Abstract
The stop-signal task is a well-established assessment of response inhibition, and in humans, proficiency is linked to dorsal striatum D2 receptor availability. Parkinson's disease (PD) is characterized by changes to efficiency of response inhibition. Here, we studied 17 PD patients (6 female and 11 male) using the stop-signal paradigm in a single-blinded d-amphetamine (dAMPH) study. Participants completed [18F]fallypride positron emission topography (PET) imaging in both placebo and dAMPH conditions. A voxel-wise analysis of the relationship between binding potential (BPND) and stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) revealed that faster SSRT is associated with greater D2-like BPND in the amygdala and hippocampus (right cluster qFDR-corr = 0.026, left cluster qFDR-corr = 0.002). A region of interest (ROI) examination confirmed this association in both the amygdala (coefficient = −48.26, p = 0.005) and hippocampus (coefficient = −104.94, p = 0.007). As healthy dopaminergic systems in the dorsal striatum appear to regulate response inhibition, we interpret our findings in PD to indicate either nigrostriatal damage unmasking a mesolimbic contribution to response inhibition, or a compensatory adaptation from the limbic and mesial temporal dopamine systems. These novel results expand the conceptualization of action-control networks, whereby limbic and motor loops may be functionally connected.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT While Parkinson's disease (PD) is characteristically recognized for its motor symptoms, some patients develop impulsive and compulsive behaviors (ICBs), manifested as repetitive and excessive participation in reward-driven activities, including sex, gambling, shopping, eating, and hobbyism. Such cognitive alterations compel a consideration of response inhibition in PD. To investigate inhibitory control and assess the brain regions that may participate, we assessed PD patients using a single-blinded d-amphetamine (dAMPH) study, with [18F]fallypride positron emission topography (PET) imaging, and stop-signal task performance. We find a negative relationship between D2-like binding in the mesial temporal region and top-signal reaction time (SSRT), with greater BPND associated with a faster SSRT. These discoveries indicate a novel role for mesolimbic dopamine in response inhibition, and advocate for limbic regulation of action control in this clinical population.
Introduction
Parkinson's disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative disorder characterized by dopaminergic and monoaminergic neuronal loss. Although motor symptoms respond to dopamine therapy, non-motor symptoms are more variable and arise as greater sources of clinical burden to patients (Martinez-Martin et al., 2011). Impulsivity is one well-described clinical phenomenon in PD, whereby some patients develop impulsive and compulsive behaviors (ICBs; Weintraub et al., 2010), and others experience motor impulsiveness that contributes to falls (Ahlskog, 2010).
While generally understood as an inability to suppress an impetuous drive, a unitary definition must be eschewed, and impulsivity must instead be recognized as a multifaceted cognitive process (Bechara et al., 2000). Two common distinctions are cognitive impulsivity, which is grounded in reward-based decision-making, and motor impulsivity, which focuses on response inhibition (Bechara et al., 2000). Cognitive neuroscience paradigms that assess action control can be conceptually linked to interpretations of motor impulsivity. One such tool is the stop-signal task, which serves as the benchmark for measuring response inhibition. The paradigm involves participants making speeded choice reactions to “go” stimuli, and occasionally inhibiting their reactions on the occurrence of a “stop” signal (Logan and Cowan, 1984). The task assesses the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT), or the speed with which one cancels the preparation of an already initiated response.
Although the range of neuromodulatory systems associated with stop-signal task performance is not fully elucidated, dopamine has been considered a significant contributor to action control (Mink, 1996). Studies of clinical populations suffering from dopaminergic dysfunction have demonstrated slower inhibitory processes with longer SSRTs (Colzato et al., 2007; Enticott et al., 2008). Moreover, reduced spontaneous eyeblink rate, an index of dopaminergic function, is correlated with slower SSRT (Colzato et al., 2009). In rodents, the role of dopaminergic activity on SSRT has been recognized in the dorsomedial striatum, with D2-specific blockade increasing SSRT (Eagle et al., 2011). Similarly, correlations between D2 binding potential (BPND) and SSRT have been localized to the caudate and putamen in healthy humans (Ghahremani et al., 2012; Robertson et al., 2015).
Poor response inhibition often accompanies psychiatric disorders that manifest with impulsivity (Moeller et al., 2001). However, in PD patients with ICBs, action control is maintained, such that they demonstrate faster SSRTs than either healthy controls or PD patients without ICBs (Claassen et al., 2015). Self-reported measures disclose that ICB patients present with increased cognitive, but not motor, impulsivity on the Barret Impulsivity Scale-11 (BIS-11; Aumann et al., 2020). The preservation or even enhancement of inhibitory motor control in ICB patients suggests key distinctions in the pattern of dopamine alterations among PD patients. Such a possibility is consistent with current evidence for differences to mesocorticolimbic dopamine regulation in ICB patients, with strengthened network connectivity, reduced D2-like BPND in the ventral striatum and putamen, and low dopamine tone in the anterior cingulate cortex (Ray et al., 2012; Petersen et al., 2018; Stark et al., 2018a). Additionally, following administration of dopamine agonist medication, PD patients have shown increased regional cerebral blood flow in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, accompanied by improved inhibitory control (Trujillo et al., 2019). Motor inhibition in PD patients may involve mesolimbic networks because of the predominant damage of dopaminergic innervation in the striatum.
In this study, we tested the hypothesis that D2-mediated striatal and extrastriatal networks may function as a compensatory system for facilitating inhibitory control in PD through an examination of SSRT values and D2-like BPND. To measure baseline ability and sensitivity to modulation by dopamine release, PD patients, with and without ICBs, completed the stop-signal task following placebo and d-amphetamine (dAMPH) administration. To determine the association of different brain regions, participants also underwent two positron emission topography (PET) imaging scans with [18F]fallypride, a high affinity D2/3 receptor ligand, under placebo and dAMPH. This assessment was performed in a single-blinded fashion. Given the proficiency of SSRT performance in PD-ICB patients, we hypothesized that ventral striatal D2 BPND would be associated with faster SSRT and that dAMPH administration would improve performance.
Materials and Methods
Participants
All PD patients (n = 17, sex = 6 female/11 male) were recruited from the Movement Disorders Clinic at Vanderbilt University Medical Center. Patients met United Kingdom Brain Bank criteria for idiopathic PD (Hughes et al., 1992) and were diagnosed by a movement disorder neurologist (D.O.C.). All patients were screened to confirm that they met inclusion criteria, and further did not have an implanted deep brain stimulator, an unstable medical condition, a comorbid neurologic disorder, dementia, a history of major psychiatric illness, or a history of substance abuse. Additionally, no patients had taken psychostimulants over the previous year, and were not currently using cocaine, nicotine or excessive alcohol. The majority of participants were taking DA agonist medications along with concomitant levodopa therapy. Dopamine medications were converted to levodopa equivalent dose (LEDD; Tomlinson et al., 2010). The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and all subjects provided written, informed consent before participating in the study in compliance with the standards of ethical conduct in human investigation regulated by the local Institutional Review Board.
Along with a physical examination, neurologic examination, EKG, urinalysis, and metabolic panel, a set of questionnaires was also administered during study screening. Patients completed part II of the Movement Disorders Society-United Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS), which serves as a self-evaluation of motor activities of daily living, and part III (in the Off-medication condition), which serves as a clinician-scored motor examination (Goetz et al., 2008; Weintraub et al., 2012). Symptoms of depression were assessed using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale Revised (CESD-R). Finally, the Questionnaire for Impulsive-Compulsive Disorders in Parkinson's Disease-Rating Scale (QUIP-RS) was conducted to screen for ICBs. To define the clinical presence of ICBs, formal interviews with patients and caregivers were performed, with an ICB designation based on previously described DSM-5 criteria (Voon et al., 2006). Table 1 presents demographic and clinical information.
Demographic and clinical evaluation of the PD participants
Participants attended two testing visits while off dopaminergic therapies. The sessions were conducted within approximately one week of each other. Before each session, participants completed a 36-h withdrawal from dopamine agonist therapy and a 16-h withdrawal from levodopa therapy, and resumed medication following the completion of the imaging study. This period was considered sufficient to eliminate effects from dopaminergic therapies, while minimizing potential patient discomfort. During the first session, participants were administered oral placebo, and in the second, they were administered a 0.43 mg/kg oral dose of dAMPH. The order of these visits was blinded to the participant only (i.e., single blind).
Stop-signal task
Approximately 30 min after medication (placebo or dAMPH) administration, but before [18F]fallypride injection, patients performed a manual version of the stop-signal task (Logan and Cowan, 1984). Participants, wearing corrective lenses if prescribed, were seated at a comfortable distance from a laptop screen and provided with directions before proceeding. On Go trials, which comprised 75% of the trials, patients were presented with a fixation point followed by a left-pointing or right-pointing gray arrow in the center of the screen. They were instructed to make a left-hand button press with external response grips when they observed a left-pointing arrow and to make a right-hand button press when they observed a right-pointing arrow. Responses were to be made as quickly and accurately as possible. The time it took for a participant to press the response button following the appearance of the target was recorded as go reaction time (GoRT). Once a choice had been executed or 1200 ms had passed, the gray arrow disappeared. Next, an interstimulus interval between 1250 and 1750 ms occurred while the fixation point remained visible.
On Stop trials, which comprised 25% of the trials, the gray arrow changed to a purple arrow soon after appearing on the screen. Patients were instructed to attempt to restrain themselves from pressing a button after perceiving the purple arrow. The stop-signal delay, or the time between the presentation of the gray arrow and its color change was systematically regulated with a staircase design that adjusts according to the participant's performance (Levitt, 1971). Beginning at 200 ms, the delay was thereafter tuned incrementally by 50 ms, increasing if stopping was successful and decreasing if stopping was unsuccessful. In situations when stopping was unsuccessful and the participant responded, the time it took for a participant to press the button following the appearance of the target was noted as the signal-respond reaction time. Each administration of the stop-signal task involved 48 practice trials, followed by two blocks of 104 experimental trials.
Estimating SSRT
A parametric model of the stop-signal task was applied, with model parameters calculated using the Bayesian Estimation of ex-Gaussian Stop-Signal Reaction Time Distribution (BEESTS) software (Matzke et al., 2013). Reaction times that were less than or greater than three standard deviations away from the mean were removed from the analysis during preprocessing. Similar to the traditional mean and integration approaches for analyzing stop-signal task data, this model assumes that the distribution of signal-respond reaction times, or reaction times on failed stop trials, can be considered as a censored GoRT distribution. BEESTS fits the data according to an ex-Gaussian distribution, outputting µ as the mean of the Gaussian component, σ as the standard deviation of the Gaussian component, and τ as the mean and standard deviation of the exponential component for both GoRT and SSRT distributions (Matzke et al., 2013). Mean GoRT and SSRT can then be calculated as the sum of µ and τ. This method also provides an estimation of the probability of trigger failures, which represents an inability to initiate the stop process (Matzke et al., 2017). The model was run for 20,000 samples with a thinning of 5. GoRT and SSRT served as the primary assessments, representing response initiation and inhibitory control, respectively.
PET imaging protocol
[18F]fallypride was synthesized in the radiochemistry core as outlined in a previously described method (Stark et al., 2018b). Data were collected on a Philips Vereos PET/CT scanner with a three-dimensional acquisition and transmission attenuation correction. Approximately 2 h after medication (placebo or dAMPH) administration, serial PET scans were acquired simultaneously with a 5.0 mCi bolus injection of [18F]fallypride over a 30-s period. Scan time ran to ∼3.5 h after injection with two breaks of 15 min between emission scans (Stark et al., 2018b).
Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) protocol
Anatomical T1-weighted MR images were acquired to allow region definition for region of interest (ROI) analysis and spatial normalization for voxel-wise analyses. All MRI scans were completed with a 3.0T Philips MRI scanner with a body coil transmission and 32-channel SENSE array reception. Structural images were acquired using a T1-weighted high-resolution anatomic scan (MPRAGE; spatial resolution 1 × 1 × 1 mm3; TR/TE = 8.9/4.6 ms).
PET image processing
[18F]fallypride non-displaceable binding potential (BPND) was quantified following motion-correction with Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM12; Wellcome Trust Center for Neuroimaging, London, United Kingdom; https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/). Parametric BPND images were estimated using the simplified reference tissue model in PMOD software (PMOD Technologies; Stark et al., 2018b). The cerebellum was used as a reference region because of its limited D2/3 receptor expression (Camps et al., 1989). Parametric BPND images were co-registered to the participant's T1-weighted MR image using FSL's FLIRT with 6 degrees of freedom and a mutual information cost function (FSL v6.0; FMRIB). For voxel-level analyses, parametric BPND images were non-linearly registered to standard Montreal Neurologic Institute (MNI152) space using FSL's FNIRT.
ROI analyses
A priori bilateral subcortical ROIs were manually defined on each participant's T1 MRI image according to established anatomic definitions (Stark et al., 2018b). These ROIs included the caudate (head), putamen, globus pallidus, ventral striatum, substantia nigra, amygdala, and cerebellum. Additionally, the hypothalamus and ventromedial orbitofrontal cortex were manually defined according to a previously described method and proposed anatomic landmarks, respectively (Mackey and Petrides, 2009; Klomp et al., 2012). The hippocampus and thalamus were segmented with FSL FIRST, and the anterior cingulate cortex and insula with FreeSurfer (v6.0; Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Charlestown, MA; https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki).
Experimental design and statistical analyses
First, GoRT and SSRT data from the placebo and dAMPH sessions of PD patients were compared using the Wilcoxon signed-rank test. A Wilcoxon rank-sum test was used to compare the GoRT and SSRT placebo data between ICB and non-ICB patients.
To investigate the relationship between [18F]fallypride BPND and stop-signal task performance, a voxel-wise analysis was completed using SPM12. The regression of D2/3 BPND on SSRT across cortical and subcortical areas included age and sex as covariates (Mukherjee et al., 2002; Pohjalainen et al., 1998). Significance criteria consisted of an uncorrected p < 0.005 and cluster-level false discovery rate (FDR) controlled at 0.05 to correct for multiple comparisons. A general linear regression model (GLM) was applied to further explore the correlation between [18F]fallypride BPND and SSRT in striatal and extrastriatal ROIs. For this model, SSRT served as the dependent variable, mean ROI BPND as the independent variable, and age and sex as covariates. An FDR of 0.05 was used to correct for multiple comparisons (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995).
An additional GLM was used to test the hypothesis that D2/3 receptor expression in the significant regions from the first GLM may also play a role in disparate effects of dAMPH on stop-signal task performance. For this analysis, change in SSRT between placebo and dAMPH sessions served as the dependent variable, mean ROI BPND as the independent variable, and age and sex as covariates. Again, an FDR of 0.05 was used to correct for multiple comparisons. All statistical analyses were performed using R (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, 2016).
Results
Inhibitory control performance
In order to evaluate SSRT, it is necessary to ensure that (1) the probability of failing to inhibit a response is greater at longer stop-signal delays, and (2) mean signal-respond reaction time is faster than mean GoRT. These criteria were met, validating the independent race model and the reliability of the estimates of SSRT across participants. Table 2 summarizes the behavioral results estimated by BEESTS.
Stop-signal task performance following placebo administration
SSRT following dAMPH administration data from four participants were excluded from analyses because of technological issues during task completion. Across PD patients (n = 13), SSRT did not significantly vary between placebo administration and dAMPH administration (placebo: 274.09 ± 36.48 ms; dAMPH: 274.47 ± 28.57 ms; z = −0.135, p = 0.893). GoRT also did not vary between drug administration conditions (placebo: 613.70 ± 113.29 ms; dAMPH: 568.37 ± 98.58 ms; z = 0.955, p = 0.340). Although patients with ICBs (n = 8) displayed faster SSRTs than those without (n = 9), the difference is not statistically significant (ICB: 260.84 ± 34.05 ms; non-ICB: 285.39 ± 33.62 ms; z = −1.381, p = 0.167). Finally, placebo GoRT also did not significantly differ between patients with ICBs and those without (ICB: 604.86 ± 134.90 ms; non-ICB: 594.91 ± 80.72 ms; z = −0.140, p = 0.888). Group level comparisons for placebo SSRT are presented in Figure 1.
Group results for the stop-signal task variables. A, Placebo SSRT did not significantly differ between patients with ICBs and patients without ICBs. B, Placebo GoRT did not significantly differ between patients with ICBs and patients without ICBs.
Voxel-wise analysis of relationship between baseline [18F]fallypride BPND and placebo SSRT
Applying the voxel-wise method with age and sex as covariates resulted in negative correlations between baseline [18F]fallypride BPND and placebo SSRT in striatal and extrastriatal regions. These clusters localized to the left orbitofrontal cortex and bilaterally to the amygdala and hippocampus, surviving cluster-level FDR correction at 0.05. A cluster localized to the left caudate also emerged but was not statistically significant at cluster-level FDR correction at 0.05. Figure 2 shows a map of significant clusters overlaid on coronal and axial slices of a standard-space brain, along with coordinate and statistical information for the significant clusters. No significant clusters emerged when testing for an association between baseline [18F]fallypride BPND and GoRT.
Voxel-wise regression of baseline [18F]fallypride BPND on placebo SSRT. A, Map of significant clusters where SSRT was negatively correlated with [18F]fallypride BPND in PD patients, overlaid on coronal and axial slices of an MNI template brain. All survived cluster-level FDR correction at p < 0.05. Areas include the amygdala, hippocampus, and left orbitofrontal cortex. Brains are displayed according to radiologic convention, with the left side of the brain shown on the right side of the image. B, Regions showing significance for a negative correlation between [18F]fallypride BPND and SSRT.
ROI-based analysis of relationship between baseline [18F]fallypride BPND and placebo SSRT
Significant negative correlations between baseline [18F]fallypride BPND and placebo SSRT were found in ROIs, including the amygdala (coefficient = −48.26, p = 0.005) and hippocampus (coefficient = −104.94, p = 0.007). Both the amygdala and hippocampus survived FDR correction for multiple comparisons at 0.05 (pcorrected = 0.039, pcorrected = 0.039). Figure 3 displays the significant relationships between [18F]fallypride BPND and SSRT. Table 3 presents mean regional [18F]fallypride BPND values for all ROIs, along with the coefficient and p values from the GLM analysis.
[18F]Fallypride BPND (baseline) effects on SSRT
Scatterplots with lines of best fit displaying the relationship between placebo SSRT and baseline [18F]fallypride BPND. A GLM was applied with SSRT as the dependent variable and mean ROI BPND as the independent variable. Age and sex served as covariates. A significant negative correlation between BPND and SSRT was observed for the (A) amygdala (p = 0.005) and (B) hippocampus (p = 0.007), indicating a negative relationship between D2/3 expression and stopping control in these limbic areas.
ROI-based analysis of relationship between baseline [18F]fallypride BPND and SSRT change in response to dAMPH
The ROIs of the hippocampus and amygdala were included in this GLM. While the p values (p = 0.075) were not significant, the data support a negative correlation between baseline hippocampus D2-like receptor binding, and dAMPH effects on SSRT (95% CI = [−205.987, −2.867]). Figure 4 displays this relationship between [18F]fallypride BPND in the hippocampus and SSRT change.
Scatterplot with line of best fit displaying the relationship between SSRT change (SSRT placebo – SSRT dAMPH) and baseline [18F]fallypride BPND. A GLM was applied with SSRT change as the dependent variable and mean ROI BPND as the independent variable. Age and sex served as covariates. A negative correlation between BPND and SSRT change was observed for the hippocampus (p = 0.045), but it did not survive FDR correction for multiple comparisons at 0.05.
Discussion
This study investigated the neural correlates of inhibitory control in persons with PD. As in our past work (Claassen et al., 2015), analysis of SSRT makes evident that ICB patients do not possess a deficit in stopping speed, and indeed appeared slightly faster than patients without ICBs (although this difference was not significant in this small sample). It seems increasingly apparent that motor control is not a primary contributor to ICBs in PD, and other impulsive-compulsive processes are likely more central to these deficits. An analysis of task performance in relation to dopamine receptor availability across all participants established a negative correlation in the amygdala and hippocampus, such that greater D2-like BPND is associated with better stopping control.
Novel relationships between BPND in the hippocampus, amygdala, and SSRT
While previous studies have identified a negative correlation between SSRT and D2-like BPND in the dorsal, but not ventral, striatum in healthy subjects (Ghahremani et al., 2012; Robertson et al., 2015), this study is the first to specifically examine this association in persons with PD. Interestingly, with this clinical population, a different relationship emerges, as the most robust associations emerged bilaterally in the amygdala and hippocampus of the voxel-wise and ROI-based analyses.
The manifestation of a role for limbic areas in inhibitory control in PD patients may be explained by preservation of ventral striatal and mesolimbic dopamine networks in PD. The dorsal striatum, with input from the substantia nigra, suffers a substantial loss of dopaminergic innervation early in the disease and throughout its progression (Damier et al., 1999; Björklund and Dunnett, 2007). On the other hand, while the mesolimbic system, with input from the ventral tegmental area, degenerates during the disease course, it declines to a lesser degree (MacDonald and Monchi, 2011; Alberico et al., 2015). Patients with higher D2-like BPND in the hippocampus and amygdala may have better preserved ventral tegmental area to mesial temporal dopamine networks, accounting for their superior inhibitory control. We consider that in healthy dopaminergic circuits, D2-type dopamine receptors in the dorsal striatum serve as critical modulators of response inhibition. It is possible that nigrostriatal degeneration unmasks relationships between motor action control and mesolimbic dopamine. Alternatively, these relationships may indicate a compensatory mechanism, with PD patients developing a greater reliance on limbic dopamine networks to regulate action control. The results align with previous findings of comparatively conserved dopaminergic mesolimbic networks and faster SSRTs in patients with ICBs (Claassen et al., 2015; Stark et al., 2018a).
Dopamine and the limbic cortex in PD
These results highlight a novel relationship between hippocampal and limbic regulation of action control, emphasizing previous anatomic findings that the limbic and motor loops are functionally connected. Notably, a recent study in rodents proposed that these two circuits are linked in a single direction, such that the limbic ventral striatum regulates primary motor cortex function (Aoki et al., 2019). Rather than adopting the standard notion that limbic and sensorimotor information are processed in parallel, Aoki and colleagues introduce an open cortico-basal ganglia loop. According to this interpretation, the ventral striatum, which receives inputs from the ventral tegmental area, amygdala, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and orbitofrontal cortex, is able to inhibit substantia nigra neurons projecting to the motor thalamus (Choi et al., 2017; Aoki et al., 2019). Our data appear to illustrate an important translational example of limbic regulation of motor control through the amygdala and hippocampus in humans.
Amphetamine effects on action control
The results of this study reflect the notion that persons with PD experience differential effects to dAMPH with respect to their performance on the stop-signal task. As seen in Figure 4, some demonstrate improvement (positive SSRT change) and reduced SSRT, while others do not.
Previous work has likewise shown conflicting effects of dAMPH on SSRT performance. Various rodent studies have illustrated dose-dependent and baseline-dependent SSRT changes with amphetamine (Feola et al., 2000; Eagle and Robbins, 2003; Eagle et al., 2007). Furthermore, studies with healthy volunteers have seen similar conditional results (de Wit et al., 2000; Dolder et al., 2018). Doses of 5, 10, and 20 mg of dAMPH, used by Weafer and de Wit, significantly reduced SSRT, implying that low to moderate doses can modulate action control (Weafer and de Wit, 2013). A consistent theme is that dAMPH appears to improve performance in participants with longer baseline SSRTs (Feola et al., 2000; Weafer and de Wit, 2013). Similar results are observed with this clinical population, implying that PD patients may reach an optimal level of performance, after which a stimulant is ineffectual.
When examining the effect of dAMPH on stop-signal task performance, greater hippocampal D2-like BPND is associated with more negative SSRT change (worse performance with dAMPH). It is likely that the positive influence of D2-like BPND on action control, as previously discussed, acts on hippocampal networks. Previous studies investigating the effect of dopamine on working memory have suggested an inverted-U shaped role for exogenous dopamine (Cools and D'Esposito, 2011). This model indicates that at optimal baseline working memory, additional dopamine would only worsen performance. Similarly, we see that patients with greater D2-like BPND in the hippocampus and amygdala have already attained their best SSRTs and cannot see further improvement with dAMPH. Conversely, dAMPH appears to be rescuing poor stop-signal task performance and improving SSRTs. The clinical implication of such dopaminergic actions suggests that as dAMPH alters monoamine levels, the stimulant is more likely to benefit patients with slow baseline SSRTs.
Our discovery of relationships between D2-like BPND and placebo SSRT, along with SSRT change, emphasizes a role of D2-like receptors in the hippocampus and amygdala on action-control. Previous studies have suggested that dopaminergic communication in the hippocampus bolsters stimulus generalization, as D2 receptor-blockade has been shown to modulate similarity processing in humans (Kahnt and Tobler, 2016). While the stop-signal paradigm is not implemented as a reward task, participants may perceive and, accordingly, respond to the color-changing arrows with varying degrees of discrimination. Moreover, there is increasing support for the idea that the hippocampus is involved in visual perception, in addition to its established implication in memory (Lee et al., 2012). Although the response inhibition task has not been considered in terms of spatial perception, the configuration of directional stimuli is undoubtedly spatial in nature, and thus may in fact prompt hippocampal involvement. Research into the function of D2 receptors in the amygdala is consistent with our data, as rodent experiments have concluded that D2 expression in the central nucleus of the amygdala regulates impulsive behavior (Kim et al., 2018). As our results highlight both the hippocampus and amygdala, it is prudent to try to disentangle their respective roles in accordance with the stop-signal task. Over the course of the paradigm, it is likely that participants may begin to predict the incidence of a stop-signal, and may even consider “go” as a rewarding or positive stimulus and “stop” as a punishing or negative stimulus. In this case, the amygdala may function to learn and process reward for impulsivity, while the hippocampus may serve to discriminate and generalize between stimuli.
In conclusion, although this study benefitted from the ability to gather behavioral information on both placebo and dAMPH, it is possible that the stop-signal task was conducted too soon after dAMPH administration. By assessing inhibition early in the context of dAMPH exposure, we may not have assessed the true effect of increased dopamine on SSRT. Future work should try to mitigate this by implementing a delay of ∼2 h between drug administration and behavioral testing (Fillmore et al., 2005).
Furthermore, the apparent effects of dAMPH on inhibitory control may not be dopaminergic in nature. While dAMPH predominantly acts by communicating with dopamine neurons, it nevertheless is capable of interacting with other neurotransmitters, including serotonin, acetylcholine, and norepinephrine (Moore, 1977). In light of this, the inference that dopamine is the main contributor to these results must be made with a caveat that other monoamines acting on hippocampal and amygdala regions may modulate action control in humans. Finally, we acknowledge the limitations of a small sample size, given the arduous PET and dAMPH methods, and are optimistic that future studies with larger patient bases will further substantiate and build on this work.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging Grant 1K24AG064114 (to D.O.C.) and the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grant 5R01NS097783 (to D.O.C.).
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Daniel O. Claassen at daniel.claassen{at}vumc.org