Abstract
Transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS) over cortical areas has been shown to acutely improve performance in sensory detection tasks. One explanation for this behavioral effect is stochastic resonance (SR), a mechanism that explains how signal processing in nonlinear systems can benefit from added noise. While acute noise benefits of electrical RNS have been demonstrated at the behavioral level as well as in in vitro preparations of neural tissue, it is currently largely unknown whether similar effects can be shown at the neural population level using neurophysiological readouts of human cortex. Here, we hypothesized that acute tRNS will increase the responsiveness of primary motor cortex (M1) when probed with transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Neural responsiveness was operationalized via the well-known concept of the resting motor threshold (RMT). We showed that tRNS acutely decreases RMT. This effect was small, but it was consistently replicated across four experiments including different cohorts (total N = 81, 46 females, 35 males), two tRNS electrode montages, and different control conditions. Our experiments provide critical neurophysiological evidence that tRNS can acutely generate noise benefits by enhancing the neural population response of human M1.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT A hallmark feature of stochastic resonance (SR) is that signal processing can benefit from added noise. This has mainly been demonstrated at the single-cell level in vitro where the neural response to weak input signals can be enhanced by simultaneously applying random noise. Our finding that transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS) acutely increases the excitability of corticomotor circuits extends the principle of noise benefits to the neural population level in human cortex. Our finding is in line with the notion that tRNS might affect cortical processing via the SR phenomenon. It suggests that enhancing the response of cortical populations to an external stimulus might be one neurophysiological mechanism mediating performance improvements when tRNS is applied to sensory cortex during perception tasks.
Introduction
Transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS) is a non-invasive electrical brain stimulation technique whereby currents are randomly drawn from a predefined range of intensities and frequencies (Antal and Herrmann, 2016). Until now most studies applied tRNS for several minutes over primary motor cortex (M1), which typically leads to an increase in corticomotor excitability relative to baseline for up to 60 min after stimulation (Terney et al., 2008; Chaieb et al., 2011, 2015; Abe et al., 2019; Moret et al., 2019) with occasional reports suggesting inhibitory effects for low intensities (Moliadze et al., 2012). The exact mechanism causing this temporary facilitation of cortical activity is unknown; however, it has been hypothesized to reflect neuroplastic changes (Terney et al., 2008).
By contrast, acute (i.e., online) effects of tRNS have been studied much less. One general hypothesis is that the brain responds to electrical noise according to a stochastic resonance (SR) phenomenon (Terney et al., 2008; Miniussi et al., 2013; van der Groen and Wenderoth, 2016; van der Groen et al., 2018, 2019; Pavan et al., 2019). SR is a general mechanism that enhances the response of nonlinear systems to weak subthreshold signals by adding an optimal level of random noise (Gingl et al., 1995; McDonnell and Abbott, 2009). One key indicator of the SR phenomenon in a broad sense (McDonnell and Abbott, 2009) is that the investigated system “benefits” from noise, which usually refers to better detection, transmission or processing of the input signal than when no noise is present. In humans, SR effects have been mainly demonstrated via behavioral signal detection tasks whereby noise was added to the periphery. For example, the detection of low-contrast visual stimuli was significantly enhanced when the stimuli were superimposed with visual noise (Simonotto et al., 1997). Recently, a similar enhancement of visual perception has been reported when noise was directly added to visual cortex via tRNS, which improved the detection of low contrast visual stimuli (van der Groen and Wenderoth, 2016), visual decision-making (Ghin et al., 2018; Pavan et al., 2019; van der Groen et al., 2019), binocular rivalry (van der Groen et al., 2018), and visual training in healthy participants (Fertonani et al., 2011; Pirulli et al., 2013) and patients (Moret et al., 2018; Herpich et al., 2019). However, until now the beneficial effect of adding external electrical random noise to neural activity has mainly been studied via behavioral outcome measures in humans or via physiological single cell studies in animals (Onorato et al., 2016; Remedios et al., 2019). By contrast, it is largely unknown whether tRNS causes acute benefits when applied in vivo to neural populations within the cortex. Here, we seek to answer this question by delivering electrical noise transcranially to human M1. We hypothesized that if M1 benefits from externally added noise in accordance to the SR phenomenon, neural responsiveness to transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS; i.e., reflecting the processing and/or transmission of the external stimulation) would be increased (McDonnell and Abbott, 2009). Neural responsiveness was operationalized via the well-known concept of the resting motor threshold (RMT), which is defined as the stimulation intensity required to evoke motor evoked potentials (MEPs) of a given amplitude in at least 50% of trials, i.e., with a probability of 0.5. Accordingly, our hypothesis would be supported if RMTs were lower during tRNS application when compared with no stimulation because random noise increased the probability of evoking MEPs of sufficient size (Fig. 1).
Materials and Methods
Participants
Eighty-one healthy volunteers (46 females, 35 males, mean age = 25.4 ± 5.1; range, 18–46) took part in this study, which consisted of 4 experiments. A new group of participants was recruited for each experiment. All were right-handed and had no identified contraindications for participation according to established TMS exclusion criteria (Wassermann, 1998; Rossi et al., 2009). All provided written informed consent. Upon study conclusion, they were debriefed and financially compensated for their time and effort. None of the participants reported any major side effects resulting from the stimulation. All research procedures were approved by the Cantonal Ethics Committee Zurich (KEK-ZH-Nr. 2014-0242, KEK-ZH-Nr. 2014-0269 and BASEC Nr. 2018-01078) and were performed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration of the World Medical Association (2013 WMA Declaration of Helsinki).
General study design
To evaluate the acute influence of tRNS on the excitability of motor cortex we performed a series of four experiments in which we combined high frequency (100–500 Hz) tRNS with single-pulse TMS over left M1. tRNS was applied for only 3 s per trial, with jittering of the TMS pulse between 1.3 and 1.7 s after tRNS onset (for an overview of the different experiments, see Fig. 2). Short tRNS duration was used based on the previously demonstrated acute behavioral effects of 2- to 5-s tRNS (van der Groen and Wenderoth, 2016; van der Groen et al., 2019) and reports showing that 4-s tDCS results in an excitability change during stimulation without producing after-effects (Nitsche et al., 2003). In all experiments, the TMS intertrial interval was set to 8 s with 20% temporal variability. During the measurement participants sat comfortably at rest and directed their gaze toward a fixation cross. MEPs of the right first dorsal interosseous (FDI) muscle were used as a primary physiological read-out. Our main outcome parameter in all experiments was the probability of eliciting MEPs with an amplitude ≥0.05 mV [i.e., response probability; p(MEP0.05 mV)]. All experiments consisted of conditions where tRNS was applied to M1 in random order (see below) and no noise control conditions, with the experimenter holding the TMS coil blinded regarding condition type. Since we used a very brief stimulation time (3 s only), fade in/out periods were not possible. Accordingly, some participants were able to distinguish the stimulation conditions (see Results, Additional control analyses, Tactile sensation). We accounted for this possible bias via various control analyses and by including one experiment where we introduced an active stimulation control condition (see experiment 4).
tRNS
Random noise (100–500 Hz) was delivered through a battery-driven electrical stimulator (DC-Stimulator PLUS, NeuroConn GmbH). Electroconductive gel was applied to the contact side of the rubber electrodes (5 × 7 cm) to reduce skin impedance. Depending on the experiment, the stimulation intensity varied between 0.5- and 2-mA amplitude (peak-to-baseline), resulting in maximum current densities ranging from 14.29 to 57.14
tRNS and no noise control conditions were randomized throughout all of the experiments to prevent cumulative effects of tRNS and to minimize the effect of general changes in corticomotor excitability.
TMS
Single-pulse monophasic TMS was delivered using a 70-mm figure-of-eight coil connected to the Magstim 200 stimulator. For all subjects the coil was positioned over the hotspot of the FDI muscle. Depending on the tRNS electrode montage the coil was placed either on top of the electrode (experiments 1 and 2) or on the scalp in between the electrodes (experiments 3 and 4; see Fig. 2). The hotspot was defined as the stimulation site where TMS delivery resulted in the most consistent and largest MEPs in the resting muscle. The coil was held tangential to the surface of the scalp with the handle pointing backward and laterally at 45° away from the nasion-inion mid-sagittal line, resulting in a posterior-anterior direction of current flow in the brain. Such a coil orientation is thought to be optimal for inducing the electric field perpendicular to the central sulcus resulting in the stimulation of M1 neurons (Mills et al., 1992; Rathelot and Strick, 2009). The optimal coil location was marked with a semi-permanent marker on the head and registered using the neuronavigation software (Brainsight Frameless, Rogue Research Inc.). The position of the participant's head and TMS coil was constantly monitored in real-time with the Polaris Vicra Optical Tracking System (Northern Digital Inc.). This ensured that the center of the coil was kept within 2 mm of the determined hotspot, and that the coil orientation was consistent throughout the experiment. For each participant we determined the RMT, defined as the lowest intensity to elicit MEPs with peak-to-peak amplitude ≥0.05 mV in the relaxed muscle, in five out of 10 consecutive trials (Rossini et al., 1994). Pooling data across all experiments, the mean RMT at baseline corresponded to 44 ± 9% of the maximum stimulator output (MSO).
Electromyography (EMG)
The muscle response was recorded by a surface EMG electrode (Bagnoli DE-2.1 EMG Sensors, Delsys) placed over the right FDI muscle. Raw signals were amplified (sampling rate, 5 kHz), digitized with a CED micro 1401 AD converter and Signal software V2.13 (both Cambridge Electronic Design), and stored on a personal computer for off-line analysis. Timing of the TMS delivery, remote control of the tRNS stimulator and EMG data recording were synchronized via the CED. Muscular relaxation was constantly monitored through visual feedback of EMG activity and participants were instructed to relax their muscles if necessary.
Data processing and analysis
The EMG data were bandpass filtered (30–800 Hz, notch filter = 50 Hz). Filtering was applied separately for the pre-TMS background EMG (bgEMG) and post-TMS period containing peak-to-peak MEP amplitude to avoid “smearing” the MEP into bgEMG data. MEP amplitude was defined as the peak-to-peak amplitude between 15 and 60 ms after the TMS pulse. Next, we excluded trials in which the unwanted background muscle activation could influence the measured MEP amplitude. Trials with root mean square bgEMG above 0.01 mV were removed from further analyses to control for unwanted bgEMG activity (Hess et al., 1986; Devanne et al., 1997). For the remaining trials, the mean and SD of the bgEMG was calculated for each participant. Trials with bgEMG > mean + 2.5 SDs were also excluded. Based on these criteria 96.7% of all data were included for further analysis (96.7% in experiment 1, 97.7% in experiment 2, 96.2% in experiment 3, and 97.6% in experiment 4).
In all experiments our main outcome parameter was the probability of eliciting MEPs with an amplitude ≥0.05 mV [for each condition
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were performed with repeated measures ANOVAs (rmANOVAs) in IBM SPSS Statistics version 23.0 (IBM Corp.) unless otherwise stated. All data were tested for normal distribution using Shapiro–Wilk test of normality. Sphericity was tested using Mauchly's sphericity test. If sphericity was violated, Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied. The threshold for statistical significance was set at α = 0.05. All post hoc tests were corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction. Effect sizes are reported for each experiment in the form of partial η2 (
Additional information for specific experiments
Experiment 1, testing effects of 1-mA tRNS
In the first experiment, we tested whether tRNS induces an increase in MEP probability when TMS was applied to M1 at RMT intensity. Sixteen right-handed participants (self-report) took part in the experiment (nine females, seven males, mean age = 24.7 ± 5, range: 19–35). tRNS electrodes were placed over (1) the hotspot of the right FDI as determined by single-pulse TMS and (2) the contralateral, right M1 (Fig. 2A). This bilateral montage was chosen based on modeling results which indicated similar current densities but less current spread for the M1-M1 montage (used here) compared with the M1-supraorbital cortex montage as used in previous studies (Terney et al., 2008; Chaieb et al., 2011). tRNS intensity was set to 1-mA peak-to-baseline amplitude and applied in each trial for 3 s. TMS pulses were delivered over left M1 at RMT intensity (mean RMT 49 ± 13% MSO, range: 36–84%), starting 1.3, 1.5, or 1.7 s after tRNS onset. Testing was split into 4 blocks (60 trials in each block, 240 trials total). We measured MEPs in the FDI muscle either during the 1-mA tRNS condition (180 trials total) or the no tRNS condition (control, 60 trials total). We compared MEP probability between the 1-mA tRNS versus no tRNS conditions using a paired t test. Although we adjusted the TMS intensity to the individuals' RMT before the main experiment, closer inspection of the data revealed that RMTs could drift during the experiment (Karabanov et al., 2015) so that some participants were stimulated at sub-threshold TMS intensities (i.e., MEP0.05 mV probability < 0.5 in the no tRNS condition) while others were stimulated at supra-threshold intensities (MEP0.05 mV probability > 0.5 in the no tRNS condition). Therefore, we evaluated whether tRNS-induced changes in MEP probability were associated with MEP probability in the no tRNS condition using Pearson correlation. The tRNS-induced modulation was calculated as a ratio between p(MEP0.05 mV) in the 1-mA tRNS condition and p(MEP0.05 mV) in the no tRNS condition. Thus, we correlated x with
Given that correlating a fractional increase from x with x itself can be problematic as it might lead to spurious correlations (Pearson, 1897; Tu, 2016), we applied a statistical correction method suggested by Tu (2016; Eqs. 4–6). This method compares the above correlation rx,y/x to the expected “null correlation” (rnull) which is revealed by
The observed correlation rx,y/x is then compared with rnull using a z-test of the following form:
This allowed us to assess whether there is a statistically significant association between p(MEP0.05 mV) in the no tRNS condition and tRNS-induced modulation.
Experiment 2, testing dose–response effects
The second experiment aimed to determine whether there is an optimal tRNS intensity to modulate MEP probability in M1. Twenty-three right-handed [Edinburgh Handedness Inventory mean laterality quotient (LQ) = 78.3 ± 13.8; Oldfield, 1971] participants were recruited. We had to exclude one participant because of technical problems with data acquisition. Twenty-two participants (13 females, nine males, mean age = 25.4 ± 5.4, range: 20–46) were included in the subsequent analysis. At the beginning of the session, we measured the RMT of each participant (mean RMT = 43.4 ± 7.5% MSO, range: 31–54%). Based on the results of experiment 1 and behavioral effects of tRNS (van der Groen and Wenderoth, 2016; van der Groen et al., 2018), we decided to probe MEP with subthreshold TMS, as the response toward stimuli presented below the threshold are postulated to be particularly susceptible for SR effects (Gingl et al., 1995; Moss et al., 2004). TMS over left M1 was always applied with an intensity slightly below RMT, which was operationally defined as the intensity evoking MEPs with an amplitude of at least 0.05 mV in three out of 10 trials [p(MEP0.05 mV) = 0.3; mean subthreshold intensity: 42 ± 7.3% MSO, range: 30–53%]. The TMS pulse was randomly applied either 1.5 or 1.7 s after tRNS onset to avoid anticipation of the pulse (Fig. 2A). The tRNS electrodes were placed as in experiment 1 (Fig. 2A). We varied tRNS intensity (0.5, 1, 1.5, or 2 mA vs no tRNS control condition) to examine whether there is a dose effect on the tRNS induced enhancement of MEP probability. We recorded 10 trials per tRNS condition in randomized order in 3 blocks. Throughout the experiment we monitored MEP probability in the no tRNS control condition and, if required, we adjusted the TMS intensity between blocks to ensure subthreshold level stimulation [p(MEP0.05 mV) = 0.3; Fig. 9]. This procedure was chosen because it has been shown that state-dependent changes of RMT can occur in absence of overt activity or task involvement and need to be considered to keep TMS intensity comparable throughout the experiment (Karabanov et al., 2015). Therefore, we determined MEP probability for the no tRNS condition after each completed block. Since we targeted MEP probability of 0.3, TMS intensity was reduced by 1% MSO when MEP probability was ≥0.5 or increased when MEP probability was ≤0.1. Note that adjusting TMS intensities only between blocks ensured subthreshold TMS throughout the course of the experiment without confounding the comparison between the different tRNS conditions that were sampled equally within each block. 30 MEPs were collected per condition resulting in a total number of 150 TMS pulses. For 19 participants, we collapsed MEPs across the three blocks to estimate the MEP probability for each of the noise conditions. For three participants, post hoc analysis revealed that they met the criteria for subthreshold TMS during the no tRNS control condition only in two blocks, i.e., MEP probabilities were estimated from 20 MEPs per condition. MEP probabilities were subjected to a rmANOVA with the within-subject factor tRNS intensity (no tRNS, 0.5-, 1-, 1.5-, and 2-mA tRNS).
Before starting the main experiment, all experimental tRNS intensities (0.5–2 mA) were presented to the participant (for 20 s in a randomized order) and subjectively assessed on a scale from 0 (no sensation) to 10 (strong pain) to make sure that the stimulation did not cause any unpleasant sensations.
Experiment 3, threshold estimation
The third experiment was a conceptual replication of experiment 2, but this time we used a modified electrode montage and tested whether tRNS applied online can influence RMT. The required sample size was estimated based on experiment 2 (
RMT was defined at the beginning of the session (mean RMT = 41.1 ± 7.4% MSO, range: 28–55%). We then applied TMS intensities centered around the individual RMT level during the main experiment (namely: RMT–2%, RMT–1%, RMT, RMT+1%, RMT+2%). Based on the results of experiment 2, we tested only tRNS intensity of 2 mA versus no tRNS. This time we used a modified electrode montage as described by Rawji et al. (2018), whereby electrodes were positioned 7 cm anterior and posterior to the FDI muscle hotspot along the coil axis (45° away from the nasion-inion mid-sagittal line). As described by the authors, in this arrangement current oscillates perpendicular to the central sulcus, which has been hypothesized to be more efficient in modulating corticospinal excitability (Rawji et al., 2018). The electric field modeling showed that this arrangement provides more focal electrical stimulation and induces a slightly higher electric field over M1 (Fig. 2B). Additionally, this montage enables positioning the TMS coil directly on the scalp and avoids delivering TMS pulses through the electrode (see Fig. 2). Different electrode montages can change the directionality of the electric field which has been shown to strongly influence the effect of tDCS (Nitsche et al., 2008). However, noise benefits induced by tRNS should be polarity independent (Pirulli et al., 2016). Here, we test whether the increased M1 responsiveness caused by applying tRNS generalize across electrode montages targeting M1. We tested 10 different experimental conditions (no tRNS vs 2-mA tRNS × five TMS intensities) in total. The experiment consisted of six blocks, with four trials per condition presented within each block in a randomized order (24 MEPs in each of the 10 conditions, 240 TMS pulses total). TMS was randomly applied 1.3 or 1.7 s after tRNS onset within each trial (Fig. 2B). We monitored the MEP probability in the control condition (no tRNS with TMS intensity targeted at RMT) and TMS intensity was adjusted between blocks by 1% MSO: it was (1) decreased if the probability of MEPs in the no tRNS condition was ≥0.75; or (2) increased if the probability of MEPs was ≤0.25, to stay as close as possible to the target RMT level [p(MEP0.05 mV) = 0.5; Fig. 9].
Post hoc inspection of the data revealed that two participants showed response probabilities consistently above RMT. Thus, they did not fulfil our criteria of probing MEP with TMS intensities centered around the RMT and were excluded from subsequent analyses. Additionally, one outlier was excluded during data analysis because of a tRNS-induced decrease in RMT >2 SD of the group mean. Data from the final sample of twenty participants (10 females, 10 males, mean age = 27.5 ± 6, range: 18–42) was entered into a 2 × 5 rmANOVA with the within-subject factors tRNS (no tRNS vs 2-mA tRNS) and TMS intensity (RMT–2%, RMT–1%, RMT, RMT+1%, RMT+2%). Additionally, we performed a threshold estimation analysis to determine whether tRNS influenced RMT. To do so, for each participant we calculated the response probability for each of the five TMS intensities when either 2-mA tRNS or no tRNS was applied. Next, we fitted separate linear models [y = ax + b, with y denoting p(MEP0.05 mV) and x the stimulation intensity] to each of the datasets and determined RMTFit = (0.5-b)/a, i.e., the intensity which would be used to evoke a sufficiently large MEP with a probability of 0.5 (see Fig. 3). Note that this method provides a more accurate estimation of RMT than manually adjusting TMS intensity until p(MEP0.05 mV) = 0.5 is reached, partly because the model is informed by more data.
Before the start of the main experiment, participants were familiarized with tRNS and we assessed the detectability of potential sensations. The detection task consisted of 20 trials. Participants received either tRNS (2 mA over M1, with and without TMS on half of the trials) or no tRNS (with and without TMS). Their task on each trial was to indicate (after an auditory cue) whether they felt something underneath the tRNS electrodes (ignoring TMS pulses) by pressing the appropriate button on a keyboard.
Experiment 4, threshold estimation with active control condition
The final experiment aimed to replicate the results obtained in experiment 3, but this time we compared tRNS applied over M1 to tRNS applied over right primary visual cortex (V1) to control for unspecific stimulation effects. The sample size estimation was based on the effect size of
We applied tRNS either over the left M1 or the control stimulation site and probed the muscle response with single-pulse TMS using five intensities around threshold level (RMT–2%, RMT–1%, RMT, RMT+1%, RMT+2%). As in experiment 3, the TMS intensity was adjusted between blocks if necessary, based on the data obtained during the control stimulation over right V1 (Fig. 9).
From the original sample of twenty-nine right-handed (mean LQ = 85 ± 20.3; Oldfield, 1971) participants, six were excluded for various reasons. One could not complete the session because of technical problems. Three participants revealed response probabilities consistently above RMT and one substantially below RMT during the experiment. Additionally, one outlier was removed post hoc because tRNS caused the RMT to decrease by >2 SD of the group mean. This resulted in the final sample of twenty-three participants (14 females, nine males, mean age = 24.3 ± 3.9, range: 19–34; mean RMT = 44.1 ± 7.2% MSO, range: 33–59%).
Similar to the previous experiment, we used the threshold estimation analysis (RMTFit) and a 2 × 5 rmANOVA with the within-subject factors tRNS (tRNS over M1 vs V1) and TMS intensity (RMT–2%, RMT–1%, RMT, RMT+1%, RMT+2%) for the statistical analysis of MEP probability. As in experiment 3, we assessed the detectability of skin sensation because of electrical stimulation via a detection task, which was performed before and after the main experiment. Detection task was similar to experiment 3, but included three stimulation conditions: no tRNS, 2-mA tRNS over M1 or 2-mA tRNS over right V1.
Results
Experiment 1, tRNS induced increase in MEP probability for subthreshold TMS
In the first experiment, we investigated whether tRNS modulates the probability of eliciting MEPs. We measured MEPs during 1-mA tRNS versus no tRNS and calculated the probability of evoking MEPs with an amplitude ≥ 0.05 mV [p(MEP0.05 mV) 1-mA tRNS and p(MEP0.05 mV) no tRNS, respectively]. We did not observe a significant difference between the overall MEP probability in the noise versus control condition [t(15) = 0.31, p = 0.77, mean difference (MD) = 0.007 ± 0.09].
Although we aimed for stimulating each participant with a TMS intensity corresponding to RMT as determined before the main experiment, post hoc analysis of the no tRNS condition revealed that some participants were stimulated with subthreshold TMS intensities [i.e., p(MEP0.05 mV) no tRNS < 0.5], while others were stimulated with suprathreshold TMS intensities [i.e., p(MEP0.05 mV) no tRNS > 0.5]. Therefore, we calculated a Pearson correlation to test whether a potential increase in p(MEP0.05 mV) in the 1-mA tRNS condition (i.e., indicating a noise benefit) depended on the MEP probability in the no tRNS condition (Fig. 4). We found a clear negative correlation rx,y/x = −0.68, which was highly significant when compared with the null model rnull = 0.158 (z = −3.56, p < 0.001; see Materials and Methods for details). This finding suggests that tRNS modulates M1's response probability most strongly when TMS stimuli are delivered with an intensity slightly below the individual RMT [i.e., p(MEP0.05 mV) no tRNS < 0.5], a result that is consistent with van der Groen and Wenderoth (2016), who showed that tRNS enhances detection performance for subthreshold stimuli.
Experiment 2, increase in MEP probability for higher tRNS intensity over M1
Next, we aimed to examine whether there is an optimal tRNS intensity to enhance the probability of evoking MEPs. Therefore, we applied tRNS over both M1s at intensities ranging from 0.5, 1, 1.5 to 2 mA versus no tRNS control condition (Fig. 2A). Based on our previous finding, we ensured that M1 was probed with TMS at sub-RMT intensities. Accordingly, p(MEP0.05 mV) = 0.26 ± 0.06 for the no tRNS condition but gradually increased for higher tRNS intensities as indicated by a significant main effect of tRNS intensity (F(4,84) = 4.57, p = 0.002,
Experiment 3, tRNS over M1 induced decrease in RMT
Next, we performed a conceptual replication of the previous experiment and applied 2-mA tRNS versus no tRNS (control condition) but changed the electrode placement (Fig. 2B) to probe whether acute noise benefits on responsiveness generalize across different electrode montages targeting M1. Although specific electrode positions can possibly affect the directionality of the electrical field to target neurons and result in divergent effects regarding polarity and potentially also the effective amount of the induced current, we again observed tRNS-induced enhancement in cortical responsiveness. We found a general increase in MEP probability when 2-mA tRNS was applied over M1 (main effect of tRNS, F(1,19) = 7.58, p = 0.01,
Experiment 4, decrease in RMT is specific for M1 stimulation
In experiment 4, we enrolled a new cohort of participants to control for the potentially unspecific effects of tRNS (e.g., arousal or tactile stimulation; Fertonani et al., 2015) by comparing 2-mA tRNS over M1 to 2-mA tRNS over right V1, with the latter serving as a control area that is unlikely to influence RMT. Our results confirmed the principal finding from the previous experiment, revealing that the probability of evoking MEPs is higher when stimulating M1 compared with the control site (main effect of tRNS, F(1,22) = 13.09, p = 0.002,
Additional control analyses
MEP amplitude
We analyzed whether tRNS over M1 at 0.5 up to 2 mA versus the control condition (i.e., no tRNS or 2-mA tRNS over right V1) influenced average MEP amplitude. Over all experiments this effect did not reach significance (all p ≥ 0.23), except for experiment 3, where we found a significant increase in MEP amplitude in the 2-mA tRNS versus no tRNS condition (main effect of tRNS, F(1,19) = 10, p = 0.005,
Tactile sensation
In experiments 2–4, we performed assessments of the possible tactile effects caused by electrical stimulation. We found that most of our participants could distinguish between no tRNS and tRNS conditions. In experiment 2 we recorded the subjective assessment (on a 0–10 scale) of the tactile sensation evoked by tRNS (mean for 0.5 mA = 0.3 ± 0.7; 1 mA = 0.6 ± 1.4; 1.5 mA = 1.2 ± 1.8; 2 mA = 1.9 ± 1.9). In experiment 3, we measured the detectability of potential sensations because of 2-mA tRNS via a detection task (mean accuracy = 89 ± 18%). We extended the detectability estimation in experiment 4, by repeating the task before (pre) and after (post) the experiment (mean accuracy pre = 85 ± 17%; mean accuracy post = 77 ± 18%, resulting in general average accuracy of 80 ± 16%). Additionally, we distinguished hit rates (HRs) for correct detection of M1 (mean = 0.71 ± 0.4) versus V1 stimulation (mean = 0.74 ± 0.3), showing that sensation detectability did not significantly differ between stimulated areas (t(22) = −0.27, p = 0.79). In order to test whether our TMS results might have been driven by the tactile sensation, we reanalysed our main outcome parameters (MEP probability and RMTFit change) from experiments 2–4 after adding sensation (experiment 2) or detection accuracy (experiments 3 and 4) as a covariate (all covariates were z-scored because of non-normal distribution). After the covariates were added, all the effects reported in experiments 2–4 remained significant (all p ≤ 0.04) making it unlikely that tactile sensation was the main driver of our results. Furthermore, strength or accuracy of stimulation-induced sensations did not correlate with the measured effects, i.e., MEP probability and RMTFit change in experiments 2 and 3 (experiment 2: r = 0.22, p = 0.34; experiment 3: r = 0.04, p = 0.88). Only experiment 4 showed a significant correlation (r = 0.5, p = 0.02), which, however, could not have driven our results because better detection of tRNS diminished its effect on lowering RMTFit. Similarly, there was also no significant correlation between the monitored impedance between the electrodes (which was always <15 kΩ) and the observed effects related to tRNS (all r ≤ 0.21, p ≥ 0.4).
bgEMG
Analysis of bgEMG across the experimental conditions demonstrated that muscle activity was generally low (on average <0.0025 mV across experiments). Moreover, additional analyses using bgEMG as a covariate in experiments 2–4 revealed that all reported TMS effects remained significant (all p ≤ 0.03).
tRNS induced effects do not depend on MEP criteria
The above results used MEP probability as the main outcome measurement. Importantly, the reported effects were not driven by our MEP amplitude criterion (i.e., MEP amplitude ≥0.05 mV), as control analyses with slightly different criteria (i.e., MEP cutoff amplitudes of 0.03–0.07 mV) revealed a similar pattern of results in all our experiments. Namely, regardless of the adopted MEP criterion, we confirmed tRNS-induced enhancement in MEP probability in experiment 1 (all r ≤ −0.56, z ≤ −2.86, p ≤ 0.004), we found a gradual increase in MEP probability for higher tRNS intensities in experiment 2 (all F ≥ 2.5, p ≤ 0.05) and observed the decrease in RMTFit during 2-mA tRNS over M1 (vs no tRNS in experiment 3: all t ≥ 2.3, p ≤ 0.03 and versus 2-mA tRNS over V1 in experiment 4: all t ≥ 2.3, p ≤ 0.03).
TMS intensity adjustments
It is well known that the individual responsiveness to TMS can drift slightly throughout a TMS experiment because of state-dependent changes, which affect measurements close to RMT intensities in particular (Karabanov et al., 2015). As recommended by Karabanov et al. (2015), we adjusted TMS intensities between experimental blocks to ensure subthreshold stimulation [i.e., p(MEP0.05 mV) no tRNS ≤ 0.3 or ≤ 0.5] throughout experiments 2–4 as shown in Figure 9. Importantly, once TMS intensity was adjusted, we collected an equal amount of data for each of the tRNS conditions ensuring that the direct comparison of tRNS versus the control condition was not confounded by the between-block adjustments.
Next, we analyzed whether the intensity adjustments followed a systematic trend by pooling these data across experiments and comparing the first to the last block. We found no systematic changes in TMS intensity (t(63) = 1.15, p = 0.25, MD = 0.22 ± 1.5%; Fig. 9) making it unlikely that tRNS or other aspects of our experimental procedure caused any longer-lasting effects on M1 responsiveness.
Electric field measurement
Finally, we also tested electric field induced by TMS pulse with and without tRNS. The induced electric field was measured by oscilloscope using a search coil placed under the TMS coil. tRNS was delivered to a conducting phantom medium soaked in saline solution through two electrodes placed on either side of the coils (impedance = 2 kΩ). We measured 20 TMS pulses with and without electrical noise stimulation (2-mA tRNS) in an alternating manner. We showed that tRNS did not influence the electric field induced by the TMS pulse (t(19) = 0.6, p = 0.53, MD = 0.05 ± 0.3 μV). This confirmed that all changes in MEP probability originated from the modulation of M1 responsiveness and not stronger magnetic stimulation.
Discussion
This study provides direct evidence that online tRNS over M1 enhances the responsiveness of cortical motor circuits via a shift in response threshold. Across four separate experiments we showed that acute tRNS effects manifested as (1) a higher probability of evoking MEPs when TMS was applied with intensities at or slightly below RMT (Figs. 4, 5, 6A, 7A); and (2) lower estimated RMTs (RMTFit shown in Figs. 6B,C, 7B,C). Importantly, the observed effects appeared to be specific for tRNS delivery over M1 but not over V1 (Fig. 7), making it unlikely that our results were driven by any unspecific tRNS effects. Our findings consistently indicate that tRNS enhances the responsiveness of cortical motor circuits by lowering the response threshold and provide important proof-of-principle evidence of noise benefits at the neural population level in humans.
Acute tRNS-induced noise benefits are partly consistent with SR theory
We consistently observed that adding electrical noise to M1 can acutely increase its responsiveness to TMS. One potential explanation for the observed results is signal enhancement during acute noise delivery to a nonlinear system. Such noise benefits are one hallmark feature of SR theory (McDonnell and Abbott, 2009). A second important feature is that noise benefits are a function of noise intensity exhibiting an inverted U-shape dose–response relationship, i.e., too much noise is detrimental. While our neurophysiological study provided proof-of-principle evidence of noise benefits at the neural population level in human cortex, we did not demonstrate the inverted U-shape function for higher tRNS intensities (Fig. 5) and can draw no inference about how much noise would be optimal for increasing the responsiveness of M1.
Previous studies (van der Groen and Wenderoth, 2016; van der Groen et al., 2018; Pavan et al., 2019) demonstrated an inverted U-shape function of tRNS effects using behavioral outcome parameters that are typically acquired in accordance with detection theory. One explanation for the detrimental effect of excessive noise is that it causes the system to respond even if there is no signal to detect and reduces the detection rate by causing too many “false alarms”. However, in our experiments a false alarm would mean that tRNS alone would occasionally evoke MEPs. Thus, probing the detrimental effect of adding too much noise to the resting motor system via tRNS would require much higher intensities than used here, which would certainly cause strong discomfort in human participants (Fertonani et al., 2015).
Accordingly, we cannot claim that we added the optimal level of noise to M1 based on our data. In fact, the absolute changes in RMT were relatively small (although statistical effect sizes were medium to large). This can be partly attributed to the fact that we aimed to modulate RMT, which is one of the most robust and reliable TMS measurements (Ngomo et al., 2012; Dissanayaka et al., 2018). Indeed, RMT has rarely been modulated by other forms of electrical stimulation (Nitsche et al., 2005), including long-lasting effects of tRNS (Terney et al., 2008). However, another explanation for the relatively small absolute effects is that tRNS induced a suboptimal amount of noise.
Alternative accounts of the observed acute tRNS-induced noise benefits
Until now, a change in corticomotor excitability measurements has been demonstrated after prolonged tRNS delivery and has been hypothesized to reflect long-lasting neuroplastic changes (Terney et al., 2008; Chaieb et al., 2011, 2015; Abe et al., 2019; Moret et al., 2019). However, neuroplastic changes are unlikely to have driven the acute tRNS effects in our study since tRNS conditions were always interleaved with no noise or other control conditions, thereby minimizing the influence of long-term excitability changes. Moreover, there was no systematic change in adjusted TMS intensity for the no tRNS condition over time, making it unlikely that our results were affected by long-term neuroplasticity (Fig. 9).
Another possible mechanism is that repeated subthreshold stimulations with tRNS induced consecutive openings of sodium channels which might lead to temporal summation of small membrane potentials, cause depolarization of the neural membrane because of an increased influx of inward sodium currents and/or prevent the homeostasis of the system (Terney et al., 2008; Fertonani et al., 2011). This may affect excitability of M1 in a similar manner as reported here.
Finally, the effects of tRNS may also be attributed to the increased synchronization of neural firing through amplification of subthreshold oscillatory activity, reducing the amount of endogenous noise (Miniussi et al., 2013).
Possible neurophysiological substrate mediating tRNS-induced noise benefits
We showed that a short tRNS bout of <3 s acutely influences the neurophysiology of human motor cortex. Using threshold estimation analysis as our primary outcome measurement (Vucic et al., 2018), we showed that tRNS specifically affected the response threshold of cortical motor circuits. By contrast, tRNS had no systematic effect on MEP amplitude. It has been argued that motor threshold and MEP amplitude reflect independent neural processes (Paulus et al., 2008; Vucic and Kiernan, 2017; Vucic et al., 2018).
From a neurophysiological point of view, the motor threshold reflects the efficacy of a chain of synapses from cortical interneurons in Layers II/III to the muscles (Kobayashi and Pascual-Leone, 2003). Pharmacological studies have implicated voltage-gated sodium channels as a major determinant of motor threshold since blocking sodium channels increases RMT (Tergau et al., 2003; Sommer et al., 2012; Ziemann et al., 2015).
Although the neurophysiological mechanism of tRNS is not completely understood, previous studies (Onorato et al., 2016) suggested that such enhancing a neurons response via electrical noise occurred by the concurrent activation of voltage-gated sodium channels. A recent study measured sodium currents in somatosensory and auditory pyramidal neurons in vitro while stimulating the cells with different levels of electrical random noise (Remedios et al., 2019). The authors showed that some neurons exhibited higher peak amplitudes of sodium currents, elicited by a voltage-clamp-ramp protocol, which correlated with shorter latencies of the neuronal response during brief electrical noise delivery. In other words, in those neurons, less voltage was needed to evoke the sodium current peak when the optimal level of electrical noise was delivered. Additionally, tRNS might affect neuronal populations by increasing the probability of synchronized firing within neuronal populations (Miniussi et al., 2013). In this regard, it has been estimated that alternating currents at 100 Hz polarize a single neuron by a relatively small amount (Deans et al., 2007). However, networks of many synaptically connected active neurons reveal higher sensitivity to field modulation than single cells, thus, amplifying the stimulation effect (Fröhlich and McCormick, 2010; Reato et al., 2010). Accordingly, even subthreshold stimulation that induces very weak electric fields in cortex can modulate membrane potentials (Gluckman et al., 1996; Francis et al., 2003; Bikson et al., 2006). Based on these findings we propose that tRNS might have reduced RMTFit by slightly increasing the responsiveness of voltage-gated sodium channels (Ziemann et al., 2015) in large populations of cortical cells.
Interestingly, unlike short 4-s bouts of tDCS (Nitsche et al., 2003; but see example for large variability of tDCS effects on MEP amplitude, Jonker et al., 2021), acute tRNS induced no changes in MEP amplitudes. In line with this finding and the supposed role of sodium channels, previous pharmacological studies have shown that blocking voltage-gated sodium channels had no or only inconsistent effects on MEP amplitudes (Paulus et al., 2008; Vucic et al., 2018).
Increased cortical responsiveness via tRNS is unlikely to result from tactile stimulation
In our study, many participants felt a slight but noticeable skin sensation (Fertonani et al., 2015). This constitutes a potential confound because participants were not blinded to tRNS conditions and because some effects might be driven by transcutaneous stimulation of peripheral nerves rather than by transcranial stimulation of cortical neurons (Asamoah et al., 2019). We reanalysed all our experiments accounting for tactile sensation and showed that they did not contribute to the observed effects. Most importantly, in the final experiment we used an active control condition. Behavioral results revealed that the intensity of the skin sensation was similar but only M1 stimulation lowered the response threshold of cortical motor circuits (Fig. 7). Moreover, a study in non-human primates found significant neuromodulation effects despite blocking or substantially suppressing somatosensory input (Vieira et al., 2020). Thus, we argue that the effects observed in our study are most likely caused by adding electrical noise to M1 rather than by unspecific effects of tRNS.
In conclusion, our results demonstrate that tRNS changes the responsiveness of M1 circuits. We observed acute modulation of RMTFit, which seems to reflect immediate signal enhancement rather than neuroplastic changes. Such increase in responsiveness of a nonlinear system in the presence of noise seems to be consistent with one of the two hallmarks of SR theory showing noise benefits at the neural population level in humans (McDonnell and Abbott, 2009). Our study provides evidence that online tRNS influences the neurophysiology of the human motor cortex and sheds new light on understanding the impact of acute electrical noise stimulation on neural processing at the network level.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation Grant 320030_175616 and by the National Research Foundation, Prime Minister's Office, Singapore under its Campus for Research Excellence and Technological Enterprise (CREATE) program (FHT). We thank all the participants for their time and effort, Daniel Woolley for his valuable help and feedback on the manuscript, Ernest Mihelj for his contribution to the data analysis, and Marta Stepien, Lena Salzmann, Alexandra Burgler, and Luana Giacone for their assistance during data collection.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Weronika Potok at weronika.potok{at}hest.ethz.ch or Nicole Wenderoth at nicole.wenderoth{at}hest.ethz.ch