Abstract
Control of protein intake is essential for numerous biological processes as several amino acids cannot be synthesized de novo, however, its neurobiological substrates are still poorly understood. In the present study, we combined in vivo fiber photometry with nutrient-conditioned flavor in a rat model of protein appetite to record neuronal activity in the VTA, a central brain region for the control of food-related processes. In adult male rats, protein restriction increased preference for casein (protein) over maltodextrin (carbohydrate). Moreover, protein consumption was associated with a greater VTA response, relative to carbohydrate. After initial nutrient preference, a switch from a normal balanced diet to protein restriction induced rapid development of protein preference but required extensive exposure to macronutrient solutions to induce elevated VTA responses to casein. Furthermore, prior protein restriction induced long-lasting food preference and VTA responses. This study reveals that VTA circuits are involved in protein appetite in times of need, a crucial process for animals to acquire an adequate amount of protein in their diet.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Acquiring insufficient protein in one's diet has severe consequences for health and ultimately will lead to death. In addition, a low level of dietary protein has been proposed as a driver of obesity as it can leverage up intake of fat and carbohydrate. However, much remains unknown about the role of the brain in ensuring adequate intake of protein. Here, we show that in a state of protein restriction a key node in brain reward circuitry, the VTA, is activated more strongly during consumption of protein than carbohydrate. Moreover, although rats' behavior changed to reflect new protein status, patterns of neural activity were more persistent and only loosely linked to protein status.
Introduction
Ensuring appropriate intake of the three macronutrients (carbohydrate, fat, protein) is a compelling problem for survival of all animals, including humans. Of the three macronutrients, protein intake is thought to be the most tightly regulated, as many amino acids cannot be synthesized de novo (Berthoud et al., 2012). Concordantly, many species, including invertebrates (Mayntz et al., 2005) and mammals (Theall et al., 1984), adjust their behavior to ensure adequate intake of dietary protein. In humans, inadequate protein levels in diet may contribute to obesity, by leveraging up the amount of calories consumed from fats and sugar (Simpson and Raubenheimer, 2005; Hall, 2019; Raubenheimer and Simpson, 2019). Recently, we developed a rodent model of protein appetite in which animals (rodents) maintained on a protein-restricted (PR) diet developed a strong preference for a protein-rich solution, relative to a carbohydrate-rich solution (Murphy et al., 2018; see also Hill et al., 2019), indicating that animals can specifically direct feeding and food-seeking behavior toward protein sources in times of need. However, the neural mechanisms by which diets that are low in protein might shift behavior are not understood.
The VTA and its projections play a central role in food-seeking behaviors, food preference, and in the motivation to eat (Ikemoto and Panksepp, 1999; Berridge, 2007; Bromberg-Martin et al., 2010). VTA neurons are sensitive to numerous food-related signals, including ingestive and post-ingestive processes (de Araujo et al., 2008; Domingos et al., 2011; Beeler et al., 2012; Ferreira et al., 2012; McCutcheon et al., 2012a; Alhadeff et al., 2019), and peripheral hormones (Di Chiara and Abizaid, 2009; Mebel et al., 2012; Mietlicki-Baase et al., 2013, 2014; Cone et al., 2014), allowing the formation of future food preferences (Sclafani et al., 2011). Despite abundant data on the involvement of VTA activity in mediating responses to fat- or carbohydrate-containing food, the role of this region in regulation of protein appetite is still unexplored.
Here, we use in vivo fiber photometry to record the activity of VTA neurons during consumption of isocaloric protein- and carbohydrate-containing solutions in an animal model of protein preference (Murphy et al., 2018; Naneix et al., 2020, 2021). We find that, in PR animals, protein consumption is associated with elevated neural activation, relative to carbohydrate consumption. We then show that, when physiological state is reversed, behavioral protein preference shifts to reflect the new state more rapidly than neural activity in the VTA.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
Adult male Sprague Dawley rats (Charles River Laboratories, n = 15) weighing 250-300 g on arrival were used. Rats were housed in pairs in individually ventilated cages (46.2 × 40.3 × 40.4 cm), in a temperature (21 ± 2°C) and humidity (40%-50%) controlled environment with a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 A.M.) and with water and food available ab libitum. All testing occurred in the light phase. Data are not reported for 7 rats because of poor or nonexistent photometry signal resulting from lack of viral expression, misplacement of fiber, or poor connection between patch cable and ferrule. Two rats were removed from the study because of aggressive behavior in the week following the initial dietary manipulation, which led to them being singly housed, rather than in pairs. Procedures were performed in accordance with the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 and conducted under Project License 70/8069/PFACC16E2.
Virus injection and fiber implantation
For fiber photometry recording, rats received a unilateral injection of a GCaMP6s-expressing virus in the VTA and were implanted with fiber optic cannulas targeting the injection site (see Fig. 1A). One to two weeks after their arrival, rats were anesthetized with isoflurane (5% induction, 2%-3% maintenance) and mounted in a stereotaxic frame (David Kopf Instruments) in a flat skull position. The scalp was shaved, cleaned with chlorhexidine, and locally anesthetized with bupivacaine (150 µl, s.c.). Rats also received intraperitoneal injection of nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory meloxicam (1 mg/kg). Core body temperature, oxygen saturation, and heart rate were monitored throughout the surgery. A hole was drilled above the VTA at the following coordinates: AP −5.8 mm, ML 0.7 mm relative to bregma (Paxinos and Watson, 1998). A 10 µl Hamilton syringe placed in a motorized syringe pump (Harvard Apparatus Pump 11 Elite) was loaded with the GCaMP6s virus (AAV9.Syn1.GCaMP6s.WPRE.SV40, ≈1.9 × 1013 GC/ml, Penn Vector Core; RRID:Addgene_100843) and was slowly lowered into VTA (DV −8.1 mm relative to brain surface); 1 μl of virus was delivered over 10 min (100 nl/min), and the syringe was left in place for 5 additional minutes before being slowly removed. An optic fiber cannula (ThorLabs CFM14L10, 400 μm, 0.39 NA, 10 mm length) was implanted at the same coordinates, 0.1 mm above the injection site (DV −8.0 mm relative to brain surface). The cannula was secured in place by dental cement (C&B Supabond followed by regular dental acrylic, Prestige Dental) overlaying four small skull-screws. Rats were housed in pairs immediately for recovery. Rats were allowed at least 4 weeks to recover before the start of behavioral testing to allow ample time for virus expression.
Diets
All rats were initially maintained on standard laboratory chow diet (EURodent Diet 5LF2, LabDiet) containing 14% protein. Four weeks after surgery, 8 of the rats were randomly assigned to the PR diet condition. For these rats, standard chow was switched to a modified AIN-93G diet containing 5% protein from casein (#D15100602, Research Diets) (Murphy et al., 2018). Remaining rats were maintained under standard laboratory chow diet (non-restricted group [NR]). Behavioral testing started 1 week following protein restriction.
Flavor conditioning and casein preference tests
Animals were trained in two identical conditioning chambers (30.5 × 24.1 × 21.0 cm; Med Associates), each located inside a sound- and light-attenuated aluminum outer chamber (1200 × 700 × 700 cm). Each conditioning chamber was equipped with a house light located on the left wall, 2 retractable sippers located on the right wall, and 2 light cues located above each sipper hole. Each bottle placed on a retractable sipper was connected to a contact lickometer (Med Associates) used to measure intake of flavored solution. The house light was turned on at the beginning of each daily session and turned off at the end of it. Conditioning chamber apparatus was controlled via a computer running Med-PC IV Software Suite (Med Associates). Sessions were video recorded at either 5 or 10 Hz using a webcam (Microsoft LifeCam) that interfaced with fiber photometry software.
Initially, all rats were pretrained with 2 bottles containing 0.2% sodium saccharin (Sigma Millipore, #S1002). First, rats had continuous access to both bottles in the chambers until they reached >1000 licks during the daily 60 min session (1-3 d). Then, each saccharin bottle was presented individually in a pseudorandom order (intertrial interval 10-30 s, mean 20 s) during 45 trials. On each trial, if no licks were made, then sippers remained available for 30 s. However, once a lick was made, sippers remained extended for 5 s before retraction (see Fig. 1B). This protocol trained rats over a small number of sessions to approach and drink from sippers when available. Coincident with sipper activation, the cue light located above the sipper hole was turned on and remained on until the sipper was retracted. Sippers took ∼2 s from activation until the rat could reach them to drink. Following saccharin pretraining, during the next 4 d, all rats were trained to associate a specific flavored solution (0.05% cherry or grape Kool-Aid with 0.2% saccharin) with a different nutrient in daily sessions lasting a maximum of 60 min. During conditioning sessions, only one bottle was available and was presented during 45 individual trials, as described above. Bottles were filled with either protein-containing solution (4% casein sodium salt from bovine milk, Sigma Millipore, #C8654; 0.21% L-methionine, Sigma Millipore, #M9625; 0.2% saccharin; 0.05% flavored Kool-Aid) or isocaloric carbohydrate-containing solution (4% maltodextrin, Sigma Millipore, #419672; 0.2% saccharin; 0.05% flavored Kool-Aid), as previously described (Murphy et al., 2018). Bottle positions, presentation order, and flavor-macronutrient associations were counterbalanced between rats. Bottle position was alternated between days.
Twenty-four hours after the last conditioning session, rats received a first preference test (Preference test 1). Both casein and maltodextrin-flavored solutions were available during the test. The test started with 45 trials during which each bottle was presented in pseudorandom order (forced choice trials; intertrial interval 10–30 s, mean 20 s). These trials were followed by 20 presentations of the two bottles simultaneously (free choice trials).
Immediately after Preference test 1, diet conditions were switched between experimental groups. NR rats were now given protein-restricted diet (NR → PR), whereas protein-restricted rats were given standard chow diet (PR → NR). Seven days after the diet switch, a second preference test was conducted (Preference test 2). This test was followed by 4 d of additional conditioning sessions, as described above, before a final preference test (Preference test 3).
Fiber photometry recordings
To assess the activity of VTA neurons during the consumption of differently flavored macronutrient solutions, the “bulk” fluorescence signal generated by GCaMP6s expressing cells was recorded using fiber photometry (see Fig. 1) (Gunaydin et al., 2014; Lerner et al., 2015). Signal processing and acquisition hardware (RZ5P; Tucker Davis Technologies) were used to control two light sources: a 470 nm LED (ThorLabs, M470F3) modulated at 211 Hz and a 405 nm LED (ThorLabs, M405F1) modulated at 539 Hz. A fluorescence minicube (Doric Lenses) combined both wavelengths, which were transmitted through an optical patch cable to the rat's optic cannula implant. LED power was set at 30-60 µW. Emitted light was delivered through the same patch cable back to the minicube where it was filtered for GFP emission wavelength (525 nm) and sent to a photoreceiver (#2151 Femtowatt Silicon Photoreceiver, DC-750 Hz; Newport). Demodulation of the two light sources allowed dissociation of calcium-dependent GCaMP6s signals (470 nm) and calcium-independent changes resulting from autofluorescence and motion artifacts (isosbestic 405 nm wavelength). All signals were acquired using Synapse Essentials software (Tucker Davis Technologies). Signals were sampled at 6.1 kHz (before demodulation) and 1017 Hz (after demodulation). Behavioral events (e.g., licks and sipper presentations) were time stamped by registering TTLs generated by the Med-PC system. The demodulated signals were filtered by using FFT to convert each signal from the time domain into the frequency domain, subtracting the 405 signal from the 470 signal, and then converting back into the time domain (Konanur et al., 2020). This corrected signal was expressed as a change in fluorescence, relative to total fluorescence, and used for all further analyses.
Subsequently, data were divided into discrete trials by alignment with time stamps representing the first lick in each trial and binning into 100 ms bins. z scores were calculated for each trial by taking the mean divided by the SD of a baseline period lasting for 10 s preceding the first lick in each trial. Area under the curve (AUC) was calculated for the 5 s following the first lick before the sipper retracted and for the 5 s following sipper retraction. Baseline activity for each session was estimated by calculating the AUC of the epoch at the start of the session before the first trial began.
Histology
After completion of behavioral testing and recordings, rats were deeply anesthetized using 5% isoflurane followed by pentobarbital (50 mg/ml) before being transcardially perfused with cold 0.1 m PBS followed by 4% PFA solution. Brains were then postfixed overnight in ice cold 4% PFA before being transferred in 0.1 m PBS solution with 30% sucrose for at least 48 h at 4°C. Serial coronal sections (40 µm thick) were cut on a freezing microtome and stored in PBS solution containing 0.02% sodium azide. VTA-containing sections were selected to check virus spread and the position of the fiber track. Free-floating sections were transferred to 6-well plates filled with PBS. First, sections were rinsed in 0.1 m PBS (3 × 5 min) before being incubated for 1 h in blocking solution (3% goat serum, 3% donkey serum, 3% Triton in 0.1 m PBS). Next, sections were incubated overnight at room temperature with primary antibody to detect GCaMP (chicken anti-GFP, A10262, Thermo Fisher Scientific; RRID:AB_2534023; 1:1000 in blocking solution). After rinses in 0.1 m PBS (3 × 5 min), sections were incubated with secondary antibody solution (goat anti-chicken IgG AlexaFluor-488 conjugate, A-11039, Thermo Fisher Scientific; RRID:AB_2534096; 1:250 in 0.1 m PBS) for 90 min at room temperature. Finally, sections were rinsed with 0.1 m PBS (3 × 5 min) and mounted in VectorShield Hard Set mounting medium and coverslipped. Images were taken using an epifluorescence microscope (Leica Microsystems, DM2500) using 2.5×, 10×, and 20× objectives and a R6 Retiga CCD camera (QImaging). Fiber position and virus spread were determined according to neuroanatomical landmarks (Paxinos and Watson, 1998).
Experimental design and statistical analysis
Behavioral data (lick time stamps) were extracted from data files and analyzed using custom Python scripts that measured numbers of licks for each solution and latencies from sipper extension. Position of rats in the chamber was determined using DeepLabCut (Mathis et al., 2018; Nath et al., 2019) to track body parts (nose, ears, base of tail) of rats in every frame across the preference session.
For statistical analysis of within session behavioral and neural variables, two-way mixed repeated-measures ANOVA was used with Diet group as a between-subject variable (e.g., PR vs NR) and Solution as a within subject variable (casein vs maltodextrin). Choice data were analyzed by comparing diet groups using an unpaired t test and for preference within each diet group using one-sample t tests versus no preference (0.5). For comparison of behavioral and neural latencies, these values were pooled for individual trials across all rats. Pearson's correlation coefficients were calculated between latency for neural activity to peak and latency to lick (from sipper extension). Differences for each type of latency were compared between solutions using Mann–Whitney U test.
For summary data, across all sessions, two-way mixed repeated-measures ANOVA was used with Diet as a between-subject variable and Session as a within-subject variable. To examine neural activity for each rat individually, AUC of casein trials was compared with AUC of maltodextrin trials using an unpaired two-tailed t test. Resulting p values were used to construct pie charts.
For data from conditioning sessions, three-way mixed repeated-measures ANOVA was used with Diet group as a between-subject variable (e.g., PR vs NR) and Solution and Session as within subject variables (casein vs maltodextrin; Session 1 vs Session 2). For body weight, two-way mixed repeated-measures ANOVA was used with Diet as a between-subject variable and Day as a within-subject variable, and planned t tests were used to compare groups on the first and last day. For food intake, unit of statistic was “cage” as all rats were group housed and average food intake per rat across all days was compared with t test.
Significant effects and interactions were followed by estimating effect sizes between subgroups. Effect sizes were determined by comparison to bootstrapped sampling distributions, which are shown in bottom panels for each comparison; 5000 bootstrap samples were taken. CIs are bias corrected and accelerated and are shown on the same plots and reported in the text. Reported p values are permutation p values resulting from t tests comparing 5000 reshuffles.
Data and software availability
All data files are available at Figshare (https://doi.org/10.25392/leicester.data.7636268). These experiments used a combination of software tools: Python (data extraction, analysis, and plotting) and R (statistics). Estimation plots were adapted from dabest v0.3.01 (Ho et al., 2019). All code is available at Github (https://github.com/mccutcheonlab/PPP_analysis/releases/tag/v1.0).
Results
VTA neurons were targeted by injecting an AAV encoding the calcium sensor GCaMP6s (under control of the synapsin promoter), and a fiber optic was implanted above the injection site to record neural activity in freely moving rats (n = 14; Fig. 1A,B). Three to 4 weeks after surgery, a subset of rats were switched to low protein diet (5% protein from casein; PR group, n = 8) while the remaining animals remained on regular chow (14% protein; NR group, n = 6). Analysis of body weight data for the subsequent 2 weeks, before conditioning sessions started, revealed that PR and NR rats gain weight at a slightly different rate across days (Fig. 1C; two-way ANOVA, Diet: F(1,13) = 0.09, p = 0.767; Day: F(14,182) = 25.02, p < 0.0001; Diet × Day: F(14,182) = 3.97, p < 0.0001). However, the difference between diets was minimal as planned comparisons of PR and NR rats on either the first or last day did not reveal a difference in body weight between groups (day 1: t(13) = 0.72, p = 0.486 and day 14: t(13) = 0.15, p = 0.881). Analysis of food intake showed that PR rats exhibited a mild hyperphagia as has been previously reported (Fig. 1D; mean difference in food intake between NR and PR rats: 3.77 g [95% CI 1.28, 6.88], p = 0.042) (Laeger et al., 2014).
Following 5 d of saccharin pretraining, rats received four daily conditioning sessions in which they had access to distinctly flavored solutions containing either casein (protein) or maltodextrin (carbohydrate; one session per day), alternated from day to day (Fig. 1B). Both groups similarly increased their consumption throughout conditioning (Fig. 1E; three-way ANOVA, Session: F(1,13) = 22.308, p < 0.0001) for both casein and maltodextrin (all F values < 1; all p values > 0.1). Thus, rats in both physiological states experienced the same exposure to casein and maltodextrin solutions in advance of the preference test session.
Protein preference is associated with elevated VTA response to protein over carbohydrate
Following conditioning sessions, we then recorded VTA responses during a test session (Fig. 1B). Rats first experienced 45 trials in which only one bottle was available at a time (forced choice trials), similar to conditioning sessions.
Across all forced choice trials, rats exhibited similar licking behavior for casein and maltodextrin (Fig. 2A; two-way ANOVA: all F values < 1; all p values > 0.1). However, PR rats did show shorter latencies to drink for casein than for maltodextrin (Fig. 2B; two-way ANOVA, Diet: F(1,13) = 4.83, p = 0.047; Solution: F(1,13) = 9.52, p = 0.009; Diet × Solution: F(1,13) = 5.83, p = 0.031; paired mean difference in latency between casein and maltodextrin for PR rats: −2.48 s [95% CI −3.65, −1.03], p = 0.011). In addition, PR rats on average spent more time closer to the casein sipper than the maltodextrin sipper (Fig. 2C,D; two-way ANOVA, Diet: F(1,12) = 0.20, p = 0.661; Solution: F(1,12) = 0.50, p = 0.492; Diet × Solution: F(1,12) = 5.03, p = 0.045; paired mean difference in distance to casein and maltodextrin sippers in NR rats: −33.2 mm [95% CI −96.3, 59.5], p = 0.375; paired mean difference in PR rats: 63.9 mm [95% CI −11.3, 90.1], p = 0.026). There was no difference in total distance moved between NR and PR rats (NR: 97.78 ± 13.76 mm; PR: 112.83 ± 9.20; unpaired t test: t(13) = 0.87, p = 0.400).
Photometry recordings of VTA neurons during consumption of each solution (Fig. 3A,B) showed that casein and maltodextrin consumption evoked similar VTA responses in NR rats (paired mean difference in AUC between casein and maltodextrin in NR rats: 0.80 [95% CI −0.46, 2.17], p = 0.354). In contrast, although PR rats licked similarly for both solutions (Fig. 2A), casein consumption is associated with a higher VTA response than for maltodextrin (Fig. 3C; two-way ANOVA, Diet: F(1,13) = 0.60, p = 0.454; Solution: F(1,13) = 20.73, p = 0.0005; Diet × Solution: F(1,13) = 10.39, p = 0.007; paired mean difference in AUC between casein and maltodextrin in PR rats: 4.66 [95% CI 3.27, 6.41], p = 0.0026). No differences were found in neural activity in the 5 s epoch following termination of licking (Fig. 3D; two-way ANOVA, Diet: F(1,13) = 0.96, p = 0.346; Solution: F(1,13) = 1.80, p = 0.203; Diet × Solution: F(1,13) = 0.05, p = 0.824). Moreover, differences in VTA responses were not attributable to differences in baseline activity between the two diet conditions (Fig. 3E; unpaired t test: t(13) = 0.30, p = 0.769).
We examined whether there were differences in how long the photometry signal took to peak during each trial and whether this was correlated with the latency to lick (Fig. 4). We found that in NR rats there was no difference between casein and maltodextrin trials in latency to peak calcium response (Fig. 4A; from sipper extension; Mann–Whitney U: p = 0.743). Moreover, on a trial-to-trial basis, the latency to peak showed a moderate but significant correlation with latency to lick for both casein trials (Pearson correlation coefficient: r = 0.27, p = 0.0014) and maltodextrin trials (r = 0.20, p = 0.020).
In contrast, for PR rats, the latency for the photometry signal to peak did differ between casein and maltodextrin trials (Fig. 4B; Mann–Whitney U: p < 0.001); furthermore, there was a highly significant correlation with latency to lick on casein trials (r = 0.45, p < 0.0001), but no correlation for maltodextrin trials (r = 0.11, p = 0.148). These findings for PR rats are likely because of the neural activation at time of licking on maltodextrin trials being greatly reduced for this group of rats.
Following these forced choice trials, rats were presented with 20 trials in which both bottles were available at the same time (free choice trials) to confirm the existence of protein preference in the PR group (Murphy et al., 2018; Naneix et al., 2020). In free choice trials, PR rats significantly licked more for casein than for maltodextrin (Fig. 5A; two-way ANOVA, Diet: F(1,13) = 5.12, p = 0.041; Solution: F(1,13) = 1.75, p = 0.208; Diet × Solution: F(1,13) = 14.96, p = 0.002; mean paired difference in licks between casein and maltodextrin for PR rats: 442.22 [95% CI 127.33, 587.22], p = 0.006), whereas NR rats did not (mean paired difference in licks between casein and maltodextrin for NR rats: −216.67 [95% CI −464.67, −16.16], p = 0.121). Consistent with this result, PR and NR rats exhibited differential casein preference, as calculated by the number of times they chose casein during the free choice trials (mean difference in choice preference between NR and PR rats: difference between groups: 0.49 [95% CI 0.23, 0.66], p = 0.004). As such, NR rats showed no preference for one solution over the other (preference for NR rats: 0.37 [95% CI 0.23, 0.52], p = 0.121 vs 50%), but PR rats displayed a strong preference for casein (Fig. 5B; preference for PR rats: 0.85 [95% CI 0.58, 0.95], p = 0.0064 vs 50%).
Preference toward protein develops with minimal experience in a newly protein-restricted state
Next, we were interested in what would happen to behavior and neural activity when rats' protein needs changed. First, we investigated what happened when rats from the control group were switched to the PR diet (hereafter, NR → PR rats). Importantly, we retested rats at two time points: 1 week after diets were switched but before any intervening experience of the casein and maltodextrin solutions (Fig. 6A; Preference test 2); and 1 week after this, once rats had experienced an extra block of conditioning sessions (Fig. 6G; Preference test 3).
As reported in Preference test 1 (see above), animals licked similarly for casein and maltodextrin during forced choice trials in Preference test 2 (Fig. 6B; mean paired difference in licks between casein and maltodextrin: 3.5 [95% CI −69.5, 36.0], p = 0.817) but slightly increased the licking for casein in Preference test 3 (Fig. 6H; mean paired difference: 81.50 [95% CI 50.00, 111.17], p < 0.001). Similarly, analysis of latencies indicated no difference during Preference test 2 (Fig. 6C; mean paired difference in latency between casein and maltodextrin: −0.07 s [95% CI −0.94, 0.73] p = 0.974) but showed shorter latencies to drink from the casein sipper during Preference test 3 (Fig. 6I; mean paired difference: −2.22 s [95% CI −3.91, −1.23], p = 0.030).
On free choice trials, NR → PR rats licked more for casein than maltodextrin during both Preference test 2 (Fig. 6D; mean paired difference in licks between casein and maltodextrin: 330.00 [95% CI 176.33, 440.17], p < 0.001) and Preference test 3 (Fig. 6J; mean paired difference: 623.17 [95% CI 511.17, 689.83], p < 0.001). As expected, this pattern resulted in strong casein preference over maltodextrin on Preference test 2 (preference: 0.71 [95% CI 0.60, 0.83], p = 0.030 vs 50%) and Preference test 3 (preference: 0.95 [95% CI 0.83, 0.98], p = 0.030 vs 50%).
The casein preference reported in Preference test 2 and Preference test 3 in NR → PR rats strongly contrasts with behavior during Preference test 1 (Fig. 5). Interestingly, photometry recordings during forced choice trials did not show any difference in VTA responses to casein and maltodextrin in either Preference test 2 (Fig. 6E,F; mean paired difference in AUC between casein and maltodextrin: 1.59 [95% CI −0.92, 4.95] p = 0.381) or Preference test 3 (Fig. 6K,L; mean paired difference: 2.53 [95% CI −1.84, 4.37], p = 0.097).
In summary, NR → PR rats developed a rapid behavioral preference to protein over carbohydrate that was observed even before they had gained extensive experience with each solution. Activity in VTA, however, was slower to change to reflect the rats' new physiological state and behavior.
Protein preference and differences in associated VTA activity disappear after experience with nutrient solutions in protein replete state
We also investigated the effect of protein repletion on casein preference and VTA responses using a similar diet switch design in rats that were initially protein-restricted and were changed changed to NR diet (hereafter, PR → NR rats). Again, rats were tested 1 week following the diet switch but before being given additional experience with solutions (Preference test 2; Fig. 7A) and then, again, after a block of conditioning sessions (Preference test 3; Fig. 7G).
During forced choice trials, there was no difference in the number of licks for casein and maltodextrin in Preference test 2 (Fig. 7B; mean paired difference in licks between casein and maltodextrin: 34.67 [95% CI −42.44, 100.44], p = 0.386) or Preference test 3 (Fig. 7H; mean paired difference: −25.00 [95% CI −141.78, 64.33], p = 0.682). The latency to drink from the casein sipper was still shorter than the latency for maltodextrin in Preference test 2 (Fig. 7C; mean paired difference in latency between casein and maltodextrin: −2.11 s [95% CI −2.95, −1.22], p = 0.003), but this difference disappeared in Preference test 3 after additional conditioning sessions (Fig. 7I; mean paired difference: −0.24 s [95% CI −0.91, 0.65], p = 0.561).
On free choice trials, there was now no significant difference in the number of licks between casein and maltodextrin during Preference test 2 (Fig. 7D; mean paired difference in licks between casein and maltodextrin: 189.22 [95% CI 19.89, 380.44], p = 0.099), although when number of choices was considered, as a group, PR → NR rats still showed a moderate preference for casein over maltodextrin (preference: 0.68 [95% CI 0.57, 0.79], p = 0.020 vs 50%). In Preference test 3 after additional conditioning sessions, casein preference was completely abolished for both licking (Fig. 7J; mean paired difference in licks between casein and maltodextrin: −11.78 [95% CI −294.11, 279.67], p = 0.922) and choices (preference: 0.48 [95% CI 0.26, 0.68], p = 0.889 vs 50%).
When VTA neural activity was analyzed during forced choice trials, we found that there was still greater VTA activation on casein trials than maltodextrin trials during Preference test 2, although the effect size was more variable than on Preference test 1 (Fig. 7E,F; mean paired difference in AUC between casein and maltodextrin 3.86 [95% CI 1.54, 8.17], p = 0.028). Consistent with the abolition of casein preference reported during Preference test 3, analysis of VTA neural activity also now showed no reliable difference between casein and maltodextrin in forced choice trials, although there was a high degree of variability (Fig. 7K,L; mean paired difference in AUC between casein and maltodextrin 3.24 [95% CI 0.47, 6.37], p = 0.091). Thus, the protein preference and associated VTA responses that developed when rats were protein-restricted was markedly reduced once rats had gained additional experience with the nutrient solutions in the new protein replete state.
Behavior and VTA activity become uncoupled after diet switch
To compare across all sessions for each group of rats, we examined how protein preference changed from Preference test 1 to Preference test 3. After the switch from NR to PR state (NR → PR rats), there was a clear shift in behavior across the three sessions as shown by a main effect of Session (Fig. 8A; one-way repeated-measures ANOVA: F(2,10) = 27.01, p < 0.0001). Further comparisons showed that after diet switch NR → PR rats' behavior differed both before additional conditioning sessions (mean paired difference in preference between Preference test 2 and Preference test 1: 0.34 [95% CI 0.16, 0.52], p = 0.007) and after (mean paired difference between Preference test 3 and Preference test 1; 0.58 [95% CI 0.43, 0.73], p = 0.001). However, consistent with our earlier analysis, VTA responses to casein and maltodextrin did not significantly change between the three preference tests (Fig. 8B; two-way repeated ANOVA: Session F(2,10) = 0.49, p = 0.625; Solution (F(1,5) = 5.74, p = 0.06; Session × Solution F(2,10) = 0.67, p = 0.534).
In contrast, protein repletion (PR → NR rats) induced a gradual decrease in casein preference across the three tests (Fig. 8D; one-way repeated ANOVA: F(2,16) = 5.99, p = 0.011). Between sessions comparisons showed that casein preference in the second test session, when rats had not received additional conditioning, was no different to the first test session (mean paired difference in preference between Preference test 2 and Preference test 1: −0.17 [95% CI −0.31, 0.09], p = 0.119). However, by the third test session, there was a significant decrease in casein preference compared with the first session (mean paired difference in preference between Preference test 3 and Preference test 1: −0.37 [95% CI −0.60, −0.09], p = 0.018). This shift in casein preference is associated with a trend toward a decrease in VTA responses to both casein and maltodextrin through the three sessions (Fig. 8E; two-way repeated-measures ANOVA: Session F(2,16) = 3.80, p = 0.08; Session × Solution F(2,16) = 0.48, p = 0.624). However, VTA responses to casein remained higher than responses to maltodextrin (Solution: F(1,8) = 12.77, p = 0.007).
The relationship between casein preference and neural activation to each solution is summarized in Figure 8C, F. Performing a simple linear regression between behavior (casein preference) and photometry (difference in z score between casein and maltodextrin trials) yielded weak to moderate correlations for each group with this relationship being significant for PR → NR rats (Pearson's correlation, r = 0.41, p = 0.034; Fig. 6C) but not for NR → PR rats (r = 0.23, p = 0.350; Fig. 6F).
Next, we performed multivariate linear regression on these data including test day as a predictor and found higher β coefficients associated with behavior than with photometry supporting our finding that protein preference changed more readily across the dietary manipulations than did neural activity (β coefficients for behavior: 2.51 and −1.55 for NR → PR rats and PR → NR rats, respectively; β coefficients for photometry: 0.02 and 0.02 for NR → PR rats and PR → NR rats, respectively). In addition, β coefficients for behavior were oppositely signed in each diet group reflecting the bidirectional change in behavior.
Finally, to check whether behavior and photometry measurements were more closely related to state of protein deprivation we recoded data based on each animal's current dietary state and reran the regression. Once again, we found that higher β values were associated with behavior than with photometry (behavior: 1.32 and 0.76 for NR → PR rats and PR → NR rats, respectively; photometry: −0.01 and 0.01 for NR → PR rats and PR → NR rats, respectively).
These analyses and visual inspection of the data suggested that changes in VTA responses after diet switch may have been obscured by interindividual variability in responses. To explore this further, we chose to look at differences in VTA activity on a rat-by-rat basis. By comparing activity on individual trials, rather than the mean of these trials, we calculated for each rat whether there was significantly greater activation to casein or to maltodextrin (Fig. 8G). For NR → PR rats, no rats showed a significantly greater activation to either nutrient on Preference test 1. However, after switching to the PR diet, a progressively greater proportion showed significantly greater activation to casein (33% for Preference test 2, 66% for Preference test 3). For PR → NR rats, results were strikingly different. On Preference test 1, a majority of rats (56%) showed significantly greater activation on casein trials than on maltodextrin trials. After switching to control diet, this changed little, with a large proportion continuing to show greater activation on casein than on maltodextrin trials (44% on both Preference test 2 and Preference test 3).
In summary, protein preference behavior changed strongly and rapidly in a bidirectional manner in both groups of rats, whereas shifts in VTA neural activity were not as apparent, especially in PR → NR rats.
Discussion
Animals prioritize protein intake over the intake of other macronutrients (Morrison and Laeger, 2015). However, the neural mechanisms underpinning this behavioral process are not well understood. Here, for the first time, we show that protein restriction changes neural activity in the VTA during the consumption of protein or carbohydrate to reflect the initial protein preference. Furthermore, we also demonstrate that protein preference is dependent on current physiological state and can be induced or abolished according to protein needs. Interestingly, VTA nutrient-related responses are highly dependent on the animal's prior experience in protein-restricted or non-restricted state, appearing slower than behavior to adapt to new physiological status.
Protein appetite is associated with increased VTA activity
Consistent with our earlier studies (Murphy et al., 2018; Naneix et al., 2020), PR rats developed a strong preference for protein-containing solution over carbohydrate-containing solution. Protein preference did not coincide with a general aversion to the carbohydrate as rats consumed similar amounts of both casein and maltodextrin during conditioning and forced choice trials. This differs from responses seen to diets lacking single amino acids that can lead to development of conditioned taste aversion for foods with imbalanced amino acid content (Maurin et al., 2005; Gietzen and Aja, 2012).
VTA neurons play a complex role in the control of food-related behaviors (Berridge, 2007; Bromberg-Martin et al., 2010; Brown et al., 2012; Zessen et al., 2012; Root et al., 2020). Previous studies show that dopamine signaling originating in the VTA is involved in establishing carbohydrate-based flavor preferences (Sclafani et al., 2011; de Araujo et al., 2012; McCutcheon, 2015; Hsu et al., 2018). Here, we show, for the first time, that protein appetite involves VTA circuits and that VTA activation is modulated by both the macronutrient content of the food and the rats' protein status during the initial preference test (Fig. 2). Specifically, VTA responses are greater during consumption of protein (casein) compared with carbohydrate (maltodextrin) selectively in PR rats. These differences in VTA activity are observed during forced choice trials, in which only one solution is available, but this difference in activity reflects future food preference in the subsequent free choice trials. Importantly, this difference is not the result of different behavioral activation as rats exhibited similar levels of licking. Differences in VTA responses to the consumption of each nutrient may reflect reward value and be used to guide food preferences (Berridge, 2007; Roitman et al., 2008; Bromberg-Martin et al., 2010; McCutcheon et al., 2012b; Salamone and Correa, 2012). In addition, PR rats exhibited a shorter latency for casein consumption (Fig. 2), suggesting an increase in incentive properties of this solution (Barbano and Cador, 2005). We previously reported that protein appetite was associated with increased casein palatability (Murphy et al., 2018; Naneix et al., 2020).
Using ex vivo voltammetry recordings, we recently showed that protein restriction increased evoked dopamine release in the NAc, but not dorsal striatum (Naneix et al., 2021). Similar changes have been reported with other nutrients (McCutcheon, 2015) and hunger states (Heffner et al., 1980), which may be used to reinforce and guide food-seeking behaviors toward the most relevant source of food. While firing of dopamine neurons does not always reflect terminal release (Sulzer et al., 2016; Mohebi et al., 2019), this result is consistent with our present in vivo observation in PR rats. There is now a need to characterize whether these increased VTA responses also translate into increased dopamine release in vivo, precisely where this release occurs in the forebrain, and how dopamine cell bodies or terminals may be able to detect dietary amino acids (Karnani et al., 2011).
VTA responses do not follow changes in initial protein preference
Changes in protein status after an initial nutrient preference resulted in different behavioral adaptations depending on the direction of diet shift. Rats experiencing a new protein deficiency (NR → PR; Fig. 4) rapidly shifted their preference toward casein even without additional conditioning, suggesting that protein appetite can manifest independently of prior experience with protein-containing food in a restricted state. Previous studies have demonstrated that an immediate specific appetite exists for another essential nutrient, sodium (Krause and Sakai, 2007). As such, sodium depletion induces immediate and unlearned alterations in how sodium is perceived and how animals respond to stimuli previously associated with sodium (Robinson and Berridge, 2013). However, sodium appetite is rapidly terminated once sodium levels are restored (Krause and Sakai, 2007). Such fine regulation was not observed with protein intake (PR → NR; Fig. 5) as casein preference only decreased in newly protein-replete (PR → NR) rats after experiencing additional conditioning sessions.
VTA responses to both casein and maltodextrin became more complex and did not immediately follow changes in protein preference. Newly PR rats (NR → PR; Fig. 4) exhibited delayed changes in VTA responses to casein and maltodextrin consumption, despite increased preference for protein. Previous studies have shown that unconditioned VTA dopamine responses to food or specific nutrients (Cone et al., 2014, 2016) update immediately, independently of prior experience of the physiological state (e.g., sodium depletion, hunger). In contrast, dopamine responses to food- or nutrient-predictive cues require multiple associations under physiological conditions in which the food is rewarding (Bassareo and Di Chiara, 1997; Day et al., 2007; Cone et al., 2016). Thus, our results suggest that VTA activity may track the value of the flavor paired with protein rather than the protein content itself (Sclafani et al., 2011; McCutcheon, 2015).
Protein repletion (PR → NR; Fig. 5) had a delayed impact on VTA activity, as rats continued to show elevated VTA responses to casein despite a progressive decrease of their protein preference. These results contrast starkly with those from studies of sodium appetite where VTA dopamine responses to conditioned cues are flexibly expressed in a state-dependent manner once learned (Cone et al., 2016). Instead, elevated VTA responses to casein, even after the initial behavioral preference was reversed, suggest a long-lasting neurobiological impact of protein restriction that may require extended time and prolonged learning to be reversed.
Methodological considerations
In this study, we used a targeting strategy that was not selective for dopamine neurons. As such, it is likely that some of the photometry signal resulted from activity in nondopamine populations of VTA neurons, including local GABA interneurons and projecting GABA or glutamate neurons (Dobi et al., 2010; Morales and Margolis, 2017), although, by number, dopamine neurons represent the largest proportion of VTA neurons (Nair-Roberts et al., 2008). In addition, the increases in neural activity evoked by behavioral events are qualitatively similar to those others have observed when recording only dopamine neurons (e.g., with TH::Cre rats) (Parker et al., 2016) or when recording dopamine release using voltammetry (Phillips et al., 2003). As other VTA neuronal populations are involved in different aspects of food-related behaviors (Brown et al., 2012; Zessen et al., 2012; Morales and Margolis, 2017; Root et al., 2020), future cell-specific targeting will be required to tease apart responses from these neuronal subtypes.
This study used only male rats, consistent with our previous study (Murphy et al., 2018). Protein (and other macronutrient) requirements differ in male and female rats at adulthood and through development (Leibowitz et al., 1991); and, in addition, total food intake changes across the estrus cycle with resulting effects on the proportion of protein intake (Wurtman and Baum, 1980). Moreover, physiological state influences the activity of VTA neurons in a sex-dependent manner (Godfrey and Borgland, 2020). Thus, a better understanding of brain mechanisms underlying protein appetite warrants further investigation in both males and females.
In conclusion, a key remaining question is how VTA midbrain circuits detect the nutrient content of food and integrate this with physiological state to regulate protein homeostasis. Previous work suggests that the VTA must receive taste information (Hajnal et al., 2004; Roitman et al., 2008; McCutcheon et al., 2012b). Protein can be detected via umami receptors expressed on taste buds (Chaudhari et al., 2009; Liman et al., 2014), but the link between protein sensing by the tongue and VTA neuronal populations remains to be explored. VTA circuits are also sensitive to the caloric content of food (de Araujo et al., 2008; Domingos et al., 2011; Beeler et al., 2012; Ferreira et al., 2012; McCutcheon et al., 2012a), and this information is relayed to forebrain regions controlling food-seeking behaviors (Tellez et al., 2016). Whether VTA neurons are sensitive to protein or amino acids directly is not known, but individual amino acid levels can be detected by hypothalamic, cortical, and hindbrain regions connected to the VTA (Karnani et al., 2011; Anthony and Gietzen, 2013; Heeley and Blouet, 2016; Tsang et al., 2020). Furthermore, recent work showed that fibroblast growth factor 21 (FGF21), a hepatic hormone, is released in response to reduction in dietary protein (Laeger et al., 2014), and its central action is necessary for development of protein preference in mice (Hill et al., 2019).
Given the potential effects of inadequate protein diet in utero or after birth on neurodevelopmental disorders (Grissom and Reyes, 2013; Gould et al., 2018) and obesity (Simpson and Raubenheimer, 2005), our results highlight neurobiological substrates that may underlie protein appetite in normal and pathologic conditions.
Footnotes
This work was supported by Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council Grant BB/M007391/1 to J.E.M., European Commission Grant GA 631404 to J.E.M., Leverhulme Trust Grant RPG-2017-417 to J.E.M. and J.A.-S., and Tromsø Research Foundation Grant 19-SG-JMcC to J.E.M. We thank the staff of the Division of Biomedical Services, Preclinical Research Facility, University of Leicester, for technical support and care of experimental animals; Heramb Chadchankar and Michelle Murphy for preliminary work; Vaibhav Konanur for developing the analytical method used to correct fluorescence traces; Leon Lagnado for kindly loaning equipment used in initial photometry experiments; and Andrew MacAskill for useful discussions regarding analysis.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to James Edgar McCutcheon at j.mccutcheon{at}uit.no
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.