Ln proficiency. To test both the replicability and generalizability of findings, the two experiments were contrasted on several factors such as the early language experience of the participants (monolingual vs bilingual) and the language currently being learned, and the L1–Ln pairs in the two experiments had contrasting degrees of overlap in language families, phonology, and orthography. We hypothesized that (1) lateralization of comprehension would be more variable across individuals but production would be left lateralized; and (2) with increasing language proficiency, comprehension may display changes in hemispheric dominance, while production would remain left lateralized. We further expected that L1–Ln associations would change with increasing Ln proficiency, and that the pattern of changes would differ across the language systems.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Experiment I: basic versus advanced level language learners (cross-sectional)

The final experiment sample consisted of 29 right-handed native Spanish adults (mean age = 43.7 ± 9.7 years; 15 female) studying Basque in the same language school at either the basic (A2 level, n = 14) or advanced level (C1 level, n = 15). The proficiency levels correspond to those specified by the Common European Framework of Reference for Languages (CEFR). Participants were from the Basque Country, Spain; they grew up primarily exposed to Spanish at home and in school, with little early Basque exposure, and had limited knowledge of English or other languages (no difference between groups, p = 0.83). The two groups of learners were matched on age, gender, IQ, and Spanish proficiency (Table 1). Data from five other participants were discarded because of excessive head motion during MRI scanning, and these were not counted in the final sample.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Participant demographics and linguistic scores

Experiment II: intermediate language learners (longitudinal)

The final experimental group consisted of 19 right-handed native Spanish adolescents (mean age = 17.2 ± 0.6 years; 16 female) taking part in a 3 month English immersion-style after school program for B1 level students. Participants were from the Basque Country, Spain; they were native speakers of Spanish and acquired Basque in school (age of acquisition = 2.6 ± 2.06 years). The medium of instruction in school was Spanish/Basque; English was learned as a foreign language, with little exposure outside of classes. The students had intermediate English proficiency (Table 1). Data from five other participants were discarded because of excessive head motion during MRI scanning, and these were not counted in the final sample.

Experiments I and II

In both experiments, language proficiency was assessed using picture-naming tasks—an adaptation of the Boston Naming Test (Kaplan et al., 1983) controlled for cognates across Spanish, Basque, and English. Participant groups in Experiment I differed significantly in their Basque proficiency, and participants in Experiment II exhibited a significant increase in English proficiency after language training (Table 1). All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and no history of neurologic or psychiatric disorders. In compliance with the ethical regulations established by the Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language (BCBL) Ethics Committee and the guidelines of the Helsinki Declaration, all participants gave written informed consent before taking part in the experiment and received monetary compensation for their participation.

fMRI task

Inside the MRI scanner, participants performed the following two tasks: a comprehension and a production task. The order of tasks was counterbalanced across participants.

Language comprehension task

The participants performed semantic animacy judgment (living/nonliving) with single-word text and speech stimuli in each of their languages. Participants were instructed to fixate on a white cross in the middle of a black screen, and on the presentation of stimuli, to indicate their responses as quickly and as accurately as possible via button presses (counterbalanced across participants) using their dominant (right) hand. Stimuli were high-frequency, concrete, imageable nouns (e.g., house, dog, table) with an even split between living and nonliving items. Visual stimuli were presented in white letters on a black screen and were five to eight letters long. Auditory stimuli were presented through headphones and lasted an average of 565 ms (SD = 86 ms). Each run had 48 stimuli with intermixed reading and listening trials. The fMRI design was event-related with six/four runs (Experiment I: 2 languages × 3 runs; Experiment II: 2 languages × 2 runs). To avoid language switching, the languages were separated and their order was counterbalanced across participants.

Language production task

The participants performed a paced form of the semantic verbal fluency task in each language. Participants were instructed to fixate on a white cross in the middle of a black screen and respond overtly to semantic category words (e.g., fruits, animals, clothes) presented on the screen. Each word was displayed eight times, each requiring a novel response, or, failing this, an overt response saying “pass” in the relevant language. Fluency was scored as the percentage of valid answers of eight possible responses for each category. Repetitions, inflections of the same word and erroneous responses were removed, and responses were scored only for correctness and not accent or pronunciation. In the control condition, participants repeated the word presented on the screen. The task had a block design with two runs per language, each run containing eight semantic categories. To avoid language switching, the languages were separated and their order was counterbalanced across participants.

MRI data acquisition

Whole-brain MRI data were acquired using a 3 T Siemens Magnetom Trio whole-body MRI scanner and a 32-channel head coil at the BCBL. Padded headphones were used to dampen background scanner noise and enable clear transmission of the auditory stimuli. Participants viewed the print stimuli on a screen via a mirror mounted on the head coil. To limit head movement, the head coil was padded with foam and participants were asked to remain as still as possible. Structural T1-weighted images were acquired with an MPRAGE sequence [repetition time (TR) = 2530 ms, echo time (TE) = 2.97 ms, inversion time = 1100 ms, flip angle (FA) = 7°, field of view (FOV) = 256 × 256 mm, 176 slices and voxel size = 1 mm3].

Language comprehension task

Functional MRI was acquired in the course of six/four separate runs using a gradient echo echoplanar pulse sequence with the following parameters: TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, 32 axial slices with a 3.4 × 3.4 × 4 mm voxel resolution, 0% interslice gap, FA = 80°, FOV = 220 mm, 64 × 64 matrix. A total of 186 volumes were collected in each of the functional runs. Before each scan, four volumes were discarded to allow for T1 equilibration effects. To improve estimation of the resting baseline in functional analyses, functional runs contained three silent fixation periods of 20 s each. Within each functional run, the order of the trials (reading and listening conditions) and the intertrial intervals of variable duration corresponding to the baseline MR frames (30% of total collected functional volumes) were determined by an algorithm designed to maximize the efficiency of the recovery of the blood oxygenation level-dependent response (optseq2; Dale, 1999).

Language production task

Functional MRI was acquired in the course of four separate runs using a gradient echo echoplanar pulse sequence with the following parameters: TR = 3000 ms, TE = 25 ms, 43 axial slices with a 3.0 × 3.0 × 3.0 mm voxel resolution, 10% interslice gap, FA = 90°, FOV = 192 mm, 64 × 64 matrix. Two hundred forty volumes were collected for each of the functional runs. Before each scan, four volumes were discarded to allow for T1-equilibration effects.

MRI data analysis

Preprocessing

Standard SPM8 (Penny et al., 2011) preprocessing routines and analysis methods were used. Images were first corrected for differences in the timing of slice acquisition and then realigned to the first volume using rigid-body registration. Each subject's functional volumes were spatially smoothed with a 4 mm full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. Next, motion parameters obtained from realignment were used to inform a volume repair procedure (ArtRepair; Mazaika et al., 2009) that identified bad volumes on the basis of scan-to-scan movement (>1 mm) and signal fluctuations in global intensity (>1.3%), and that corrected bad volumes via interpolation between the nearest nonrepaired scans. Data from subjects requiring >20% of volumes to be repaired were discarded. The number of corrected volumes was similar between groups (Experiment I: comprehension task, p = 0.34; production task, p = 0.63) and scans (Experiment II: comprehension task, p = 0.75; production task, p = 0.46). After volume repair, functional volumes were coregistered to the T1 images using 12-parameter affine transformation and spatially normalized to the MNI space by applying nonlinear transforms estimated by deforming the MNI template to each individual's structural volume. During normalization, the volumes were sampled to 3 mm cubic voxels. The resulting volumes were then spatially smoothed with a 7 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. Finally, time series were temporally filtered to eliminate contamination from slow frequency drift (high-pass filter with a cutoff period of 128 s).

Subject-level analyses

Statistical analyses were performed on individual subject data using the general linear model (GLM). fMRI time series data were modeled by a series of impulses convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF). Six motion parameters for translation (x, y, z) and rotation (yaw, pitch, roll) were included as covariates of noninterest in the GLM. In the event-related design comprehension task, each trial was modeled as an event that was time locked to the onset of the presentation of each stimulus, and error responses were modeled separately. In the block design production task, each trial was modeled as an epoch of 31 s each, time locked to the beginning of the presentation of each block. The remaining functions were used as covariates in the GLM, along with a basic set of cosine functions that high-pass filtered the data, and a covariate for session effects. The least-squares parameter estimates of the height of the best fitting canonical HRF for each experimental condition were used in pairwise contrasts.

Laterality analyses

For every subject, lateralization of activation in the classical language network regions was calculated for each task × language. Laterality is typically quantified as a normalized ratio of left and right hemisphere contributions, ranging between +1 (fully left-lateralized activation) and −1 (fully right-lateralized activation). Each subject's whole-brain t-maps were masked with anatomic language regions from the AAL atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002)—six bilateral regions from classical language models (Friederici, 2012; Hagoort, 2013): inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) pars orbitalis, IFG pars triangularis, IFG pars opercularis, superior temporal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, and inferior parietal lobule. Since laterality indices are highly threshold dependent, in line with the latest recommendations (Bradshaw et al., 2017a), a threshold-independent bootstrapping method was used to calculate the laterality index using the LI-toolbox (Wilke and Lidzba, 2007), in which 10,000 indices were iteratively calculated at different thresholds, yielding a robust mean laterality index. Three analyses were conducted to examine the proficiency-dependent plasticity of (1) L1–Ln correlation, (2) hemispheric dominance, and (3) modality clustering. L1–Ln correlations were calculated for each group × task, and Cohen's q was used to quantify the difference in L1–Ln correlation between basic/advanced proficiency and before/after training in each modality. To examine hemispheric dominance, a lateralized dissociation index was calculated such that: Embedded Image Embedded Image that is, the absolute difference between laterality indices for each language and a factor, hem, to code whether the two languages were lateralized to the same or opposite hemispheres. Positive values indicated that languages were lateralized to opposite hemispheres, while negative values indicated that the languages were lateralized to the same hemisphere. Cohen's d was used to measure the magnitude of proficiency-dependent change in each modality: difference between medians in cross-sectional Experiment I, and difference in repeated measures in longitudinal Experiment II. To examine the modality-wise clustering of the joint L1–Ln distribution, 85% data ellipses were plotted for each modality, and the joint distribution difference (JDD) between any two modalities was calculated as: Embedded Image that is, standardized difference between the bivariate L1–Ln group means and the difference between joint spread of the data. This index lies between 0 and 1, with higher values indicating a greater difference between modalities. The maximum Euclidean distance between centroids was considered to be 1 for laterality data, and the maximum angle between the axes is 90°. Proficiency-group differences were measured in terms of the percentage difference in the difference index.

Results

In-scanner behavioral performance

Experiment I: basic versus advanced level language learners (cross-sectional)

A series of mixed-model ANOVAs was conducted on the behavioral measures of the fMRI tasks: comprehension task accuracy, production task fluency, and comprehension task reaction times (Fig. 1A). The comprehension task accuracy ANOVA with between-subjects factor Group (basic, advanced) and within-subject factors Language (L1, Ln) and Modality (reading, speech) showed a significant Group × Language interaction (F(1,26) = 16.18, p = 0.0004). The production task fluency ANOVA with between-subjects factor Group (basic, advanced) and within-subjects factor Language (L1, Ln) also showed a significant Group × Language interaction (F(1,23) = 31.36, p = 0.00,001). Post hoc simple-effect analyses (two-sample t tests) of these Group × Language interactions showed that the advanced proficiency group had significantly higher Ln task accuracy than the basic proficiency group in both comprehension (t(18.08) = 3.20, p = 0.002, one-sided) and production (t(22.28) = 5.502, p = 0.000008, one-sided), but there was no significant difference between groups in L1 task accuracy (comprehension: t(25.48) = −0.93, p = 0.360, two-sided, production: t(21.683) = 1.03, p = 0.31, two sided). Finally, the ANOVA for comprehension task reaction times showed a main effect of Language, and both groups were significantly slower in their Ln than their L1 (F(1,26) = 40.41, p = 0.000001).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

A, B, Behavioral measures accuracy and response time for in-scanner semantic tasks plotted as a function of Group, Language, and Modality in Experiment I (A), and as a function of Training, Language, and Modality in Experiment II (B). Error bars represent the SD, and asterisks represent statistically significant differences (***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05).

Experiment II: intermediate language learners (longitudinal)

A series of repeated-measures ANOVAs was conducted on the following behavioral measures of the fMRI tasks: comprehension task accuracy, production task fluency, and comprehension task reaction times (Fig. 1B). The comprehension task ANOVAs with three within-subject factors, Training (before, after), Language (L1, Ln), and Modality (reading, speech), showed the main effects of Language (L1 > Ln, F(1,17) = 338.64, p = 0.000000000001) and Modality (reading > speech, F(1,17) = 30.05, p = 0.00,004) on task accuracy. The production task fluency ANOVA with two within-subject factors Training (before, after), and Language (L1, Ln) showed a main effect of Language (L1 > Ln, F(1,15) = 146.01, p = 0.000000004). The comprehension task reaction times ANOVA revealed a significant Training × Language interaction (F(1,17) = 5.48, p = 0.031). Post hoc simple-effect analyses (paired t tests) showed that reaction times decreased significantly after training in Ln (t(17) = 2.83, p = 0.006, one-sided), but not in L1 (t(17) = 0.21, p = 0.836, two sided).

Language lateralization

Lateralization in comprehension and production

Laterality indices were calculated for the language network regions in each task and language using a threshold-free method, with values between +1 (left lateralization) and −1 (right lateralization). In both experiments, Wilcoxon signed-rank tests of paired samples revealed significant differences between each of the modalities. Comprehension and production displayed robust differences in lateralization, with significant differences between both reading and verbal production (Experiment I: W = 421, p = 0.000000000006; Experiment II: W = 1099, p = 0.0000000004) as well as between speech comprehension and verbal production (Experiment I: W = 824, p = 0.000002; Experiment II: W = 729.5, p = 0.000000000000006). Reading and speech comprehension also differed significantly (Experiment I: W = 1998.5, p = 0.021; Experiment II: W = 3840, p = 0.012). In reading and speech comprehension, lateralization was highly variable and indices spanned the full range of possible values between the two languages, while verbal production was clearly left lateralized. At the group level, comprehension appeared bilateral and production was left lateralized. This result was consistent across the cross-sectional and longitudinal experiments (Fig. 2).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

A, B, Laterality indices plotted as a function of Group, Language, and Modality in Experiment I (A), and Training, Language, and Modality in Experiment II (B). Laterality indices were obtained from individual whole-brain activation in the neuroanatomical language network, and the respective line graphs display the mean and SD of laterality indices across participants in each Modality and Language.

Learning-dependent changes in lateralization

To examine patterns of learning-dependent changes in lateralization while accounting for the high interindividual variability across tasks and languages, L1 lateralization was used as a baseline for each subject's Ln lateralization, and the linear association between L1 and Ln was assessed using Pearson's r. In lower-proficiency learners, L1 and Ln lateralized similarly, regardless of left/right lateralization. However, with increasing proficiency, this pattern reversed for comprehension, and L1 and Ln lateralized to opposite hemispheres. This learning-dependent change was not observed in verbal production (Fig. 3). Cohen's q was used to quantify the proficiency-dependent change in L1–Ln correlation for each task, confirming that, across both studies, learning-dependent change in lateralization was large in reading comprehension, medium in speech comprehension, and small in verbal production.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

A, B, Linear associations between L1 and Ln lateralization indices (Pearson's r) as a function of Group and Modality in Experiment I (A) and of Training and Modality in Experiment II (B). Cohen's q quantified the learning-dependent changes in L1–Ln correlation in each modality.

To examine whether increasing proficiency involved changes in hemispheric dominance for each modality, lateralized dissociation indices were calculated for each subject such that absolute values indicated the magnitude of L1–Ln difference, and direction (i.e., positive or negative) indicated whether the languages were lateralized to the same or opposite hemispheres (positive = opposite hemispheres, negative = same hemisphere). There was a significant proficiency-related increase in absolute dissociation between L1 and Ln lateralization across modalities (Experiment I: Mann–Whitney U tests: across modalities: W = 584.5, p = 0.013; reading comprehension: W = 41, p = 0.007; speech comprehension: W = 88, p = 0.579; verbal production: W = 53.5, p = 0.022; Experiment II: Wilcoxon signed-rank tests: across modalities: V = 374.5, p = 0.023; reading comprehension: V = 15, p = 0.004; speech comprehension: V = 53, p = 0.142; verbal production: V = 78, p = 0.330), and Cohen's d was used to quantify learning-dependent change in hemispheric dominance for each modality. In both experiments, the same pattern of changes emerged: large in reading comprehension, medium in speech comprehension, and small in verbal production (Fig. 4).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

A, B, Lateralized dissociation indices (LDIs) as a function of Group and Modality in Experiment I (A), and Training and Modality in Experiment II (B). Positive values indicate that L1 and Ln lateralized to opposite hemispheres, and negative values indicate that L1 and Ln lateralized to the same hemisphere. Cohen's d quantified the learning-dependent changes in LDI in each modality. Error bars represent SD.

Finally, modality-wise clustering of joint L1–Ln lateralization was plotted using 85% data ellipses to examine overlap between modalities. One-way multivariate ANOVAs (MANOVAs) and a joint distribution difference index were used to test and quantify the separation between the following: (1) comprehension (both reading and speech) and production (Fig. 5I); and (2) reading and speech comprehension (Fig. 5II). The effects of proficiency were tested using nonparametric two-sample/paired tests of difference/change in cluster separation between modalities (Euclidean distance) and quantified with the percentage change in the JDD. The one-way MANOVA modeled the joint L1–Ln distribution differences between modalities, and the index quantified this difference by taking into account the difference in both bivariate mean and spread of data, with values between 0 (overlapping distributions) and 1 (no similarities). MANOVAs revealed significant differences between comprehension and production (Experiment I: basic proficiency group: F(1.8,65.2) = 11.73, p = 0.0005; advanced proficiency group: F(1.9,63.8) = 22.96, p = 0.00000002; Experiment II: before training: F(1.7,73.2) = 21.67, p = 0.0000002, after training: F(1.7,70.7) = 38.94, p = 0.0000000000004), and with increasing proficiency, comprehension, and production dissociated further (Experiment I: the advanced proficiency group displayed 1042.35% greater comprehension–production dissociation than the basic proficiency group, Mann–Whitney U test of group difference in cluster separation: W = 67 398, p = 0.000000000003; Experiment II: participants displayed 47.38% increase in comprehension–production dissociation after training, Wilcoxon signed-rank test of post-training change in cluster separation: V = 101769, p = 0.0000000000000002). There were no significant differences in L1–Ln joint distribution between reading and speech comprehension (Experiment I: basic proficiency group: F(1.9,45.5) = 1.84, p = 0.18; advanced proficiency group: F(1.7,41.4) = 0.32, p = 0.71; Experiment II: before training: F(1.9,71.2) = 1.98, p = 0.15; after training: F(1.8,60.5) = 2.09, p = 0.13), and reading and speech comprehension converged further with increasing proficiency (Experiment I: the advanced group displayed 87.27% greater comprehension–production overlap than the basic group, Mann–Whitney U test of group difference in cluster separation: W = 18073, p = 0.177; Experiment II: participants displayed 27.13% increase in comprehension–production overlap after training, Wilcoxon signed-rank test of post-training change in cluster separation: V = 39 306, p = 0.0005).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

IA–IIB, Modality-wise clustering (comprehension vs production, I; reading vs speech comprehension, II) in joint distributions of L1–Ln lateralization indices plotted as a function of Group in Experiment I (A) and as a function of Training in Experiment II (B). A joint distribution difference index with values between 0 and 1 quantified overlap in each group, with higher values indicating larger separation between modalities. Asterisks represent statistically significant differences (p < 0.001).

Discussion

In the present work, we examined hemispheric specialization and learning-dependent plasticity of the language network concurrently in three language systems: reading, speech comprehension, and verbal production. We conducted cross-sectional and longitudinal fMRI experiments in separate populations of immersed language learners. Both experiment samples had the same L1 (Spanish), but were contrasted in other factors: (1) early language experience: monolingual versus sequential bilingual; (2) language being learned: Basque versus English; (3) phonological similarity with native language: high overlap versus low overlap; and (4) orthographic depth: transparent versus opaque. Across these contrasting experimental designs and participant groups, we found a highly consistent pattern of results in both experiments: (1) across native and non-native languages, lateralization for language comprehension was variable but language production was strongly left lateralized; and (2) with increasing non-native language proficiency, reading and speech comprehension displayed significant changes in hemispheric dominance (reading > speech), while verbal production remained left lateralized. The converging results from separate experiments provide unique insight into the long-standing debate on hemispheric specialization of language and the effects of language experience (Gainotti, 1993; Price, 1998, 2012; Jung-Beeman, 2005; Hickok and Poeppel, 2007; Friederici, 2012; Hervé et al., 2013; Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2017).

The first result showing variably lateralized (bilateral at the group-level) comprehension and left lateralized verbal production across different languages suggested that comprehension is flexible while verbal production is hardwired to be left lateralized. Previously, conflicting evidence from studies in monolinguals had led to a range of different conclusions and models of comprehension: from left lateralized to partly bilateral, bilateral, or right-lateralized function (Booth et al., 2000; Gaillard et al., 2003; Jung-Beeman, 2005; Hickok and Poeppel, 2007; Lidzba et al., 2011). Few studies have compared different modalities in the same participants, and, though lateralization was seen to be highly modality-dependent in the current study, it did not appear to depend on the exact task used, since lateralization for the single-word overt tasks in the current study was consistent with results from far more complex discourse-level covert tasks in previous studies (Dehaene et al., 1997; Lidzba et al., 2011; Bhattasali et al., 2019). There were also subtle differences between the two experiments, with similar Ln lateralization but differing central tendencies for L1 laterality. This pattern is consistent with the literature on the influence of early language experience: meta-analyses have found that early bilinguals (L2 acquired before age 6 years) typically show bilateral hemispheric involvement, while monolinguals and late bilinguals show greater left hemisphere dominance (Hull and Vaid, 2006, 2007; Bloch et al., 2009; Liu and Cao, 2016). Thus, the convergent results in the present work indicate that interindividual variability in lateralization for language comprehension is not an artifact of task or methodology, but that instead, language comprehension is differently lateralized across individuals. Lesion studies in children have found dissociative effects of lesion side on comprehension and production: while lesions in the left hemisphere were associated with more severe delays in production compared with comprehension, comprehension delays were more common—but not universal—in children with right hemisphere damage (Marchman et al., 1991; Bates, 1993; Thal et al., 1991). In line with these findings, developmental neuroimaging studies all found left-lateralized language production, but reached conflicting conclusions on comprehension, leading to a suggestion of differing maturational mechanisms for comprehension and production (Hervé et al., 2013). Clinical studies have recommended that both comprehension and production tasks be used in determining language lateralization for clinical purposes (Wilke et al., 2010; Lidzba et al., 2011; Vilasboas et al., 2017; Woodhead et al., 2018). Modality-dependent lateralization (i.e., variably lateralized comprehension vs left-lateralized production) could explain the long-standing conflicts among previous studies that used tasks tapping into different modalities and shed new light on the question of functional specialization for language.

Our analytic approach to examining learning-dependent changes in language lateralization built on the observed interindividual variability and used within-subject measures calculated with each subject's L1 as a baseline for their Ln. We used three measures—L1–Ln correlation, L1–Ln distance, and modality clustering—and quantified the change within each language system. These revealed that (1) L1 and Ln were similarly lateralized in lower-proficiency language learners and tended to dissociate with increasing Ln proficiency; (2) the change was largest in reading, smaller in speech comprehension, and smallest in verbal production; and (3) with increasing proficiency, comprehension, and production dissociated, while reading and speech comprehension converged.

Convergence and dissociation of neural activation for different languages and language systems has been of considerable research interest. Neuroimaging studies of language have by and large come to the conclusion that all languages do indeed recruit the same language regions, and that language experience modulates the amount of overlap, leading to the “convergence hypothesis” (Perani and Abutalebi, 2005; Gurunandan et al., 2019). The current study built on this finding, and characterized lateralization patterns for L1 and Ln within the common language network, finding that increasing Ln proficiency led to increasing dissociation in lateralization between the two languages. There has been much debate on whether language control in bilinguals is language specific or domain general, with mixed evidence (Hernández et al., 2013), and it is possible that, apart from any changes in the involvement of language control regions, the greater hemispheric separation of languages in more proficient nonmonolinguals also contributes to their improved language control. Future studies looking concurrently at dissociation within the language network and recruitment of language control regions are needed to test this idea. Comprehension and production also dissociated with increasing Ln proficiency. In lower-proficiency learners, there was lower separation between modalities, possibly indicating variable strategies of Ln access and variable activation profiles (Dehaene et al., 1997), but, as individuals attained higher proficiency, their activation profiles stabilized and became more uniform. Turning to the question of convergence between language systems, print–speech convergence has been considered a universal signature of native language proficiency (Shankweiler et al., 2008; Rueckl et al., 2015; Preston et al., 2016), but it is less well studied in multilinguals. In the current study, we found increasing convergence of joint L1–Ln lateralization for reading and speech comprehension with increasing language learning, suggesting that reading–speech convergence is also sensitive to increasing Ln proficiency.

The pattern of plasticity differences between the language systems (i.e., plasticity for reading > speech comprehension > verbal production) was strikingly similar to their perceived difficulty in real-world language learning in adults. Two observations support the idea that the differential plasticity of language systems contribute to differential learning. First, learners in the longitudinal study had switched languages from same to opposite hemispheres in reading within a relatively short time frame, while fewer did so for speech comprehension, and none for production. Further, individuals who had L1 and Ln lateralized in opposite hemispheres maintained this dissociation post-training, and individuals who had L1 and Ln in the same hemisphere tended to dissociate post-training to varying degrees depending on the modality. This suggested that opposite hemispheric dominance of languages could be advantageous for language learning, and, further, that shifts in hemispheric dominance are limited by the plasticity of the sensory/motor cortices corresponding to each language system. Neuropsychological evidence from stroke recovery patterns in adults who showed greater (but not complete) recovery in comprehension than in production (Lomas and Kertesz, 1978), as well as different reorganization patterns for comprehension and production (Musso et al., 1999; Heiss and Thiel, 2006) further supports our conclusion. Though the visual, auditory, and motor cortices are all bilateral, each of them exhibits hemispheric advantages for processing specific features (Benke and Kertesz, 1989; Deruelle and Fagot, 1997; Flinker et al., 2019; Albouy et al., 2020), and previous studies with monolinguals have found differences in visual lateralization of different writing scripts (Tzeng et al., 1979; Kuo et al., 2001), asymmetrical sensitivity of the auditory cortices (Friederici and Alter, 2004; Boemio et al., 2005), and left lateralization of auditory and articulatory motor areas (Morillon et al., 2010), pointing to differential potential for post-critical period plasticity of these sensory/motor regions that matches the converging pattern of language system plasticity found in the current study. Second, the adolescent learners in the second experiment displayed substantial neural changes after just 3 months of training, while the adult learners in the first experiment displayed similar neural differences for a much larger proficiency difference between groups. This finding is compatible with age-related decrease in neural plasticity and sheds further light on the source of the difficulty of late language learning. However, despite the convergence of the neural results in Experiments I and II, the modest behavioral effect in Experiment II limited any further interpretation of the neural changes in relation to behavioral outcomes at the individual level in naturalistic language learning. In sum, taken together with previous evidence, the converging findings in the present work point to the sensorimotor cortices playing a large role in both the lateralization of language as well as the asymmetric decrease in plasticity of the language network.

Methodological studies and reviews of language lateralization have often warned against the overinterpretation of results from a single task, small regions of interest, or nonrobust analytical methods (Bradshaw et al., 2017a,b; Bain et al., 2019). These were avoided in the current study, and interpretations were based on the robust patterns of results verified by corroborating analyses that were replicated in contrasting experiments. However, the current study used classical single-word tasks, and while the lateralization results were consistent with the findings from far more complex comprehension tasks (Dehaene et al., 1997; Lidzba et al., 2011; Bhattasali et al., 2019), future studies are needed to establish whether the results presented in the current study would be as or possibly even more pronounced in sentence/discourse processing (Hagoort, 2019). Further, a priori power analysis was not conducted, nor was a replication sample examined. The two experiments involved ecologically valid language learning, and the lateralization results were sensitive to participants' real-world language-learning progress (i.e., CEFR level) rather than their performance or improvement on the in-scanner semantic tasks involving high-frequency stimuli. In fact, while performance on the tasks was relatively uniform across participants, lateralization exhibited much larger variation in both languages, supporting the idea of multifactorial modulation of hemispheric specialization (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2017), since participants were carefully selected to control for language backgrounds as much as possible, but actual experimental control on early or previous language exposure was not possible. The replication of findings in language learners at different levels of proficiency suggested that the learning-dependent neural changes were not temporary, but further studies are necessary to disentangle the effects of learning versus proficiency, and test lateralization of languages in early balanced bilinguals. Finally, the two experiments featured distinct L1–Ln language pairs that were contrasted on factors such as overlap in language families, and phonological and orthographic properties, but did not involve more sensory differences such as visual differences between writing systems or auditory perception and motor production of tones, as in, say, English–Chinese. Following from our idea that the sensorimotor cortices are the limiting factor in language learning and its associated neural changes, it is possible that late acquisition of a language that requires greater sensorimotor learning would entail smaller proficiency-dependent neural changes in lateralization and the size of the changes would decrease more sharply with increasing age than in the current study.

In conclusion, our study design with cross-sectional and longitudinal experiments in contrasting samples of real-world language learners, testing of different language systems, and a multipronged analytical approach revealed robust and converging patterns of modality-dependent lateralization and plasticity of the language network. Our findings suggest that language lateralization for reading and speech comprehension is plastic well into adulthood, while production shows strong left hemisphere specialization. Plasticity for reading was greater than for speech comprehension, which was in turn greater than for verbal production. Together with previous evidence in the literature, we propose that hemispheric specialization for language may arise from the sensorimotor cortices, and that the differential plasticity of language systems is tied to the plasticity of the associated sensorimotor systems.

Footnotes

  • The authors declare no competing interests.

  • K.G. eceived support from “la Caixa” Foundation (ID 100010434) through the fellowship LCF/BQ/DI17/11620005 and from the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie Grant 713673. J.A.-T. was supported by Basque Government predoctoral Grant PRE_2015_1_028. M.C. was supported by project APCIN-2015-061-MultiLateral, which is funded by the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (MINECO; Grant FLAG-ERA JTC 2015). P.M.P.-A. was supported by MINECO Grants RYC-2014-15440 and PGC2018-093408-B-I00, and the Neuroscience Research Projects program from the Fundacion Tatiana Perez de Guzman el Bueno. The research was also supported by the Basque Government (Grant BERC 2018–2021) and the Spanish State Research Agency through the Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language Severo Ochoa excellence accreditation (Grant SEV-2015-0490). We thank Ana Fernández, Oihana Vadillo and Diego López-Zuazo (La Anunciata Ikastetxea, Donostia) for assistance with data collection.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Kshipra Gurunandan at k.gurunandan{at}bcbl.eu or Pedro M. Paz-Alonso at p.pazalonso{at}bcbl.eu

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Ahmad Z,
    2. Balsamo LM,
    3. Sachs BC,
    4. Xu B,
    5. Gaillard WD
    (2003) Auditory comprehension of language in young children: neural networks identified with fMRI. Neurology 60:1598–1605. doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000059865.32155.86
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Albouy P,
    2. Benjamin L,
    3. Morillon B,
    4. Zatorre RJ
    (2020) Distinct sensitivity to spectrotemporal modulation supports brain asymmetry for speech and melody. Science 367:1043–1047. doi:10.1126/science.aaz3468
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    1. Bain JS,
    2. Yeatman JD,
    3. Schurr R,
    4. Rokem A,
    5. Mezer AA
    (2019) Evaluating arcuate fasciculus laterality measurements across dataset and tractography pipelines. Hum Brain Mapp 40:3695–3711. doi:10.1002/hbm.24626 pmid:31106944
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Bates E
    (1993) Comprehension and production in early language development. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 58:222–242. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5834.1993.tb00403.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Bavelier D,
    2. Neville HJ
    (2002) Cross-modal plasticity: where and how? Nat Rev Neurosci 3:443–452. doi:10.1038/nrn848
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Benke T,
    2. Kertesz A
    (1989) Hemispheric mechanisms of motor speech. Aphasiology 3:627–641. doi:10.1080/02687038908249029
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  7. ↵
    1. Bhattasali S,
    2. Fabre M,
    3. Luh W-M,
    4. Al Saied H,
    5. Constant M,
    6. Pallier C,
    7. Brennan JR,
    8. Spreng RN,
    9. Hale J
    (2019) Localising memory retrieval and syntactic composition: an fMRI study of naturalistic language comprehension. Lang Cogn Neurosci 34:491–510. doi:10.1080/23273798.2018.1518533
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  8. ↵
    1. Bloch C,
    2. Kaiser A,
    3. Kuenzli E,
    4. Zappatore D,
    5. Haller S,
    6. Franceschini R,
    7. Luedi G,
    8. Radue E-W,
    9. Nitsch C
    (2009) The age of second language acquisition determines the variability in activation elicited by narration in three languages in Broca's and Wernicke's area. Neuropsychologia 47:625–633. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.009
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Boatman D,
    2. Freeman J,
    3. Vining E,
    4. Pulsifer M,
    5. Miglioretti D,
    6. Minahan R,
    7. Carson B,
    8. Brandt J,
    9. McKhann G
    (1999) Language recovery after left hemispherectomy in children with late‐onset seizures. Ann Neurol 46:579–586. doi:10.1002/1531-8249(199910)46:4<579::AID-ANA5>3.0.CO;2-K
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Boemio A,
    2. Fromm S,
    3. Braun A,
    4. Poeppel D
    (2005) Hierarchical and asymmetric temporal sensitivity in human auditory cortices. Nat Neurosci 8:389–395. doi:10.1038/nn1409
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Booth JR,
    2. MacWhinney B,
    3. Thulborn KR,
    4. Sacco K,
    5. Voyvodic JT,
    6. Feldman HM
    (2000) Developmental and lesion effects in brain activation during sentence comprehension and mental rotation. Dev Neuropsychol 18:139–169. doi:10.1207/S15326942DN1802_1
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Bradshaw AR,
    2. Bishop DV,
    3. Woodhead ZV
    (2017a) Methodological considerations in assessment of language lateralisation with fMRI: a systematic review. PeerJ 5:e3557. doi:10.7717/peerj.3557
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Bradshaw AR,
    2. Thompson PA,
    3. Wilson AC,
    4. Bishop DV,
    5. Woodhead ZV
    (2017b) Measuring language lateralisation with different language tasks: a systematic review. PeerJ 5:e3929. doi:10.7717/peerj.3929
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  14. ↵
    1. Broca P
    (1863) Localisation des fonctions cérébrales: siége de langage articulé. Bulletins de la Société d'Anthropologie de Paris 4:200–208.
    OpenUrl
  15. ↵
    1. Dale AM
    (1999) Optimal experimental design for event‐related fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp 8:109–114. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:2/3<109::AID-HBM7>3.0.CO;2-W
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Dax G
    (1863) Observations tendant à prouver la coïncidence constante des dérangements de la parole avec une lésion de l'hémisphère gauche du cerveau. C R Acad Sci III 56:534.
    OpenUrl
  17. ↵
    1. Dehaene-Lambertz G,
    2. Dehaene S,
    3. Hertz-Pannier L
    (2002) Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in infants. Science 298:2013–2015. doi:10.1126/science.1077066
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Dehaene S,
    2. Dupoux E,
    3. Mehler J,
    4. Cohen L,
    5. Paulesu E,
    6. Perani D,
    7. van de Moortele PF,
    8. Lehéricy S,
    9. Le Bihan D
    (1997) Anatomical variability in the cortical representation of first and second language. Neuroreport 8:3809–3815. doi:10.1097/00001756-199712010-00030
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Deruelle C,
    2. Fagot J
    (1997) Hemispheric lateralisation and global precedence effects in the processing of visual stimuli by humans and baboons (Papio papio). Laterality 2:233–246. doi:10.1080/135765097397468
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Duffau H,
    2. Denvil D,
    3. Capelle L
    (2002) Long term reshaping of language sensory and motor maps after glioma resection: a new parameter to integrate in the surgical strategy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 72:511–516. doi:10.1136/jnnp.72.4.511 pmid:11909913
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Duffau H,
    2. Capelle L,
    3. Denvil D,
    4. Sichez N,
    5. Gatignol P,
    6. Lopes M,
    7. Mitchell M-C,
    8. Sichez J-P,
    9. Van Effenterre R
    (2003) Functional recovery after surgical resection of low grade gliomas in eloquent brain: hypothesis of brain compensation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 74:901–907. doi:10.1136/jnnp.74.7.901
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Enge A,
    2. Friederici AD,
    3. Skeide MA
    (2020) A meta-analysis of fMRI studies of language comprehension in children. Neuroimage 215:116858. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116858
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  23. ↵
    1. Flinker A,
    2. Doyle WK,
    3. Mehta AD,
    4. Devinsky O,
    5. Poeppel D
    (2019) Spectrotemporal modulation provides a unifying framework for auditory cortical asymmetries. Nat Hum Behav 3:393–405. doi:10.1038/s41562-019-0548-z
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Friederici AD
    (2012) The cortical language circuit: from auditory perception to sentence comprehension. Trends Cogn Sci 16:262–268. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.001
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Friederici AD,
    2. Alter K
    (2004) Lateralization of auditory language functions: a dynamic dual pathway model. Brain Lang 89:267–276. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00351-1
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Gaillard WD,
    2. Sachs BC,
    3. Whitnah JR,
    4. Ahmad Z,
    5. Balsamo LM,
    6. Petrella JR,
    7. Braniecki SH,
    8. McKinney CM,
    9. Hunter K,
    10. Xu B,
    11. Grandin CB
    (2003) Developmental aspects of language processing: fMRI of verbal fluency in children and adults. Hum Brain Mapp 18:176–185. doi:10.1002/hbm.10091
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Gainotti G
    (1993) The riddle of the right hemisphere's contribution to the recovery of language. Int J Lang Commun Disord 28:227–246. doi:10.3109/13682829309060038
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Gurunandan K,
    2. Carreiras M,
    3. Paz-Alonso PM
    (2019) Functional plasticity associated with language learning in adults. Neuroimage 201:116040. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116040
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  29. ↵
    1. Hagoort P
    (2013) MUC (memory unification control) and beyond. Front Psychol 4:416.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Hagoort P
    (2019) The neurobiology of language beyond single-word processing. Science 366:55–58. doi:10.1126/science.aax0289
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    1. Heiss WD,
    2. Thiel A
    (2006) A proposed regional hierarchy in recovery of post-stroke aphasia. Brain Lang 98:118–123. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2006.02.002
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Hernández M,
    2. Martin CD,
    3. Barceló F,
    4. Costa A
    (2013) Where is the bilingual advantage in task-switching? J Mem Lang 69:257–276. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2013.06.004
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  33. ↵
    1. Hervé PY,
    2. Zago L,
    3. Petit L,
    4. Mazoyer B,
    5. Tzourio-Mazoyer N
    (2013) Revisiting human hemispheric specialization with neuroimaging. Trends Cogn Sci 17:69–80. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Hickok G,
    2. Poeppel D
    (2007) The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat Rev Neurosci 8:393–402. doi:10.1038/nrn2113
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Hope TMH,
    2. Leff AP,
    3. Prejawa S,
    4. Bruce R,
    5. Haigh Z,
    6. Lim L,
    7. Ramsden S,
    8. Oberhuber M,
    9. Ludersdorfer P,
    10. Crinion J,
    11. Seghier ML,
    12. Price CJ
    (2017) Right hemisphere structural adaptation and changing language skills years after left hemisphere stroke. Brain 140:1718–1728. doi:10.1093/brain/awx086
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Horowitz-Kraus T,
    2. Grainger M,
    3. DiFrancesco M,
    4. Vannest J,
    5. Holland SK
    (2015) Right is not always wrong: DTI and fMRI evidence for the reliance of reading comprehension on language-comprehension networks in the right hemisphere. Brain Imaging Behav 9:19–31. doi:10.1007/s11682-014-9341-9
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  37. ↵
    1. Hull R,
    2. Vaid J
    (2006) Laterality and language experience. Laterality 11:436–464. doi:10.1080/13576500600691162
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Hull R,
    2. Vaid J
    (2007) Bilingual language lateralization: a meta-analytic tale of two hemispheres. Neuropsychologia 45:1987–2008. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.002
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Jung-Beeman M
    (2005) Bilateral brain processes for comprehending natural language. Trends Cogn Sci 9:512–518. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.09.009
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Kaplan EF,
    2. Goodglass H,
    3. Weintraub S
    (1983) The Boston naming test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.
  41. ↵
    1. Kleim JA,
    2. Jones TA
    (2008) Principles of experience-dependent neural plasticity: implications for rehabilitation after brain damage. J Speech Lang Hear Res 51:S225–S239. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2008/018)
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Kovelman I,
    2. Shalinsky MH,
    3. Berens MS,
    4. Petitto LA
    (2008) Shining new light on the brain's “bilingual signature”: a functional near infrared spectroscopy investigation of semantic processing. Neuroimage 39:1457–1471. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.017
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Kuo WJ,
    2. Yeh TC,
    3. Duann JR,
    4. Wu YT,
    5. Ho LT,
    6. Hung D,
    7. Hsieh JC
    (2001) A left-lateralized network for reading Chinese words: a 3 T fMRI study. Neuroreport 12:3997–4001. doi:10.1097/00001756-200112210-00029
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Lidzba K,
    2. Schwilling E,
    3. Grodd W,
    4. Krägeloh-Mann I,
    5. Wilke M
    (2011) Language comprehension vs language production: age effects on fMRI activation. Brain Lang 119:6–15. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2011.02.003
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Liu H,
    2. Cao F
    (2016) L1 and L2 processing in the bilingual brain: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Brain Lang 159:60–73.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  46. ↵
    1. Lomas J,
    2. Kertesz A
    (1978) Patterns of spontaneous recovery in aphasic groups: a study of adult stroke patients. Brain Lang 5:388–401. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(78)90034-2
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Marchman VA,
    2. Miller R,
    3. Bates EA
    (1991) Babble and first words in children with focal brain injury. Appl Psycholinguist 12:1–22. doi:10.1017/S0142716400009358
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  48. ↵
    1. Mårtensson J,
    2. Eriksson J,
    3. Bodammer NC,
    4. Lindgren M,
    5. Johansson M,
    6. Nyberg L,
    7. Lövdén M
    (2012) Growth of language-related brain areas after foreign language learning. Neuroimage 63:240–244. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.043
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Mazaika PK,
    2. Hoeft F,
    3. Glover GH,
    4. Reiss AL
    (2009) Methods and software for fMRI analysis of clinical subjects. Neuroimage 47:S58. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(09)70238-1
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  50. ↵
    1. Morillon B,
    2. Lehongre K,
    3. Frackowiak RS,
    4. Ducorps A,
    5. Kleinschmidt A,
    6. Poeppel D,
    7. Giraud AL
    (2010) Neurophysiological origin of human brain asymmetry for speech and language. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:18688–18693. doi:10.1073/pnas.1007189107
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  51. ↵
    1. Musso M,
    2. Weiller C,
    3. Kiebel S,
    4. Müller SP,
    5. Bülau P,
    6. Rijntjes M
    (1999) Training-induced brain plasticity in aphasia. Brain 122:1781–1790. doi:10.1093/brain/122.9.1781
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Papanicolaou AC,
    2. Moore BD,
    3. Levin HS,
    4. Eisenberg HM
    (1987) Evoked potential correlates of right hemisphere involvement in language recovery following stroke. Arch Neurol 44:521–524. doi:10.1001/archneur.1987.00520170049021
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Park HR,
    2. Badzakova-Trajkov G,
    3. Waldie KE
    (2012) Language lateralisation in late proficient bilinguals: a lexical decision fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 50:688–695. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.005
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  54. ↵
    1. Payne BR,
    2. Lomber SG
    (2001) Reconstructing functional systems after lesions of cerebral cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:911–919. doi:10.1038/35104085
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Penny WD,
    2. Friston KJ,
    3. Ashburner JT,
    4. Kiebel SJ,
    5. Nichols TE
    (2011) Statistical parametric mapping: the analysis of functional brain images. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
  56. ↵
    1. Perani D,
    2. Abutalebi J
    (2005) The neural basis of first and second language processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15:202–206. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.007
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Perani D,
    2. Saccuman MC,
    3. Scifo P,
    4. Anwander A,
    5. Awander A,
    6. Spada D,
    7. Baldoli C,
    8. Poloniato A,
    9. Lohmann G,
    10. Friederici AD
    (2011) Neural language networks at birth. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:16056–16061. doi:10.1073/pnas.1102991108
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. ↵
    1. Preston JL,
    2. Molfese PJ,
    3. Frost SJ,
    4. Mencl WE,
    5. Fulbright RK,
    6. Hoeft F,
    7. Landi N,
    8. Shankweiler D,
    9. Pugh KR
    (2016) Print-speech convergence predicts future reading outcomes in early readers. Psychol Sci 27:75–84. doi:10.1177/0956797615611921
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Price CJ
    (1998) The functional anatomy of word comprehension and production. Trends Cogn Sci 2:281–288. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01201-7
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Price CJ
    (2012) A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech spoken language and reading. Neuroimage 62:816–847. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.062
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Rueckl JG,
    2. Paz-Alonso PM,
    3. Molfese PJ,
    4. Kuo W-J,
    5. Bick A,
    6. Frost SJ,
    7. Hancock R,
    8. Wu DH,
    9. Mencl WE,
    10. Duñabeitia JA,
    11. Lee J-R,
    12. Oliver M,
    13. Zevin JD,
    14. Hoeft F,
    15. Carreiras M,
    16. Tzeng OJL,
    17. Pugh KR,
    18. Frost R
    (2015) Universal brain signature of proficient reading: evidence from four contrasting languages. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112:15510–15515. doi:10.1073/pnas.1509321112
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  62. ↵
    1. Schlegel AA,
    2. Rudelson JJ,
    3. Tse PU
    (2012) White matter structure changes as adults learn a second language. J Cogn Neurosci 24:1664–1670. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00240
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Shankweiler D,
    2. Mencl WE,
    3. Braze D,
    4. Tabor W,
    5. Pugh KR,
    6. Fulbright RK
    (2008) Reading differences and brain: cortical integration of speech and print in sentence processing varies with reader skill. Dev Neuropsychol 33:745–775. doi:10.1080/87565640802418688
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Skeide MA,
    2. Friederici AD
    (2016) The ontogeny of the cortical language network. Nat Rev Neurosci 17:323–332. doi:10.1038/nrn.2016.23
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  65. ↵
    1. Szaflarski JP,
    2. Holland SK,
    3. Schmithorst VJ,
    4. Byars AW
    (2006) fMRI study of language lateralization in children and adults. Hum Brain Mapp 27:202–212. doi:10.1002/hbm.20177
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Thal DJ,
    2. Marchman V,
    3. Stiles J,
    4. Aram D,
    5. Trauner D,
    6. Nass R,
    7. Bates E
    (1991) Early lexical development in children with focal brain injury. Brain Lang 40:491–527. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(91)90145-Q
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Tzeng OJ,
    2. Hung DL,
    3. Cotton B,
    4. Wang WS
    (1979) Visual lateralisation effect in reading Chinese characters. Nature 282:499–501. doi:10.1038/282499a0
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Tzourio-Mazoyer N,
    2. Landeau B,
    3. Papathanassiou D,
    4. Crivello F,
    5. Etard O,
    6. Delcroix N,
    7. Mazoyer B,
    8. Joliot M
    (2002) Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single- subject brain. Neuroimage 15:273–289. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0978
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Tzourio-Mazoyer N,
    2. Perrone-Bertolotti M,
    3. Jobard G,
    4. Mazoyer B,
    5. Baciu M
    (2017) Multi-factorial modulation of hemispheric specialization and plasticity for language in healthy and pathological conditions: a review. Cortex 86:314–339. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2016.05.013
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  70. ↵
    1. Vigneau M,
    2. Beaucousin V,
    3. Hervé P-Y,
    4. Jobard G,
    5. Petit L,
    6. Crivello F,
    7. Mellet E,
    8. Zago L,
    9. Mazoyer B,
    10. Tzourio-Mazoyer N
    (2011) What is right-hemisphere contribution to phonological lexico-semantic and sentence processing?: Insights from a meta-analysis. Neuroimage 54:577–593. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.036
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Vilasboas T,
    2. Herbet G,
    3. Duffau H
    (2017) Challenging the myth of right nondominant hemisphere: lessons from corticosubcortical stimulation mapping in awake surgery and surgical implications. World Neurosurg 103:449–456. doi:10.1016/j.wneu.2017.04.021
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  72. ↵
    1. Vingerhoets G,
    2. Van Borsel J,
    3. Tesink C,
    4. van den Noort M,
    5. Deblaere K,
    6. Seurinck R,
    7. Vandemaele P,
    8. Achten E
    (2003) Multilingualism: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 20:2181–2196. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.029
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Wilke M,
    2. Lidzba K
    (2007) LI-tool: a new toolbox to assess lateralization in functional MR-data. J Neurosci Methods 163:128–136. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.01.026
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Wilke M,
    2. Pieper T,
    3. Lindner K,
    4. Dushe T,
    5. Holthausen H,
    6. Krägeloh-Mann I
    (2010) Why one task is not enough: functional MRI for atypical language organization in two children. Eur J Paediatr Neurol 14:474–478. doi:10.1016/j.ejpn.2010.05.002
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Woodhead ZV,
    2. Rutherford HA,
    3. Bishop DV
    (2018) Measurement of language laterality using functional transcranial Doppler ultrasound: a comparison of different tasks. Wellcome Open Res 3:104. doi:10.12688/wellcomeopenres.14720.2
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  76. ↵
    1. Xiang H,
    2. van Leeuwen TM,
    3. Dediu D,
    4. Roberts L,
    5. Norris DG,
    6. Hagoort P
    (2015) L2-proficiency-dependent laterality shift in structural connectivity of brain language pathways. Brain Connect 5:349–361. doi:10.1089/brain.2013.0199
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 40 (50)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 40, Issue 50
9 Dec 2020
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Converging Evidence for Differential Specialization and Plasticity of Language Systems
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Converging Evidence for Differential Specialization and Plasticity of Language Systems
Kshipra Gurunandan, Jaione Arnaez-Telleria, Manuel Carreiras, Pedro M. Paz-Alonso
Journal of Neuroscience 9 December 2020, 40 (50) 9715-9724; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0851-20.2020

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Converging Evidence for Differential Specialization and Plasticity of Language Systems
Kshipra Gurunandan, Jaione Arnaez-Telleria, Manuel Carreiras, Pedro M. Paz-Alonso
Journal of Neuroscience 9 December 2020, 40 (50) 9715-9724; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0851-20.2020
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • bilingualism
  • comprehension
  • hemispheric specialization
  • language learning
  • laterality
  • production

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Mechanisms of Dominant Electrophysiological Features of Four Subtypes of Layer 1 Interneurons
  • Neural index of reinforcement learning predicts improved stimulus-response retention under high working memory load
  • Activity-dependent Nr4a2 induction modulates synaptic expression of AMPA receptors and plasticity via a Ca2+/CRTC1/CREB pathway
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Neural index of reinforcement learning predicts improved stimulus-response retention under high working memory load
  • Random Tactile Noise Stimulation Reveals Beta-Rhythmic Impulse Response Function of the Somatosensory System
  • Accelerating maturation of spatial memory systems by experience – evidence from sleep oscillation signatures of memory processing
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.