Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Systems/Circuits

Blockage of NMDA- and GABA(A) Receptors Improves Working Memory Selectivity of Primate Prefrontal Neurons

Paul Rodermund, Stephanie Westendorff and Andreas Nieder
Journal of Neuroscience 12 February 2020, 40 (7) 1527-1537; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2009-19.2019
Paul Rodermund
Animal Physiology, Institute of Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, D-72076 Tübingen, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Stephanie Westendorff
Animal Physiology, Institute of Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, D-72076 Tübingen, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Andreas Nieder
Animal Physiology, Institute of Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, D-72076 Tübingen, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Andreas Nieder
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The ongoing activity of prefrontal neurons after a stimulus has disappeared is considered a neuronal correlate of working memory. It depends on the delicate but poorly understood interplay between excitatory glutamatergic and inhibitory GABAergic receptor effects. We administered the NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801 and the GABA(A) receptor antagonist bicuculline methiodide while recording cellular activity in PFC of male rhesus monkeys performing a delayed decision task requiring working memory. The blockade of GABA(A) receptors strongly improved the selectivity of the neurons' delay activity, causing an increase in signal-to-noise ratio during working memory periods as well as an enhancement of the neurons' coding selectivity. The blockade of NMDA receptors resulted in a slight enhancement of selectivity and encoding capacity of the neurons. Our findings emphasize the delicate and more complex than expected interplay of excitatory and inhibitory transmitter systems in modulating working memory coding in prefrontal circuits.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Ongoing delay activity of prefrontal neurons constitutes a neuronal correlate of working memory. However, how this delay activity is generated by the delicate interplay of synaptic excitation and inhibition is unknown. We probed the effects of excitatory neurotransmitter glutamate and inhibitory neurotransmitter GABA in regulating delay activity in rhesus monkeys performing a delayed decision task requiring working memory. Surprisingly, the blockade of both glutamatergic NMDA and GABA(A) receptors improved neuronal selectivity of delay activity, causing an increase in neuronal signal-to-noise ratio. Moreover, individual neurons were similarly affected by blockade of both receptors. This emphasizes the delicate and more complex than expected interplay of excitatory and inhibitory transmitter systems in modulating working memory coding in prefrontal circuits.

  • GABA
  • glutamate
  • iontophoresis
  • pharmacology
  • PFC
  • working memory

Introduction

Working memory, the ability to retain, manipulate, and recall information after ceasing of its sensory trace, is a vital cognitive function for everyday behavior. Lesion studies in nonhuman primates have established that the PFC is obligatory for working memory in delayed response tasks (Blum, 1952; Mishkin, 1957). A physiological correlate of working memory is persistent activity of neurons in the PFC after removal of the sensory stimulus (Goldman-Rakic, 1996; Dash et al., 2007; Fuster, 2015) and maintenance of behavioral principles (Miller et al., 2003; Vallentin et al., 2012; Eiselt and Nieder, 2013). The persistent activity of these neurons is stimulus specific, commonly leading to elevated responses to a preferred memorandum and inhibitory responses to nonpreferred ones (Goldman-Rakic, 1999). It has been suggested that the persistent activity of excitatory glutamatergic pyramidal cells found in cortical layer III of the lateral PFC of nonhuman primates arises from recurrent excitation of neurons tuned to the same memorandum (Goldman-Rakic, 1995). These pyramidal cells are thought to preserve response specificity by lateral inhibition from interneurons (Goldman-Rakic, 1995; Arnsten et al., 2012).

Glutamate and GABA are the most prominent excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmitters, respectively, in the PFC (Dash et al., 2007). One of the important glutamate receptors is the NMDA receptor. At a behavioral level, systemic and local administration of antagonists of NMDA and GABA receptors disrupted working memory performance in rodents and nonhuman primates (Sawaguchi et al., 1989; Cole et al., 1993; Chrobak et al., 2008; Smith et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2013; Auger and Floresco, 2014).

NMDA receptors play a major role in working memory activity (Wang, 1999; Skoblenick and Everling, 2012; Wang et al., 2013). Such effects of NMDA receptors could be mediated by NMDA receptor subtype 2B subunits (NR2B) that show slow gaiting kinetics and saturation properties well suited for causing sustained elevated firing rates (Wang, 1999, 2001, 2002; Compte et al., 2000; Wang et al., 2013; Wang and Arnsten, 2015). However, the effects of general NMDA antagonists, such as MK-801 (MK), are contradictory. Iontophoretic administration of MK in the PFC of behaving monkeys reduced task-related firing, but not spontaneous activity per se (Wang et al., 2013). While some studies with rodents found that MK preferentially blocked receptors on interneurons, others found stronger effects on pyramidal cells (Jackson et al., 2004; Homayoun and Moghaddam, 2007; Rotaru et al., 2011). As the excitatory effects of MK were only seen after systemic but not after local injection of NMDA antagonists in the rodent PFC, it has been suggested that they are based on blockage of inhibitory interneurons in other brain areas that project to the PFC (Suzuki et al., 2002; Lorrain et al., 2003; Skoblenick and Everling, 2012).

Inhibitory effects of GABA are also thought to play a role in coding working memory and attention (Funahashi et al., 1989; Compte et al., 2000; Yoon et al., 2016; Auger and Floresco, 2017; Bast et al., 2017). Bicuculline methiodide (Bic) is a common GABA(A) antagonist that reduced the tuning of task related neurons in the PFC by elevating the firing rate to nonpreferred stimuli and decreasing signal-to-noise ratio (Rao et al., 2000). However, the behavioral effects elicited by GABA antagonists are not consistent. For example, studies with rodents have shown that GABA antagonists in some cases did not impair working memory, memory acquisition, or retention learning, and had a supportive effect on memory consolidation (Luft et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2012; Auger and Floresco, 2014; Farahmandfar et al., 2017).

To investigate the effects of glutamate and GABA on neuronal response properties in the PFC, we iontophoretically blocked NMDA and GABA(A) receptors while recording from PFC neurons of macaques performing a perceptual decision task. In this task, the monkeys had to decide and memorize over a delay period whether or not they had seen a stimulus displayed with different near-threshold intensities (Merten and Nieder, 2012). Based on the previous literature, we hypothesized that both MK and Bic would decrease the coding capacity of delay-selective PFC neurons. However, we observed the opposite effect.

Materials and Methods

Subjects and surgery.

We trained 2 male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) on a delayed perceptual decision task. Monkey Q was 7 years old and weighed 8.5 kg; Monkey Z was 9 years old and weighed 8.1 kg. Monkeys were implanted with titanium head posts for head fixation and a recording chamber above the right lateral PFC, centered over the principal sulcus (see Fig. 1d). Surgery was conducted using aseptic techniques under general anesthesia. Structural MRI was performed before implantation to locate anatomical landmarks. All procedures were authorized by the relevant authority, the Regierungspräsidium Tübingen, Germany.

Experimental design.

Monkeys were trained to report the presence or absence of a visual stimulus by responding to an associated set of response rule-cues varying in color or shape (see Fig. 1a). The sample stimulus consisted of a gray circle (1.5° of visual angle) presented at six intensity levels. Intensity levels were individually adapted to the monkeys: three sample stimuli were salient and three sample stimuli were around the perceptual threshold of the monkey (see Fig. 1c). Intensity of threshold sample stimuli for Monkey Q were slightly reduced after 14 of 70 recording days to ensure they were still around perceptual threshold. Stimulus intensities are depicted in RGB values, stimulus intensity levels in ordinal numbers, with lower values representing lower stimulus intensities. Given values do not represent actual physical intensity levels of the presented stimuli.

Throughout a trial, the monkeys fixated on a central white fixation spot (0.1° of visual angle) and kept their gaze within 3° of visual angle. Eye movements were monitored with an infrared eye-tracking system (ISCAN). CORTEX program (National Institute of Mental Health) was used for experimental control and behavioral data acquisition.

The animals initiated each experimental trial by grasping a lever and fixating a central fixation target (fixation period). After 500 ms, a visual stimulus (gray circle) was displayed for 500 ms in half of the trials (stimulus period); in the other half, no stimulus was shown. Both trial types were randomly intermixed. After the delay period (2000 ms), a response-rule cue was presented. Two response-rule cues were associated with the presence of a sample stimulus (red square or gray triangle) and two response-rule cues with its absence (blue square or gray cross). If a sample stimulus had been presented, a red square or a gray triangle as response-rule cue required the monkey to release the lever within 1200 ms to receive a fluid reward, whereas a blue square or gray cross as response-rule cue required the monkey to keep fixation and hold on to the lever for another 1200 ms. The associated response rule-cues applied in the inverse way if no sample stimulus had been presented (see Fig. 1b).

Electrophysiology.

We performed extracellular single-cell recordings in the right lateral PFC centered on the principal sulcus. In each recording session, up to three custom-made electrode-pipette combinations were inserted transduraly using a modified electrical microdrive (NAN Instruments) (Jacob et al., 2013). Neurons were recorded at random; no attempt was made to preselect neurons according to particular response properties. Signal acquisition, amplification, filtering, and digitalization were accomplished with the MAP system (Plexon). Waveform separation was performed offline (Offline Sorter; Plexon).

Iontophoresis.

MK and Bic were applied iontophoretically (MVCS iontophoresis system; npi electronic) using custom-made tungsten-in-glass electrodes flanked by two pipettes each (Thiele et al., 2006). Electrode impedances were usually ∼1 mΩ, full range 0.2–6.4 mΩ (measured at 500 Hz; Omega Tip Z; World Precision Instruments). Pipette resistances depended on the pipette opening diameter, drug, and solvent used. Typical resistances were between 15 and 60 mΩ, full range 10–168 mΩ.

Retention currents were −7 nA for both drugs. Ejection currents for MK (0.01 mol/L in double-distilled water, pH 3.8 with HCl; Sigma Millipore) were usually at 25 nA, full range 15–25 nA, which was comparable with Wang et al. (2013). Ejection currents for Bic (0.002 mol/L in double-distilled water, pH 3.9 with HCl; Sigma Millipore) were usually at 15 nA, full range 15–25 nA, which was analogous to Rao et al. (2000). If only one drug was administered per electrode, the other flanking pipette was filled with 0.9% NaCl, pH 7. Electrode impedance and pipette resistance were measured after each recording session.

Trial blocks without drugs alternated with trial blocks during which drugs were continuously applied. Both control and drug blocks lasted between 8 and 26 min, depending on the time the monkeys needed to reach a sufficient number of correct trials. The first block and all odd-number blocks were control conditions; in the even-number blocks, the drug was administered. In the subset of neurons that were tested with both MK and Bic in sequence, the washout period was the duration of the control blocks (i.e., between 8 and 26 min).

In previous iontophoretic experiments using exactly the same apparatus and methods, we have ensured that neuronal effects are not caused by positive ejection currents (Jacob et al., 2013; Ott et al., 2014). In such control experiments with 0.9% physiological NaCl and ejection currents of 25 nA (as used here), or even higher values of 50 nA, none of the tested neuronal responses (either spontaneous activity or any of the selective responses) was affected by ejection currents alone (Jacob et al., 2013; Ott et al., 2014). In addition, we show here that application of MK and Bic resulted in opposite effects on the neurons' spontaneous firing rates, even though both drugs were ejected by the same amounts of positive currents. Together, this confirms that the effects observed in the present study were caused by pharmacological receptor blockade.

Statistical analysis.

Data analysis was performed using MATLAB (The MathWorks). All significance levels were α = 0.05. Behavioral performance was assessed by signal detection theory, classifying the monkeys' responses as correct (hits and correct rejections) or wrong (misses and false alarms). To obtain psychometric detection curves, we calculated the ratio of correct to wrong responses for each stimulus intensity. We compared psychometric performance using two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs with main factors drug condition and stimulus intensity. The amount of aborted trials was compared across drug conditions using Wilcoxon signed-rank tests for paired data.

For neuronal analyses, we sorted spikes offline and studied the responses of all well-isolated neurons. Neurons that had a mean firing rate >1 Hz and were recorded for at least 8 trials each when the stimulus was present and absent during control and at least one drug condition were analyzed. For spike density population plots, spike rates were normalized by subtracting the mean baseline firing rate in control trials and dividing by the respective SD.

Neurons were classified as either broad-spiking (BS) or narrow-spiking (NS) cells (i.e., putative pyramidal cells or interneurons) with a linear classifier (k-means, k = 2, squared Euclidean distance) (Diester and Nieder, 2008). Only cells that had a downward deflection in voltage before an upward deflection were classified. More precisely, the minimum of the extracted waveform had to occur between 200 and 400 μs and the maximum after >300 μs. A total of 49 of 281 units did not fulfill these criteria. Waveforms were normalized by their difference between maximum and minimum voltage deflection and aligned to their minimum.

To examine drug effects on spontaneous firing rates, we compared firing rate of these neurons during the fixation period between control and respective drug condition using a Wilcoxon signed-rank test for paired data. To investigate modulation of spontaneous firing rates at the transition from control to drug phase, we normalized baseline firing rates by dividing with the mean firing rate during control condition. All reported neuronal analyses are based on correct trials only.

As most neurons did not have an elevated firing rate throughout the whole delay period, we used a sliding-window approach. We calculated spike densities by convolving each spike with a Gaussian kernel (σ = 50 ms). Thus, 95% of the area under the Gaussian correspond to a window size of 196 ms. The obtained spike density functions were sampled with 10 ms resolution. Next, we calculated two-way ANOVAs with the main factors stimulus condition (present/absent) and drug condition (control/drug) for each 10 ms bin of the spike densities in the delay phase. To ensure that neurons were no longer responsive to presentation of the sample stimulus, we excluded the first 100 ms of the delay. Cells that selectively responded to the stimulus condition for at least 300 consecutive milliseconds entered further analyses. In other words, the null hypothesis had to be rejected for at least 30 tests in a row. The longest selective time span was used as the analysis window for the respective neuron.

We quantified selectivity of the stimulus-selective neurons (defined by the procedure described above) using receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis on the firing rates of the neuron for stimulus absent and present trials (Green and Swets, 1966). The area under the ROC curve (AUROC) depicts the discriminability of two distributions, where 1 indicates perfect discriminability and 0.5 signals no separation. Stimulus present and absent conditions were labeled as preferred or nonpreferred based on the respective AUROC values. If a neuron fired more strongly to the stimulus present condition (resulting in an AUROC value > 0.5), the stimulus present condition was the preferred condition. However, if a neuron responded more strongly to the stimulus absent condition, the stimulus absent condition was the preferred condition. For drug trials, we kept the same analysis window and labels regarding stimulus preference as for control trials and calculated AUROC values again. AUROC values in control and drug condition were compared with a paired t test.

We calculated mean firing rates of stimulus-selective neurons during their respective analysis window and compared drug effects on stimulus absence- and presence-preferring neurons with two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs with drug condition and stimulus intensity as main factors. Next, we compared mean firing rates and Fano factors for preferred and nonpreferred stimulus conditions across drug conditions with paired t tests (Fano, 1947). Mean firing rate differences between control and drug condition were calculated separately for preferred and nonpreferred stimulus conditions and also compared with a paired t test.

The ROC analysis was repeated for those neurons that were tested in control and both drug (MK and Bic) conditions (n = 53). We used a binomial test to examine whether the amount of neurons that increased their AUROC values with administration of either drug was expected by the amount of neurons that increased their AUROC values with one of the drugs.

Results

We investigated 2 rhesus monkeys trained on a delayed perceptual decision task. At the beginning of a trial, either a sample stimulus of varying intensity was flashed or no sample stimulus was shown (stimulus absent trials) (Fig. 1a). The monkeys had to decide whether or not they had seen a stimulus. Intensity levels of the sample stimuli were individually adapted to the monkeys such that three sample stimuli were salient (stimulus present trials) and three sample stimuli were around the perceptual threshold (stimulus threshold trials). This challenged the monkeys' stimulus present/stimulus absent judgments and forced subjective decisions. The monkeys maintained their decision in working memory to later report their decisions as a function of subsequent response-rule cues. Importantly, the response-rule cues informed the monkeys whether or not a motor act was required depending on their memorized decisions (Fig. 1b). Neuronal activity in the delay period was thus working memory-related and dissociated from potential motor preparation.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Behavioral protocol, performance, and recording site. a, After grasping a lever and holding fixation for 500 ms, a sample stimulus of varying contrast was flashed for 500 ms in 50% of the trials (top branch), and no stimulus was shown in the remaining 50% (bottom branch). After a delay of 2000 ms, one of four target stimuli was shown and, depending on whether or not a sample stimulus was presented, either instructed the monkey to release the bar or to withhold a response. b, Instructed response rules (left column) with respect to stimulus condition (middle and right column) by the respective rule cues. c, Psychometric detection curves for Monkey Q (top, 70 sessions) and Monkey Z (bottom, 59 sessions) subdivided in control (Cntl), Bic, and MK trials. d, Lateral view of a macaque monkey brain. Circled area represents the area of extracellular recording and iontophoresis targeting GABA(A) and NMDA receptors at the principal sulcus.

The monkeys' behavioral performance was classified according to signal detection theory. Hits and correct rejections were rewarded, whereas misses and false alarms were not reinforced. Not reinforcing misses of stimuli that were presented around perceptual threshold leads to a small bias of the monkeys to erroneously report the presence of a stimulus in some of the stimulus absent trials. Both monkeys were able to detect the salient stimuli in >90% of the cases and correctly rejected >80% of the trials in which no stimulus was shown. Psychometric detection curves are depicted in Figure 1c.

While the monkeys performed the task, we recorded 281 single neurons from the lateral PFC (118 from Monkey Q and 163 from Monkey Z; Fig. 1d). These neurons were classified into NS and BS neurons based on their waveform characteristics (Diester and Nieder, 2008; Viswanathan and Nieder, 2015). During recordings, trial blocks without pharmacological manipulation (control condition) alternated with blocks in which either MK or Bic was applied to the vicinity of the recorded cells by iontophoresis (drug condition). A total of 186 neurons fulfilled the criteria in control and MK conditions, 193 neurons in control and Bic conditions, and 98 neurons in control and both drug conditions.

We first explored the potential effects of the drugs on the monkeys' behavior. Administration of either drug improved psychometric performance for Monkey Q (F(2) = 3.75, p = 0.029, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA), but not for Monkey Z (F(2) = 0.49, p = 0.614, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA) (Fig. 1c). In addition, both monkeys aborted significantly more trials in both drug conditions, independent of the respective stimulus intensity. Specifically, Monkey Q aborted 43.80% of all trials during MK administration, compared with 20.26% during respective control trials (Z = −6.27, p < 0.001, paired Wilcoxon signed-rank test), and 42.31% during Bic administration, compared with 20.55% during respective control trials (Z = −6.27, p < 0.001, paired Wilcoxon signed-rank test). Similarly, Monkey Z aborted 42.96% of all trials during MK administration, compared with 20.51% during respective control trials (Z = −6.15, p < 0.001, paired Wilcoxon signed-rank test), and 41.84% during Bic administration, compared with 20.71% during respective control trials (Z = −6.21, p < 0.001, paired Wilcoxon signed-rank test).

NMDA receptor blockade reduced spontaneous firing rate, whereas GABA(A) receptor blockade increased it

We first evaluated the general effect of MK and Bic on the PFC neurons' spontaneous firing rates. Application of MK slightly decreased spontaneous firing rates (Z = −2.41, p = 0.016, paired Wilcoxon signed-rank test), whereas Bic significantly increased them (Z = 7.39, p < 0.001, paired Wilcoxon signed-rank test) (Fig. 2c,d). We next separated the neurons into broad-spiking (i.e., putative pyramidal cells, BS) and narrow-spiking cells (i.e., putative interneurons, NS) based on the waveform of the extracellularly measured spikes (Fig. 2a,b; see Materials and Methods). The effects of MK and Bic were similar for the subpopulation of BS and NS. BS tended to decrease their firing rate with application of MK (n = 106, Z = −1.87, p = 0.061, paired Wilcoxon signed-rank test); NS were not modulated by MK (n = 46, Z = −1.61, p = 0.107, paired Wilcoxon signed-rank test). Bic increased firing rates for BS (n = 115, Z = 6.35, p < 0.001, paired Wilcoxon signed-rank test) as well as for NS (n = 47, Z = 4.42, p < 0.001, paired Wilcoxon signed-rank test). The number of BS and NS do not add up to the total of 186 neurons analyzed for MK, and the total of 193 neurons analyzed for Bic, because the waveform of some neurons could not be classified as neither broad nor narrow and were excluded.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Waveform separation and drug effects on spontaneous firing rate. a, Waveforms of recorded neurons. b, Distribution of BS and NS neurons. c, d, Spontaneous firing rate during the fixation period in control and drug conditions: c, MK, 186 neurons; d, Bic, 193 neurons. e, Normalized spontaneous firing rate for BS and NS neurons for the last 50 correct trials in control condition before drug on switch and the first 50 correct trials after drug on switch.

Next, we examined the time course of modulation of firing rates at the transition from control to drug phases (Fig. 2e). In the pooled data, drug effects of MK are no longer significant. The continuously increasing firing rate enhancement caused by Bic is clearly visible, and was stronger for BS compared with NS.

NMDA and GABA(A) receptor blockade each improved selectivity to preferred stimulus condition

Many of our recorded neurons showed selective activity either for stimulus present trials or stimulus absent trials. We used a sliding window ANOVA to assess selectivity of the neurons to stimulus condition in the delay period (see Materials and Methods) and refer to the stimulus condition that caused a significantly elevated firing rate as the preferred stimulus condition, whereas the other condition is referred to as the nonpreferred stimulus condition. Of 186 neurons recorded in the MK condition, 72 were stimulus-selective; of those, 46 preferred stimulus presence and 26 preferred stimulus absence. Likewise, of the 193 neurons recorded under the Bic conditions, 83 were stimulus-selective; of those, 56 preferred stimulus presence and 27 preferred stimulus absence. Of the 98 neurons recorded in both drug conditions, 53 were stimulus-selective, with 34 preferring stimulus presence and 19 preferring stimulus absence.

Next, we explored the neuromoteric functions with and without drugs for the stimulus-absent and stimulus-present neuron population separately. To that aim, we compared the mean firing rates of neurons preferring stimulus presence or absence in control and drug condition across all stimulus intensities (Fig. 3). Significant firing rate differences after drug applications were observed for both neurons that preferred the stimulus-present condition and neurons that preferred the stimulus-absent condition. Bic, in particular, caused a clear upward parallel shift of the neurometric functions for both cell categories (Fig. 3b).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Drug effects on firing rate for neurons preferring stimulus absence or presence. Because the monkeys worked with different absolute intensity values, the data are plotted in relative intensity values according to the monkeys' individual psychometric functions (Fig. 1c). Relative stimulus intensity levels 0–6 correspond to absolute stimulus intensities 0, 6, 7, 8, 15, 20, 30 for Monkey Q and 0, 10, 11, 12, 20, 30, 40 for Monkey Z. Firing rate means were derived during each neuron's respective analysis window. a, Red represents firing rates with MK relative to control discharges in black and gray. b, Blue represents firing rates with Bic relative to control firing rates. Solid lines and darker colors represent neurons preferring stimulus presence. Dashed lines and lighter colors neurons preferring stimulus absence. Error bars indicate SEM.

Specifically, we found no effect of MK on the firing rate of stimulus presence-preferring neurons (F(1) < 0.01, p = 0.999, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA), and no interaction of firing rate with intensities (F(6) = 1.11, p = 0.355). We also found no effect of MK on the firing rate of stimulus absence-preferring neurons (F(1) = 2.30, p = 0.142, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA), but a marginal interaction of firing rate with intensities (F(6) = 2.25, p = 0.042). Post hoc testing revealed significant differences at stimulus intensities 3 and 4. In contrast, we found significant differences in firing rate following Bic administration for stimulus presence-preferring (F(1) = 23.51, p < 0.001, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA), without significant interaction (F(6) = 1.22, p = 0.294). Similarly, Bic administration significantly changed activity for stimulus absence-preferring neurons (F(1) = 5.41, p = 0.028, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA), again without significant interaction (F(6) = 0.78, p = 0.587).

We further analyzed the impact of both drugs on the encoding of the stimulus condition of these stimulus-selective neurons. To evaluate the drug effects on the population of PFC cells, we derived and compared the area under the ROC curve (AUROC) as a measure of neuronal selectivity. Here, the AUROC is a nonparametric measure of the discriminability of two distributions of firing rates recorded in the stimulus present and stimulus absent conditions, respectively. Values of 0.5 indicate no separation, and values of 1 signal perfect discriminability. Figure 4 depicts example cells showing improved stimulus selectivity after blocking NMDA (MK condition; Fig. 4a,b) and GABA(A) receptors (Bic condition; Fig. 4c,d). In the former case, the AUROC value increases from 0.87 in control condition to 0.97 in MK condition; and in the latter case, the AUROC value increases from 0.61 in control condition to 0.77 in Bic condition. Comparing AUROC values during drug and control conditions for the population of stimulus-selective neurons yielded significantly higher AUROC values in both drug conditions. Mean AUROC values increased from 0.71 (SE 0.01) in control to 0.74 (SE 0.01) with administration of MK (t(71) = −2.20, p = 0.031, paired t test), and from 0.68 (SE 0.01) in control to 0.74 (SE 0.01) with administration of Bic (t(82) = −4.19, p < 0.001, paired t test). The blockade of both NMDA and GABA(A) receptors thus improved working memory selectivity to stimulus condition (Fig. 5). The distribution of analysis window durations during which neurons exhibited stimulus selectivity is depicted in Figure 6. For both MK (Fig. 6a) and Bic (Fig. 6b), neurons show a mixture of brief and sustained selectivity.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Drug effects on selectivity for example neurons. Raster plots (row represents a trial; dot represents a spike) and spike density histograms (time-resolved average firing rates) representing the activity of an example neuron with increased delay selectivity during MK administration (a,b) and an example neuron with increased delay selectivity during Bic administration (c,d). Dashed vertical lines indicate beginning of stimulus presentation and delay period. Shaded backgrounds represent the analysis window. Dark colors represent stimulus-present trials. Light colors represent stimulus-absent trials. Shaded areas around the spike density histograms represent respective SEs. a, b, An NS cell preferring stimulus-absent trials from Monkey Z in control (a) and MK (b) condition. c, d, An NS cell preferring stimulus-absent trials from Monkey Q in control (c) and Bic (d) condition.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Drug effects on the selectivity for the population of stimulus-selective neurons. AUROC values represent discriminability between preferred and nonpreferred stimulus condition in control and drug condition (a: MK, 72 neurons; b: Bic, 83 neurons). Left, AUROC values for each neuron. Right, Mean (and SE) for the respective population.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Distribution of analysis windows of stimulus-selective neurons. Stimulus-selective neurons in MK (a) and Bic (b) condition are sorted according to the beginning of their analysis window.

As every recording session started with a control block, we wanted to ensure that drug effects were not based on duration of recording time. Therefore, we compared AUROC values of the first and second control block separately for both drug conditions. AUROC values were comparable for neurons recorded in MK condition (t(10) = 0.47, p = 0.647, paired t test) as well as for neurons recorded in Bic condition (t(20) = −0.72, p = 0.478, paired t test).

To elucidate mechanisms that lead to enhanced selectivity, we further investigated drug effects on firing rates in the preferred and nonpreferred stimulus condition separately. For stimulus-selective neurons modulated with MK, firing rates in control trials were by definition higher for the preferred stimulus condition (5.92 ± 0.79) than for the nonpreferred condition (2.88 ± 0.43) (Fig. 7). MK did not change absolute firing rates of these neurons in either the preferred stimulus condition (6.3912 ± 0.77, t(71) = −0.60, p = 0.552, paired t test; Fig. 7a) or in the nonpreferred stimulus condition (2.82 ± 0.44, t(71) = 0.02, p = 0.981, paired t test; Fig. 7b). Firing rate differences between drug and control trials for preferred (0.47 ± 0.85) and nonpreferred (−0.06 ± 0.47) stimulus conditions were comparable (t(71) = 0.97, p = 0.334, paired t test; Fig. 7c). The absence of MK effects at the population level is also depicted in Figure 8 showing the normalized spike density histograms of the population of stimulus-selective neurons treated with MK. An analysis of the Fano factor, a measure of firing rate dispersion and variability, revealed no significant difference between control and MK trials for either the preferred (t(71) = 0.287, p = 0.775, paired t test) or the nonpreferred stimulus condition (t(68) = 1.44, p = 0.156, paired t test). Together, these results suggest that the improved selectivity with MK arises from a combination of nonsignificant changes in the activity of selective neurons.

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

MK effects on the firing rate for preferred and nonpreferred stimuli. a, b, Firing rates for control and MK condition separated for preferred (a) and nonpreferred (b) stimulus condition. Left, Firing rates for each neuron. Right, Mean firing rates for the respective population. c, Differences in mean firing rate between MK and control condition for the preferred and nonpreferred stimulus condition. Error bars indicate SEs. n.s., not significant.

Figure 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 8.

Time-resolved effects of MK on the preferred and nonpreferred stimulus condition. Spike density histograms represent the activity of the population of stimulus-selective neurons in control (left) and after MK administration (right). Dashed vertical lines indicate beginning of stimulus presentation and delay period. Solid lines indicate preferred stimulus conditions. Dashed lines indicate nonpreferred stimulus conditions. Shaded areas around the spike density histograms represent respective SEMs.

Next, we analyzed how Bic achieved the improved selectivity reported above. Again, by definition, mean firing rates in control trials were higher for the preferred stimulus condition (5.67 ± 0.94) than for the nonpreferred condition (2.81 ± 0.59) for stimulus-selective neurons modulated with Bic (Fig. 9). Bic did not increase absolute firing rates in either the preferred stimulus condition (t(82) = −1.61, p = 0.111, paired t test; Fig. 9a) or in the nonpreferred condition (t(82) = −1.27, p = 0.207, paired t test; Fig. 9b). A comparison of firing rate differences between control and drug trials for preferred and nonpreferred stimulus condition revealed that Bic tended to increase firing rates stronger for the preferred stimulus condition (2.47 ± 1.54) than for the nonpreferred condition (1.24 ± 0.97) (t(82) = 1.79, p = 0.077, paired t test; Fig. 9c). Figure 10 depicts normalized spike density histograms of the population of stimulus-selective neurons treated with Bic that illustrate these effects across time. The Fano factors for Bic were comparable between control and drug trials, both for the preferred condition (t(82) = 0.10, p = 0.920, paired t test) and the nonpreferred condition (t(79) = 0.61, p = 0.547, paired t test).

Figure 9.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 9.

Bic effects on firing rate for preferred and nonpreferred stimulus condition. a, b, Firing rates for control and Bic condition separated for preferred (a) and nonpreferred (b) stimulus condition. Left, Firing rates for each neuron. Right, Mean firing rates for the respective population. c, Differences in mean firing rate between Bic and control condition for the preferred and nonpreferred stimulus condition. Error bars indicate SEs. n.s., not significant.

Figure 10.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 10.

Time-resolved effects of Bic on the preferred and nonpreferred stimulus condition. Spike density histograms represent the activity of the population of stimulus-selective neurons in control (left) and after Bic administration (right). Dashed vertical lines indicate beginning of stimulus presentation and delay period. Solid lines indicate preferred stimulus conditions. Dashed lines indicate nonpreferred stimulus conditions. Shaded areas around the spike density histograms represent respective SEs.

Individual neurons increased selectivity in response to both NMDA and GABA(A) receptor blockade

To find out how individual neurons were affected by blockage of NMDA and GABA(A) receptors, we further analyzed the subset of 53 stimulus-selective neurons in sequential MK and Bic conditions. Figure 11a–c depicts an example cell showing enhanced delay selectivity with administration of either drug. MK in this case increases selectivity by diminishing the firing rate stronger for the stimulus absent condition than for the stimulus present condition.

Figure 11.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 11.

Stimulus-selective neurons modulated by Bic and MK. a–c, Raster plots and spike density histograms representing the activity of a broad spiking cell preferring stimulus-present trials from Monkey Q in control (a), Bic (b), and MK (c) conditions. Dashed vertical lines indicate beginning of stimulus presentation and delay period. Shaded backgrounds represent the analysis window. Dark colors represent stimulus-present trials. Light colors represent stimulus-absent trials. Shaded areas around the spike density histograms represent respective SEs. d, Amount of neurons that increased/decreased their selectivity with drug administration. <, >, show whether the AUROC value, signifying discriminability between preferred and nonpreferred stimulus condition, was higher in the respective drug or control condition.

A population analysis applying again an ROC analysis of selectivity to stimulus condition during the respective analysis windows in the delay revealed that 33 of these neurons enhanced their selectivity with administration of MK, 38 did so by administration of Bic, and 28 neurons enhanced selectivity with either drug compared with control. Other possible combinations were infrequent, with 10 neurons decreasing selectivity with MK but increasing it with Bic, 5 neurons increasing selectivity with MK but decreasing it with Bic, and 10 neurons decreasing selectivity with either drug (Fig. 11d). Statistical testing revealed that the proportion of cells that improved their selectivity with both drugs (52.83%) was within the range expected by the proportion of cells that improved their selectivity with one of the drugs (44.64%) (p = 0.143, one-sided binomial test). Overall, 85% (28 of 33) of the neurons that improved their selectivity with application of MK also improved their selectivity when Bic was applied. Likewise, 74% (28 of 38) of those neurons that improved their selectivity with application of Bic also improved their selectivity when MK was applied.

Effects were comparable for the subset of BS and NS. Ten of the 53 neurons could not be classified as neither NS nor BS, 26 were BS, and 17 were NS. Of the 26 BS, 17 improved their selectivity with administration of MK, 19 did so with administration of Bic, and 13 with either drug. The proportion of BS that improved selectivity with both drugs (50.00%) was within the range expected by the proportion of cells that improved their selectivity with one of the drugs (47.78%) (p = 0.489, one-sided binomial test). Of the 17 NS, 11 improved their selectivity with administration of MK, 13 did so with administration of Bic, and 9 with either drug. Again, the proportion of NS that improved selectivity with both drugs (52.94%) was within the range expected by the proportion of cells that improved their selectivity with one of the drugs (49.48%) (p = 0.483, one-sided binomial test).

Discussion

In rhesus monkeys performing a delayed decision task requiring working memory, we probed the effects of the excitatory glutamatergic and inhibitory GABAergic transmitter systems on single neurons by combining single-cell recordings and simultaneous micro-iontophoretic drug applications. As major results, we found that both the blockade of excitatory glutamatergic synapses with NMDA-receptor antagonists MK, as well as the inactivation of inhibitory synapses by GABA(A)-receptor antagonist Bic increased prefrontal neurons' stimulus selectivity during the delay period, albeit only subtle for NMDA receptors. These results contrast previous findings in behaving monkeys (Rao et al., 2000; Wang et al., 2013). Moreover, we probed individual neurons not only with one of the receptor antagonists, but with both. We found that most neurons (both putative pyramidal cells as well as inhibitory interneurons) that improved their signal-to-noise ratio with one of the drugs also increased their selectivity with the other.

Performance changes by the drugs were only seen in 1 of the 2 tested monkeys; administration of either drug improved performance of Monkey Q, but not Monkey Z. Additionally, we observed that both monkeys significantly aborted more trials in drug conditions. Overall, these behavioral findings were rather unexpected, given that the effect of micro-iontophoresis is very focal (Herz et al., 1969). We thus assume that the drugs affected small clusters or micro-networks of neurons, rather than individual cells alone.

Blocking NMDA receptors increases spontaneous responses and improves stimulus selectivity

We show a significant, albeit mild reduction of spontaneous firing rate after administration of glutamatergic NMDA-receptor antagonist MK in awake behaving monkeys. Only previous in vitro studies have shown a decrease in spontaneous firing rate after administration of MK (Huettner and Bean, 1988; Rotaru et al., 2011) or studies in anesthetized rats after previous administration of NMDA (Zhang et al., 1992; Wang et al. 2013) found that iontophoretic administration of MK in awake behaving monkeys produced no significant decrease in spontaneous firing rate, albeit a marked reduction of firing rates in all task epochs for delay selective neurons. The discrepancy of our findings to Wang et al. (2013) might be explained because MK only acts on open channels, and its effectiveness thus depends on the presence of glutamate (Huettner and Bean, 1988).

Whether MK has a net excitatory or inhibitory effect on clusters of neurons should depend on the ratio of recruited pyramidal cells and interneurons and may possibly be a function of drug dosage. Indeed, high dosages of MK caused excitatory effects in rodents, whereas low dosages did not (Jackson et al., 2004). Such excitatory effects of MK are suspected to be mediated by an inhibition of glutamatergic input in inhibitory interneurons, which would lead to net disinhibition of local circuits.

An important finding of the current study is that blockade of NMDA receptors by MK improved neuronal selectivity during working memory phases. In agreement with our finding, Jackson et al. (2004) found improved working memory performance at low doses of MK in the PFC of rats. In the same study, performance was, however, impaired at high doses. Wang et al. (2013) found impaired performance as well as reduced task related firing of delay cells of monkeys in an oculomotor delayed response (ODR) task after systemic administration of different doses of the NMDA antagonist ketamine. In the ODR task, monkeys have to make a saccade to a remembered spatial location after a memory delay period. In this ODR task, iontophoretic application of MK mimicked the neuronal effects of systemic ketamine administration by reducing the activity of delay cells, however more so for the preferred direction. This effect caused an impairment of stimulus coding during the ODR task, as opposed to an enhancement of stimulus selectivity as observed in the current study.

The disparate findings on working memory activity in ODR tasks versus feature-based delayed response tasks may relate to anatomically distinct PFC neurons that have been described for spatial and feature-based working memory (Wilson et al., 1993), even though many single neurons represent both spatial and visual information (Rao et al., 1997). An alternative explanation is that the delay activity in the ODR task might reflect mainly motor preparation signals or allocation of spatial attention rather than maintenance of signals in working memory (Lebedev et al., 2004; Takeda and Funahashi, 2004; Markowitz et al., 2015). This is because the monkeys know from the onset of the sample location where they have to make a saccade in the subsequent test phase. In our delayed decision task, however, the monkeys lacked information during the delay period that would have allowed them to prepare an action. Thus, the ODR task might entail specific circuits engaged in preparatory motor signaling, which could be differentially modulated by glutamatergic and GABAergic receptors. Such disparate pharmacological findings on working memory activity in ODR tasks versus feature-based delayed response tasks have also been reported for the dopaminergic modulatory system (Ott and Nieder, 2017, 2019; Vijayraghavan et al., 2016; Stalter et al., 2020).

The precise mechanisms behind the increase in stimulus selectivity remains elusive. In our study, MK effects for preferred and nonpreferred stimuli were comparable, suggesting that NMDA receptors are not only active for preferred stimulus, but at least partially also for the nonpreferred stimulus. We believe that it was an interplay between modest (but in themselves nonsignificant) changes in firing rates and firing variability that added up to significant changes in selectivity as detected by the ROC analysis.

Blocking GABA(A) receptors increases neuronal selectivity preferentially by disinhibiting preferred stimuli

Iontophoretic administration of GABA(A)-antagonist Bic generally increased the spontaneous firing rates of pyramidal cells and interneurons in lateral PFC. Bic had a stronger effect on pyramidal cells than on interneurons. Both findings are in line with similar previous studies (e.g., Rao et al., 2000).

As a major finding of the current study, blockade of GABA(A) receptors by Bic also improved neuronal selectivity during working memory periods. The supportive effect of blocking GABA receptors on memory is in partial agreement with behavioral studies in rodents that found improved memory retention and consolidation after administration of GABA antagonists (Luft et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2012). The blockade of GABA(A) receptors by Bic tended to increase the firing rate stronger for the preferred than the nonpreferred stimulus condition and thereby improved selectivity. This suggests that GABA downregulates memory processing by diminishing coding of preferred stimuli.

Contrary to our findings, Rao et al. (2000) found that iontophoretic administration of Bic in PFC of macaque monkeys performing an ODR task cancels spatial tuning of pyramidal cells and interneurons, especially in the delay phase. In their study, Bic impaired tuning by disinhibition of the nonpreferred direction as well as directions neighboring the preferred one, but also created tuning in a subset of previously untuned cells.

As discussed previously, the disparate findings in the study by Rao et al. (2000) compared with our study may be based on differences in spatial versus feature-based task protocols, as well as different types of cognitive signals (premotor vs pure working memory aspects) activated during the delay periods. In addition, GABA most likely affects working memory in a dose-dependent manner, and possibly follows an inverted-U response curve. This idea is supported by the findings that too much as well as too little GABA impaired optimal behavioral performance (Pezze et al., 2014; Bast et al., 2017; Ferguson and Gao, 2018).

NMDA and GABA(A) effects are mediated by the same neurons

In previous iontophoretic studies with monkeys, only one pharmacological substance was explored per neuron. We were therefore interested to find out if and how individual neurons would react to application of both NMDA and GABA antagonists. We found that most neurons that improved their signal-to-noise ratio with one of the drugs also increased their selectivity with the other. This was true for both putative pyramidal cells and inhibitory interneurons. This leads to the conclusion that both major classes of cortical neurons contained glutamatergic NMDA and GABA receptors. The observed selectivity-promoting effects of MK and Bic were hence also visible at the single-cell level, so that more than half of our recorded neurons increase their selectivity with either drug.

The observed drug effects were comparable for the subset of BS and NS neurons (i.e., putative pyramidal cells and inhibitory interneurons). This is to be expected when the input of a recorded neuron is affected. However, the net output effect of both cell types on the postsynaptic neurons is expected to be in the opposite direction. Since iontophoresis does not only affect one particular neuron at the recording site but also nearby neurons as parts of cortical microcircuits, the observed effects may not necessarily be due to direct effects on the respective receptors of the recorded neurons but could also stem from secondary effects mediated via microcircuits. This could explain some of the variance found at the single-neuron level and emphasizes the importance of larger neuron population analyses.

Differentiating NMDA and GABA effects hence gets more complicated as glutamatergic transmission is controlled by GABA receptors; and vice versa, GABAergic transmission is controlled by NMDA receptors (Del Arco and Mora, 2002; Higley, 2014; Farahmandfar et al., 2017). Because of this delicate interplay of excitation and inhibition in cortical networks, neuropsychiatric conditions, such as schizophrenia, have also been associated with impairments in the NMDA system as well as the GABA system (Benes, 1995; Gonzalez-Burgos and Lewis, 2012; Datta and Arnsten, 2018). This suggests that a better understanding of these transmitter systems, also with respect to potential psychiatric therapies, requires an investigation of glutamatergic and GABAergic effects back-to-back.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft Grant NI 618/5-2 to A.N.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Andreas Nieder at andreas.nieder{at}uni-tuebingen.de

References

  1. ↵
    1. Arnsten AF,
    2. Wang MJ,
    3. Paspalas CD
    (2012) Neuromodulation of thought: flexibilities and vulnerabilities in prefrontal cortical network synapses. Neuron 76:223–239. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.038 pmid:23040817
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Auger ML,
    2. Floresco SB
    (2014) Prefrontal cortical GABA modulation of spatial reference and working memory. Int. J Neuropsychopharmacol 18:pyu013. doi:10.1093/ijnp/pyu013 pmid:25552433
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Auger ML,
    2. Floresco SB
    (2017) Prefrontal cortical GABAergic and NMDA glutamatergic regulation of delayed responding. Neuropharmacology 113:10–20. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2016.09.022 pmid:27678413
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Bast T,
    2. Pezze M,
    3. McGarrity S
    (2017) Cognitive deficits caused by prefrontal cortical and hippocampal neural disinhibition. Br J Pharmacol 174:3211–3225. doi:10.1111/bph.13850 pmid:28477384
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Benes FM
    (1995) Altered glutamatergic and GABAergic mechanisms in the cingulate cortex of the schizophrenic brain. Arch Gen Psychiatry 52:1015–1024. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950240033007 pmid:7492253
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Blum RA
    (1952) Effect of subtotal lesions of frontal granular cortex on delayed reaction in monkeys. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 67:375–386. doi:10.1001/archneurpsyc.1952.02320150108012 pmid:14894008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Chrobak JJ,
    2. Hinman JR,
    3. Sabolek HR
    (2008) Revealing past memories: proactive interference and ketamine-induced memory deficits. J Neurosci 28:4512–4520. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0742-07.2008 pmid:18434529
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Cole BJ,
    2. Klewer M,
    3. Jones GH,
    4. Stephens DN
    (1993) Contrasting effects of the competitive NMDA antagonist CPP and the non-competitive NMDA antagonist MK 801 on performance of an operant delayed matching to position task in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 111:465–471. doi:10.1007/BF02253537 pmid:7870988
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Compte A,
    2. Brunel N,
    3. Goldman-Rakic PS,
    4. Wang XJ
    (2000) Synaptic mechanisms and network dynamics underlying spatial working memory in a cortical network model. Cereb Cortex 10:910–923. doi:10.1093/cercor/10.9.910 pmid:10982751
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Dash PK,
    2. Moore AN,
    3. Kobori N,
    4. Runyan JD
    (2007) Molecular activity underlying working memory. Learn Mem 14:554–563. doi:10.1101/lm.558707 pmid:17690339
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Datta D,
    2. Arnsten AF
    (2018) Unique molecular regulation of higher-order prefrontal cortical circuits: insights into the neurobiology of schizophrenia. ACS Chem Neurosci 9:2127–2145. doi:10.1021/acschemneuro.7b00505 pmid:29470055
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Del Arco A,
    2. Mora F
    (2002) NMDA and AMPA/kainate glutamatergic agonists increase the extracellular concentrations of GABA in the prefrontal cortex of the freely moving rat: modulation by endogenous dopamine. Brain Res Bull 57:623–630. doi:10.1016/S0361-9230(01)00758-4 pmid:11927365
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Diester I,
    2. Nieder A
    (2008) Complementary contributions of prefrontal neuron classes in abstract numerical categorization. J Neurosci 28:7737–7747. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1347-08.2008 pmid:18667606
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Eiselt AK,
    2. Nieder A
    (2013) Representation of abstract quantitative rules applied to spatial and numerical magnitudes in primate prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 33:7526–7534. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5827-12.2013 pmid:23616557
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Fano U
    (1947) Ionization Yield of Radiations. II. The Fluctuations of the Number of Ions. Phys Rev 72:26–29.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  16. ↵
    1. Farahmandfar M,
    2. Akbarabadi A,
    3. Bakhtazad A,
    4. Zarrindast MR
    (2017) Recovery from ketamine-induced amnesia by blockade of GABA-A receptor in the medial prefrontal cortex of mice. Neuroscience 344:48–55. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.02.056 pmid:26944606
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Ferguson BR,
    2. Gao WJ
    (2018) PV interneurons: critical regulators of E/I balance for prefrontal cortex-dependent behavior and psychiatric disorders. Front Neural Circuits 12:37. doi:10.3389/fncir.2018.00037 pmid:29867371
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Funahashi S,
    2. Bruce CJ,
    3. Goldman-Rakic PS
    (1989) Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol 61:331–349. doi:10.1152/jn.1989.61.2.331 pmid:2918358
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Fuster JM
    (2015) The prefrontal cortex, Ed 5. London: Academic.
  20. ↵
    1. Goldman-Rakic PS
    (1995) Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron 14:477–485. doi:10.1016/0896-6273(95)90304-6 pmid:7695894
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Goldman-Rakic PS
    (1996) Regional and cellular fractionation of working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93:13473–13480. doi:10.1073/pnas.93.24.13473 pmid:8942959
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Goldman-Rakic PS
    (1999) The “psychic” neuron of the cerebral cortex. Ann N Y Acad Sci 868:13–26. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb11270.x pmid:10414278
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Gonzalez-Burgos G,
    2. Lewis DA
    (2012) NMDA receptor hypofunction, parvalbumin positive neurons, and cortical gamma oscillations in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 38:950–957. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbs010 pmid:22355184
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Green DM,
    2. Swets JA
    (1966) Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley.
  25. ↵
    1. Herz A,
    2. Zieglgänsberger W,
    3. Färber G
    (1969) Microelectrophoretic studies concerning the spread of glutamic acid and GABA in brain tissue. Exp Brain Res 9:221–235. doi:10.1007/bj00234456 pmid:5371952
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Higley MJ
    (2014) Localized GABAergic inhibition of dendritic Ca2+ signalling. Nat Rev Neurosci 15:567–572. doi:10.1038/nrn3803 pmid:25116141
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Homayoun H,
    2. Moghaddam B
    (2007) NMDA receptor hypofunction produces opposite effects on prefrontal cortex interneurons and pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci 27:11496–11500. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2213-07.2007 pmid:17959792
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    1. Huettner JE,
    2. Bean BP
    (1988) Block of N-methyl-D-aspartate-activated current by the anticonvulsant MK801: selective binding to open channels. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 85:1307–1311. doi:10.1073/pnas.85.4.1307 pmid:2448800
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Jackson ME,
    2. Homayoun H,
    3. Moghaddam B
    (2004) NMDA receptor hypofunction produces concomitant firing rate potentiation and burst activity reduction in the prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:8467–8472. doi:10.1073/pnas.0308455101 pmid:15159546
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. ↵
    1. Jacob SN,
    2. Ott T,
    3. Nieder A
    (2013) Dopamine regulates two classes of primate prefrontal neurons that represent sensory signals. J Neurosci 33:13724–13734. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0210-13.2013 pmid:23966694
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    1. Kim DH,
    2. Kim JM,
    3. Park SJ,
    4. Cai M,
    5. Liu X,
    6. Lee S,
    7. Shin CY,
    8. Ryu JH
    (2012) GABA(A) receptor blockade enhances memory consolidation by increasing hippocampal BDNF levels. Neuropsychopharmacology 37:422–433. doi:10.1038/npp.2011.189 pmid:21900885
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Lebedev MA,
    2. Messinger A,
    3. Kralik JD,
    4. Wise SP
    (2004) Representation of attended versus remembered locations in prefrontal cortex. PLoS Biol 2:e365. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0020365 pmid:15510225
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Lorrain DS,
    2. Baccei CS,
    3. Bristow LJ,
    4. Anderson JJ,
    5. Varney MA
    (2003) Effects of ketamine and N-methyl-D-aspartate on glutamate and dopamine release in the rat prefrontal cortex: modulation by a group II selective metabotropic glutamate receptor agonist LY379268. Neuroscience 117:697–706. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00652-8 pmid:12617973
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Luft T,
    2. Pereira GS,
    3. Cammarota M,
    4. Izquierdo I
    (2004) Different time course for the memory facilitating effect of bicuculline in hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, and posterior parietal cortex of rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem 82:52–56. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2004.03.002 pmid:15183170
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Markowitz DA,
    2. Curtis CE,
    3. Pesaran B
    (2015) Multiple component networks support working memory in prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112:11084–11089. doi:10.1073/pnas.1504172112 pmid:26283366
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    1. Merten K,
    2. Nieder A
    (2012) Active encoding of decisions about stimulus absence in primate prefrontal cortex neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:6289–6294. doi:10.1073/pnas.1121084109 pmid:22460793
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    1. Miller EK,
    2. Nieder A,
    3. Freedman DJ,
    4. Wallis JD
    (2003) Neural correlates of categories and concepts. Curr Opin Neurobiol 13:198–203. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00037-0 pmid:12744974
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Mishkin M
    (1957) Effects of small frontal lesions on delayed alternation in monkeys. J Neurophysiol 20:615–622. doi:10.1152/jn.1957.20.6.615 pmid:13476217
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Ott T,
    2. Nieder A
    (2017) Dopamine D2 receptors enhance population dynamics in primate prefrontal working memory circuits. Cereb Cortex 27:4423–4435. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhw244 pmid:27591146
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Ott T,
    2. Nieder A
    (2019) Dopamine and cognitive control in prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 23:213–234. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2018.12.006 pmid:30711326
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Ott T,
    2. Jacob SN,
    3. Nieder A
    (2014) Dopamine receptors differentially enhance rule coding in primate prefrontal cortex neurons. Neuron 84:1317–1328. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.11.012 pmid:25482027
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Pezze M,
    2. McGarrity S,
    3. Mason R,
    4. Fone KC,
    5. Bast T
    (2014) Too little and too much: hypoactivation and disinhibition of medial prefrontal cortex cause attentional deficits. J Neurosci 34:7931–7946. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3450-13.2014 pmid:24899715
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    1. Rao SC,
    2. Rainer G,
    3. Miller EK
    (1997) Integration of what and where in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science 276:821–824. doi:10.1126/science.276.5313.821 pmid:9115211
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    1. Rao SG,
    2. Williams GV,
    3. Goldman-Rakic PS
    (2000) Destruction and creation of spatial tuning by disinhibition: GABAA blockade of prefrontal cortical neurons engaged by working memory. J Neurosci 20:485–494. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-01-00485.2000 pmid:10627624
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    1. Rotaru DC,
    2. Yoshino H,
    3. Lewis DA,
    4. Ermentrout GB,
    5. Gonzalez-Burgos G
    (2011) Glutamate receptor subtypes mediating synaptic activation of prefrontal cortex neurons: relevance for schizophrenia. J Neurosci 31:142–156. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1970-10.2011 pmid:21209199
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  46. ↵
    1. Sawaguchi T,
    2. Matsumura M,
    3. Kubota K
    (1989) Delayed response deficits produced by local injection of bicuculline into the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in Japanese macaque monkeys. Exp Brain Res 75:457–469. doi:10.1007/bf00249897 pmid:2744104
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Skoblenick K,
    2. Everling S
    (2012) NMDA antagonist ketamine reduces task selectivity in macaque dorsolateral prefrontal neurons and impairs performance of randomly interleaved prosaccades and antisaccades. J Neurosci 32:12018–12027. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1510-12.2012 pmid:22933786
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. ↵
    1. Smith JW,
    2. Gastambide F,
    3. Gilmour G,
    4. Dix S,
    5. Foss J,
    6. Lloyd K,
    7. Malik N,
    8. Tricklebank M
    (2011) A comparison of the effects of ketamine and phencyclidine with other antagonists of the NMDA receptor in rodent assays of attention and working memory. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 217:255–269. doi:10.1007/s00213-011-2277-5 pmid:21484239
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Stalter M,
    2. Westendorff S,
    3. Nieder A
    (2020) Dopamine gates visual signals in monkey prefrontal cortex neurons. Cell Rep 30:164–172.e4.
    OpenUrl
  50. ↵
    1. Suzuki Y,
    2. Jodo E,
    3. Takeuchi S,
    4. Niwa S,
    5. Kayama Y
    (2002) Acute administration of phencyclidine induces tonic activation of medial prefrontal cortex neurons in freely moving rats. Neuroscience 114:769–779. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00298-1 pmid:12220577
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Takeda K,
    2. Funahashi S
    (2004) Population vector analysis of primate prefrontal activity during spatial working memory. Cereb Cortex 14:1328–1339. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh093 pmid:15166104
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Thiele A,
    2. Delicato LS,
    3. Roberts MJ,
    4. Gieselmann MA
    (2006) A novel electrode-pipette design for simultaneous recording of extracellular spikes and iontophoretic drug application in awake behaving monkeys. J Neurosci Methods 158:207–211. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.05.032 pmid:16843532
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Vallentin D,
    2. Bongard S,
    3. Nieder A
    (2012) Numerical rule coding in the prefrontal, premotor, and posterior parietal cortices of macaques. J Neurosci 32:6621–6630. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5071-11.2012 pmid:22573684
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  54. ↵
    1. Vijayraghavan S,
    2. Major AJ,
    3. Everling S
    (2016) Dopamine D1 and D2 receptors make dissociable contributions to dorsolateral prefrontal cortical regulation of rule-guided oculomotor behavior. Cell Rep 16:805–816. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2016.06.031 pmid:27373147
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Viswanathan P,
    2. Nieder A
    (2015) Differential impact of behavioral relevance on quantity coding in primate frontal and parietal neurons. Curr Biol 25:1259–1269. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.025 pmid:25913409
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Wang M,
    2. Arnsten AF
    (2015) Contribution of NMDA receptors to dorsolateral prefrontal cortical networks in primates. Neurosci Bull 31:191–197. doi:10.1007/s12264-014-1504-6 pmid:25754145
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Wang M,
    2. Yang Y,
    3. Wang CJ,
    4. Gamo NJ,
    5. Jin LE,
    6. Mazer JA,
    7. Morrison JH,
    8. Wang XJ,
    9. Arnsten AF
    (2013) NMDA receptors subserve persistent neuronal firing during working memory in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuron 77:736–749. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.032 pmid:23439125
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Wang XJ
    (1999) Synaptic basis of cortical persistent activity: the importance of NMDA receptors to working memory. J Neurosci 19:9587–9603. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-21-09587.1999 pmid:10531461
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  59. ↵
    1. Wang XJ
    (2001) Synaptic reverberation underlying mnemonic persistent activity. Trends Neurosci 24:455–463. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01868-3 pmid:11476885
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Wang XJ
    (2002) Probabilistic decision making by slow reverberation in cortical circuits. Neuron 36:955–968. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01092-9 pmid:12467598
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Wilson FA,
    2. Scalaidhe SP,
    3. Goldman-Rakic PS
    (1993) Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. Science 260:1955–1958. doi:10.1126/science.8316836 pmid:8316836
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  62. ↵
    1. Yoon JH,
    2. Grandelis A,
    3. Maddock RJ
    (2016) Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex GABA concentration in humans predicts working memory load processing capacity. J Neurosci 36:11788–11794. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1970-16.2016 pmid:27852785
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  63. ↵
    1. Zhang J,
    2. Chiodo LA,
    3. Freeman AS
    (1992) Electrophysiological effects of MK-801 on rat nigrostriatal and mesoaccumbal dopaminergic neurons. Brain Res 590:153–163. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(92)91091-R pmid:1422830
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 40 (7)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 40, Issue 7
12 Feb 2020
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Blockage of NMDA- and GABA(A) Receptors Improves Working Memory Selectivity of Primate Prefrontal Neurons
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Blockage of NMDA- and GABA(A) Receptors Improves Working Memory Selectivity of Primate Prefrontal Neurons
Paul Rodermund, Stephanie Westendorff, Andreas Nieder
Journal of Neuroscience 12 February 2020, 40 (7) 1527-1537; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2009-19.2019

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Blockage of NMDA- and GABA(A) Receptors Improves Working Memory Selectivity of Primate Prefrontal Neurons
Paul Rodermund, Stephanie Westendorff, Andreas Nieder
Journal of Neuroscience 12 February 2020, 40 (7) 1527-1537; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2009-19.2019
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • GABA
  • glutamate
  • iontophoresis
  • pharmacology
  • PFC
  • working memory

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Increased neuronal expression of the early endosomal adaptor APPL1 replicates Alzheimer’s Disease-related endosomal and synaptic dysfunction with cholinergic neurodegeneration.
  • Presynaptic mu opioid receptors suppress the functional connectivity of ventral tegmental area dopaminergic neurons with aversion-related brain regions
  • Change of spiny neuron structure in the basal ganglia song circuit and its regulation by miR-9 during song development
Show more Research Articles

Systems/Circuits

  • Presynaptic mu opioid receptors suppress the functional connectivity of ventral tegmental area dopaminergic neurons with aversion-related brain regions
  • V2b neurons act via multiple targets to produce in phase inhibition during locomotion
  • The Neurobiology of Cognitive Fatigue and Its Influence on Effort-Based Choice
Show more Systems/Circuits
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.