Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Systems/Circuits

Dynamics of Neural Microstates in the VTA–Striatal–Prefrontal Loop during Novelty Exploration in the Rat

Ashutosh Mishra, Nader Marzban, Michael X Cohen and Bernhard Englitz
Journal of Neuroscience 11 August 2021, 41 (32) 6864-6877; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2256-20.2021
Ashutosh Mishra
1Synchronisation in Neural Systems Laboratory, Department of Neuroinformatics, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, 6500 HB, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
2Computational Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Neurophysiology, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, 6525 AJ, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Nader Marzban
1Synchronisation in Neural Systems Laboratory, Department of Neuroinformatics, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, 6500 HB, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Michael X Cohen
1Synchronisation in Neural Systems Laboratory, Department of Neuroinformatics, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, 6500 HB, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Michael X Cohen
Bernhard Englitz
2Computational Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Neurophysiology, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, 6525 AJ, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Neural activity at the large-scale population level has been suggested to be consistent with a sequence of brief, quasistable spatial patterns. These “microstates” and their temporal dynamics have been linked to myriad cognitive functions and brain diseases. Most of this research has been performed using EEG, leaving many questions, such as the existence, dynamics, and behavioral relevance of microstates at the level of local field potentials (LFPs), unaddressed. Here, we adapted the standard EEG microstate analysis to triple-area LFP recordings from 192 electrodes in rats to investigate the mesoscopic dynamics of neural microstates within and across brain regions during novelty exploration. We performed simultaneous recordings from the prefrontal cortex, striatum, and ventral tegmental area in male rats during awake behavior (object novelty and exploration). We found that the LFP data can be accounted for by multiple, recurring microstates that were stable for ∼60–100 ms. The simultaneous microstate activity across brain regions revealed rhythmic patterns of coactivations, which we interpret as a novel indicator of inter-regional, mesoscale synchronization. Furthermore, these rhythmic coactivation patterns across microstates were modulated by behavioral states such as movement and exploration of a novel object. These results support the existence of a functional mesoscopic organization across multiple brain areas and present a possible link of the origin of macroscopic EEG microstates to zero-lag neuronal synchronization within and between brain areas, which is of particular interest to the human research community.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The coordination of neural activity across the entire brain has remained elusive. Here we combine large-scale neural recordings at fine spatial resolution with the analysis of microstates (i.e., short-lived, recurring spatial patterns of neural activity). We demonstrate that the local activity in different brain areas can be accounted for by only a few microstates per region. These microstates exhibited temporal dynamics that were correlated across regions in rhythmic patterns. We demonstrate that these microstates are linked to behavior and exhibit different properties in the frequency domain during different behavioral states. In summary, LFP microstates provide an insightful approach to studying both mesoscopic and large-scale brain activation within and across regions.

  • LFP microstates
  • object exploration
  • PFC
  • rodent
  • striatum
  • VTA

Introduction

The coordination of neural activity across the entire brain has remained incompletely understood. At the same time, recent research has demonstrated the far-reaching influence of behavior and other state changes on the activity in distant and substantial parts of the brain (McGinley et al., 2015; Musall et al., 2019). Previous work has demonstrated that neural activity is structured into short-lived epochs of coordinated activity on the local scale (e.g., of small ensembles of cells; Luczak et al., 2009, 2015) and on the large scale (e.g., measured noninvasively; Lehmann et al., 1987; Pascual-Marqui et al., 1995).

On the local level, transient spatiotemporal patterns have been observed in visual cortex (Arieli et al., 1995; Chiu and Weliky, 2001; Kenet et al., 2003; Fiser et al., 2004; Omer et al., 2019), auditory cortex (Luczak et al., 2009; Sakata and Harris, 2009), and other cortical areas in the awake, anesthetized, and sleep state in various species using different recording techniques (Mao et al., 2001; Petersen et al., 2003; Battaglia et al., 2004; Massimini et al., 2004).

On the large-scale level, electroencephalography (EEG) has been a valuable tool to study whole-brain activity in humans. Here, transient spatiotemporal patterns have been identified and termed “EEG microstates.” EEG microstates are defined by clustering topographical maps around peaks in global field power (GFP; Lehmann et al., 1987) using modified k-means clustering (Murray et al., 2008). A large number of studies have shown that four clusters account for a large fraction of variance and are observed consistently across myriad studies spanning nearly 4 decades (Michel and Koenig, 2018). Microstates are used to study human cognition, including visual processing (Britz and Michel, 2011), perceptual awareness (Britz et al., 2014), neuropsychiatric disorders including schizophrenia (Dierks et al., 1997; Lehmann et al., 2005; Kindler et al., 2011; Tomescu et al., 2015; da Cruz et al., 2020), and resting-state networks (Britz et al., 2010; Musso et al., 2010; Yuan et al., 2012; Khanna et al., 2015; Bréchet et al., 2019; Zanesco et al., 2020).

The relation between large-scale EEG microstates and small-scale neural population spike correlations is poorly understood, in part because different analysis methods are applied at different scales (Alishbayli et al., 2019). Here we combined multichannel recordings (up to 192 electrodes across three brain areas) with the analysis techniques developed in the context of EEG microstates to investigate the dynamics of neural activity on the meso-scale [i.e., local field potentials (LFPs)]. We recorded LFPs simultaneously from the prefrontal cortex (PFC), striatum (STR), and ventral tegmental area (VTA) in rats, while they moved freely in an arena in which novel objects were introduced.

We observed reliable LFP microstates in each region that shared characteristics previously identified in EEG microstates, including robustness, temporal dynamics, temporal continuity, and relation to global field power (Khanna et al., 2015; Milz et al., 2016; Mishra et al., 2020). Using concepts from EEG microstate analysis, we verified the validity of these meso-scale, LFP microstates. For example, the LFP microstates explain a large proportion of the variance of the signal, in contrast to their temporally shuffled surrogates. Furthermore, we linked different microstate properties like occurrence rate, temporal coverage, and state duration to behavioral states. A striking finding was that LFP microstates appear to be coordinated across regions: microstate time series from distant brain regions exhibited distinct cross-correlation patterns that depended on the behavioral state, suggesting functional interactions of these brain regions, manifested by the microstate activation patterns. Functional coupling of these regions is a well established result (Gao et al., 2007; Zhang et al., 2016), which can be analyzed from a larger perspective using LFP microstates. Thus, we propose that the EEG microstate method is an insightful approach to identify, define, and characterize more local, transient states of neural activation. As proposed by Luczak et al. (2015), these microstates might form a vocabulary of neural activation that could help structure our understanding of whole-brain dynamics.

Materials and Methods

All experimental procedures were performed in accordance with the European Union directive on animal experimentation (2010/63/EU), and the Dutch nationally approved ethics project 2015–0129. All recordings were performed in the laboratory of M.X.C. We included four male Long–Evans TH:Cre rats (age, ∼3 months; weight, 350–450 g at the time of recordings). Rats were housed singly in Makrolon type III cages (40 × 30 × 35 cm; UNO) in a room with controlled temperature (21 ± 2°C) and humidity (60 ± 15%). Animals were kept on 12 h light/dark cycles, and the experiments were performed under the light phase.

Electrode implants

Each silicon probe was loaded on a plastic 3D-printed drive purchased from 3Dneuro (https://www.3dneuro.com). The 3Drives were lightweight (0.43 g), with a small footprint and base dimensions of 5 × 6 mm and height of 16.7 mm, allowing the travel distance of 9 mm and resolution of 62.5 µm per one-quarter turn. Rats were anesthetized with 5% isoflurane (2–3% maintenance) and placed in a stereotaxic frame. They were given a dose of Rimadyl/Carprofen (0.5 mg/kg) and Xylocaine (maximum, 0.4 ml) subcutaneously at the incision site. Small craniotomies on the skull were made above the VTA, STR, and PFC. The animals were implanted with custom-designed high-density silicon probes (NeuroNexus) with specific electrode layout of densely packed recording sites designed for each target region (Fig. 1A, red dots inside the coronal slices).

In PFC and STR, each probe contained four laminar shanks, having 16 recording electrodes per shank in forms of tetrodes for spike recordings and single sites for LFP recordings, thus creating 64 recording sites per probe. Since the focus of the current investigation is on LFP signals, spike recordings are not analyzed here. Sixty-four electrodes covered an area of 1 × 2 mm with typical spacing of 225 µm in each shank and 330 µm between shanks in PFC.

STR electrodes also covered an area of 1 × 2 mm with the same shank distance (330 µm). However, two shanks contained only tetrodes, and two shanks had only single sites with typical spacing of 130 µm between single sites and 660 µm between tetrodes.

VTA contained eight shanks of eight electrodes each and covered an area of 1.5 × 0.14 mm. All electrodes were referenced to a reference screw implanted on the skull over the cerebellum.

Implants were surrounded by and grounded to a copper mesh Faraday cage built around the implants and was fixed to the skull surface using dental cement (Vandecasteele et al., 2012). Rats were monitored daily for 1 week following surgery and recordings began at least 1 week after surgery. After recovery (minimum, 7 d), the electrodes were advanced into the brain toward a final target location of 4 mm below the skull for PFC, 5 mm for striatum, and 8 mm for the VTA.

Experiment design and apparatus

Animals were handled and trained for 1 week to freely move and get habitualized in a black plastic square box (length × base × height, 60 × 40 × 40 cm) open field covered with bedding in the bottom of the enclosure. The square enclosure was placed on an electrically grounded Faraday cage inside the recording room. In each session, rats were allowed to move freely in the box. A novel object (e.g., a cup or a toy) was presented in the middle of the box in the second session, and the same object was repeated in the fourth session (see Fig. 4A). Rats were allowed to explore the novel object freely. A camera was placed above the box to track movement. Camera resolution was 320 × 240 (width × height) and captured at a frame rate of 30 Hz. The alignment between camera and OpenEphys acquisition system was implemented using event markers. All sessions within a trial were recorded without any intersession delay. A maximum of one trial per animal was recorded on a single day. There were 27 recording trials in total.

Recording and cleaning of electrophysiology data

Electrophysiological data were sampled at 30 kHz via OpenEphys hardware and software solutions (Siegle et al., 2017). Offline, data were downsampled to 1 kHz and stored in the EEGLAB (Delorme and Makeig, 2004) format in MATLAB (MathWorks). Excessively noisy data segments were removed manually, and excessively noisy channels were removed from the data based on visual inspection of raw signals and variance thresholds (6.6 ± 5.2 channels per dataset per region). Per recording, removed segments comprised 6.2 ± 5.0% (median ± median absolute deviation) of all movement states and 2.0 ± 1.5% of all stationary states. Similarly, removed segments comprised 2.8 ± 2.8% of all object interaction states and 2.7 ± 2% of nonobject interaction states. Thus, removed noisy segments are significantly smaller than the rest of the datasets (p < 1e-14, Wilcoxon signed-rank test) in each behavioral state, ensuring that sufficient data are available for subsequent behavioral state-specific analyses. More importantly, several microstate features that we analyzed were normalized to the amount of data (e.g., the rate of occurrence), and the surrogate-based permutation tests shuffle the retained data and are thus robust for the amount of data. Thereafter, average referencing was performed separately for the set of electrodes located in each region. The datasets were then bandpass filtered in the range of 1–30 Hz, using a Hamming window sinc FIR (finite impulse response) filter, to be consistent with the common practice in EEG microstate analysis (Milz et al., 2017).

Microstate analysis

The microstate analysis was performed using the Microstate EEGLAB Toolbox (Poulsen et al., 2018). Before running the microstate analysis, data from all sessions were concatenated to create one continuous epoch of ∼20 min. Extracting microstates on the aggregated data improves the quality of the decomposition and facilitates a direct comparison of microstate dynamics across sessions.

Detailed descriptions of microstate extraction can be found in previous publications (Pascual-Marqui et al., 1995; Murray et al., 2008; Milz, 2016; Michel and Koenig, 2018; Poulsen et al., 2018; Mishra et al., 2020). Here we provide a brief overview; the following procedure was performed separately for each animal and each recording session. The first step is to compute GFP time series, which is the SD across channels. Microstates are extracted at local maxima of GFP, with the constraint that two GFP peaks must be separated by at least 10 ms. The second step is to cluster the spatial maps at GFP peaks according to a modified k-means method that has been adapted for EEG microstate analysis. We selected four clusters for subsequent analysis. The number of clusters was chosen based on the global explained variance (GEV) criterion (Eq. 2; Fig. 1C1–C3). We noticed that the incremental change in GEV saturated after three to five clusters. Moreover, cluster centers with similar spatial patterns started emerging after four clusters in most cases. Although the “correct” number of clusters is difficult to determine exactly, a consistent number of clusters across all recordings facilitates analyses and comparisons. Thus, we decided to extract four clusters for all datasets. Because k-means involves random initializations, we repeated the clustering algorithm 10 times using 10 different random starting values; the iteration with the highest total GEV was retained for the final analyses. The third step in microstate analysis is to “backfit” the microstates onto the data, which provides a time series containing the dominant microstate map at each time point; thus, successive time points with the same map are termed “microstates.” This is achieved by correlating the spatial map at each time point with the four microstate maps, and labeling each time point according to the map with the largest absolute correlation (absolute correlation is used to ensure polarity invariance of the backfitting). We set the minimum duration of a microstate to be 25 ms, and any microstates shorter than this are absorbed into the neighboring microstate.

Microstate properties estimation

We quantified the temporal pattern of occurrence of microstates using the following five properties.

Mean spatial correlation.

Defined as the mean Pearson correlation between LFP signals and their corresponding microstate map. We used the absolute value of the correlation for polarity-independent analyses, as follows: M=∑n=1N|Corr(xn,aln)|N,(1) where xn is the channel vector at time point n, aln is the microstate map l, and GFPn is global field power, where N is the length of the signal.

GEV.

Global explained variance is a measure of similarity between the data and the microstates. GEV for the nth sample of the data is defined as follows: GEVn=Corr(xn,aln)2GFPn2∑n=1NGFPn2.(2)

Occurrence rate.

The mean number of appearances of a microstate per second.

Temporal coverage.

The fraction of total time covered by all occurrences of a given microstate.

State duration.

Defined as the median duration of each microstate occurrence.

Although GEV and spatial correlation are correlated, they are separate measures, and we therefore report both.

Animal movement tracking and behavioral state determination

Animal movement tracking was performed using DeepLabCut (Mathis et al., 2018). We used this deep neural network-based method to track five body markers (i.e., snout, left ear, right ear, head center, and tail start; see Fig. 4A1,A2). We also tracked the location of the object center, for sessions in which an object was present. This was done because the objects could be moved around slightly as the animals explored it. A moving median filter with a window length of 13 frames was used to postprocess the movement-tracking results. This improved accuracy by removing isolated video frames with incorrect location of body markers and objects.

To determine the video frames where the animal was not moving, a threshold on the speed of the animal was manually set based on visual inspection (see Fig. 4D2).

We defined a radius around the object center, based on the size and shape of the object and determined separately for each object. For the extraction of frames where the rats were interacting with the object, the distance between the snout and the object center was computed. “Object interaction” was defined as frames in which this distance was less than the interaction radius. Two binary vectors, associated with two types of behavioral states–object interaction and movement, were upsampled to 1 kHz to match with the LFP data.

Microstate activation cross-correlation and their power spectra

The ephys data were projected onto the extracted microstates to generate microstate activation time series. Cross-correlations were performed on all pairs (within and across regions) of these microstate activation time series, up to a maximum lag of ±2000 ms (using xcorr in MATLAB). The power spectra of the cross-correlograms [cross-spectral density (CSD)] were estimated by taking the square of the discrete Fourier transform (fft in MATLAB) divided by the length of cross-correlograms. To facilitate comparison and subsequent analyses, power spectra were z-normalized by taking the SD over frequencies.

Power-spectrum clustering

The cross-correlation power spectrum profiles were clustered using the k-means method. Squared Euclidean distance criteria was used as the distance metric for the clustering. The optimal number of clusters, determined using silhouette measure, was two. Clustering was performed separately for each dataset.

Surrogate data for permutation testing

To test the statistical significance of GEV by four microstates, and the effect on microstate properties by the change in behavioral states, we generated surrogate datasets in which the local temporal structure per channel was preserved, while the temporal alignment between channels was randomized. This was achieved in the LFP data by choosing a random point in time for each channel and swapping the time series around these time points. This surrogate technique was chosen because it not only destroys the spatial alignment of the activity, but preserves the local temporal structure (see Fig. 3A–B). A complete randomization of the time points would remove all spatial and temporal structures, and thus result in a “straw-man” null-hypothesis against which any empirical microstate would be highly significant. One hundred surrogates were produced, and GEV values were calculated for each surrogate to compare with the GEV of the empirical data. We also generated surrogate movement data using the same method. Again, 100 surrogates were produced. Microstate properties for each microstate were calculated for the surrogate movement data and compared against their empirical value to compute the significance (see Fig. 5C1–C3).

To determine a significance threshold for the cross-correlation analyses, we used 50 randomly shuffled surrogates per dataset (total of 50 × 27 = 1350 shufflings), and computed microstates and cross-correlation spectra from each shuffled dataset, as described above. From each of 27 datasets, we computed the 99th percentile of the shuffled CSD distribution. Finally, we took the median of the 99th percentile spectra across the 27 datasets as our final statistical threshold seen in Figure 6, E and F.

Statistical analysis involving behavior

Statistical analysis on microstate properties and their relationship with the behavioral states was performed without distributional assumptions, by using permutation tests. Statistical analysis on behavioral state durations and power spectra peak frequencies were performed using a two-sided Wilcoxon rank-sum test.

Results

We investigated the existence and dynamics of coordinated neural activity over three brain regions (PFC, STR, and VTA) in chronic recordings in behaving rats (N = 4; Fig. 1A). Recordings were collected simultaneously using up to 192 electrodes (64 per region) in conjunction with video recordings. We used microstate analysis, an established technique for detecting short-lived states of joint activation from the EEG literature (Lehmann, 1989; Michel and Koenig, 2018; Mishra et al., 2020) and deep learning-based video tracking (Mathis et al., 2018) to relate the occurrence of the detected microstates with ongoing behavioral states. We start by introducing the analysis technique to isolate microstates in multichannel LFP data.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

LFP signals recorded from multiple sensors can be represented by LFP microstates. A, Neural recordings were concurrently collected from three brain regions (PFC (navy), STR (maroon), VTA (orange) in awake behaving rats. Large-scale electrode arrays (red dots) covered a coronal plane in each brain area (slices show approximate anteroposterior locations, taken from the Sprague Dawley rat atlas by Papp et al., 2014). The probe contained 16 recording electrodes per shank in PFC and STR. B, LFP data excerpt from all channels recorded in each brain region (3 × 64, top) and GFP (bottom). Microstate maps were extracted based on the local peaks of the GFP, using a modified k-means clustering algorithm (Michel and Koenig, 2018; Poulsen et al., 2018; Mishra et al., 2020; for details, see Materials and Methods). C1–C3, Cumulative global explained variance saturated after three to five microstates in all three regions. Gray lines indicate results from individual datasets, thick lines indicate the mean. D, The spatial patterns of LFP signals (top left) can be captured by a small number of microstate maps (top right; e.g., data and corresponding microstates at two GFP peaks, 331 and 751 ms). We focused on the four dominant microstates in each region (bottom, different colors). The current microstate is determined by correlation between microstates and the current LFP. E, Microstate maps extracted from each region while the animal was freely moving in an open field environment (A, top left). The left-most maps also show the electrode locations. Map values were spatially spline interpolated between electrode locations. Maps in D and E have the same color scale as that shown in D. D and E are from rat 1, recording day 1.

Large-scale LFP signals are accounted for by a small set of microstates

Multiarray, multiarea LFP signals were acquired simultaneously (Fig. 1B) and subjected to microstate analysis separately per region. The microelectrode array geometry enabled the acquisition of neural signals from a wide area (0.15−2 mm2). In each area, the spatial activity could be accounted for by a small number of microstates (i.e., joint activity patterns), whose residence time ranged between a few tens and a few hundreds of milliseconds (Fig. 1D, sample from PFC). Microstates are recurring spatial patterns of neural recordings. Since all similar maps can be grouped together, the representative structures from each group can be considered as the representation of the entire neural signal. Thus, the segmentation of the LFP signal into microstates is based on spatial correlation of neural activity (for details, see Materials and Methods). Qualitatively, this becomes apparent when the LFP data and corresponding microstate are visualized together (Fig. 1D, top; quantified in Table 1). The first four microstates accounted for 68% of the variance in the example shown (Table 1, details; see Fig. 8, region-specific details). The exact number of significant microstates from a clustering analysis can be difficult to ascertain (Murray et al., 2008; Michel and Koenig, 2018); we chose to use four microstates per region for each recording day, based on the total amount of explained variance (Fig. 1C1–C3), to facilitate analyses across days. Although other choices may yield slightly different results, our experience is that the patterns of findings are robust to the exact number of microstates.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1

Summary of microstate properties

Microstate topographies exhibited a mixture of distributed spatial features (Fig. 1E, left, Microstate1 for STR) and restricted local activity (Fig. 1E, middle left, Microstate2 for STR). Importantly, electrodes in the array were individually inspected for spurious recordings (e.g., caused by unreasonably high variance or large differences in impedance; for details, see Materials and Methods). Electrodes with excessive variance or artifacts were excluded from subsequent analysis, ensuring that the microstates reflected spatiotemporally coherent activation patterns based on well sampled neural activity.

Microstate maps are robust over multiple days

We repeated the experiment in the same animals over multiple days to determine the stability of microstate maps over time. The microstate analysis was performed separately on the recording of each day (between 5 and 9 d/animal). We found that spatial correlations of microstate maps across days were high (Fig. 2), indicating that many of the maps recurred on different days. Other microstates were less stable and were identified on some but not all days (Fig. 2A1, blue/green, Microstate3/4 in PFC). It is possible that these “missing” microstates were sorted into smaller clusters that were excluded from the primary analysis because of lower signal-to-noise ratios or infrequent occurrences.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Microstate maps are reproducible across different days. Simultaneous LFP signals were recorded on six different days (each day with a new object) in PFC, STR, and VTA. Microstate maps from each region on the first day were chosen as reference maps. Maps are rotated counterclockwise by 90° for PFC and STR and rescaled for visualization purposes. A1, In the PFC, the squared correlations among the four maps on each day and average microstate maps were computed, and are shown with red, black, blue, and green lines for microstates 1–4, respectively. Lower correlation values on certain days can be loosely interpreted as the absence of the Microstate on that day (in reference to the average Microstate). A2, Microstate (MS) maps are reordered based on their best match (A1). Most microstate maps retained the spatial features on each recording day. (A1 and A2 are from animal 1 in A3). A3, The fraction of maps that were repeated over time for each of four animals. B, C, These rows show the same results as described above, but for data from the striatum and VTA.

To assess the presence of microstate maps on experiment repeats, we averaged all highly similar maps (per region and per animal) together as their first principal component. We refer to these maps as subject average microstate maps. Squared spatial correlations between the subject average microstate maps and microstate maps on each experiment day were computed as an indicator of microstate recurrence (Fig. 2A1,B1,C1, example from one animal). We used a threshold of 0.5 (squared spatial correlation) to determine that a microstate repeated, although the exploration of a range of threshold values (0.4–0.7) yielded qualitatively similar results. In all three regions, the same set of microstates, within the same animal, showed a stronger repeatability over separate recording days (Fig. 2A3,B3,C3).

Spatiotemporal LFP structure underlies the explanatory power of microstate maps

The previous analyses demonstrate that microstates are rather stable over multiple days. We next investigated how much variance in the total signal is explained by the microstates. We quantified this using the GEV, which reflects the correlation between the signal at each time point and the microstate maps. Using four microstates per region accounted for 71 ± 6%,71 ± 9%, and 64 ± 10% (mean ± SD) of the global variance in PFC, STR, and VTA, respectively. We tested whether these GEV values were statistically significant by generating a null-hypothesis distribution, generated by shuffling the data in a way that preserved the local temporal structure but distorted global temporal structure (see Materials and Methods; Fig. 3A–B, illustration). Microstate analysis was rerun 100 times with different random surrogate datasets, and subsequently the GEV was recomputed.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Microstate maps represent the spatiotemporal structure of LFP activity. A1, Surrogate LFP datasets with conserved local structure were created by pivoting the data around a random time point (independently selected per trial; blue, before pivot point; red, after pivot point). A2, The data were swapped around these pivot points to generate a surrogate dataset. A1 and A2 illustrate the principle of the shuffling method; the actual recordings were ∼20 min long. The shuffling strategy disrupts the spatial structure while imposing minimal disruption to the time series within each channel (vertical line at 300 ms as an illustration). B, Microstate analysis was performed on these surrogate datasets, and GEV was computed. The corresponding GEV distribution for one of the recordings (C, red circle) showed a significant decrease in GEV in the surrogate data, indicating that the original temporal alignment between channels is underlying the microstate maps explanatory value. C, GEVs of the empirical data were always higher than the corresponding median GEV from surrogate datasets in all three brain regions, showing statistical significance of the microstate results.

We found that this removal of spatiotemporal LFP patterns in the surrogate data resulted in a significant drop in GEV (p < 1e-35; Fig. 3C). The GEV of the shuffled data were only 41 ± 9%, 37 ± 5%, and 31 ± 4% compared with ∼70% for the original data (see above), corresponding to a significant reduction by 43%, 47%, and 51% (p < 1e-35, permutation test) in PFC, STR, and VTA, respectively. GEV values differed considerably across regions (Fig. 3C), but in all cases using the spatial surrogates dropped the GEV significantly. In conclusion, microstates capture repeatedly occurring spatiotemporal activation patterns, a small number of which are sufficient to explain up to ∼80% of the total LFP variance.

Region-wise correlations between shuffled data GEVs and empirical data GEVs were not significant (Fig. 3C; PFC: r = 0.33, p = 0.09; STR: r = 0.23, p = 0.24; VTA: r = 0.058, p = 0.78).

Linking microstates to behavioral states

During the recordings, animals interacted with a novel or a familiar object in the arena. We subdivided the recording session along the following two behavioral dimensions: (1) “movement related” (i.e., stationarity or in motion); and (2) “object exploration related” (i.e., interacting or not interacting with the object). Tracking of the animal and the object was performed using DeepLabCut (version 2.1; Mathis et al., 2018), and each time point was assigned a binary value for each of the two dimensions based on empirically determined thresholds (Fig. 4; for details, see Materials and Methods). Each recording day comprised four sessions (Fig. 4B); movement tracking was performed separately for each session and then later pooled.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Automated movement tracking to segregate behavioral states. A1, An example video frame during the experiment. A2, Animal movement is accurately tracked using DeepLabCut, shown here using the same frame as in A1. Animal movement was tracked using five body markers (blue, black, red, green, and yellow dots represent, respectively, snout, tail start, left ear, right ear, and head center). B, Each experimental recording consisted of four sessions (∼5 min each session). Sessions 1 and 3 were open field recordings, while session 2 included a novel object (different each day) in the middle of the box. In session 4, the same object was present. Rats spend less time interacting with familiar objects than novel objects (each dot represents snout location in each frame; red dots show time points labeled as interaction). A small number of erroneous object locations were filtered out before analysis. C, The distribution of temporal coverage of each behavioral state (rest, movement, object interaction) across sessions was dominated by periods of rest. Object interaction can take place during rest as well as during movement, represented as an overlap in behavioral states. D1, Median temporal coverage from all recordings captures the trends over all sessions, recordings, and animals (N = 4). D2, Median distribution of animal speed movement from all the recordings. The dashed line shows the motion speed threshold separating resting from movement.

Animals spent most of their time in the stationary state (median ± SD, 88 ± 5%) followed by movement state (12 ± 5%). Next, we classified these two behavioral states further into object interaction and no object interaction and found that the fraction of time spent on object interaction mostly took place during stationary state (5 ± 4%) compared with interaction during moving state (0.9 ± 0.7%; Fig. 4C). Per recording, we removed 6.2 ± 5.0% (median ± median absolute deviation) of all movement states and 2.0 ± 1.5% of all stationary states. Similarly, removed segments consisted of 2.8 ± 2.8% of all object interaction states and 2.7 ± 2% of non-object interaction states. The rejected segment lengths are significantly lower than the remaining data in each behavioral state (p < 1e-14, Wilcoxon signed-rank test). Thus, overall only a small fraction of data from all the behavioral states is removed during data cleaning.

We next examined the evolution of behavioral states as time progressed. First, we compared the sessions in which the object was not present (sessions 1 and 3). We found that the animals spent more time in a stationary state in the later session (session 3, 93 ± 6%) compared with the earlier session (session 1, 85 ± 8%; p = 0.00,017, two-sided Wilcoxon signed-rank test). Next, we compared the time spent in the stationary state when the object was introduced (sessions 2 and 4). We again found that animals spent more time in the stationary state in the later session (90 ± 5%) compared with the earlier session (83 ± 6%; p = 0.00,017, two-sided Wilcoxon signed-rank test). A novel object was first presented in session 2, and the same object was again presented in session 4. The fraction of time spent on object exploration in session 4 (9 ± 12%) was significantly less (p = 0.0021, two-sided Wilcoxon signed-rank test) than in session 2 (15 ± 12%). There were no trends along experiment repeats, indicating that the animals remained interested in the novel objects each day.

Microstate dynamics were modulated by behavioral state

We quantified three microstate properties during each behavioral state—occurrence rate, temporal coverage, and state duration (for definitions, see Materials and Methods)—and reported their summaries in Table 1 (see Fig. 8). We separated these features for each microstate and brain region, and for the behavioral states “stationary” and “movement.” All three microstate properties in all three brain regions were modulated by behavioral state (Fig. 5A, example from one recording in PFC). Furthermore, these modulations were consistent across the different recording days (Fig. 5B1–B3, one animal in PFC). Microstates followed the same direction of state-related changes in most of the repeats (PFC, 76 ± 13%; STR, 82 ± 14%; VTA 82 ± 15%), indicating a systematic relationship between microstate properties and behavioral states.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Microstate parameters are affected by behavioral state. A, Example results from the recording in PFC (rat 1, recording day 3). A1, Temporal coverage fraction for all four microstates differed in the two behavioral states (statistics are in C1). A2, Occurrence rate during the two behavioral states (solid bars on the left refer to stationary periods, and lighter bars on the right refer to movement periods; the four numbers refer to the different microstates). A3, Similarly, microstate durations were modulated by behavioral state. B1–B3, The modulation of microstate properties by behavioral state (expressed as stationary minus movement) was consistently in the same direction (PFC, 76 ± 13%) across 6 different recording days (each dot corresponds to a day; only microstates with r2 > 0.5 repeatability are shown here). C1–C3, Similar to B but for data pooled across 27 novel objects in four rats. Significance was assessed by comparison against surrogate datasets (thin horizontal lines indicate a significance threshold of z = ±1.96). There was a high fraction of significant results for all three microstate properties in all three brain regions.

We assessed the statistical significance of the behavioral state dependency by generating a null-hypothesis distribution based on surrogate behavioral states, obtained by random temporal cuts of the time series of behavioral labels (Fig. 3A; also see Materials and Methods). Temporal coverage changed significantly between behavioral states, as all three regions showed a high fraction of significant changes (|z| > 1.96) exceeding 0.05 (fraction of significant tests: PFC, 0.76; STR, 0.79; VTA, 0.66). The occurrence rate showed a similar trend. The changes in the occurrence rate were also significant in all three regions (PFC, 0.35; STR, 0.44; VTA, 0.52). State duration was less affected by behavior (PFC, 0.22; STR, 0.23; VTA, 0.08; Fig. 5C). We also repeated these analyses for object exploration-related behavioral states and observed qualitatively similar results.

In summary, temporal dynamics of microstates were significantly affected by changes in behavioral state. The most significant changes were observed for temporal coverage.

Relationship of microstate dynamics across brain regions

We speculated that if microstates reflect a mesoscale organizational principle, then functional connectivity across brain regions might manifest as correlated microstate temporal dynamics. To investigate this possibility, we computed cross-correlations across all pairs of microstate activation time series, defined as the projection of sensor-level signals at each time point onto the microstate basis vectors, in the three regions (144 cross-correlation pairs in total, of which 12 are autocorrelations and 66 are inter-regional, omitting pairs that only differ by sequence; Fig. 6A). All cross-correlation and autocorrelation curves followed a damped oscillatory nature with an average peak of 9 ± 6 ms. The peak amplitude of cross-correlations varied substantially across microstate combinations and across animals (mean ± SD; absolute correlation value, 0.17 ± 0.07).

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Microstate activation time series are rhythmically correlated within and between brain areas. A–E2, Illustrative results from one recording session (rat 1, recording day 2). A, Microstate time series cross-correlations show damped oscillatory dynamics across microstates both within regions and across regions. B, Example cross-correlation between activation time series of microstate2 from STR and microstate2 from VTA (this is a zoomed-in version of the boxed plot in A). The typical cross-correlation structure is absent in shuffled data (red line). C, The z-normalized power spectrum of the cross-correlation (cross-spectral density) shows a peak at ∼8 Hz. The shuffled data do not exhibit a dominant peak (red line). D, The z-normalized peak power from all combinations of microstate activation cross-correlations illustrates varying degrees of correlation within and between areas. E1, E2, The z-normalized power spectra from all combinations of microstates within and between areas were clustered into two groups, one with a lower-frequency peak (∼2-4 Hz; E1, black lines,) and one with a higher-frequency peak (∼8 Hz; E2, blue lines). Thick lines indicate cluster centers, and thin lines are individual power spectra. The red lines indicate CSD power for statistical significance calculated from permutation testing (p < 0.01). F1–F2, Results from all recordings in all animals show a consistent grouping of power spectra based on the frequency of the peak in power. The red lines indicate CSD power for statistical significance calculated from permutation testing (p < 0.01).

Remarkably, the cross-correlations exhibited robust oscillations, suggesting rhythmic patterns of temporal microstatic activation coordination across microstates and regions. The z-normalized power spectral profiles (Fig. 6C) contained a distinguishable set of peaks at different frequencies. Typically, one or multiple peaks were <10 Hz, along with smaller peaks in higher frequencies (10–30 Hz). These higher-frequency peaks appeared to be harmonics of the lower-frequency peaks, and we therefore focused on the lower frequencies. As expected from the cross-correlation profiles, peak power varied for different microstate combinations, suggesting the varying strength of cross-region interaction in different microstate combinations (Fig. 6D). To test the statistical reliability of these cross-correlation profiles, we repeated the analysis on shuffled surrogate data (see Materials and Methods). The damping oscillatory nature of cross-correlation structure was absent in the shuffled data (Fig. 6B, red line), indicating that the cross-correlation magnitude reflects microstate interactive temporal dynamics and not random fluctuations.

The power spectral profiles of the cross-correlations were grouped into two clusters, based on k-means and the silhouette method (Rousseeuw, 1987). The first cluster had a dominant peak at 2–4 Hz, while the second was dominated by peaks at ∼8–10 Hz (Fig. 6E1,E2). These spectral profiles were consistent both within animals (across experiment repeats) and across animals. All individual spectral profiles showed a consistent shape (Fig. 6F1,F2).

The occurrence of large-scale EEG patterns could be enabled by underlying co-occurrences of microstates with near-zero lag. While volume conduction prevents such an analysis in EEG recordings, our data allowed investigations into near zero-lag connectivity. For this purpose, we identified the peak in the cross-correlation with a lag closest to zero (“near-zero peak lag”). We found that the peak lag values were largely repeatable over experiment days within each animal. Computing confidence intervals (95%; Jackknife method) for each inter-region microstate pair on each experiment day led to the finding that >92% of inter-region microstate pairs across all experimental days in each animal included 0 ms in the 95% confidence interval around the peak, suggesting a substantial fraction of co-occurrence of microstates across regions.

In summary, microstate activations were temporally synchronized across brain regions both in slow (2–4 Hz) and theta/alpha (8–10 Hz) ranges. Moreover, most of these synchronizations indicated correlation peaks at zero or near-zero lag. This suggests that mesoscopic-scale networks interact in a synchronized rhythmic fashion across the brain areas.

Inter-regional interactions and behavioral states

We investigated the possible behavioral relevance of the microstate activation correlations by separating them according to the following two behavioral states: object interaction and long stationary periods. We defined a “long stationary period” as the trial segments where the animals remained stationary for >90th percentile of all stationary segment lengths (2.3 ± 0.9 s; calculated separately for each trial). While it is easier to distinguish moments of object interaction and no interaction, shorter segments of stationarity can be indistinguishable with movement preparation. Hence, to compare against the dynamics during the “true” stationarity, we chose to focus on longer stationary periods. Next, we defined dominant microstates during these two behavioral states in terms of their temporal coverage (Fig. 7A1–A3). We focused on temporal coverage because it showed the most robust dependence on object exploration in all three regions. All three microstate properties exhibited dependence on the behavioral state (Fig. 5), and their results were qualitatively similar. However, temporal coverage is proportional to both state duration and occurrence rate and, hence, was considered as the most suitable property for the subsequent analyses. The cross-correlation power spectra during long resting periods dominated the 8–10 Hz peak, whereas the power spectra during object interaction dominated the 2–4 Hz peak (Fig. 7B1–C2).

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Power spectra of microstate activation cross-correlations are grouped into two clusters that are differentially modulated by behavior. A1–C2, Illustrative data from one recording (rat 1, recording day 2). A1–A3, Temporal coverage by each microstate during longer resting periods (left bars) and object interaction (right bars shown in light colors) indicates the dominant microstate during each behavioral state. B1, Dominant microstates (yellow) during longer stationary periods are shown across areas. All dominant microstates were grouped for further cross-correlation spectrum analysis. B2, Power spectra of cross-correlation of relevant microstates (shown in B1) during longer rest periods show a peak at ∼8 Hz (thick line shows the mean power spectrum). C1, Same as B1 for object interaction microstates. C2, Cross-correlation power spectra of dominant microstates (C1, yellow) during object interaction show an earlier peak at ∼2–4 Hz (thick line shows the mean power spectrum). D, Mean power spectra from all recordings show higher affinity to one of the spectral cluster centers (Fig. 6F1,F2), except very few spectra from object interaction microstate combinations that show relatively greater similarity to the cluster more populated by rest microstates.

Furthermore, power spectra associated with long resting periods were found to have a higher squared correlation with the 2–4 Hz cluster (r2 = 0.87 ± 0.22, mean + SD) compared with the 8–10 Hz cluster (r2 = 0.36 ± 0.32, p = 7.4e-05, two-sided Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Fig. 7D). On the other hand, the power spectra associated with object interaction correlated better with the higher-peak frequency cluster centers (r2 = 0.76 ± 0.24) compared with the low-peak frequency cluster (r2 = 0.50 ± 0.32, p = 0.024, two-sided Wilcoxon signed-rank test) in terms of squared correlation (Fig. 7D). Thus, each behavioral state exhibited a distinct power spectrum profile. The two clusters of power spectral profiles (Fig. 6F) were modulated by object exploration and movement.

In summary, the dependence of microstate properties on two contrasting behavioral states (object exploration and longer stationary periods) is supported by both the significant changes in microstate properties (Fig. 5) and the distinct nature of interaction patterns (cross-correlation spectra) across regions. In fact, these two types of interaction indicators relate to the two behavioral states. This suggests the specificity of the underlying neural mechanism of the two behavioral states (Figs. 8, 9).

Figure 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 8.

Distributions of microstate properties in all three regions in all recording sessions provide insights on temporal and spatial features of microstates (Table 1). A, Temporal coverage fraction shows temporal area covered by each microstate. B, Occurrence rate shows that microstates in all three areas appeared approximately three to four times per second on average. C, The top four microstates exhibited a high global explained variance (60–80%) in all three brain regions. D, The top four microstates exhibited high spatial correlation (Pearson) with the electrode topography (∼0.5). E, State duration of microstates followed power law distributions in PFC and STR, and had a characteristic peak of ∼100 ms in the VTA.

Figure 9.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 9.

LFP microstates are a small set of recurring spatial patterns of LFP signals recorded in a single brain region [here PFC (navy), STR (maroon), VTA (orange)]. These microstates, originating from different brain regions coordinate during different behavioral states. This is exhibited through their distinct behavioral state-specific cross-spectral density profiles. During the stationary state, a subset of microstates coordinates with 8–10 Hz cross-spectral density (csd) profile (pink), while during object interaction another subset of microstates shows a peak of ∼2–4 Hz in their csd profile (gray). Data from one recording session (rat 1, recording day 2).

Discussion

We extended a spatiotemporal analysis method developed for EEG data to characterize the dynamics of mesoscale signals (LFP) in multiple brain regions, and to search for states of activation and their interactions, during novelty seeking in awake behaving rats. We identified microstates analogous to EEG microstates, which were physiologically interpretable and modulated by the behavioral state. This establishes a novel link between network-level activations and dynamics and concurrent behavioral states, and may facilitate bridging human EEG and rodent invasive research.

“States” and scales in brain activity

The concept of a “brain state” appears in multiple domains of the neuroscience literature, and spans multiple spatial scales and levels of analysis. For example, the “resting state” in fMRI (Biswal et al., 1995; Raichle et al., 2001) refers to coordination across widespread brain regions during rest. Large-spatial scale states have also been defined based on temporally precise measurements such as EEG and MEG. Examples include defining entropy as the basis of states (Kannathal et al., 2005), using the embedded dimension in a state-space interaction to define cortical synchrony (Carmeli et al., 2005); hidden Markov models (Baker et al., 2014; Hunyadi et al., 2019); and the clustering-based approach of EEG data segmentation into quasi-stable states (Lehmann et al., 1987; Lehmann, 1989; Michel et al., 1999; Michel and Koenig, 2018).

At a finer spatial scale, neural states have been defined based on population spiking activity. For example, “up states” and “down states” in the context of depolarization and hyperpolarization (Steriade et al., 1993), the concept of “vocabulary” in population spike patterns (Luczak et al., 2009), propagating wave patterns (Townsend and Gong, 2018), and several others (Greenberg et al., 2008; Poulet and Petersen, 2008; Curto et al., 2009; Okun et al., 2010).

Our approach was to adapt methods developed in the EEG literature to multichannel LFP recordings. Although it is difficult for LFP microstates to directly connect with human EEG microstates, many features of the identified LFP microstates appear to be consistent with EEG microstate findings (Table 1; Michel and Koenig, 2018), including the number of microstates, their temporal characteristics, and their relation to ongoing behavior. Interestingly, state durations in the range of 50–100 ms were observed in our data, in the EEG microstates literature (Michel et al., 1999), and in population spike recordings (Luczak et al., 2009). One of the findings of this study is the presence of zero-and near-zero lag synchronization across the brain areas via microstate activations in combination with statistically robust nonzero correlation magnitudes (Fig. 6). An open question in EEG microstate research is the amount of true synchronization because of volume conduction (Michel and Koenig, 2018), as the volume conduction can elevate cross-region coherence (Srinivasan et al., 2007). The cross-correlation analysis in the current study presents the novel finding that zero-lag neuronal synchronization within and between brain areas could contribute to EEG microstates, as the multiregion LFP recordings remain largely unaffected by volume conduction. Furthermore, this hints at a possible common link underlying observable brain states across different spatial scales.

Microstates and dimensionality reduction approaches

LFP microstates are identified by a data-driven dimensionality reduction approach that is based on long, continuous recordings and yet can be linked to transient behaviors. We found that four microstates can explain 60–80% of the LFP signals. Overall, this is consistent with other dimensionality reduction approaches, for example independent component analysis of LFP in the hippocampal CA1 (Makarov et al., 2010), or principal component analysis in rat barrel cortex (Einevoll et al., 2007), monkey PFC (Machens et al., 2010), and rat orbitofrontal cortex (Kobak et al., 2016). It is remarkable that the conclusion of three to six large components persists over many different approaches. It seems that multichannel LFP data occupy low-dimensional subspaces, and that different dimensionality reduction methods may identify different basis vectors to span a limited set of the most relevant subspaces. The EEG microstate method uses clustering of the empirical data, which facilitates physiological and behavioral interpretations (Khanna et al., 2015; Michel and Koenig, 2018).

LFP microstates exhibit both stability and variability over time

The reproducibility of the microstate maps across 5–9 d suggests that the microstates are stable over time within animals, perhaps reflecting the activation of circuits that are anatomically patterned. On the other hand, some microstate maps were not in the top four components in all recording sessions. These might have a lower signal-to-noise ratio and thus were relegated to lower-ranked microstates, or they might reflect state-specific functional networks that were less constrained by stable anatomic connectivity.

EEG microstate studies have shown that four “standard” microstate maps are observed consistently over individuals and across many studies (Michel and Koenig, 2018). In our LFP data, only some of the maps were topographically reproducible across animals. However, this apparent discrepancy must be interpreted with caution: the LFP electrodes are much smaller than EEG electrodes, and therefore even small (submillimeter) differences in the placement of probes can cause topographical discrepancies across animals. Furthermore, EEG microstates reflect a macroscopic phenomenon (Betzel and Bassett, 2017), and the relationship between macroscopic and mesoscopic dynamics like LFP is nontrivial (Nunez and Srinivasan, 2006; Alishbayli et al., 2019). Generally, LFPs can be more sensitive to local effects compared with EEG signals, which are spatiotemporally filtered manifestations of a multitude of LFP signals (Buzsáki et al., 2012).

LFP microstates reflect mesoscale rhythmic coordination across brain regions

We identified a novel manifestation of rhythmic synchronization between mesoscopic-scale networks in spatially distinct brain regions (Fig. 9). Remarkably, the interactions exhibited two prominent peaks in the lower-frequency spectrum that mapped onto different aspects of behavior. The ∼2 Hz peak was more strongly modulated by interacting with the novel object, whereas the ∼8 Hz peak was more strongly modulated by movement. This suggests that partly overlapping networks use spectral multiplexing to coordinate inter-regional interaction during novelty using two distinct routes of mesoscopic information exchange (Akam and Kullmann, 2014).

The PFC is crucial for higher cognition (Uylings et al., 2003; Laubach et al., 2018) and is involved in executive functions, including learning and attention (Passetti et al., 2002), decision-making (De Bruin et al., 2000; Sul et al., 2010), working memory (Bekinschtein and Weisstaub, 2014), and inhibitory response control (Brown and Bowman, 2002; Hardung et al., 2017). The PFC also plays a crucial role in communicating with other cortical and subcortical regions to implement executive control (Pasupathy and Miller, 2005; Antzoulatos and Miller, 2014; Feingold et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2016; Gao et al., 2017). Interactions among the PFC, VTA, and striatum are also crucial, particularly for learning and working memory, through dopaminergic pathways (Karreman and Moghaddam, 1996; Zahrt et al., 1997; Peters et al., 2004; Bentivoglio and Morelli, 2005; Björklund and Dunnett, 2007; Gao et al., 2007; Fujisawa and Buzsáki, 2011; Ott et al., 2014).

Although traditional analyses often involve synchronization between a selected pair of electrodes, our multichannel recordings allowed us to examine synchronization at a larger spatial scale. Because the microstates reflect a weighted combination of data from all channels, these can be considered as functional patterns that span multiple millimeters and reflect coordinated activity across possibly thousands of cells. Because the microstates are defined independently in each region, the synchronization results were not biased by selecting electrode pairs that exhibit a particular pattern of correlation. One major advantage of this method is the ability to decompose multiple partially spatially overlapping networks. Indeed, it is possible that traditional electrode-based synchronization methods mix neurally and cognitively distinct interactions because a single electrode measures activity from multiple networks (Cunningham and Yu, 2014; Cohen, 2021).

Implications for understanding novelty seeking

The experimental paradigm used here is a variant of the commonly used object recognition test (Aggleton, 1985). Novelty seeking has been shown to evoke exploratory behavior (Berlyne, 1950). The reduction of exploratory behavior during the repetition of a previously novel object is taken as a measure of single-trial memory formation. The role and interplay of PFC, STR, and VTA is well known for memory and motivation (Karreman and Moghaddam, 1996; Palmiter, 2008; Liljeholm and O'Doherty, 2012; Antzoulatos and Miller, 2014; Bekinschtein and Weisstaub, 2014; Morales and Margolis, 2017). The access to these brain regions makes this experimental paradigm suitable for exploring the neural states underlying the novelty-seeking behavior. Moreover, the prolonged stationary behavioral state, as the trials progressed, allowed us to investigate and compare the neural activity during novelty seeking against the “background activity.” In this study, we propose a role for mesoscale microstates in novelty-seeking behavior, which can be attributed to the activation of different networks during novelty processing.

Limitations and future directions

First, although LFP microstates have a clear methodological basis, their precise involvement in cognition remains to be determined. EEG microstates have been theorized to reflect building blocks of brain-wide events (Lehmann, 1990). LFP microstates may also be a mesoscopic equivalent of default-mode networks (Biswal et al., 1995). Second, the true number of microstates and their overall shape (the spatial structure) during different sensory stimulation and spontaneous conditions is difficult to determine even with multielectrode arrays. Future studies with larger and denser arrays will help to map the spatial reach of microstates. Third, the relationship between intermicrostate synchronization, as we observed it, with other manifestations of neural synchronization—and their relation to various behaviors—remain to be explored. Third, it remains unclear whether the rhythmic coordination that we observed reflects long-range excitation or inhibition, although these patterns do seem to play a meaningful role in relation to behavior. Finally, the exact relationship between LFP microstates and microstates at smaller (e.g., spiking) and larger (EEG) spatial scales remains to be investigated.

Footnotes

  • M.X.C. is funded by European Research Council Timeframe Starting Grant 638589 awarded to M.X.C. A.M. is funded by a joint doctoral grant awarded to M.X.C. and B.E. by the Donders Center of Neuroscience. B.E. was supported by European Commission Marie Curie Grant 660328, NWO Vidi Grant 016.189.052, and NWO ALW Open Grant ALWOP.346. We thank Dr. Paul Anderson for contributions to the surgeries and data collection process.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Ashutosh Mishra at a.mishra{at}donders.ru.nl

SfN exclusive license.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Aggleton JP
    (1985) One-trial object recognition by rats. Q J Exp Psychol B 37:279–294. doi:10.1080/14640748508401171
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  2. ↵
    1. Akam T,
    2. Kullmann DM
    (2014) Oscillatory multiplexing of population codes for selective communication in the mammalian brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 15:111–122. doi:10.1038/nrn3668 pmid:24434912
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Alishbayli A,
    2. Tichelaar JG,
    3. Gorska U,
    4. Cohen MX,
    5. Englitz B
    (2019) The asynchronous state's relation to large-scale potentials in cortex. J Neurophysiol 122:2206–2219. doi:10.1152/jn.00013.2019 pmid:31642401
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Antzoulatos EG,
    2. Miller EK
    (2014) Increases in functional connectivity between prefrontal cortex and striatum during category learning. Neuron 83:216–225. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.005 pmid:24930701
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Arieli A,
    2. Shoham D,
    3. Hildesheim R,
    4. Grinvald A
    (1995) Coherent spatiotemporal patterns of ongoing activity revealed by real-time optical imaging coupled with single-unit recording in the cat visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 73:2072–2093. doi:10.1152/jn.1995.73.5.2072 pmid:7623099
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Baker AP,
    2. Brookes MJ,
    3. Rezek IA,
    4. Smith SM,
    5. Behrens T,
    6. Probert Smith PJ,
    7. Woolrich M
    (2014) Fast transient networks in spontaneous human brain activity. Elife 3:e01867. doi:10.7554/eLife.01867 pmid:24668169
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Battaglia FP,
    2. Sutherland GR,
    3. McNaughton BL
    (2004) Hippocampal sharp wave bursts coincide with neocortical “up-state” transitions. Learn Mem 11:697–704. doi:10.1101/lm.73504 pmid:15576887
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Bekinschtein P,
    2. Weisstaub N
    (2014) Role of PFC during retrieval of recognition memory in rodents. J Physiol Paris 108:252–255. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.03.001 pmid:24726917
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Bentivoglio M,
    2. Morelli M
    (2005) The organization and circuits of mesencephalic dopaminergic neurons and the distribution of dopamine receptors in the brain. In: Dopamine (Dunnett SB, Bentivoglio M, Björklund A, Hökfelt T, eds), pp 1–107. Burlington, MA: Elsevier.
  10. ↵
    1. Berlyne DE
    (1950) Novelty and curiosity as determinants of exploratory behaviour. Br J Psychol Gen Sect 41:68–80. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1950.tb00262.x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  11. ↵
    1. Betzel RF,
    2. Bassett DS
    (2017) Multi-scale brain networks. Neuroimage 160:73–83. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.006 pmid:27845257
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Biswal B,
    2. Yetkin FZ,
    3. Haughton VM,
    4. Hyde JS
    (1995) Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med 34:537–541. doi:10.1002/mrm.1910340409
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Björklund A,
    2. Dunnett SB
    (2007) Dopamine neuron systems in the brain: an update. Trends Neurosci 30:194–202. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.006 pmid:17408759
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Bréchet L,
    2. Brunet D,
    3. Birot G,
    4. Gruetter R,
    5. Michel CM,
    6. Jorge J
    (2019) Capturing the spatiotemporal dynamics of self-generated, task-initiated thoughts with EEG and fMRI. Neuroimage 194:82–92. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.029 pmid:30902640
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Britz J,
    2. Michel CM
    (2011) State-dependent visual processing. Front Psychol 2:370. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00370 pmid:22203809
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Britz J,
    2. Van De Ville D,
    3. Michel CM
    (2010) BOLD correlates of EEG topography reveal rapid resting-state network dynamics. Neuroimage 52:1162–1170. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.052 pmid:20188188
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Britz J,
    2. Díaz Hernàndez L,
    3. Ro T,
    4. Michel CM
    (2014) EEG-microstate dependent emergence of perceptual awareness. Front Behav Neurosci 8:163. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00163 pmid:24860450
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Brown VJ,
    2. Bowman EM
    (2002) Rodent models of prefrontal cortical function. Trends Neurosci 25:340–343. doi:10.1016/s0166-2236(02)02164-1 pmid:12079756
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Buzsáki G,
    2. Anastassiou CA,
    3. Koch C
    (2012) The origin of extracellular fields and currents–EEG, ECoG, LFP and spikes. Nat Rev Neurosci 13:407–420. doi:10.1038/nrn3241 pmid:22595786
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Carmeli C,
    2. Knyazeva MG,
    3. Innocenti GM,
    4. De Feo O
    (2005) Assessment of EEG synchronization based on state-space analysis. Neuroimage 25:339–354. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.049 pmid:15784413
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Chiu C,
    2. Weliky M
    (2001) Spontaneous activity in developing ferret visual cortex in vivo. J Neurosci 21:8906–8914. pmid:11698602
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Cohen MX
    (2021) A tutorial on generalized eigendecomposition for source separation in multichannel electrophysiology. arXiv:2104.12356.
  23. ↵
    1. Cunningham JP,
    2. Yu BM
    (2014) Dimensionality reduction for large-scale neural recordings. Nat Neurosci 17:1500–1509. doi:10.1038/nn.3776 pmid:25151264
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Curto C,
    2. Sakata S,
    3. Marguet S,
    4. Itskov V,
    5. Harris KD
    (2009) A simple model of cortical dynamics explains variability and state dependence of sensory responses in urethane-anesthetized auditory cortex. J Neurosci 29:10600–10612. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2053-09.2009 pmid:19710313
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    1. da Cruz JR,
    2. Favrod O,
    3. Roinishvili M,
    4. Chkonia E,
    5. Brand A,
    6. Mohr C,
    7. Figueiredo P,
    8. Herzog MH
    (2020) EEG microstates are a candidate endophenotype for schizophrenia. Nat Commun 11:3089. doi:10.1038/s41467-020-16914-1 pmid:32555168
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Delorme A,
    2. Makeig S
    (2004) EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:9–21. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 pmid:15102499
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. De Bruin JPC,
    2. Feenstra MGP,
    3. Broersen LM,
    4. Van Leeuwen M,
    5. Arens C,
    6. De Vries S,
    7. Joosten R
    (2000) Role of the prefrontal cortex of the rat in learning and decision making: effects of transient inactivation. In: Cognition, emotion and autonomic responses: the integrative role of the prefrontal cortex and limbic structures (Uylings HBM, ed), pp 103–113. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
  28. ↵
    1. Dierks T,
    2. Jelic V,
    3. Julin P,
    4. Maurer K,
    5. Wahlund LO,
    6. Almkvist O,
    7. Strik WK,
    8. Winblad B
    (1997) EEG-microstates in mild memory impairment and Alzheimer's disease: possible association with disturbed information processing. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 104:483–495. doi:10.1007/BF01277666 pmid:9295180
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Einevoll GT,
    2. Pettersen KH,
    3. Devor A,
    4. Ulbert I,
    5. Halgren E,
    6. Dale AM
    (2007) Laminar population analysis: estimating firing rates and evoked synaptic activity from multielectrode recordings in rat barrel cortex. J Neurophysiol 97:2174–2190. doi:10.1152/jn.00845.2006 pmid:17182911
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Feingold J,
    2. Gibson DJ,
    3. DePasquale B,
    4. Graybiel AM
    (2015) Bursts of beta oscillation differentiate postperformance activity in the striatum and motor cortex of monkeys performing movement tasks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112:13687–13692. doi:10.1073/pnas.1517629112
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    1. Fiser J,
    2. Chiu C,
    3. Weliky M
    (2004) Small modulation of ongoing cortical dynamics by sensory input during natural vision. Nature 431:573–578. doi:10.1038/nature02907 pmid:15457262
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Fujisawa S,
    2. Buzsáki G
    (2011) A 4 Hz oscillation adaptively synchronizes prefrontal, VTA, and hippocampal activities. Neuron 72:153–165. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.018 pmid:21982376
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Gao M,
    2. Liu C-L,
    3. Yang S,
    4. Jin G-Z,
    5. Bunney BS,
    6. Shi W-X
    (2007) Functional coupling between the prefrontal cortex and dopamine neurons in the ventral tegmental area. J Neurosci 27:5414–5421. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5347-06.2007 pmid:17507563
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    1. Gao P,
    2. de Munck JC,
    3. Limpens JHW,
    4. Vanderschuren LJMJ,
    5. Voorn P
    (2017) A neuronal activation correlate in striatum and prefrontal cortex of prolonged cocaine intake. Brain Struct Funct 222:3453–3475. doi:10.1007/s00429-017-1412-4 pmid:28393262
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Greenberg DS,
    2. Houweling AR,
    3. Kerr JND
    (2008) Population imaging of ongoing neuronal activity in the visual cortex of awake rats. Nat Neurosci 11:749–751. doi:10.1038/nn.2140 pmid:18552841
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Hardung S,
    2. Epple R,
    3. Jäckel Z,
    4. Eriksson D,
    5. Uran C,
    6. Senn V,
    7. Gibor L,
    8. Yizhar O,
    9. Diester I
    (2017) A functional gradient in the rodent prefrontal cortex supports behavioral inhibition. Curr Biol 27:549–555. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.052 pmid:28190729
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Hunyadi B,
    2. Woolrich MW,
    3. Quinn AJ,
    4. Vidaurre D,
    5. De Vos M
    (2019) A dynamic system of brain networks revealed by fast transient EEG fluctuations and their fMRI correlates. Neuroimage 185:72–82. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.082 pmid:30287299
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Kannathal N,
    2. Choo ML,
    3. Acharya UR,
    4. Sadasivan PK
    (2005) Entropies for detection of epilepsy in EEG. Comput Methods Programs Biomed 80:187–194. doi:10.1016/j.cmpb.2005.06.012 pmid:16219385
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Karreman M,
    2. Moghaddam B
    (1996) The prefrontal cortex regulates the basal release of dopamine in the limbic striatum: an effect mediated by ventral tegmental area. J Neurochem 66:589–598. doi:10.1046/j.1471-4159.1996.66020589.x pmid:8592128
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Kenet T,
    2. Bibitchkov D,
    3. Tsodyks M,
    4. Grinvald A,
    5. Arieli A
    (2003) Spontaneously emerging cortical representations of visual attributes. Nature 425:954–956. doi:10.1038/nature02078 pmid:14586468
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Khanna A,
    2. Pascual-Leone A,
    3. Michel CM,
    4. Farzan F
    (2015) Microstates in resting-state EEG: current status and future directions. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 49:105–113. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.12.010 pmid:25526823
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Kindler J,
    2. Hubl D,
    3. Strik WK,
    4. Dierks T,
    5. Koenig T
    (2011) Resting-state EEG in schizophrenia: auditory verbal hallucinations are related to shortening of specific microstates. Clin Neurophysiol 122:1179–1182. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2010.10.042 pmid:21123110
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Kobak D,
    2. Brendel W,
    3. Constantinidis C,
    4. Feierstein CE,
    5. Kepecs A,
    6. Mainen ZF,
    7. Qi X-L,
    8. Romo R,
    9. Uchida N,
    10. Machens CK
    (2016) Demixed principal component analysis of neural population data. Elife 5:e10989. doi:10.7554/eLife.10989
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Laubach M,
    2. Amarante LM,
    3. Swanson K,
    4. White SR
    (2018) What, if anything, is rodent prefrontal cortex? Eneuro 5:ENEURO.0315-18.2018.
  45. ↵
    1. Lehmann D
    (1989) Microstates of the brain in EEG and ERP mapping studies. In: Brain dynamics (Başar E, Bullock TH, eds), pp 72–83. Berlin: Springer.
  46. ↵
    1. Lehmann D
    (1990) Brain electric microstates and cognition: the atoms of thought. In: Machinery of the mind (John ER, Harmony T, Prichep LS, Valdés-Sosa M, Valdés-Sosa PA, eds), pp 209–224. Boston: Birkhäuser.
  47. ↵
    1. Lehmann D,
    2. Ozaki H,
    3. Pal I
    (1987) EEG alpha map series: brain micro-states by space-oriented adaptive segmentation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 67:271–288. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(87)90025-3][PMC] [2441961
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Lehmann D,
    2. Faber PL,
    3. Galderisi S,
    4. Herrmann WM,
    5. Kinoshita T,
    6. Koukkou M,
    7. Mucci A,
    8. Pascual-Marqui RD,
    9. Saito N,
    10. Wackermann J,
    11. Winterer G,
    12. Koenig T
    (2005) EEG microstate duration and syntax in acute, medication-naive, first-episode schizophrenia: a multi-center study. Psychiatry Res 138:141–156. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2004.05.007 pmid:15766637
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Liljeholm M,
    2. O'Doherty JP
    (2012) Contributions of the striatum to learning, motivation, and performance: an associative account. Trends Cogn Sci 16:467–475. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.07.007 pmid:22890090
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Luczak A,
    2. Barthó P,
    3. Harris KD
    (2009) Spontaneous events outline the realm of possible sensory responses in neocortical populations. Neuron 62:413–425. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.014 pmid:19447096
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Luczak A,
    2. McNaughton BL,
    3. Harris KD
    (2015) Packet-based communication in the cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 16:745–755. doi:10.1038/nrn4026 pmid:26507295
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Machens CK,
    2. Romo R,
    3. Brody CD
    (2010) Functional, but not anatomical, separation of “what” and “when” in prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 30:350–360. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3276-09.2010
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  53. ↵
    1. Makarov VA,
    2. Makarova J,
    3. Herreras O
    (2010) Disentanglement of local field potential sources by independent component analysis. J Comput Neurosci 29:445–457. doi:10.1007/s10827-009-0206-y pmid:20094907
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Mao BQ,
    2. Hamzei-Sichani F,
    3. Aronov D,
    4. Froemke RC,
    5. Yuste R
    (2001) Dynamics of spontaneous activity in neocortical slices. Neuron 32:883–898. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00518-9 pmid:11738033
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Massimini M,
    2. Huber R,
    3. Ferrarelli F,
    4. Hill S,
    5. Tononi G
    (2004) The sleep slow oscillation as a traveling wave. J Neurosci 24:6862–6870. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1318-04.2004
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. ↵
    1. Mathis A,
    2. Mamidanna P,
    3. Cury KM,
    4. Abe T,
    5. Murthy VN,
    6. Mathis MW,
    7. Bethge M
    (2018) DeepLabCut: markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learning. Nat Neurosci 21:1281–1289. doi:10.1038/s41593-018-0209-y pmid:30127430
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. McGinley MJ,
    2. David SV,
    3. McCormick DA
    (2015) Cortical membrane potential signature of optimal states for sensory signal detection. Neuron 87:179–192. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.038 pmid:26074005
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Michel CM,
    2. Koenig T
    (2018) EEG microstates as a tool for studying the temporal dynamics of whole-brain neuronal networks: a review. Neuroimage 180:577–593. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.062 pmid:29196270
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Michel CM,
    2. Seeck M,
    3. Landis T
    (1999) Spatiotemporal dynamics of human cognition. News Physiol Sci 14:206–214. doi:10.1152/physiologyonline.1999.14.5.206
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Milz P
    (2016) Keypy—an open source library for EEG microstate analysis. Eur Psychiatr 33:S493. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.01.1812
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  61. ↵
    1. Milz P,
    2. Faber PL,
    3. Lehmann D,
    4. Koenig T,
    5. Kochi K,
    6. Pascual-Marqui RD
    (2016) The functional significance of EEG microstates–associations with modalities of thinking. Neuroimage 125:643–656. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.023 pmid:26285079
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Milz P,
    2. Pascual-Marqui RD,
    3. Achermann P,
    4. Kochi K,
    5. Faber PL
    (2017) The EEG microstate topography is predominantly determined by intracortical sources in the alpha band. Neuroimage 162:353–361. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.058 pmid:28847493
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Mishra A,
    2. Englitz B,
    3. Cohen MX
    (2020) EEG microstates as a continuous phenomenon. Neuroimage 208:116454. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116454 pmid:31841679
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Morales M,
    2. Margolis EB
    (2017) Ventral tegmental area: cellular heterogeneity, connectivity and behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci 18:73–85. doi:10.1038/nrn.2016.165 pmid:28053327
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Murray MM,
    2. Brunet D,
    3. Michel CM
    (2008) Topographic ERP analyses: a step-by-step tutorial review. Brain Topogr 20:249–264. doi:10.1007/s10548-008-0054-5 pmid:18347966
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Musall S,
    2. Kaufman MT,
    3. Juavinett AL,
    4. Gluf S,
    5. Churchland AK
    (2019) Single-trial neural dynamics are dominated by richly varied movements. Nat Neurosci 22:1677–1686. doi:10.1038/s41593-019-0502-4 pmid:31551604
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Musso F,
    2. Brinkmeyer J,
    3. Mobascher A,
    4. Warbrick T,
    5. Winterer G
    (2010) Spontaneous brain activity and EEG microstates. A novel EEG/fMRI analysis approach to explore resting-state networks. Neuroimage 52:1149–1161. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.093 pmid:20139014
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Nunez PL,
    2. Srinivasan R
    (2006) Electric fields of the brain: the neurophysics of EEG. Oxford, UK: Oxford UP.
  69. ↵
    1. Okun M,
    2. Naim A,
    3. Lampl I
    (2010) The subthreshold relation between cortical local field potential and neuronal firing unveiled by intracellular recordings in awake rats. J Neurosci 30:4440–4448. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5062-09.2010 pmid:20335480
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  70. ↵
    1. Omer DB,
    2. Fekete T,
    3. Ulchin Y,
    4. Hildesheim R,
    5. Grinvald A
    (2019) Dynamic patterns of spontaneous ongoing activity in the visual cortex of anesthetized and awake monkeys are different. Cereb Cortex 29:1291–1304. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhy099 pmid:29718200
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Ott T,
    2. Jacob SN,
    3. Nieder A
    (2014) Dopamine receptors differentially enhance rule coding in primate prefrontal cortex neurons. Neuron 84:1317–1328. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.11.012 pmid:25482027
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Palmiter RD
    (2008) Dopamine signaling in the dorsal striatum is essential for motivated behaviors: lessons from dopamine-deficient mice. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1129:35–46. doi:10.1196/annals.1417.003 pmid:18591467
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Papp EA,
    2. Leergaard TB,
    3. Calabrese E,
    4. Johnson GA,
    5. Bjaalie JG
    (2014) Waxholm Space atlas of the Sprague Dawley rat brain. Neuroimage 97:374–386. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.001 pmid:24726336
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Pascual-Marqui RD,
    2. Michel CM,
    3. Lehmann D
    (1995) Segmentation of brain electrical activity into microstates: model estimation and validation. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 42:658–665. doi:10.1109/10.391164 pmid:7622149
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Passetti F,
    2. Chudasama Y,
    3. Robbins TW
    (2002) The frontal cortex of the rat and visual attentional performance: dissociable functions of distinct medial prefrontal subregions. Cereb Cortex 12:1254–1268. doi:10.1093/cercor/12.12.1254 pmid:12427677
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Pasupathy A,
    2. Miller EK
    (2005) Different time courses of learning-related activity in the prefrontal cortex and striatum. Nature 433:873–876. doi:10.1038/nature03287 pmid:15729344
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Peters Y,
    2. Barnhardt NE,
    3. O'Donnell P
    (2004) Prefrontal cortical up states are synchronized with ventral tegmental area activity. Synapse 52:143–152. doi:10.1002/syn.20015 pmid:15034920
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. Petersen CCH,
    2. Hahn TTG,
    3. Mehta M,
    4. Grinvald A,
    5. Sakmann B
    (2003) Interaction of sensory responses with spontaneous depolarization in layer 2/3 barrel cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100:13638–13643. doi:10.1073/pnas.2235811100 pmid:14595013
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  79. ↵
    1. Poulet JFA,
    2. Petersen CCH
    (2008) Internal brain state regulates membrane potential synchrony in barrel cortex of behaving mice. Nature 454:881–885. doi:10.1038/nature07150 pmid:18633351
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Poulsen AT,
    2. Pedroni A,
    3. Langer N,
    4. Hansen LK
    (2018) Microstate EEGlab toolbox: an introductory guide. BioRxiv 289850.doi:10.1101/289850
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  81. ↵
    1. Raichle ME,
    2. MacLeod AM,
    3. Snyder AZ,
    4. Powers WJ,
    5. Gusnard DA,
    6. Shulman GL
    (2001) A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98:676–682. doi:10.1073/pnas.98.2.676 pmid:11209064
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  82. ↵
    1. Rousseeuw PJ
    (1987) Silhouettes: a graphical aid to the interpretation and validation of cluster analysis. J Comput Appl Math 20:53–65. doi:10.1016/0377-0427(87)90125-7
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  83. ↵
    1. Sakata S,
    2. Harris KD
    (2009) Laminar structure of spontaneous and sensory-evoked population activity in auditory cortex. Neuron 64:404–418. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.020 pmid:19914188
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  84. ↵
    1. Siegle JH,
    2. López AC,
    3. Patel YA,
    4. Abramov K,
    5. Ohayon S,
    6. Voigts J
    (2017) Open Ephys: an open-source, plugin-based platform for multichannel electrophysiology. J Neural Eng 14:045003. doi:10.1088/1741-2552/aa5eea pmid:28169219
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  85. ↵
    1. Srinivasan R,
    2. Winter WR,
    3. Ding J,
    4. Nunez PL
    (2007) EEG and MEG coherence: measures of functional connectivity at distinct spatial scales of neocortical dynamics. J Neurosci Methods 166:41–52. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.06.026
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  86. ↵
    1. Steriade M,
    2. Nuñez A,
    3. Amzica F
    (1993) A novel slow (<1 Hz) oscillation of neocortical neurons in vivo: depolarizing and hyperpolarizing components. J Neurosci 13:3252–3265.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  87. ↵
    1. Sul JH,
    2. Kim H,
    3. Huh N,
    4. Lee D,
    5. Jung MW
    (2010) Distinct roles of rodent orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortex in decision making. Neuron 66:449–460. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.033 pmid:20471357
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  88. ↵
    1. Tomescu MI,
    2. Rihs TA,
    3. Roinishvili M,
    4. Karahanoglu FI,
    5. Schneider M,
    6. Menghetti S,
    7. Van De Ville D,
    8. Brand A,
    9. Chkonia E,
    10. Eliez S,
    11. Herzog MH,
    12. Michel CM,
    13. Cappe C
    (2015) Schizophrenia patients and 22q11.2 deletion syndrome adolescents at risk express the same deviant patterns of resting state EEG microstates: a candidate endophenotype of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res Cogn 2:159–165. doi:10.1016/j.scog.2015.04.005 pmid:29379765
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  89. ↵
    1. Townsend RG,
    2. Gong P
    (2018) Detection and analysis of spatiotemporal patterns in brain activity. PLoS Comput Biol 14:e1006643. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006643 pmid:30507937
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  90. ↵
    1. Uylings HBM,
    2. Groenewegen HJ,
    3. Kolb B
    (2003) Do rats have a prefrontal cortex? Behav Brain Res 146:3–17. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2003.09.028 pmid:14643455
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  91. ↵
    1. Vandecasteele M,
    2. M S,
    3. Royer S,
    4. Belluscio M,
    5. Berényi A,
    6. Diba K,
    7. Fujisawa S,
    8. Grosmark A,
    9. Mao D,
    10. Mizuseki K,
    11. Patel J,
    12. Stark E,
    13. Sullivan D,
    14. Watson B,
    15. Buzsáki G
    (2012) Large-scale recording of neurons by movable silicon probes in behaving rodents. J Vis Exp. Advance online publication. Retrived Mar 4, 2012. doi: 10.3791/3568.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  92. ↵
    1. Yuan H,
    2. Zotev V,
    3. Phillips R,
    4. Drevets WC,
    5. Bodurka J
    (2012) Spatiotemporal dynamics of the brain at rest–exploring EEG microstates as electrophysiological signatures of BOLD resting state networks. Neuroimage 60:2062–2072. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.031 pmid:22381593
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  93. ↵
    1. Zahrt J,
    2. Taylor JR,
    3. Mathew RG,
    4. Arnsten AF
    (1997) Supranormal stimulation of D1 dopamine receptors in the rodent prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performance. J Neurosci 17:8528–8535.pmid:9334425
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  94. ↵
    1. Zanesco AP,
    2. King BG,
    3. Skwara AC,
    4. Saron CD
    (2020) Within and between-person correlates of the temporal dynamics of resting EEG microstates. Neuroimage 211:116631. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116631 pmid:32062082
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  95. ↵
    1. Zhang Y,
    2. Pan X,
    3. Wang R,
    4. Sakagami M
    (2016) Functional connectivity between prefrontal cortex and striatum estimated by phase locking value. Cogn Neurodyn 10:245–254. doi:10.1007/s11571-016-9376-2 pmid:27275380
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 41 (32)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 41, Issue 32
11 Aug 2021
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Dynamics of Neural Microstates in the VTA–Striatal–Prefrontal Loop during Novelty Exploration in the Rat
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Dynamics of Neural Microstates in the VTA–Striatal–Prefrontal Loop during Novelty Exploration in the Rat
Ashutosh Mishra, Nader Marzban, Michael X Cohen, Bernhard Englitz
Journal of Neuroscience 11 August 2021, 41 (32) 6864-6877; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2256-20.2021

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Dynamics of Neural Microstates in the VTA–Striatal–Prefrontal Loop during Novelty Exploration in the Rat
Ashutosh Mishra, Nader Marzban, Michael X Cohen, Bernhard Englitz
Journal of Neuroscience 11 August 2021, 41 (32) 6864-6877; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2256-20.2021
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • LFP microstates
  • object exploration
  • PFC
  • rodent
  • striatum
  • VTA

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Cortically-evoked movement in humans reflects history of prior executions, not plan for upcoming movement
  • Neuronally-derived soluble Abeta evokes cell-wide astrocytic calcium dysregulation in absence of amyloid plaques in vivo
  • Effect of aging and a dual orexin receptor antagonist on sleep architecture and NREM oscillations including a REM Behavior Disorder phenotype in the PS19 mouse model of tauopathy
Show more Research Articles

Systems/Circuits

  • Development of BOLD Response to Motion in Human Infants
  • On the tonotopy of the low-frequency region of the cochlea
  • Auditory deprivation during development alters efferent neural feedback and perception
Show more Systems/Circuits
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.