Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Increased Functional Connectivity of the Intraparietal Sulcus Underlies the Attenuation of Numerosity Estimations for Self-Generated Words

Giedre Stripeikyte, Michael Pereira, Giulio Rognini, Jevita Potheegadoo, Olaf Blanke and Nathan Faivre
Journal of Neuroscience 27 October 2021, 41 (43) 8917-8927; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3164-20.2021
Giedre Stripeikyte
1Center for Neuroprosthetics, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology (EPFL), CH-1202 Geneva, Switzerland
2Brain Mind Institute, Faculty of Life Sciences, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology (EPFL), CH-1015 Lausanne, Switzerland
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Michael Pereira
1Center for Neuroprosthetics, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology (EPFL), CH-1202 Geneva, Switzerland
2Brain Mind Institute, Faculty of Life Sciences, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology (EPFL), CH-1015 Lausanne, Switzerland
3Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition, CNRS, Univ. Grenoble Alpes, CNRS, LPNC, 38000 Grenoble, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Giulio Rognini
1Center for Neuroprosthetics, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology (EPFL), CH-1202 Geneva, Switzerland
2Brain Mind Institute, Faculty of Life Sciences, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology (EPFL), CH-1015 Lausanne, Switzerland
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jevita Potheegadoo
1Center for Neuroprosthetics, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology (EPFL), CH-1202 Geneva, Switzerland
2Brain Mind Institute, Faculty of Life Sciences, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology (EPFL), CH-1015 Lausanne, Switzerland
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Olaf Blanke
1Center for Neuroprosthetics, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology (EPFL), CH-1202 Geneva, Switzerland
2Brain Mind Institute, Faculty of Life Sciences, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology (EPFL), CH-1015 Lausanne, Switzerland
4Department of Neurology, University of Geneva, CH-1211 Geneva, Switzerland
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Nathan Faivre
1Center for Neuroprosthetics, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology (EPFL), CH-1202 Geneva, Switzerland
2Brain Mind Institute, Faculty of Life Sciences, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology (EPFL), CH-1015 Lausanne, Switzerland
3Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition, CNRS, Univ. Grenoble Alpes, CNRS, LPNC, 38000 Grenoble, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Previous studies have shown that self-generated stimuli in auditory, visual, and somatosensory domains are attenuated, producing decreased behavioral and neural responses compared with the same stimuli that are externally generated. Yet, whether such attenuation also occurs for higher-level cognitive functions beyond sensorimotor processing remains unknown. In this study, we assessed whether cognitive functions such as numerosity estimations are subject to attenuation in 56 healthy participants (32 women). We designed a task allowing the controlled comparison of numerosity estimations for self-generated (active condition) and externally generated (passive condition) words. Our behavioral results showed a larger underestimation of self-generated compared with externally generated words, suggesting that numerosity estimations for self-generated words are attenuated. Moreover, the linear relationship between the reported and actual number of words was stronger for self-generated words, although the ability to track errors about numerosity estimations was similar across conditions. Neuroimaging results revealed that numerosity underestimation involved increased functional connectivity between the right intraparietal sulcus and an extended network (bilateral supplementary motor area, left inferior parietal lobule, and left superior temporal gyrus) when estimating the number of self-generated versus externally generated words. We interpret our results in light of two models of attenuation and discuss their perceptual versus cognitive origins.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT We perceive sensory events as less intense when they are self-generated compared with when they are externally generated. This phenomenon, called attenuation, enables us to distinguish sensory events from self and external origins. Here, we designed a novel fMRI paradigm to assess whether cognitive processes such as numerosity estimations are also subject to attenuation. When asking participants to estimate the number of words they had generated or passively heard, we found bigger underestimation in the former case, providing behavioral evidence of attenuation. Attenuation was associated with increased functional connectivity of the intraparietal sulcus, a region involved in numerosity processing. Together, our results indicate that the attenuation of self-generated stimuli is not limited to sensory consequences but is also impact cognitive processes such as numerosity estimations.

  • attenuation
  • cognition
  • connectivity
  • fMRI
  • intraparietal sulcus
  • numerosity

Introduction

The ability to distinguish self-generated versus externally generated stimuli is crucial for self-representation (Kircher and David, 2003; Legrand, 2006). A typical mechanism by which stimuli generated by oneself and those caused by external sources are distinguished is sensory attenuation, whereby self-generated stimuli are perceived as less intense. Indeed, previous studies have shown that self-produced stimuli in the auditory (Baess et al., 2011; Timm et al., 2014), visual (Hughes and Waszak, 2011; Benazet et al., 2016), and somatosensory (Shergill et al., 2013; Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2020) domains, are perceived as less intense compared with the same stimuli when they are externally generated. Such attenuation was shown at the behavioral level and at the neural level in sensory cortical regions (e.g., auditory cortex; Rummell et al., 2016; Whitford, 2019) as well as the thalamus, cerebellum, supplementary motor area (SMA), and inferior parietal cortex (Hickok, 2012; Lima et al., 2016; Bansal et al., 2018; Brooks and Cullen, 2019).

Previous studies have shown that attenuation not only applies to stimuli that are generated by overt actions, but also extends to covert processes such as motor imagery (Kilteni et al., 2018) or inner speech (Scott et al., 2013; Whitford et al., 2017). In the latter case, attenuation of the electrophysiological (Whitford et al., 2017; Jack et al., 2019) and behavioral (Scott et al., 2013) responses corresponding to the test stimulus (phoneme) was observed when it matched the imagined cued phoneme. These studies used phonemes that are integrated into the early stages of hierarchical speech processing implying primary sensory cortices (Liebenthal et al., 2005; DeWitt and Rauschecker, 2012), where attenuation effects have been demonstrated (Rummell et al., 2016). An outstanding question is whether the attenuation of self-generated stimuli is limited to sensory consequences (e.g., weakened percept) or whether it can also impact cognitive processes that are nonperceptual in nature such as abstract judgments or performance monitoring.

To answer this question, we investigated the cognitive function of numerosity estimations, defined as approximate judgments when counting is not involved (Dehaene, 1997). Properties of numerosity estimations such as innateness, amodality, or precision that linearly decreases with increasing numerosity have been described (Anobile et al., 2016; Burr et al., 2018). Extensive neuroimaging work has also established that brain areas in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) region play a key role in numerosity processing (for review, see Arsalidou and Taylor, 2011). If attenuation for self-related functions extends to higher-level cognitive processes such as numerosity estimations, we would expect the number of items that are self-generated to be underestimated compared with externally generated items in relation to modulation of IPS activity. Thus, this study aimed at investigating whether numerosity estimations of self-generated words were attenuated compared with numerosity estimations of passively heard words, thereby demonstrating that self-attenuation applies to cognitive processes beyond sensory processing.

To investigate which brain regions and networks showed activity related to attenuation processes, we designed a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) paradigm allowing the controlled comparison of numerosity estimations of either self-generated words (active condition) during a phonetic verbal fluency task or externally generated words (passive condition) while passively listening to a stream of words. Additionally, we asked participants to evaluate the error they could have made in their estimation (performance monitoring). Assuming that attenuation occurs during numerosity estimations (active condition), we predicted that participants would underestimate the number of self-generated versus externally generated words and explored possible effects on performance monitoring. As performance monitoring is better for self-initiated versus observed processes (Pereira et al., 2020), we expected to find a stronger relationship between the reported and actual number of words in the active versus passive condition. At the neural level, considering that sensory attenuation of self-generated stimuli involves the corresponding sensory brain regions (e.g., primary auditory cortex for attenuated sounds; Rummell et al., 2016; Whitford, 2019), we expected to find a reduced BOLD signal during the active condition versus passive condition in areas responsible for numerosity processing including the IPS.

Materials and Methods

Participants.

Three independent participant groups were tested in this study. First, we performed a behavioral pilot experiment in a mock scanner, where we tested 17 participants (6 women; age range, 18–28 years; mean age, 23 years; 95% CI = 23, 24; schooling level, between 13 and 21 years; mean schooling level = 17 years; 95% CI = 16, 18). During the main fMRI experiment, we studied 25 participants (14 women; age range, 18–37 years; mean age, 23 years; 95% CI = 22, 26; schooling level, between 12 and 22 years; mean age, 17 years; 95% CI = 16, 18). An additional control experiment in the mock scanner was performed, where 14 participants (12 women) were tested (age range, 18 participants 29 years; mean age, 24 years; 95% CI = 22, 26; schooling level, between 10 and 23 years; mean schooling level, 16 years; 95% CI = 14, 18). All participants were right handed according to the Edinburgh Hand Preference Inventory (Oldfield, 1971) and native French-speaking healthy volunteers with no history of neurologic or psychiatric disease and no recent reported history of drug use. Participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and no claustrophobia. All participants were naive to the purpose of the study, gave informed consent in accordance with institutional guidelines and the Declaration of Helsinki, and received monetary compensation (20 ₣/h). The study was approved by the local ethical committee of the canton of Geneva (protocol ID, 2015–00092).

Experimental task.

The experiment was performed in a mock scanner environment (both for the behavioral pilot experiment and the training for the main fMRI experiment) and in the MRI scanner (main fMRI experiment). The task consisted of phonetic verbal fluency and passive word-listening parts, followed by numerosity and error estimations (Fig. 1). The task comprised two conditions (20 trials each), during which participants either covertly generated words (active condition) or listened to prerecorded words (passive condition). Each trial started with a randomly jittered intertrial interval varying between 4 and 4.75 s, followed by an audio cue (2 s) indicating which of the active or passive condition will follow, and a recorded cue letter (2 s). Next, the word generation phase lasted between 20 and 35 s; in the active condition, participants had to covertly generate words starting with the cued letter (Fig. 1, top). For each word they covertly generated, participants were asked to press a response button, which gave us access to the actual number of generated words. In the passive condition, a series of prerecorded audio words were played to the participants, who were asked to press the response button for every word that contained the cue letter (Fig. 1, bottom). We avoided asking participants to detect words starting with the cued letter, as this could have influenced word generation in the next active trials with the same cued letter. In both conditions, the end of the generation/listening phase was indicated by an audio cue (0.5 s). After that, participants first reported the estimation of a total number of words generated (active condition) or heard (passive condition). For this, they used two buttons that moved a slider displayed on the screen. The slider was presented as a random integer (range, 0–20), which could be changed in value by pressing the response box buttons (left button, decrease the value; right button, increase the value). This was followed by an error estimation, where participants were asked to evaluate their performance on the numerosity estimation by estimating the magnitude of the error they thought they might have made (in number of words). For this, an automatically sliding bar was presented, and the participant had to select with one button press the desired value (e.g., ±2 words error). Values varied from ±0 words (e.g., numerosity response judged as correct) to ±5 words mistaken. The total time for numerosity and error estimations was restricted to a maximum of 7 s. Except for the numerosity and error estimation periods, participants were asked to perform the task with eyes closed.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Schematic representation of the general task flow: active (top) and passive (bottom) conditions consisting of instruction, word generation/listening, and numerosity and error estimations. Each condition was repeated four times per block. Auditory and visual instructions were provided in French. Simulated distribution of derived behavioral performance measures using numerosity and error estimation values are depicted in Extended Data Figure 1-1.

Figure 1-1

Data simulation of behavioral performance measures. Data were simulated with pseudorandom generation of an actual number of words and numerosity estimations ranging between 5 and 20. Error estimation varied between 0 and 5 as set during the task design. A, Simulated numerosity performance. Increasing intensity of red represent linear increase of overestimation (positive values of numerosity performance), while increasing intensity in blue represents a linear increase of underestimation (negative values of numerosity performance). B, Simulated performance monitoring when the number of words is constant (10 or 15 words). The performance monitoring value is depicted in the heatmap changing from blue (being correct) to red (worse performance monitoring). Download Figure 1-1, TIF file.

Stimuli.

All stimuli were prepared and presented using MATLAB 2016b (MathWorks) and the Psychtoolbox-3 Toolbox (psychtoolbox.org; Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997; Kleiner et al., 2007). Twenty different cue letters were used for active and passive conditions. The same cue letter was used once during the active and once during the passive condition in the counterbalanced order. Words played back during the passive condition were chosen from a list of 420 French words (Ferrand and Alario, 1998). The audio stimuli presented during the task were recorded by male and female native French speakers in a neutral manner and registered in wav format with a 11,025 Hz sampling frequency. The gender of the voice pronouncing words in the passive condition was matched to the participants' gender. During the experiment, participants were equipped with MRI-compatible earphones and report buttons for the right hand.

Procedure.

Participants were trained in a mock scanner before the main fMRI experiment to familiarize themselves with the task. They were asked to perform four trials of the task during the training (twice for each condition), with the cue letters “j” and “k.” These letters were not used later during the main fMRI experiment. We note that participants were instructed by the experimenter not to count or use any strategy to try to remember the number of words generated/heard while performing numerosity and error estimations.

The main experiment consisted of three runs lasting ∼15 min, each with short breaks in between. The total duration of a trial varied between 40 and 55 s because of pseudorandomized time for the word generation/listening phase. This time variability was introduced to avoid habituation and predictability for the number of generated/heard words and to decorrelate hemodynamic activity related to word generation/listening and numerosity estimations. The experiment was designed in 10 blocks with four trials of the same condition per block. The blocks of the active condition trials always preceded the blocks of the passive condition, allowing us to use the number and pace of generated words that were recorded based on a participant's button presses to play back the words during the next block of the passive condition. The order of the number of words played with their corresponding pace during the passive condition was shuffled within the block. This was done to ensure that participants could not recognize whether the number and pace of the played words matched the preceding active condition block. Additionally, to avoid repetition effects between blocks, the cue letters between two consecutive blocks were different, meaning that the cue letters in a given active block were not used in the following passive block.

After the main experiment, a standard phonemic verbal fluency test (generation time, 60 s; cue letter, “p”; Lezak, 1995) was performed overtly to verify that subjects understood the task correctly. Overall, the experiment lasted ∼1 h and 30 min (MRI session) and 1 h in the mock scanner (pilot session). The pilot mock scanner study contained the same procedure as the main MRI experiment, except for the shorter breaks between the runs since there was no scanning involved.

The sample size for the main fMRI experiment was based on a recent study from our group in which similar conditions were contrasted (e.g., judgment of self-generated vs observed decisions; Pereira et al., 2020).

Control experiment.

An additional control experiment in the mock scanner was performed to rule out the possibility that repetition effects occurred between consecutive blocks. It consisted only of passive condition trials, which were divided into “random” and “repeated” blocks. The random block trials were designed by pseudorandomly generating the number of words played (mean, 10 words/block; range, 6–20 words/trial) and their playing pace (total time of playing, between 20 and 35 s) to match the main fMRI experiment. Repeated blocks consequently followed random blocks, with the same number of words and word-playing pace as in the preceding random block, but with a shuffled order. This control experiment consisted of two random and two repeated blocks, with four trials/block, and lasted ∼30 min.

Behavioral performance measures.

Most statistical frameworks to analyze performance monitoring have been developed for discrimination tasks with a binary response (for review, see Fleming and Lau, 2014). In the following, we propose two indices of numerosity performance and performance monitoring to analyze ordinal data (e.g., the number of words). In addition to the prerequisites described below, these indices were defined at the single-trial level, so they could serve as parametric regressors of interest in the fMRI analysis.

The numerosity performance index is a normalized accuracy ratio reflecting how correctly participants estimated the number of words during the generation/listening phase. For each trial, we wanted signed numerosity performance to be proportional to the difference between the reported number of generated/heard words [numerosity estimation (N)] and the actual number of words generated/heard (W). We normalized this difference by the sum of numerosity estimation and the actually generated/heard number of words (N + W; Eq. 1) to give more weight to errors made about low numbers of words (e.g., an error of ±2 given a numerosity estimation of 8 has higher magnitude than an error of ±2 given a numerosity estimation of 16; Extended Data Fig. 1-1A). This normalization allowed us to assess attenuation effects independent of the number of generated words, given that the precision of numerosity estimation linearly decreases with an increasing number of items (Piazza et al., 2004). Negative numerosity performance values thus reflected an underestimation of generated/heard words, and positive values reflected an overestimation of generated/heard words. In contrast, null numerosity performance values reflected correct answers about the number of generated/heard words, as follows: Numerosity performance =N−WN + W.(1)

In addition to the numerosity performance index, we derived separate measures of accuracy (N – W) and accuracy ratio [(N – W)/W] to assess the generalizability of our findings.

Performance monitoring reflected how well participants estimated an error about their previous performance. We defined it as the absolute value of the difference between the error estimation (E) and accuracy (N – W), normalized by the sum of the numerosity estimation and words generated/heard (N + W; Eq. 2). Normalization was performed to consider the difficulty; the same error made when estimating the low number of words or high number of words should be penalized proportionally. A performance monitoring value of 0 reflected ideal error tracking, whereby participants correctly estimated the error made during the numerosity estimation. An increase in performance monitoring value represented an increase in error magnitude while estimating the difference between numerosity estimation and the actual number of words generated/heard (Extended Data Fig. 1-1B), as follows: Performance monitoring =|E−|N−W|N+W|.(2)

Data cleaning was performed before statistical analysis: trials for which participants did not generate at least five words or failed to answer numerosity or error estimations within the time limit were excluded from behavioral and fMRI analysis (in total, 2.44 ± 1.87 trials/subject were excluded). The threshold of five words was selected according to the working memory capacity of 5 ± 2 items (Cowan, 2010). Considering all participants' data, 6.1% of all trials were discarded.

Behavioral data analysis.

All continuous variables (numerosity performance, accuracy, accuracy ratio, and performance monitoring) were analyzed using linear mixed-effects regressions with condition (“active”, “passive”, or “random”, “repeat” for the control experiment) as a fixed effect and a random intercept by participant and condition. The inclusion of additional random effects was guided by model comparison and selection based on Bayesian information criteria. Analyses were performed using the lme4 (Bates et al., 2015) and lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al., 2017) packages in R (https://www.r-project.org/). The significance of fixed effects was estimated using Satterthwaite's approximation for degrees of freedom of F statistics (Luke, 2017).

Bayesian analysis was performed using the brms package (Bürkner, 2017) in R to examine the occurrence of possible repetition effects between random and repeated passive blocks in the control experiment. Namely, we conducted a Bayesian linear model of numerosity performance with condition (random, repeated) as a fixed effect and a random intercept by participant and condition with four chains of 10,000 iterations including 2000 warmup samples. We made a prior assumption that underestimation would be observed in the passive condition (prior with Gaussian distribution of mean = −0.032 and SD = 0.156), based on the difference between numerosity performance during the active and passive conditions observed in the main fMRI experiment.

Data simulation of absolute accuracy during numerosity estimations.

To examine whether the absolute accuracy (correct numerosity estimations of words generated/heard) of numerosity estimations observed in the experimental data may be approximated by a noisy sampling process rather than counting, we performed data simulations. More specifically, we simulated 1000 numerosity estimations (as the main experiment contained 40 trials/subject * 25 subjects) from a normal distribution centered on 10 and an SD varying from 0.5 to 6 with 0.1 steps. We rounded each item to the nearest integer and counted how many times the number 10 was obtained. The simulation was iterated 10,000 times.

fMRI data acquisition.

MRI data were acquired using a Siemens Magnetom Prisma 3 T scanner with a 64-channel head coil. T1-weighted (1 mm isotropic) scans were acquired using an MPRAGE (magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo) sequence [TR = 2300 ms; TI = 900 ms; TE = 2.25 ms; flip angle = 8°; GRAPPA (generalized autocalibrating partial parallel acquisition) = 2; FOV = 256 × 256 mm; 208 slices]. Functional scans were obtained using echoplanar imaging (EPI) sequence (multiband acceleration = 6; TR = 1000 ms; TE = 32 ms; flip-angle = 58°; FOV = 224 × 224 mm; matrix = 64 × 64; slice thickness = 2 mm; 66 slices). The number of functional image volumes varied according to the experiment duration (2278 ± 61 volumes).

fMRI data preprocessing.

Anatomical and functional images were processed and analyzed using SPM-12 (Wellcome Center for Human Neuroimaging). Preprocessing steps included slice time correction, field map distortion correction, realignment, and unwarping to spatially correct for head motions and distortions, coregistration of structural and functional images, normalization of all images to common Montreal Neurologic Institute (MNI) space, and spatial smoothing with a Gaussian kernel with a full-width at half-maximum of 4 mm. Quality assurance of all EPI images was performed with the criteria of a maximum of 2 mm translation and 2° rotation between volumes. In addition, an excessive movement was estimated with the mean framewise displacement (FD; Power et al., 2012) with an exclusion threshold of 0.5 mm. None of the subjects had a higher mean FD (0.2 ± 0.06 mm) than the set threshold.

fMRI data analysis.

We used a two-level random-effects analysis. In the first-level analysis, condition-specific effects were estimated according to a general linear model (GLM) fitted for each subject. An average mask of gray matter from all subjects was built using FSL (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/) and was used to mask out white matter and nonbrain tissues. The GLM was built using six boxcar regressors corresponding to the duration of the word generation/listening phase, numerosity estimation and error estimation in the active and passive conditions. Parametric modulators of numerosity performance and performance monitoring were included in the numerosity estimation and error estimation regressors, respectively. Further, we added regressors of no interest corresponding to audio instructions, button presses, and excluded trials, plus six regressors for head motion (translation and rotation).

At the second level (group level), we performed a one-way ANOVA with F tests to assess the main effects common to active and passive conditions and t tests to analyze the difference between conditions (active vs passive) for each regressor of interest: numerosity estimation and error estimation. We used a voxel-level statistical threshold of p < 0.001 and corrected for multiple comparisons at the cluster level using familywise error (FWE) correction with the threshold of p < 0.05. We used the anatomic automatic labeling atlas for brain parcellation (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002).

Functional connectivity analysis.

Psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis was used to identify modulations of functional coupling between a seed region and other brain regions by experimental conditions (active vs passive; Friston et al., 1997). To perform this analysis, we used generalized PPI toolbox version 13.1 (McLaren et al., 2012). Spheres of 6 mm radius were formed around the peak coordinates of the right IPS cluster (x = 29; y = −65; z = 50) and the left IPS cluster (x = −27; y = −66; z = 47) that were identified in the second-level analyses. First-level (individual) GLM analyses were performed, including task regressors of numerosity estimation or error estimation (psychological term) and time course of the seed region (physiological term). As for other aforementioned fMRI analyses, we performed t tests to compare the differences between conditions.

Data availability.

The MRI data that support the findings of this study are available in a public repository on Zenodo (https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4925909). Behavioral data, analysis, and task codes are available as follows: https://gitlab.epfl.ch/lnco-public/cognitive-attenuation.

Results

Behavioral results

By design, the number of generated and heard words was matched between conditions (active: mean = 10.7; SD = 3.6; 95% CI = 10.4, 11.1; passive: mean = 10.7; SD = 3.5; 95% CI = 10.4, 11.0; F(1,884) = 0.005; p = 0.94). The duration of word generation and listening did not differ between conditions either (active: mean = 27.4; SD = 5.6; 95% CI = 26.9, 27.9; passive: mean = 28.4; SD = 7.0; 95% CI = 27.7, 29.0, F(1,26) = 2.04; p = 0.16). Having shown that conditions were similar in terms of difficulty, we turned to the analysis of numerosity performance and performance monitoring.

Numerosity performance indices revealed that globally, participants underestimated the number of words (mean = −0.05; SD = 0.11; 95% CI = −0.06, −0.05). This underestimation was significantly larger (F(1,24) = 5.85; p = 0.023; 18 of 25 subjects showed the effect) in the active condition (mean = −0.07; SD = 0.11; 95% CI = −0.08, −0.06) compared with the passive condition (mean = −0.04; SD = 0.12; 95% CI = −0.05, −0.03; Fig. 2A). Comparable results were obtained during the pilot experiment in the mock scanner in an independent group of subjects (active: mean = −0.05; SD = 0.01; 95% CI = −0.06, −0.03; passive: mean = −0.03; SD = 0.10; 95% CI = −0.04, −0.02; F(1,16) = 5.80; p = 0.016). These two experiments suggest that numerosity estimations for self-generated words are attenuated.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Behavioral results. A, Individual numerosity performance in the active (red) and passive (blue) conditions. A value of zero represents ideal performance, where the reported and actual number of words match. Each dot represents a participant mean numerosity performance. Gray lines connect performance from the same participant across conditions. B, Mixed-effects linear regression between numerosity estimation (reported number of words) and an actual number of words. The reference dashed line with a slope equal to 1 represents ideal performance. Thick lines represent the global model fit, while thin lines depict individual estimates. Individual plots are depicted in Extended Data Figure 2-1. C, Mean slope coefficient estimates with 2.5% and 97.5% confidence intervals.

Figure 2-1

Mixed-effects linear regression between numerosity estimation (reported number of words) and an actual number of words depicted for each participant. Reference dashed line with a slope equal to 1 represents ideal performance. Thick lines represent the individual model fit per condition (active, red; passive, blue). Each dot represents a participant's single-trial performance (overlapping dots are depicted in higher color intensity). Download Figure 2-1, TIF file.

We further show that the larger underestimation in the active compared with the passive condition was also found when considering the measures of accuracy (active: mean = −1.48; SD = 2.38; 95% CI = −1.69, −1.26; passive: mean = −0.94; SD = 2.59; 95% CI = −1.17, −0.71; F(1,24) = 4.43; p = 0.045) and accuracy ratio as dependent variables (active: mean = 0.89; SD = 0.20; 95% CI = 0.87, 0.91; passive: mean = 0.95; SD = 0.23; 95% CI = 0.93, 0.97; F(1,24) = 7.44; p = 0.011).

We then assessed the linear relationship between the reported (numerosity estimation) and actual number of words (Fig. 2B). In addition to a main effect of words (F(1,908) = 1042; p < 0.001) and condition (F(1,198) = 8.25; p = 0.0045), we found an interaction between condition and generated/heard words (F(1,802) = 4.3; p = 0.038). This interaction was driven by a steeper slope in the active condition (mean = 0.62; 95% CI = 0.57, 0.67) compared with the passive condition (mean = 0.55; 95% CI = 0.50, 0.60; Fig. 2C), which indicates better numerosity tracking for self-generated words during the main fMRI experiment (a slope of value 1 reflecting ideal performance). We note that this interaction did not reach significance in our pilot experiment (F(1,350) = 1.10; p = 0.66), which calls for interpreting this result with caution.

Next, we investigated whether performance monitoring varied between conditions, by quantifying how well participants were able to track the error made when estimating the number of generated/heard words. We found no differences in performance monitoring (F(1,24) = 1.09; p = 0.3) between the active condition (mean = 0.07; SD = 0.06; 95% CI = 0.07, 0.08) and the passive condition (mean = 0.08; SD = 0.06; 95% CI = 0.07, 0.09) during the main fMRI experiment or during the mock scanner pilot experiment (F(1,16) = 0.78; p = 0.39; active: mean = 0.07; SD = 0.06; 95% CI = 0.06, 0.08; passive: mean = 0.06; SD = 0.06; 95% CI = 0.06, 0.07). This confirms the absence of evidence supporting an effect of attenuation on performance monitoring.

Finally, we conducted a control experiment for word generation outside the scanner, during which participants had to perform a standard verbal fluency task overtly with the cue letter “p.” Comparing the number of words generated, starting with the letter “p” overtly (outside the scanner) and covertly (during the scanning), we did not observe any significant differences between the number of words generated overtly (mean = 12.6; SD = 3.79; 95% CI = 10.9, 14.1) and covertly (mean = 12.3; SD = 3.96; 95% CI = 10.6, 13.9; paired t test: t(24) = −0.4; p = 0.69). This control experiment confirms that subjects did generate words covertly and comparably to overt fluency, as instructed.

Control experiment

The global underestimation (mean = −0.06; SD = 0.11; 95% CI = −0.08, −0.05) was replicated as in the main experiment. This underestimation was not different (F(1,117) = 0.03; p = 0.87) between random blocks (mean = −0.06; SD = 0.12; 95% CI = −0.09, −0.04) and repeated blocks (mean = −0.07; SD = 0.12; 95% CI = −0.09, −0.04), with a Bayes factor BF10 = 0.09, supporting the absence of difference between conditions.

Data simulation of absolute accuracy during numerosity estimations

As can be seen in Figure 3, the value of 10 occurred in 19.5% of all data points when drawn from a normal distribution with SD = 2, similar to the observed empirical data.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Percentage of simulated accurate numerosity estimations (occurrences of value 10) in normal distribution centered on 10 with varying SD. Simulation was iterated 10,000 times. The reference dashed line of 19.5% absolute numerosity estimations accuracy (as observed in the experiment empirical data) occurs with a SD of 2. Each dot represents a single iteration of the data simulation (overlapping dots are depicted in higher color intensity).

fMRI results

Numerosity performance

Brain activity during the numerosity estimation phase was widespread, regardless of the experimental condition (Extended Data Table 1-1). Differences between conditions revealed widespread relative deactivations in the active compared with the passive condition in the bilateral parietal cortex, including IPS, middle-superior temporal gyri, precuneus, cerebellum, middle cingulate gyri, SMA, insula, middle-superior frontal gyri, hippocampus, caudate nucleus, and putamen (Table 1, detailed list of all areas).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Numerosity estimation: difference between active and passive conditions

Table 1-1

Numerosity estimation: main effect (active + passive conditions). Brain areas activated during the numerosity estimation phase independent of parametric modulation. Voxel level, p < 0.001 uncorrected; cluster threshold at p < 0.05, FWE corrected. BA, Brodmann area; k, cluster size; R, right hemisphere; L, left hemisphere; R, right hemisphere. Download Table 1-1, DOCX file.

To avoid contaminating the results with inherent differences between the active and passive conditions that are not specific to numerosity processes, we looked for brain activity parametrically modulated by numerosity performance. We found a single brain region with such a pattern of activity, namely the right IPS, whose activity was negatively correlated with numerosity performance (main effect: F = 20.0; pFWE = 0.042; cluster size = 148; MNI coordinates: x = 29, y = −65, z = 50; Fig. 4A,B). The same pattern was found bilaterally when using a less stringent threshold (peak level uncorrected, p < 0.005; Fig. 4C, Table 2). Unlike our prediction, IPS activation did not differ between the active and passive conditions (pFWE > 0.05), suggesting that this effect was independent of whether words were heard or actively generated.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Parametric modulation of numerosity performance (exploratory analyses at lower statistical threshold)

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Neural correlates of numerosity performance. A, Main effect (active + passive conditions) of the parametric modulation of numerosity performance during numerosity estimation in the right IPS (green). B, Box plot of the corresponding individual β estimates in the right IPS in the active (red) and passive (blue) conditions. Each dot represents a participant mean β estimate. Gray lines connect β estimates from the same participant across conditions. C, Overlap between the parametric modulation of numerosity performance during numerosity estimation (green) main effect (active + passive conditions) at the lower statistical threshold of peak level uncorrected p < 0.005 (exploratory analyses) and parametric modulation of performance monitoring during the error estimation (active vs passive condition; purple); peak level uncorrected, p < 0.001.

Using PPI analysis, we investigated whether the functional connectivity of the right IPS with other brain regions differed between the active and passive conditions. This analysis revealed that bilateral SMA, left inferior parietal lobule (IPL), and left superior temporal gyrus (STG) had increased connectivity with the right IPS in the active compared with the passive condition (Fig. 5, Table 3). No effect in the other direction was observed.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

PPI analysis: numerosity estimation

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

PPI analysis: numerosity estimation. A, Increased functional connectivity in the active versus passive condition between the right IPS seed (green) and bilateral SMA, left IPL, and STG, depicted in red. B, Box plots of the corresponding β estimates for each significant PPI cluster in the active (red) and passive (blue) conditions. Each dot represents a participant mean β estimates. Gray lines connect β estimates from the same participant across conditions.

Performance monitoring

In addition to numerosity performance, we also quantified brain activity during the error estimation phase. We found widespread cortical and subcortical activations during this phase compared with baseline, regardless of the experimental condition (Extended Data Table 4-1). Moreover, bilateral insula and right putamen were significantly less active during the active condition compared with the passive condition (Fig. 6, Table 4). The left caudate nucleus showed the opposite pattern, with higher activity in the passive condition than in the active condition.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 4.

Error estimation: difference between active and passive conditions

Table 4-1

Error estimation: main effect (active + passive conditions). Brain areas with activations during the error estimation phase, independent of parametric modulation. Voxel level, p < 0.001, uncorrected; cluster threshold at p < 0.05, FWE corrected. BA, Brodmann area; k, cluster size; R, right hemisphere; L, left hemisphere; R, right hemisphere. Download Table 4-1, DOCX file.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Neural correlates of performance monitoring. A, Decreased neural activity during the active condition versus the passive condition in the right putamen and insula bilaterally (blue). Increased activity in the active condition versus the passive condition in the left caudate nucleus (red). B, Box plots of the corresponding β estimates in the active (red) and passive (blue) conditions for each significant cluster observed. Each dot represents a participant mean β estimate. Gray lines connect β estimates from the same participant across conditions.

To avoid confounding factors between the active and passive conditions, we looked more specifically at parametric modulations of error monitoring. We observed that the BOLD signal in the left IPS was more related to performance monitoring in the active condition compared with the passive condition (t = 4.02; pFWE = 0.041; kvox = 198; MNI coordinates: x = −27, y = −66, z = 47; Fig. 7). Interestingly, this activation cluster spatially overlapped with the observed activation cluster related to the main effect of numerosity performance when applying a lower statistical threshold (exploratory analysis: peak level uncorrected, p < 0.005; cluster level, pFWE < 0.05; Fig. 4C).

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Parametric modulation of performance monitoring. A, Positive association between the trialwise performance monitoring and BOLD signal during performance monitoring in the active condition versus the passive condition in the left IPS (purple). B, Box plot of corresponding β estimates in the left IPS in the active (red) and passive (blue) conditions. Each dot represents a participant mean β estimate. Gray lines connect β estimates from the same participant across conditions.

Last, results from the PPI analysis using the left IPS as a seed ROI did not reveal any functional connectivity differences between the active and passive conditions during error estimation.

Discussion

The present study examined whether the attenuation of self-generated stimuli impacts cognitive processes beyond the traditionally investigated sensory processes, namely numerosity estimations and performance monitoring. To this end, we developed an experimental paradigm allowing the controlled comparison of numerosity estimations and performance monitoring regarding self-generated and externally generated words while acquiring fMRI data. We found that participants more strongly underestimated the number of self-generated compared with externally generated words, providing behavioral evidence that numerosity estimations are indeed subject to attenuation. As expected, numerosity performance was associated with hemodynamic activity changes in IPS. Furthermore, a network including the bilateral SMA, left IPL, and left STG showed increased functional connectivity with the right IPS during numerosity estimations for self-generated words, suggesting that numerosity-related attenuation involves this neural network. Finally, by asking participants to monitor the accuracy of their own numerosity estimations, we found equivalent performance monitoring for self-generated and externally generated words. Although no difference was found at the behavioral level, we found that performance monitoring was associated with increased IPS activity in the active condition versus the passive condition.

Behavioral and neural markers of attenuated numerosity estimations

Participants underestimated the number of words they generated or heard. Such underestimations were previously described regarding the numeric estimation of perceptual quantities (e.g., number of dots or sequences of sounds) to discrete measures (e.g., Arabic numeral; Castronovo and Seron, 2007; Reinert et al., 2019). In the present study, we found that word numerosity underestimation was stronger in the active compared with the passive condition, in line with our hypothesis that attenuation of self-generated stimuli may extend to higher-level cognitive functions (Kilteni et al., 2018; Jack et al., 2019). Of note, this underestimation could not be because of simple repetition effects between successive blocks of active and passive condition trials, as a control experiment showed no differences in numerosity estimations between repeated blocks of passive condition trials.

Attenuation of self-generated stimuli has been investigated mostly for sensory processes and refers to the diminished behavioral and neural responses associated with self-generated compared with externally generated stimuli (Timm et al., 2014; Benazet et al., 2016). Importantly, attenuation has recently also been described for imagined actions in the absence of overt actions (Kilteni et al., 2018; Jack et al., 2019). For example, imagined self-touch was felt as less intense compared with externally applied touch (Kilteni et al., 2018). Similarly, imagined speech elicited reduced electrophysiological signals related to auditory processing (Whitford et al., 2017). These studies, however, investigated cognitive processes intimately linked to sensorimotor systems (e.g., imagined movement and touch), yet, to the best of our knowledge, it was unknown whether comparable mechanisms of attenuation also affect cognition beyond sensorimotor processing, such as numerosity estimations with no or less obviously implicated sensorimotor processes (Dehaene, 1997). Here we show that differential attenuation can be observed for cognitive processes such as numerosity estimations that depend on whether they are self-generated or not. As of today, there is no consensus to explain how the brain attenuates expected (e.g., self-generated) stimuli while remaining sensitive to unexpected ones (for a recent review and unifying theoretical account, see Press et al., 2020). Among the so-called cancelation theories, the internal forward model (Miall and Wolpert, 1996; Farrer and Frith, 2002) proposes that corollary discharges related to action are used to predict the sensory consequences of that action. When such predictions match the actual sensory feedback from the action, its sensory consequences are attenuated, and the action is perceived as self-generated (Wolpert and Flanagan, 2001). The forward model thus proposes to link sensory attenuation to the sensory predictions generated by a neural comparator. An analogous mechanism has been proposed to account for attenuation for covert actions such as motor imagery (Kilteni et al., 2018) or inner speech (Tian and Poeppel, 2010). The present data show that attenuation exists beyond overt and covert actions, raising the possibility that the forward model extends to repetitive cognitive activity (e.g., fluency and related numerosity estimations). One possibility, similar to what has been described for inner speech, would be that numerosity underestimation stems from a weaker perceptual representation of self-generated words. In other words, attenuation may not impact numerosity estimations directly, but rather through a decreased representation of self-generated words, which in turn may lead to attenuation of numerosity estimations.

A second account could be that mental operations have a gating effect (Cromwell et al., 2008), independent from predictive mechanisms, thereby directly affecting the strength of the mental representations of self-generated words. A similar gating mechanism has recently been shown to offer a plausible alternative to forward model accounts of sensory self-attenuation (Thomas et al., 2020). Finally, the general mechanisms put forward by the predictive coding account of active inference to explain sensory attenuation may also apply to nonperceptual cognitive attenuation such as the one we observed (Brown et al., 2013).

At the neural level, the IPS showed activity related to numerosity performance, which is in line with previous studies (Cohen and Dehaene, 1996; Piazza et al., 2006). This relation, however, was not modulated by condition (active vs passive), as one could have expected based on previous reports showing that sensory attenuation coincides with decreased activity in the corresponding sensory area (e.g., attenuation in primary auditory cortex during self-generated auditory stimuli; Rummell et al., 2016; Whitford, 2019). Importantly, we found differences in functional coupling during numerosity estimations of the IPS with a network of brain regions between the active and passive conditions. The increase in coupling during the active condition occurred between the IPS and a network comprising the SMA, IPL, and STG, which are known to be involved in predictive processing, for example of self-generated auditory and imagery speech (Lima et al., 2016; Tian et al., 2016). Although previous neuroimaging studies have mainly shown attenuated brain activity for self-generated actions (Whitford, 2019), increases in functional connectivity such as we describe have been reported in the primary auditory cortex during attenuated speech (van de Ven et al., 2009) or in somatosensory cortex during attenuated touch (Kilteni and Ehrsson, 2020). Thus, increased functional connectivity in a network centered on the key numerosity region, IPS, and that has been associated with speech-related processing further supports that numerosity underestimation stems from a process related to attenuation when participants self-generate words.

In addition to numerosity underestimation, we observed that participants formed more accurate numerosity judgments for self-generated versus externally generated words: while participants' word estimations were lower in the active condition than in the passive condition, the relation between their estimated number of words and the actual number of words was better in the active condition compared with the passive condition, suggesting a sharper representation of the number of self-generated words. This result corroborates recent findings showing better monitoring for decisions that are committed rather than observed (Pereira et al., 2020). This improved monitoring of self-generated words could be related to the sharpening of expected representations known in the sensory domain (Kok et al., 2012) or to a self-generation effect underlying the facilitation of information encoding and enhanced recall for self-generated stimuli (Slamecka and Graf, 1978; Bertsch et al., 2007). We note that this effect was not replicated during the mock pilot experiment, which included a smaller sample of participants with a globally higher absolute accuracy. It is possible that this subtle effect could not be observed in such conditions, which warrants future investigations and cautious interpretation.

Performance monitoring is modulated at the neural but not behavioral level

Previous research has shown that both humans and nonhuman primates (Beran et al., 2006; Duyan and Balcı, 2018, 2019) can monitor the quality of their numerosity estimations. Thus, in addition to asking participants to estimate the number of words they generated or heard, we also asked them to estimate their own error during this process (performance monitoring). Although our behavioral results showed similar performance monitoring between conditions, we observed that performance monitoring was associated with hemodynamic activity in the left IPS predominantly in the active condition. Interestingly, this region is not typically associated with performance monitoring as is the case in the prefrontal cortex or the insula/inferior frontal gyrus (Vaccaro and Fleming, 2018). Since activity in the left IPS is related to numerosity estimation (Cappelletti et al., 2007; Dormal et al., 2012), this parametric modulation could therefore represent a substrate for monitoring that is specific to numerosity estimations. By investigating global fMRI activity during error estimations, we also observed decreased activation in the insula and putamen and increased activity in the caudate nucleus when comparing the active versus passive conditions. To note, the anterior insula and putamen were activated during both conditions but were attenuated in the active compared with the passive conditions. While the anterior insula activations are consistent with previous literature on performance monitoring (Ullsperger et al., 2010; Bastin et al., 2017), the findings of modulated activity in the striatum were unexpected. Both areas are known as essential for the control of goal-directed decision-making (Balleine et al., 2007; Kim and Im, 2019). Further links between performance monitoring and activity modulation between conditions in the striatum regions should be explored in future studies.

Methodological considerations

As generating versus listening to words inherently involves different behavioral and neural processes, several aspects of our design should be considered when interpreting the current findings as reflecting attenuation per se rather than experimental confounds. At the behavioral level, the larger underestimation during the active condition was not because of difficulty differences between conditions as task difficulty was matched by design, and absolute accuracy (e.g., correct numerosity estimations of words generated/heard) was similar in both conditions. Of note, a steeper slope during numerosity estimations and the number of words generated in the active condition suggest that participants tracked their performance better, even when underestimating more compared with the passive condition. Although we could not explicitly control whether participants counted the number of words they generated or heard, instructions were given not to do so. Yet, counting seems unlikely as absolute accuracy reached only 19.5% of all trials, which is comparable to a recent study with a similar setup (Serino et al., 2021), and was much lower than what is typically observed when participants are instructed to count (Kansaku et al., 2007). Furthermore, a similar level of absolute accuracy was obtained on simulated data with numerosity estimations drawn from a normal distribution with SD = 2, suggesting that behavior in this task is approximated by a noisy sampling process rather than counting. At the neural level, to ensure that the inherent differences between conditions did not contaminate our results, we focused analyses on the numerosity and error estimation phases, which were identical between conditions, instead of the word generation/listening phases. In addition, we did not base our conclusions on a direct contrast between the two conditions, but on correlations between neural activity and numerosity or error estimates.

Conclusion

Based on behavioral and neuroimaging data, we propose that higher-level cognitive functions such as numerosity estimations about the number of self-generated words are attenuated. Such attenuation involves a functional network including a key numerosity region (IPS) and speech-related regions including the SMA, IPL, and STG. While attenuating the sensory consequences of one's actions is of crucial importance for aspects of the self, such as the sense of agency, attenuating the products of one's mental activities may also be relevant to distinguish them from external sources of information. Our paradigm offers a promising tool to investigate attenuation processes related to the self in cognition and to compare and distinguish them from sensory attenuation processes. It may be of relevance to the study of clinical cases in which attenuation in sensory and cognitive domains may be altered, including patients with psychotic symptoms like thought insertion whereby thoughts are not considered as one's own, but as those of someone else.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the Bertarelli Foundation (Grant 532024), the Swiss National Science Foundation (Grant 3100A0-112493), National Center of Competence in Research “Synapsy - The Synaptic Bases of Mental Diseases” (Grant 51NF40–185897), and by two donors advised by CARIGEST SA (Fondazione Teofilo Rossi di Montelera e di Premuda and a second one wishing to remain anonymous). N.F. has received funding from the European Research Council under the European Union Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Program (Grant 803122). We thank the Foundation Campus Biotech Geneva (FCBG) Human Neuroscience Platform of the Campus Biotech for technical support during data acquisition.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Olaf Blanke at olaf.blanke{at}epfl.ch or Nathan Faivre at nathan.faivre{at}univ-grenoble-alpes.fr

SfN exclusive license.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Anobile G,
    2. Cicchini GM,
    3. Burr DC
    (2016) Number as a primary perceptual attribute: a review. Perception 45:5–31. doi:10.1177/0301006615602599 pmid:26562858
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Arsalidou M,
    2. Taylor MJ
    (2011) Is 2+2=4? Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and calculations. Neuroimage 54:2382–2393. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.009
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Baess P,
    2. Horváth J,
    3. Jacobsen T,
    4. Schröger E
    (2011) Selective suppression of self-initiated sounds in an auditory stream: an ERP study. Psychophysiology 48:1276–1283. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01196.x pmid:21449953
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Balleine BW,
    2. Delgado MR,
    3. Hikosaka O
    (2007) The role of the dorsal striatum in reward and decision-making. J Neurosci 27:8161–8165. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1554-07.2007 pmid:17670959
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Bansal S,
    2. Ford JM,
    3. Spering M
    (2018) The function and failure of sensory predictions. Ann NY Acad Sci 1426:199–220. doi:10.1111/nyas.13686
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Bastin J,
    2. Deman P,
    3. David O,
    4. Gueguen M,
    5. Benis D,
    6. Minotti L,
    7. Hoffman D,
    8. Combrisson E,
    9. Kujala J,
    10. Perrone-Bertolotti M,
    11. Kahane P,
    12. Lachaux JP,
    13. Jerbi K
    (2017) Direct recordings from human anterior insula reveal its leading role within the error-monitoring network. Cereb Cortex 27:1545–1557. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv352 pmid:26796212
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Bates D,
    2. Mächler M,
    3. Bolker B,
    4. Walker S
    (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Soft 67:1–48. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Benazet M,
    2. Thénault F,
    3. Whittingstall K,
    4. Bernier PM
    (2016) Attenuation of visual reafferent signals in the parietal cortex during voluntary movement. J Neurophysiol 116:1831–1839. doi:10.1152/jn.00231.2016 pmid:27466131
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Beran MJ,
    2. Smith JD,
    3. Redford JS,
    4. Washburn DA
    (2006) Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatto) monitor uncertainty during numerosity judgments. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 32:111–119. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.32.2.111 pmid:16634654
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Bertsch S,
    2. Pesta BJ,
    3. Wiscott R,
    4. McDaniel MA
    (2007) The generation effect: a meta-analytic review. Mem Cognit 35:201–210. doi:10.3758/bf03193441 pmid:17645161
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Brainard DH
    (1997) The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spat Vis 10:433–436. doi:10.1163/156856897X00357
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Brooks JX,
    2. Cullen KE
    (2019) Predictive sensing: the role of motor signals in sensory processing. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 4:842–850. doi:10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.06.003 pmid:31401034
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Brown H,
    2. Adams RA,
    3. Parees I,
    4. Edwards M,
    5. Friston K
    (2013) Active inference, sensory attenuation and illusions. Cogn Process 14:411–427. doi:10.1007/s10339-013-0571-3 pmid:23744445
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Bürkner PC
    (2017) brms: an R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. J Stat Softw 80:1–28.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Burr DC,
    2. Anobile G,
    3. Arrighi R
    (2018) Psychophysical evidence for the number sense. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 373:20170045. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0045
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Cappelletti M,
    2. Barth H,
    3. Fregni F,
    4. Spelke ES,
    5. Pascual-Leone A
    (2007) rTMS over the intraparietal sulcus disrupts numerosity processing. Exp Brain Res 179:631–642. doi:10.1007/s00221-006-0820-0 pmid:17216413
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Castronovo J,
    2. Seron X
    (2007) Numerical estimation in blind subjects: evidence of the impact of blindness and its following experience. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 33:1089–1106. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.33.5.1089 pmid:17924809
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Cohen L,
    2. Dehaene S
    (1996) Cerebral networks for number processing: evdence from a case of posterior callosal lesion. Neurocase 2:155–174. doi:10.1080/13554799608402394
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  19. ↵
    1. Cowan N
    (2010) The magical mystery four: how is working memory capacity limited, and why? Curr Dir Psychol Sci 19:51–57. doi:10.1177/0963721409359277
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Cromwell HC,
    2. Mears RP,
    3. Wan L,
    4. Boutros NN
    (2008) Sensory gating: a translational effort from basic to clinical science. Clin EEG Neurosci 39:69–72. doi:10.1177/155005940803900209 pmid:18450171
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Dehaene S
    (1997) The number sense. Oxford, UK: Oxford UP.
  22. ↵
    1. DeWitt I,
    2. Rauschecker JP
    (2012) Phoneme and word recognition in the auditory ventral stream. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:E505–E514. pmid:22308358
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    1. Dormal V,
    2. Andres M,
    3. Pesenti M
    (2012) Contribution of the right intraparietal sulcus to numerosity and length processing: an fMRI-guided TMS study. Cortex 48:623–629. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2011.05.019 pmid:21722888
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Duyan YA,
    2. Balcı F
    (2018) Numerical error monitoring. Psychon Bull Rev 25:1549–1555. pmid:29980995
    OpenUrlPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Duyan YA,
    2. Balcı F
    (2019) Metric error monitoring in the numerical estimates. Conscious Cogn 67:69–76. pmid:30529913
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Farrer C,
    2. Frith CD
    (2002) Experiencing oneself vs another person as being the cause of an action: the neural correlates of the experience of agency. Neuroimage 15:596–603. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.1009 pmid:11848702
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Ferrand L,
    2. Alario F-X
    (1998) Normes d'associations verbales pour 366 noms d'objets concrets. psy 98:659–709. doi:10.3406/psy.1998.28564
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  28. ↵
    1. Fleming SM,
    2. Lau HC
    (2014) How to measure metacognition. Front Hum Neurosci 8:443–449. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00443 pmid:25076880
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Friston KJ,
    2. Buechel C,
    3. Fink GR,
    4. Morris J,
    5. Rolls E,
    6. Dolan RJ
    (1997) Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. Neuroimage 6:218–229. doi:10.1006/nimg.1997.0291 pmid:9344826
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Hickok G
    (2012) Computational neuroanatomy of speech production. Nat Rev Neurosci 13:135–145. doi:10.1038/nrn3158 pmid:22218206
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Hughes G,
    2. Waszak F
    (2011) ERP correlates of action effect prediction and visual sensory attenuation in voluntary action. Neuroimage 56:1632–1640. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.057 pmid:21352924
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Jack BN,
    2. Le Pelley ME,
    3. Han N,
    4. Harris AWF,
    5. Spencer KM,
    6. Whitford TJ
    (2019) Inner speech is accompanied by a temporally-precise and content-specific corollary discharge. Neuroimage 198:170–180. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.04.038 pmid:31002966
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Kansaku K,
    2. Carver B,
    3. Johnson A,
    4. Matsuda K,
    5. Sadato N,
    6. Hallett M
    (2007) The role of the human ventral premotor cortex in counting successive stimuli. Exp Brain Res 178:339–350. doi:10.1007/s00221-006-0736-8 pmid:17051376
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Kilteni K,
    2. Ehrsson H
    (2020) Functional connectivity between the cerebellum and somatosensory areas implements the attenuation of self-generated touch. J Neurosci 40:894–906. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1732-19.2019 pmid:31811029
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    1. Kilteni K,
    2. Andersson BJ,
    3. Houborg C,
    4. Ehrsson HH
    (2018) Motor imagery involves predicting the sensory consequences of the imagined movement. Nat Commun 9:1617. pmid:29691389
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Kim BS,
    2. Im HI
    (2019) The role of the dorsal striatum in choice impulsivity. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1451:92–111. doi:10.1111/nyas.13961 pmid:30277562
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Kircher T,
    2. David A
    (2003) The self in neuroscience and psychiatry. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge UP.
  38. ↵
    1. Kleiner M,
    2. Brainard DH,
    3. Pelli DG,
    4. Broussard C,
    5. Wolf T,
    6. Niehorster D
    (2007) What's new in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception 36:S14.
    OpenUrl
  39. ↵
    1. Kok P,
    2. Jehee JFM,
    3. de Lange FP
    (2012) Less is more: expectation sharpens representations in the primary visual cortex. Neuron 75:265–270. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.034 pmid:22841311
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Kuznetsova A,
    2. Brockhoff PB,
    3. Christensen RHB
    (2017) lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. J Stat Softw 82:1–26.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Legrand D
    (2006) The bodily self: the sensori-motor roots of pre-reflective self-consciousness. Phenom Cogn Sci 5:89–118. doi:10.1007/s11097-005-9015-6
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  42. ↵
    1. Lezak MD
    (1995) Neuropsychological assessment, Ed 3. New York: Oxford UP.
  43. ↵
    1. Liebenthal E,
    2. Binder JR,
    3. Spitzer SM,
    4. Possing ET,
    5. Medler DA
    (2005) Neural substrates of phonemic perception. Cereb Cortex 15:1621–1631. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi040
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Lima CF,
    2. Krishnan S,
    3. Scott SK
    (2016) Roles of supplementary motor areas in auditory processing and auditory imagery. Trends Neurosci 39:527–542. pmid:27381836
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Luke SG
    (2017) Evaluating significance in linear mixed-effects models in R. Behav Res 49:1494–1502. doi:10.3758/s13428-016-0809-y
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. McLaren DG,
    2. Ries ML,
    3. Xu G,
    4. Johnson SC
    (2012) A generalized form of context-dependent psychophysiological interactions (gPPI): a comparison to standard approaches. Neuroimage 61:1277–1286. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.068 pmid:22484411
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Miall RC,
    2. Wolpert DM
    (1996) Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Netw 9:1265–1279. doi:10.1016/s0893-6080(96)00035-4 pmid:12662535
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Oldfield RC
    (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113. pmid:5146491
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Pelli DG
    (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis 10:437–442. pmid:9176953
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Pereira M,
    2. Faivre N,
    3. Iturrate I,
    4. Wirthlin M,
    5. Serafini L,
    6. Martin S,
    7. Desvachez A,
    8. Blanke O,
    9. Van De Ville D,
    10. Millán JDR
    (2020) Disentangling the origins of confidence in speeded perceptual judgments through multimodal imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 117:8382–8390. pmid:32238562
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  51. ↵
    1. Piazza M,
    2. Izard V,
    3. Pinel P,
    4. Le Bihan D,
    5. Dehaene S
    (2004) Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus. Neuron 44:547–555. pmid:15504333
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Piazza M,
    2. Mechelli A,
    3. Price CJ,
    4. Butterworth B
    (2006) Exact and approximate judgements of visual and auditory numerosity: an fMRI study. Brain Res 1106:177–188. pmid:16828717
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Power JD,
    2. Barnes KA,
    3. Snyder AZ,
    4. Schlaggar BL,
    5. Petersen SE
    (2012) Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage 59:2142–2154. pmid:22019881
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Press C,
    2. Kok P,
    3. Yon D
    (2020) The perceptual prediction paradox. Trends Cogn Sci 24:13–24. pmid:31787500
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Reinert RM,
    2. Hartmann M,
    3. Huber S,
    4. Moeller K
    (2019) Unbounded number line estimation as a measure of numerical estimation. PLoS One 14:e0213102. pmid:30870436
    OpenUrlPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Rummell BP,
    2. Klee JL,
    3. Sigurdsson T
    (2016) Attenuation of responses to self-generated sounds in auditory cortical neurons. J Neurosci 36:12010–12026. pmid:27881785
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    1. Scott M,
    2. Yeung HH,
    3. Gick B,
    4. Werker JF
    (2013) Inner speech captures the perception of external speech. J Acoust Soc Am 133:EL286–EL292. pmid:23556693
    OpenUrlPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Serino A,
    2. Pozeg P,
    3. Bernasconi F,
    4. Solcà M,
    5. Hara M,
    6. Progin P,
    7. Stripeikyte G,
    8. Dhanis H,
    9. Salomon R,
    10. Bleuler H,
    11. Rognini G,
    12. Blanke O
    (2021) Thought consciousness and source monitoring depend on robotically controlled sensorimotor conflicts and illusory states. iScience 24:101955. pmid:33458614
    OpenUrlPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Shergill SS,
    2. White TP,
    3. Joyce DW,
    4. Bays PM,
    5. Wolpert DM,
    6. Frith CD
    (2013) Modulation of somatosensory processing by action. Neuroimage 70:356–362. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.043
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Slamecka NJ,
    2. Graf P
    (1978) The generation effect: delineation of a phenomenon. J Exp Psychol Hum Learn Mem 4:592–604. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.4.6.592
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  61. ↵
    1. Thomas ER,
    2. Yon D,
    3. Lange FP,
    4. de, Press C
    (2020) Action enhances predicted touch. bioRxiv 2020.03.26.007559.
  62. ↵
    1. Tian X,
    2. Poeppel D
    (2010) Mental imagery of speech and movement implicates the dynamics of internal forward models. Front Psychol 1:166. pmid:21897822
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Tian X,
    2. Zarate JM,
    3. Poeppel D
    (2016) Mental imagery of speech implicates two mechanisms of perceptual reactivation. Cortex 77:1–12. pmid:26889603
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Timm J,
    2. SanMiguel I,
    3. Keil J,
    4. Schröger E,
    5. Schönwiesner M
    (2014) Motor intention determines sensory attenuation of brain responses to self-initiated sounds. J Cogn Neurosci 26:1481–1489. pmid:24392902
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Tzourio-Mazoyer N,
    2. Landeau B,
    3. Papathanassiou D,
    4. Crivello F,
    5. Etard O,
    6. Delcroix N,
    7. Mazoyer B,
    8. Joliot M
    (2002) Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15:273–289. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0978
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Ullsperger M,
    2. Harsay HA,
    3. Wessel JR,
    4. Ridderinkhof KR
    (2010) Conscious perception of errors and its relation to the anterior insula. Brain Struct Funct 214:629–643. pmid:20512371
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Vaccaro AG,
    2. Fleming SM
    (2018) Thinking about thinking: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of metacognitive judgements. Brain Neurosci Adv 2:2398212818810591. pmid:30542659
    OpenUrlPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. van de Ven V,
    2. Esposito F,
    3. Christoffels IK
    (2009) Neural network of speech monitoring overlaps with overt speech production and comprehension networks: a sequential spatial and temporal ICA study. Neuroimage 47:1982–1991. pmid:19481159
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Whitford TJ
    (2019) Speaking-induced suppression of the auditory cortex in humans and its relevance to schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 4:791–804. pmid:31399393
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Whitford TJ,
    2. Jack BN,
    3. Pearson D,
    4. Griffiths O,
    5. Luque D,
    6. Harris AWF,
    7. Spencer KM,
    8. Le Pelley ME
    (2017) Neurophysiological evidence of efference copies to inner speech. Elife 6:e28197. pmid:29199947
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Wolpert DM,
    2. Flanagan JR
    (2001) Motor prediction. Curr Biol 11:R729–R732. pmid:11566114
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 41 (43)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 41, Issue 43
27 Oct 2021
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Increased Functional Connectivity of the Intraparietal Sulcus Underlies the Attenuation of Numerosity Estimations for Self-Generated Words
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Increased Functional Connectivity of the Intraparietal Sulcus Underlies the Attenuation of Numerosity Estimations for Self-Generated Words
Giedre Stripeikyte, Michael Pereira, Giulio Rognini, Jevita Potheegadoo, Olaf Blanke, Nathan Faivre
Journal of Neuroscience 27 October 2021, 41 (43) 8917-8927; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3164-20.2021

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Increased Functional Connectivity of the Intraparietal Sulcus Underlies the Attenuation of Numerosity Estimations for Self-Generated Words
Giedre Stripeikyte, Michael Pereira, Giulio Rognini, Jevita Potheegadoo, Olaf Blanke, Nathan Faivre
Journal of Neuroscience 27 October 2021, 41 (43) 8917-8927; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3164-20.2021
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • attenuation
  • cognition
  • connectivity
  • fMRI
  • intraparietal sulcus
  • numerosity

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Identification of the Acid-Sensitive Site Critical for Chloral Hydrate (CH) Activation of the Proton-Activated Chloride Channel
  • Disentangling Object Category Representations Driven by Dynamic and Static Visual Input
  • Irrelevant Threats Linger and Affect Behavior in High Anxiety
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Disentangling Object Category Representations Driven by Dynamic and Static Visual Input
  • Irrelevant Threats Linger and Affect Behavior in High Anxiety
  • Multisession Anodal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Enhances Adult Hippocampal Neurogenesis and Context Discrimination in Mice
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.