Abstract
Human epidemiological studies implicate exposure to infection during gestation in the etiology of neurodevelopmental disorders. Animal models of maternal immune activation (MIA) have identified the maternal immune response as the critical link between maternal infection and aberrant offspring brain and behavior development. Here we evaluate neurodevelopment of male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) born to MIA-treated dams (n = 14) injected with a modified form of the viral mimic polyinosinic:polycytidylic acid at the end of the first trimester. Control dams received saline injections at the same gestational time points (n = 10) or were untreated (n = 4). MIA-treated dams exhibited a strong immune response as indexed by transient increases in sickness behavior, temperature, and inflammatory cytokines. Although offspring born to control or MIA-treated dams did not differ on measures of physical growth and early developmental milestones, the MIA-treated animals exhibited subtle changes in cognitive development and deviated from species-typical brain growth trajectories. Longitudinal MRI revealed significant gray matter volume reductions in the prefrontal and frontal cortices of MIA-treated offspring at 6 months that persisted through the final time point at 45 months along with smaller frontal white matter volumes in MIA-treated animals at 36 and 45 months. These findings provide the first evidence of early postnatal changes in brain development in MIA-exposed nonhuman primates and establish a translationally relevant model system to explore the neurodevelopmental trajectory of risk associated with prenatal immune challenge from birth through late adolescence.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Women exposed to infection during pregnancy have an increased risk of giving birth to a child who will later be diagnosed with a neurodevelopmental disorder. Preclinical maternal immune activation (MIA) models have demonstrated that the effects of maternal infection on fetal brain development are mediated by maternal immune response. Since the majority of MIA models are conducted in rodents, the nonhuman primate provides a unique system to evaluate the MIA hypothesis in a species closely related to humans. Here we report the first longitudinal study conducted in a nonhuman primate MIA model. MIA-exposed offspring demonstrate subtle changes in cognitive development paired with marked reductions in frontal gray and white matter, further supporting the association between prenatal immune challenge and alterations in offspring neurodevelopment.
Introduction
The current COVID-19 pandemic highlights an urgent need to understand the association between maternal infection during pregnancy and the subsequent increased risk of offspring neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs). Although the long-term effects of prenatal SARS-CoV-2 exposure are unknown, converging epidemiological data suggest that, for a subset of women, infections during pregnancy are associated with an increased risk of NDDs in their offspring, including both schizophrenia (SZ) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (Estes and McAllister, 2016; Kępińska et al., 2020). The diversity of viral and bacterial pathogens associated with NDDs suggests that maternal immune response is the critical link between maternal infection and altered fetal neurodevelopment (Knuesel et al., 2014). Moreover, the presence of inflammatory biomarkers in gestational biospecimens lends further support to the association between maternal immune activation (MIA) and risk of offspring NDDs (Brown and Meyer, 2018). Even in the absence of an NDD diagnosis, emerging evidence from human studies links variation in maternal cytokine levels during pregnancy with various offspring neurobehavioral outcomes, including alterations in brain growth, functional connectivity, and behavioral and cognitive development (Schepanski et al., 2018). Collectively, these studies suggest that changes in maternal cytokines during pregnancy can have long-lasting consequences, ranging from subtle differences in brain and behavioral development to severe NDDs.
The preclinical MIA model has emerged as a powerful translational tool that allows investigators to manipulate maternal cytokine levels during gestation and systematically evaluate offspring neurodevelopmental consequences in a controlled environment. MIA models use immune activating agents, such as the viral mimic polyinosinic:polycytidylic acid (Poly IC), to elicit an immune response during gestation. Across species, offspring of MIA-treated dams exhibit alterations in brain and behavioral development relevant to human neurodevelopmental and neuropsychiatric disease, supporting the initial interpretation of the MIA model as an animal model of ASD or SZ (Meyer and Feldon, 2010; Careaga et al., 2017). As preclinical research evolves toward a hypothesis-based approach (Gordon, 2019), cross-species MIA model comparisons in mice, rats, and nonhuman primates (NHPs) provide new opportunities to maximize the translational utility of this promising animal model and to explore neurobiological mechanisms underlying human NDDs (Kentner et al., 2019). Similarities in placental structure and physiology, gestational timelines, brain development, neuroanatomical organization, and behavioral complexity between humans and NHPs (Bauman and Schumann, 2018; Testard et al., 2021) provide a unique opportunity to evaluate the foundational knowledge of the rodent MIA model in a species more closely related to humans.
Our research team developed the first Poly IC-based NHP model to explore the neurodevelopmental trajectory of risk associated with prenatal immune challenge in the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta). In our previous study, pregnant rhesus monkeys injected with a modified form of Poly IC at the end of the first or second trimester exhibited a transient but potent immune response and produced offspring that deviated from species-typical behavioral development (Bauman et al., 2014; Machado et al., 2015; Rose et al., 2017). Although in vivo imaging and postmortem brain tissue studies were limited in these initial NHP MIA cohorts, the emergence of atypical behaviors as the animals matured suggested that this model system may provide an opportunity to explore circuitry relevant to NDDs that may be vulnerable to prenatal immune challenge. Indeed, these small cohorts of MIA-treated NHPs demonstrated increased striatal dopamine in late adolescence as indexed by PET (Bauman et al., 2019), aberrant dendritic morphology in the DLPFC (Weir et al., 2015), and region-specific alterations in gene expression (Page et al., 2021). We have generated a larger cohort of late first trimester MIA-treated male offspring to perform a comprehensive evaluation of brain and behavioral development from birth to late adolescence to characterize the emergence of brain and behavioral alterations in MIA-exposed NHPs. Here we present our initial findings of longitudinal structural MRI and cognitive performance data from this unique NHP MIA model.
Materials and Methods
All experimental procedures were developed in collaboration with the veterinary, animal husbandry, and environmental enrichment staff at the California National Primate Research Center (CNPRC) and approved by the University of California, Davis Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. All attempts were made (in terms of social housing, enriched diet, use of positive reinforcement strategies, and minimizing the duration of daily training/testing sessions) to promote normal social development and psychological well-being of the animals that participated in this research. Gestational timing, choice of species, source of immune activating agent, and subsequent magnitude of the MIA determine the impact on offspring neurodevelopment in preclinical MIA models. In accordance to recent guideline recommendations for improving the reporting of MIA model methods, we have completed the reporting table from Kentner et al. (2019) and will be provided upon request.
Animal selection
Pregnant dams were selected from the indoor, time-mated breeding colony based on age, weight, parity, and number of prior live births (Table 1). Candidate dams between 5 and 12 years old carrying a male fetus were assigned to MIA (n = 14) or control/saline (n = 10). Because of limited availability of male fetuses, untreated pregnant females confirmed to be carrying male fetuses were added to the control group (n = 4). One offspring from the MIA group was killed at 6 months of age because of an unrelated health condition and is not included in behavior or neuroimaging datasets after 6 months of age. A second animal from the MIA-treated group was killed at 42 months because of an unrelated health condition and is not included in the final neuroimaging time point.
Summary of dam characteristicsa
MIA and validation
MIA induction protocols are based on our previous dosing and gestational timing experiments previously described (Bauman et al., 2014, 2019; Machado et al., 2015; Weir et al., 2015). Synthetic double-stranded RNA (Poly IC stabilized with poly-L-lysine [Poly ICLC]; Oncovir; 0.25 mg/kg i.v.) or sterile saline (equivalent volume to Poly ICLC) was injected at 07:30 h in the cephalic vein in awake animals on gestational day (GD) 43, 44, and 46 (Table 2). Health and behavior observations were conducted 3 times before treatment, 6 h after each of the three injections, and 3 times after treatment. The checklist captured the presence or absence of any clinical or behavioral symptoms resulting from the infusions, including change in appetite, watery eyes or nasal discharge, liquid stool, lethargy or labored movements, and body temperature. Before infusions, programmable temperature microchips (Bio Medic Temperature Systems) were implanted subcutaneously under sedation near the left and right clavicle, and a temperature wand then scanned the microchip and displayed body temperature. Temperatures were recorded just before Poly ICLC or saline injection, and 30 min, 6 h, and 8 h after injection. Pretreatment and post-treatment baseline temperatures were taken during health and behavior observations. Blood was collected from the dams on approximately GD 40 while sedated for ultrasound (pretreatment), from awake animals on GD 44 and 46, 6 h after Poly ICLC infusion, and on GD 51 or 52 while sedated for a recheck ultrasound (posttreatment) for cytokine analysis. Blood samples were centrifuged and the serum was removed, aliquoted into 200 μl samples, and frozen at −80°C until analysis. A longitudinal analysis on the maternal IL-6 response to Poly ICLC exposure was measured in serum using an NHP multiplexing bead immunoassay (MilliporeSigma) that was analyzed using the flow-based Luminex 100 suspension array system (Bio-Plex 200; Bio-Rad Laboratories). Hair samples were collected from the dams at GD 150 to evaluate potential group differences in chronic stress during pregnancy.
Maternal response to poly ICLC
Hair cortisol analysis
Hair samples were collected from the dams at GD 150 when they were sedated for an ultrasound by shaving the nape of the neck. Samples were then placed in tubes and stored in a −80°C freezer. Samples were not collected from 3 of the untreated controls, resulting in Control (n = 11) and MIA (n = 14). Cortisol concentrations were analyzed using established protocols (Vandeleest et al., 2019) by extracting cortisol from hair using methanol and then measured using a salivary cortisol kit (Salimetrics). Intra-assay coefficient of variability was 1.6%.
Rearing conditions and husbandry
Infants were raised in individual cages with their mothers where they had visual access to other mother-infant pairs at all times. For 3 h each day, one familiar adult male and four familiar mother-infant pairs were allowed to freely interact in a large cage (3 m [length] × 1.8 m [width] × 2 m [height]) to provide enrichment and facilitate species-typical social development. The rearing groups consisted of two MIA-treated mother-infant dyads and two control mother-infant dyads. Dominance hierarchies naturally formed between the dams, and group stability was monitored throughout by trained observers. The infants were weaned from their mothers at 6 months of age and were permanently paired with a familiar peer from their rearing group. Weanlings continued the same socialization routine through ∼18 months of age. They were transferred to the large enclosures for 3 h each day with the same three weanlings from their rearing group, the familiar adult male, and an adult female who was not one of the dams involved in the study. Animal rooms were maintained at 18°C-29°C and on a 12/12 light/dark cycle (lights on at 06:00). Subjects were fed twice daily (Lab Diet #5047, PMI Nutrition International), and provided with forage scratch daily and fresh produce biweekly; they had access to water ad libitum along with a variety of enrichment devices.
Offspring physical growth, neurodevelopmental milestones, and early behavioral development
Measures of body growth (weight, crown-rump length, and head circumference) were collected at 1 and 3 months of age as well as at the neuroimaging time points (6, 12, 24, 36, and 45 months of age). The NHP offspring described in this paper underwent comprehensive assessments of social development that will be the focus of future publications. Here data are presented on a neurobehavioral neonatal assessment conducted at 1 week of age, home cage observations of the mother-infant dyad (0-6 months), and home cage observations with their age/sex/treatment-matched cage mate (6-18 months) (Table 3). All behavioral observations were conducted by trained observers demonstrating an interobserver reliability > 85% (agreements/[agreements + disagreements] × 100). Each infant received a dye mark, allowing the observers to record behaviors while remaining blind to their experimental condition. Detailed methods are provided in previous publications (Bauman et al., 2004a,b, 2013, 2014).
Home cage interaction behavioral ethogram
Neonatal assessment
The development of reflexes and basic neuromotor functioning was assessed at 7 (±1) days of age. The dam was lightly sedated with ketamine to remove the infant, and the infant was transferred to a testing room. A 5 point scale of maturity was used to rate the infant on measures of visual orientation and the following: reflexes including rooting, righting, placing, and Moro; motor maturity including head posture, coordination of movements, and prone progression; and state control (agitation and consolability). For each assessment, infants were given a score as follows: 2, present/fully developed; 1.5, mostly present/developed; 1, partially present or partially developed; 0.5, slightly developed or present; or 0, absent.
Mother-infant home cage observation (0-6 months)
Each mother-infant dyad was observed 5 d per week in the home cage by a trained observer who was familiar to the animals yet blinded to the condition of the animals. Interactions were quantified using a checklist of behaviors to describe the type and frequency of interactions between the mother and infant. Behaviors included nursing, grooming, contact, maternal behaviors (restrain, retrieve, rejection, aggression), facial expressions, environmental exploration, and stereotypy. Observations were conducted between 08:00 and 15:00 and continued until weaning at ∼6 months of age. The 1 min observation was broken into six 10 s bins, and one-zero sampling was used to record behaviors. In one-zero sampling, every behavior that is present within each bin receives a score of 1 regardless of the number of times it occurs within the bin. Behaviors that are absent within each bin receive a score of 0.
Infant-infant home cage observations (6-18 months)
Each infant-infant dyad was observed 3 d/wk for 52 weeks in the home cage by a trained observer who was familiar to the animals yet blinded to the condition of the animals. Interactions were quantified using a checklist of behaviors to describe the type and frequency of interactions between familiar peers. Behaviors included sleep, nonsocial activity, proximity, contact, play, environmental exploration, and stereotypy. Observations were conducted between 08:00 and 15:00 beginning 1 week after the dyads were formed at weaning at ∼6 months of age. The 1 min observation was scored in six 10 s bins as described above.
Cognitive assessments
Reversal learning (RL)
RL paradigms are used across a range of species, including humans, and entail assessing cognitive flexibility in animals when reward outcomes for stimuli are reversed (Izquierdo and Jentsch, 2012). Training began at 19.5 months to displace a single gray cube placed in the left, right, or center position on the Wisconsin General Testing Apparatus (WGTA) within a 30 s timeframe to reveal a reward underneath. Monkeys were transported to the testing room in a modified transport box that when secured enabled them to reach out through a Plexiglas panel and fully participate with the WGTA. Each daily training session consisted of 20 trials, and training continued until a criterion of 18 of 20 retrievals in a session. Weekly maintenance training sessions were conducted until testing began at 21 months of age. For the RL assessment, monkeys were tested in 20 daily sessions using two identical cubes differing only in color (black and white). The cubes were placed to the right and left of center on the WGTA test board. The black cube was initially designated correct, and when displaced, revealed a food reward in the well underneath. The well covered by the white cube did not contain a food reward. Right/left placement of the correct cube followed a random order for the 20 trials per session. Daily testing continued until the monkeys performed 18 of 20 trials correct in a session. Once this criterion was reached, the reward contingencies were reversed in the next testing session (the white cube was correct and the black cube was incorrect). When the 18 of 20 criterion was reached, the contingencies were again reversed. The number of reversal events monkeys were able to achieve within 20 sessions were measured. Response latency on each trial was also recorded as well as a rating of the monkey's temperament during the session. For temperament, the experimenter who tested the monkey rated its temperament shortly after the completion of the testing session. Monkeys were rated on a 0-3 scale on object orientation, goal directivity, irritability, activity, inhibition, impulsivity, stereotypies, attention to the task, and its behavior during omission errors.
Automated cognitive testing
Automated cognitive testing via computerized touchscreen devices allowed more complex assessments during the older juvenile stage. The remaining cognitive tests were conducted with the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Batteries software (CANTAB, Cambridge Cognition) on a modified desktop computer, touchscreen, and automated reward dispenser using protocols for rhesus monkeys (Weed et al., 1999; Golub et al., 2005, 2014, 2017). Sugar pellets (Bio-Serv) were used for rewards. Animals were tested in their home cage and temporarily separated in place from their partner. Testing occurred between 07:30 and 11:30, and the morning meal was fed following testing. Ad libitum access to water was available throughout testing. Touchscreen training using successive approximation began between 32 and 33 months of age as previously described (Golub et al., 2017). All monkeys then passed the box training module criteria of 10 consecutive box touches within 10 min for 2 consecutive sessions.
Continuous performance task (CPT)
Sustained attention and response inhibition were assessed at 33-34 months of age via the CPT module on the CANTAB apparatus. CPT was designed as a go/no-go task in which three different colored boxes were presented one at a time on the screen. White boxes were correct and rewarded with a sugar pellet when touched. Red and green boxes were incorrect and were not rewarded when touched. Equal numbers of the three colored boxes were presented for 3 s each across 84 trials in one daily 10 min testing session. Correct responses were thus selecting the white box and not selecting, or inhibiting a response, to the red and green boxes. Incorrect responses were failing to select the white box or selecting the red or green boxes and resulted in a longer intertrial interval (3 s blank screen timeout). Monkeys had two attempts to touch a white box 5 times within 30 s to initiate their daily testing session and ultimately had 30 testing sessions. Performance was assessed for hits (correct responses), misses (omission errors), correct rejections (not selecting an incorrect box), false alarms (selecting an incorrect box), and signal detection theory measures of response accuracy (d′), nonresponse bias, and response bias (β).
Progressive ratio break point task (PRBT)
Motivation with respect to reward efficacy was tested with the Progressive Ratio CANTAB task. Monkeys began testing at 40-41 months of age and had 10 daily sessions. For this task, they had to touch a blue rectangle to retrieve a food reward, and the number of touches increased geometrically every 8 trials over the testing session. For example, the monkeys had to touch the box once for a reward for the first 8 trials, but then two touches for the next 8 trials, four touches for the following 8 trials, 8 touches for next 8 trials, etc. Daily sessions lasted for 30 min or until 3 min passed since a box was last touched. The progressive break point was considered the point at which the animal stopped responding and the session ended.
Probabilistic RL (PRL)
A more complex RL paradigm was introduced at 44-45 months of age. PRL is the RL test usually conducted in humans because it reduces the possibility that individuals develop a simple win-stay lose-shift strategy instead of continuing to acquire information on each trial. In this CANTAB module, two stimuli did not have a binary yes/no reward contingency as they did in the WGTA RL task, but rather, one stimulus was more likely, but not guaranteed, to be rewarded instead of the other stimulus. To begin, a basic RL task with 100% reward probability (one stimulus always correct and one stimulus always incorrect) was used to establish consistent performance on the computerized task. Each 30 min daily testing session consisted of 60 trials, a 30 s stimulus presentation length, 5 s intertrial interval, and 5 s darkness timeout following incorrect responses. When the monkeys reached 90% (54 of 60) correct in a session, the PRL paradigm began the next day with two new stimuli. In this paradigm, there was a 90:10 reward ratio where one stimulus was rewarded 90% of the time and the other stimulus was rewarded 10% of the time. Thus, 10% of the time, the reward contingency was possibly unexpected based on what the monkeys learned about the reward outcomes for each stimulus. When a monkey achieved 85% (51 of 60) correct for the session, the reward contingencies for the stimuli were reversed in the next testing session. With the same pair of stimuli, the one that was previously rewarded 90% of the time was now rewarded 10% of the time, and vice versa. As with the RL paradigm in the WGTA, the number of reversal events monkeys were able to achieve within 20 sessions was assessed.
Intradimensional/extradimensional (ID/ED) shift
The ID/ED task is a computerized adaptation of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task and is designed to test cognitive flexibility with respect to being able to shift from a learned set of rules to another, or attentional set shifting. Monkeys were 46-47 months old and began this task 2 d after completion of PRL. The premise of ID/ED was that the monkeys were presented with two stimuli and they learned the rule for the correct/rewarded one, and when a criterion was reached, the rule for which stimulus to select changed. There were four stages to the ID/ED task. Each stage consisted of a rewarded stimulus acquisition and then reversal of rewarded stimulus. The first stage was a simple discrimination in which two stimuli of different shapes were presented. One shape was rewarded until a criterion of 12 correct responses out of the last 15 trials was reached. On the subsequent trial, the previously rewarded stimulus became incorrect and the previously incorrect stimulus became correct (simple discrimination reversal [SDR]). When the criterion (12 of 15) was reached in the reversal round, the task moved to the second stage. This was the compound discrimination stage, in which the same shape stimuli from Stage 1 were used and had the same reward contingencies as the reversal round, but the stimuli now had lines superimposed on them to create compound stimuli. The monkeys had to learn to select the previously rewarded shape no matter which of the two lines was superimposed on it in each trial. As with Stage 1, the rewarded stimulus was then reversed after the monkey reached criterion (compound discrimination reversal [CDR]). Following criterion on Stage 2, the task moved to Stage 3, the ID. In this stage, two new shape and line stimuli were presented, yet the same relevant dimension of shape was rewarded. The monkeys had to learn both the dimension and stimulus to select for a reward. Once criterion was reached, the other shape stimulus was rewarded for the reversal round (ID shift reversal). After criterion was reached for both rounds of the third stage, the fourth stage was the ED. Two new shape and line stimuli were presented, but now it was the line stimuli that were relevant for reward instead of the shapes. Once monkeys learned which dimension and stimulus was rewarded and reached criterion, the previously incorrect stimulus in the same dimension was now the rewarded stimulus and vice versa (ED shift reversal). Monkeys were tested in 60 min daily testing sessions. In that timeframe, there was no limit to how many trials, reversals, and stages they went through, although performance did not carry over between sessions for contribution to criterion. On subsequent testing days, the new session began on the stage last presented in the previous session. For each trial, monkeys had 30 s to make a stimulus choice, a 3 s intertrial interval, and a 5 s blank screen timeout following an incorrect response. Performance on each stage was assessed by the number of trials needed to move to the next stage, the number of correct/incorrect responses, and misses/omission errors. The number of misses, hits, and errors were also normalized to the number of trials each monkey underwent for each stage as this was variable based on performance.
Neuroimaging
MRI was performed at ∼6, 12, 24, 36, and 45 months of age using a Siemens Magnetom Skyra 3-T with an 8-channel coil optimized for monkey brain scanning (RapidMR). Twenty-four of the animals were also scanned at 1 month of age. However, because of the low gray matter (GM)/white matter (WM) contrast in the T1 images, this time point is not included in the analyses for the current paper. Three animals were scanned at 3 months of age. However, some respiratory difficulties were encountered at this age, and scans were discontinued to not put the animals at risk. It was determined that 3 of the animals were sensitive to isoflurane; therefore, at subsequent time points, these animals were scanned using propofol as the anesthetic. The rate of infusion varied to maintain the animal at a steady state of anesthesia. All other animals at all time points were sedated with ketamine for tracheal intubation, then anesthetized with isoflurane for positioning in an MR-compatible stereotaxic apparatus. Once the animal was placed in and centered at the mid-line of the stereotaxic apparatus, the 8-channel receiver coil was attached to the stereotaxic apparatus using a custom connector. The center point of the 8-channel coil was positioned at AP + 10 on the stereotaxic apparatus. The Skyra table was “landmarked” at AP + 10 so that the center of the animal's brain was at the isocenter of the MRI magnet. Anesthesia was maintained with isoflurane at 1.3%-2.0%. Fluids were maintained with a saline infusion at a rate of 10 ml/kg/h for the duration of the MRI scan.
Acquisition parameters
T1-weighted images (480 sagittal slices) were acquired with TR = 2500 ms, TE = 3.65 ms, flip angle = 7°, FOV 256 × 256, voxel size during acquisition 0.6 × 0.6 × 0.6 mm. Acquired images were interpolated during image reconstruction to 512 × 512 voxels with a final resolution of 0.3 × 0.3 × 0.3 mm. The structural imaging protocol was followed with additional sequences that will not be described in this paper.
Image processing
All images were processed by operators unaware of group assignment. T1-weighted images were aligned into common space (Shi et al., 2016), bias field corrected, and brain masked using AutoSeg_3.3.2 (Wang et al., 2014). Brain masks were manually corrected if necessary. Following this preprocessing, T1-weighted images were segmented into GM, WM, and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) using NeosegPipeline_v1.0.8 (Cherel et al., 2015) (Fig. 1). Probabilistic tissue maps from structural multi-atlas templates were applied to each subject's T1-weighted images via deformable registration. University of North Carolina lobar parcellation was used to parcellate the tissue segmentations into 24 lobar brain regions using the multi-atlas fusion in AutoSeg_3.3.2 (Wang et al., 2014). For these regions, total GM and WM volumes were extracted. Lateral ventricles volume were determined via semiautomated segmentation using the region competition deformable surface approach in ITK snap (Yushkevich et al., 2006) as applied to the probability CSF maps from the tissue segmentation (Lyall et al., 2012). All segmentation and parcellation results were visually quality controlled. No major issues were detected for any of the results.
Structural MRI analysis workflow.
ROIs
In order to limit the number of comparisons, the ROIs selected were based on a review of literature documenting brain alterations associated with MIA: prefrontal, frontal, cingulate, and temporal limbic (including amygdala and hippocampus) regions (Fig. 2), and lateral ventricles (Piontkewitz et al., 2011; Willette et al., 2011; Crum et al., 2017; Drazanova et al., 2018).
ROI visualization.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were conducted within a generalized linear mixed-effects models framework (McCulloch et al., 2008) that can accommodate traditional GLMs (e.g., ANOVA and multiple linear regression) for data that were assumed normally distributed and independent across individuals, as well as linear mixed-effects models for normally distributed, binary, or count data that were collected repeatedly for an individual (across time or conditions). This flexible approach allows the use of all available data for an individual and provides the ability to control for the effect of covariates of interest and to account for the intrinsic complexity of the data by modeling subject-specific random effects and residual correlations. Transformations were used if assumptions of the linear models were not met and nonparametric techniques (exact Wilcoxon rank-sum test) were used to compare groups when transformations were unsuccessful. All models were validated both graphically and analytically. All tests were two-sided, with α = 0.05. All analyses were conducted in SAS version 9.4. (SAS Institute).
Offspring development analyses
Developmental trajectories for weight, crown-rump, and head circumference were analyzed using linear mixed-effects models with fixed effects for group (MIA, control), age at measurement, and the interaction between age and group. Linear, quadratic, and cubic age effects were considered. To account for the within-animal dependence, random intercepts and slopes for linear and quadratic effect of age were included in the models.
Cognitive development analyses
Cognitive developmental outcomes were analyzed using repeated-measures techniques: linear mixed-effects models for continuous outcomes (after square root transforming the data to meet distributional assumptions) and logistic mixed-effects models (for events observed across trials, such as misses, correct choices, false alarms). To account for the within-animal dependence, random intercepts were included in the models. For the CPT, we modeled the number of misses and false alarms and the probability to commit a miss or a false alarm (using logistic models) across the trials in each session. Models included fixed effects for group (MIA, control), session (linear, quadratic, and cubic session effects), and the interaction between session and group. Interaction terms were not significant and were not retained in the reported models. For ID/ED shift, we modeled the miss and correct rates and the probability of a miss or correct choice (using logistic models) across the trials in each stage. Models included fixed effects for group (MIA, control), stage (categorical 8 stages), and their interaction. The interaction terms were removed if they were not significant. For RL tasks, nonparametric statistics were used to examine group differences in performance during individual sessions following the first reversal.
MRI analyses
The primary aim of the MRI analyses was to model brain growth from 6 to 45 months and to assess whether MIA-treated animals had a different developmental trajectory than the control animals. Separate models were fitted for each of the global measures (brain volume, GM, WM, lateral ventricles). We first fitted models with fixed effects for group (MIA, control), time (6, 12, 24, 36, or 45 months), and the interaction between group and time, using an exchangeable within-animal covariance (except for lateral ventricles, for which a spatial exponential covariance was used) to account for the correlated nature of the data because of the repeated measures. If the interaction did not add significantly to the model, it was removed, and the results of the model including only main effects were reported. For ROI analyses, two sets of models were fitted. The first set of models paralleled the global measures analyses; the second set included adjustment for the total brain volume. Separate models were fitted for GM and WM bilateral volumes in frontal, prefrontal, cingulate, and temporal limbic cortices. The core models included fixed effects for group (MIA, control), time (6, 12, 24, 36, or 45 months), interaction between group and time, and total brain volume (for the adjusted models). Within-animal dependence was modeled using an unstructured covariance structure. We removed the interaction from the reported models if it was not significant. Significant interactions between group and time were followed up by tests to evaluate time-specific group differences.
Maternal cytokines and offspring neurodevelopment
A comprehensive evaluation of the maternal cytokine response and offspring development (i.e., behavior, multimodal neuroimaging, immune development) will be the focus of future publications. Here we present an exploratory evaluation of the relationship between maternal IL-6 and volumetric brain growth summaries of MIA-treated offspring. Spearman's rank correlations examined the associations between maternal IL-6 response and frontal and prefrontal GM and WM brain volumes in the offspring after reducing the serial data for each individual to summaries reflecting relevant and interpretable aspects of the data. Maternal IL-6 response was summarized by the peak level (i.e., the maximum) after second and third injections, which may be interpreted as a maximum effect of the injection. Because the imaging was performed at unequal time intervals and there was small variation in the ages at scan across offspring, each monkey's GM ROI trajectory was summarized by calculating the area under the curve, standardized by the length in the study. These correlations were conducted separately in the two groups, using only the monkeys who had complete data (12 MIA, 10 Control).
Results
Validation of MIA
Blood samples collected 6 h after the second (GD 44) and third (GD 46) Poly ICLC injections confirmed a strong pro-inflammatory cytokine response as indexed by change in IL-6 from baseline samples (Table 4). Within the control group, the highest measurable IL-6 value (peak) was significantly positively correlated with dam age at conception (Spearman's rank correlation = 0.80, p = 0.005), but not significantly correlated with weight (p = 0.11). Within the MIA group, there was no significant correlation between peak IL-6 levels and maternal age (p = 0.31) and weight (p = 0.85). Dams that received Poly ICLC injections also exhibited transient sickness behaviors, including reduced appetite and fever (Tables 5 and 6; Fig. 3). There was no significant difference in hair cortisol concentration between groups collected at the end of the third trimester (GD 150) (Fig. 4). Notably, the hair cortisol concentrations observed in the MIA-treated dams during late gestation were similar to those observed in other multiparous and laboratory-housed pregnant rhesus macaques that were not undergoing any further experimental manipulations (Dettmer et al., 2015), suggesting that the protocol used for inducing and assessing MIA was not associated with lasting changes in stress as indexed by hair cortisol.
Maternal IL-6 response
Descriptive statistics for appetite changes during poly ICLC injectionsa
Descriptive statistics (mean, SD) for temperature (°C) changes during poly ICLC (for MIA) or saline (for control) injections
Average temperature for the MIA- and saline-treated dams from pre-infusion (0) through 480 min after infusion during GDs 43, 44, and 46. Vertical bars represent 1 SD.
Hair cortisol concentration from MIA-exposed (n = 14) and Control (n = 11) dams. The Control group includes all 10 dams that received saline injections and one untreated dam enrolled before hair sample collection at GD 150. Horizontal lines indicate group medians. Wilcoxon rank-sum exact test indicated the groups did not differ (p = 0.32).
Offspring development
There were no group differences in overall health, physical development (Table 7; Fig. 5) and neuromotor reflexes, behavioral maturation, and attention (Table 8). There were also no significant group differences detected in the home cage observations with their mothers from 9 to 24 weeks of age (Table 9) or when observed interacting with a treatment-matched social partner in the home cage from 34 to 78 weeks of age (Table 10).
Summary for the morphometric measures from 1 to 45 months
Summary of week 1 neuro-motor reflexes, behavioral maturation, and attentiona
Summary of pre-wean (weeks 9-24) home cage observationsa
Summary of post-wean (weeks 34-78) home cage observationsa
Average trajectory for weight (a), crown-rump length (b), and head circumference (c) for the MIA-exposed and Control offspring from 1 month through 45 months. Vertical bars represent 1 SD.
Cognitive development
RL
Overall performance was similar between groups as all monkeys achieved at least one reversal; however, the number of monkeys meeting criterion on subsequent reversals diminished steadily in a non–group-specific manner. Only the sessions up to and including the first reversal were analyzed as this was the only reversal all subjects achieved. There was no significant difference in the percent errors made between the two groups either before or after the first reversal as demonstrated in Figure 6a. However, following the first reversal, MIA-treated offspring had a significantly higher number of omission errors (Fig. 6b), a trial in which they failed to give a response within the 30 s timeframe. Temperament scores following the first reversal session rated behavior of the majority of the MIA-treated animals during omission error trials as “apathetic/inactive” (data not shown). Temperament scores for object orientation, goal directivity, irritability, activity, inhibition, impulsivity, stereotypies, and attention did not differ between groups.
Performance on RL task of MIA-exposed (n = 13) and Control (n = 14) offspring at 21 months of age. Scatterplots show the following: (a) mean error percent during the initial acquisition stage and the reversal stage and (b) number of omission errors (i.e., nonresponses) in the session following the first reversal. Horizontal lines indicate group medians. **Compared with the Control group, MIA offspring have more omission errors (p = 0.005 using Wilcoxon exact rank-sum test).
CPT
Groups performed similarly on most CPT measures, such as hit rate, number of misses (omission errors), average correct rejections, response bias (β), signal detection theory measures of response accuracy (d′), and nonresponse bias (c) (Fig. 7a–f). However, the MIA-treated group was significantly more likely to false alarm (Fig. 7g) in a mixed-effects logistic model (odds ratio = 1.81, p = 0.03). The group difference in square root transformed number of false alarms did not reach statistical significance (p = 0.08).
CPT performance of MIA-exposed (n = 13) and Control (n = 14) offspring at 33-34 months of age. Scatterplots show performance endpoints averaged across 20 testing sessions as follows: (a) hit rate (correct responses), (b) misses (omission errors), (c) correct rejections (not selecting an incorrect box), (d) β (response bias), (e) d′ (response accuracy), (f) c (nonresponse bias), and (g) false alarms (selecting an incorrect box). Horizontal lines indicate group medians. *Compared with the Control group, MIA offspring are more likely to commit false alarms (p < 0.05 using a mixed-effects logistic model).
PRBT and PRL
Performance on the PRBT was similar between groups with no differences observed on the highest ratio completed, session length, total number of responses, or total reinforcers earned between the two groups (Fig. 8a–d). The groups also had similar performance on the PRL task with each group having a similar number of sessions to reach the first reversal (Fig. 9a), performance after the first reversal (Fig. 9b), proportion of animals completing each reversal (Fig. 9d), and number of sessions to each reversal (Fig. 9e). Interestingly, there was no difference in the number of omission errors following reversals in this task (Fig. 9c). Similarly, win-stay lose-shift behavior did not differ between groups (data not shown).
PBRT performance of MIA-exposed (n = 12) and Control (n = 14) offspring at 40-41 months of age. The number of screen presses required to obtain a food reward (the “ratio”) increased geometrically over the course of the 30 min session or until the monkey stopped responding for 3 min. Scatterplots show the following: (a) highest completed ratio, (b) duration of the test, (c) total number of responses, and (d) total reinforcers earned, averaged across the sessions for each individual. Horizontal lines indicate group medians. The groups did not differ.
PRL task performance of MIA-exposed (n = 12) and Control (n = 14) offspring at 44-45 months of age. Plots show the following: (a) number of sessions required to meet criteria for the first reversal, (b) percent correct in the session following the first reversal, (c) number of omission errors following the first reversal, (d) proportion of animals in each group achieving each reversal, and (e) number of sessions required to meet criteria for each reversal. Horizontal lines indicate group medians. The groups did not differ.
ID/ED shift
Similar to past studies, both groups made significantly more errors during the ED shift than the ID shift, showing an attentional set had been formed (Baxter and Gaffan, 2007; Weed et al., 2008). No difference between groups was demonstrated by the number incorrect at each stage (Fig. 10a), the number of choice trials (Fig. 10c), or the error rate at each stage (Fig. 10d). However, during the SDR and CDR stages, the MIA-treated offspring was significantly more likely to miss (i.e., commit an omission error) in mixed-effects logistic models (SDR: odds ratio = 5.48, p = 0.01; CDR: odds ratio = 6.89, p = 0.003) (Fig. 10b). The group difference in square root transformed miss rate remained significant in linear mixed-effects model for SDR (p = 0.01) and approaches statistical significance for CDR (p = 0.051). No significant group differences in miss rate were observed for other stages.
Performance on ID/ED shift initiated at 46-47 months of age of MIA-exposed (n = 12) and Control (n = 14) offspring. Scatterplots show the following: (a) number of incorrect responses, (b) miss rate, (c) number of choice trials, and (d) error rate for each stage. Horizontal lines indicate group medians. *Compared with the Control group, MIA offspring are more likely to miss during SDR and CDR (p < 0.05 using mixed-effects logistic models). SD, Simple discrimination; SDR, Simple discrimination reversal; CD, compound discrimination; CDR, Compound discrimination reversal; ID, Intradimensional shift; IDR, ID shift reversal; ED, Extradimensional shift; EDR, ED shift reversal.
Neuroimaging
Table 11 summarizes the volumetric measures for the two groups, and Table 12 displays the results of the linear mixed-effects linear models for global measures. The two groups had parallel growth trajectories from 6 to 45 months on all global measures: that is, total brain volume, total gray and total WM volume, and lateral ventricle volume; none of the group × time interactions reached statistical significance. Total brain volume from 6 to 45 months for the MIA-treated animals was consistently smaller than for the control animals, although the difference was not significant (estimated difference [est.] = −5654 mm3, p = 0.12). The same pattern of smaller volume in the MIA-treated animals relative to controls was present in total GM (est. = −3762 mm3, p = 0.12).
Summary for the GM and WM volumetric measures (mm3) from 6 to 45 months
Parameter estimates from the linear mixed-effects models for global volumetric measuresa
Table 13 summarizes the results of the unadjusted analyses for GM and WM volumes in the four ROIs. For GM, the two groups had parallel developmental trajectories from 6 to 45 months in all four regions; none of the group × time interactions reached significance. Yet, for the GM in the frontal and prefrontal regions (Fig. 11a,b), MIA-treated monkeys had smaller volumes than controls did at 6 months, and these differences persisted at later ages. (frontal: est. = −564.6 mm3, p = 0.005, prefrontal: est. = −695.8 mm3, p = 0.04). Table 14 summarizes the results of the analyses after adjusting for total brain volume. The magnitude of the group differences decreased, but remained significant, in both frontal and prefrontal GM (frontal: est. = −403.3 mm3, p = 0.01, prefrontal: est. = −387.6 mm3, p = 0.02).
Parameter estimates from the unadjusted linear mixed-effects models for regional GM and WM volumetric measuresa
Parameter estimates from the linear mixed-effects models for gray and white ROI volumetric measures adjusted for total brain volumea
Brain volume trajectories for MIA-exposed and Control offspring for (a) GM frontal and (b) prefrontal regions, and (c) WM frontal region. Light lines indicate individual trajectories. Dark lines indicate average values for the two groups. GM group differences were significant in frontal and prefrontal regions, with lower volumes in MIA across all time points. For WM, group × time interaction was significant in the frontal region, with significantly smaller volume increases from the initial 6 month measurement to the 24, 36, and 45 month measurements in MIA relative to Control.
For frontal WM, there was an interaction between time and group in both unadjusted and adjusted analyses. The two groups had similar levels of frontal WM at 6 months, but had significantly different growth trajectories over time (Fig. 11c), resulting in the MIA group having lower volumes by 36 months (est. = −353.7 mm3, p = 0.047) and more pronounced differences at 45 months (est. = −436.3 mm3, p = 0.02). These differences persisted after adjusting for total brain volume, although the magnitude of the group differences decreased (36 months: est. = −187.0 mm3, p = 0.050, 45 months: est. = −264.1 mm3, p = 0.01). Furthermore, the growth of frontal WM volume from 6 months to 24, 36, and 45 months was significantly smaller in the MIA group relative to the Control group (Table 13). A similar effect was seen when adjusting for total brain volume, with frontal WM volume growth significantly smaller from 6 months to 12, 36, and 45 months in the MIA group relative to the Control group (Table 14).
Maternal cytokines and offspring neurodevelopment
The correlation analysis for MIA-treated animals revealed a consistent pattern of negative association between peak maternal IL-6 response and summary measures of both GM (Spearman's ρ = −0.50, p = 0.10 in prefrontal; ρ = −0.55, p = 0.06 in frontal) and WM (ρ = −0.63, p = 0.03 in prefrontal; ρ = −0.62, p = 0.03 in frontal) in prefrontal and frontal regions. No pattern of association was detected in the Control group (correlations between −0.07 and 0.02, all p > 0.85).
Discussion
Here we present initial results from a new cohort of male MIA-treated NHP offspring undergoing a comprehensive assessment of brain and behavioral development from birth through 4 years of age, a time frame spanning early infancy through late adolescence. We have previously demonstrated that rhesus monkeys exposed to prenatal immune challenge develop aberrant behaviors after a period of early typical development (Bauman et al., 2014; Machado et al., 2015; Careaga et al., 2017). Although evidence of increased striatal dopamine was detected in late adolescence (Bauman et al., 2019), other aspects of neurodevelopment have not been explored in the NHP MIA model. Here we describe striking reductions in frontal lobe volume throughout development paired with the emergence of subtle changes in cognitive performance detected in the first evaluation of cognitive development in a NHP MIA model. These findings demonstrate the translational utility of the NHP model to evaluate the emergence of neurodevelopmental changes in a species more closely related to humans and to highlight ongoing developmental changes in the anatomy of the frontal lobes as a potential marker of MIA-induced risk.
Compared with controls, the MIA-treated offspring exhibited reductions in frontal and prefrontal GM volumes at 6, 12, 24, 36, or 45 months and smaller increases in frontal WM, resulting in significantly reduced WM volumes at the latter time points. Volumetric reductions have emerged as a consistent outcome in rodent MIA models (for review, see Guma et al., 2019) and have also been reported following prenatal influenza exposure in NHPs (Short et al., 2010). Although comparing developmental trajectories across species is challenging, it is noteworthy that reduced frontal volume has been detected in both mid-gestation MIA-treated rats (Piontkewitz et al., 2012; Crum et al., 2017) and late first trimester MIA-treated NHPs. Delay in frontal WM growth volume emerging in MIA-treated offspring between 2 and 3 years of age observed in this study indicates a deviation from the species-typical increased WM volumes seen from birth through puberty (Malkova et al., 2006; Knickmeyer et al., 2010). These findings highlight the frontal lobe as a particularly vulnerable region to prenatal immune challenge in NHPs, which is consistent with our preliminary findings of subtle changes in DLPFC dendritic morphology (Weir et al., 2015). Indeed, rodent MIA models have also identified numerous changes in neuronal migration, number, density, and alterations in dendritic structure and synapse formation that could contribute to aberrant brain growth trajectories (for review, see Bergdolt and Dunaevsky, 2019). In the present study, dams received Poly ICLC injections on GD 43, 44, and 46, which corresponds to late first trimester (GD 0-55) of the 165 d gestation. In rhesus monkeys, first trimester peak neurogenesis of subcortical structures is followed by the early stages of corticogenesis that continues through the second trimester (GD 56-110) (Rakic, 1988). Emerging evidence indicates that microglia play a critical role in regulating cell production during this time and raises the possibility that MIA-induced changes in the maternal-fetal immune environment could alter the timing and trajectory of these critical neurodevelopmental processes (Barger et al., 2019).
Despite the significant reduction in GM and WM volumes observed in the MIA-treated animals as described above, the MIA-treated animals had similar overall cognitive performance to control groups with some subtle differences. The MIA-treated offspring showed evidence for increased omission errors in the RL and more misses during two stages of the ID/ED (both reversal stages), which are measures of the subject not providing a response. These behavioral changes may reflect difficulty adaptively forming and using a task set but could also reflect an increased impact of unexpected negative feedback on MIA performance. Given that MIA-treated animals performed similarly to controls on all endpoints of PRBT, motivation does not appear to be a major contributing factor. Additionally, the MIA offspring also had a significantly increased number of false alarms on the CPT. When tested with a PRL paradigm at 4 years of age, no differences were found in overall performance or number of omission errors observed. The different outcomes in omission errors in the RL task versus the PRL could be a factor of maturity or a result of the different testing parameters. In the PRL task, the monkey would have an expected slowed rate of learning requiring a more complex strategy to achieve a reward compared with the RL task, and the increased complexity could have resulted in improved task engagement (Izquierdo and Jentsch, 2012). In both the RL and ID/ED task, when the MIA offspring were engaged and participating, their performance was similar to the control group.
Overall, it appears the MIA offspring performed similarly to controls but had occasional trouble maintaining and using the rule associated with the tasks. It is important to note that the NHPs in the current study were evaluated from infancy through late adolescence, when cognitive performance strategies continue to mature (Weed et al., 1999). It is plausible that the subtle changes in early cognitive performance observed in these young MIA-treated monkeys may represent a “prodromal” phase in the model that may become more pronounced over time as the monkeys progress from adolescence to young adulthood. Indeed, although many studies using an adult rodent MIA model have described impairments in working and spatial memory (Bergdolt and Dunaevsky, 2019; Haddad et al., 2020), studies testing juvenile age groups have produced mixed results with some reporting various deficits in cognition during prepubescent time points (Vuillermot et al., 2012; Giovanoli et al., 2015), whereas others found deficits that only emerged during adulthood (Meyer et al., 2006; Richetto et al., 2014).
Our NHP Poly IC-based MIA model is designed to stimulate inflammatory cytokine response late in the first trimester that mimics a moderate to severe infection during pregnancy. The MIA-treated dams exhibit transient fever, reduced appetite, and elevated inflammatory cytokines, including IL-6, which is necessary and sufficient for MIA to alter brain development and behavior in rodent offspring (Smith et al., 2007) and has recently been associated with neurodevelopmental outcomes in monkeys (Ramirez et al., 2020) and humans (Rasmussen et al., 2021). Although the MIA model has historically been presented as a model relevant for ASD and/or SZ, emerging consensus in the field suggests that prenatal immune challenge may serve as a “disease primer” that, in combination with other genetic or environmental factors, may result in altered brain and behavior trajectories relevant to a number of NDDs (Meyer, 2019). While comparisons between animal models and clinical disorders must be made with caution, the NHP model more closely approximates the protracted period of postnatal development needed to evaluate the emergence of MIA-induced neurodevelopmental changes. The subtle impairments of cognitive performance exhibited by the MIA-treated animals does align with the premorbid phase of SZ, characterized by attentional and other cognitive deficits in later childhood and adolescence (McCutcheon et al., 2020) that become more severe over time (Guo et al., 2019). The reductions in cortical GM volume observed in this new cohort of MIA-treated monkeys also align with neuroimaging studies from adolescents and young adults diagnosed with SZ (Schwarz et al., 2019; Keshavan et al., 2020) and may serve as a biomarker of neurodevelopmental risk (Ursini et al., 2021).
Additional clinical and translational research is needed to understand how other factors, including genetic risk, gestational timing, nature and intensity of the maternal immune response, and additional postnatal events, determine which (if any) disease phenotype results from prenatal exposure to MIA. The vast majority of MIA models are conducted in rodents, although there is increasing interest in cross-species approaches using other species, including ferrets and pigs (Li et al., 2018; Rymut et al., 2020). Although the results of the present study extend the results of rodent MIA models into a species more closely related to humans, there are inherent logistic constraints to NHP studies. Limitations of the current study include a relatively modest sample size and exclusion of female offspring. We recognize that sex differences are emerging as a critical factor in MIA model studies (Coiro and Pollak, 2019) and will be focusing on the impact of MIA on the female NHP brain in our upcoming studies. As the NHP model requires 165 d gestation followed by 4 years of data acquisition, the complete characterization of offspring brain and behavioral development will require additional time for coding and analysis. However, the initial observation of reduced frontal GM and WM volume paired with subtle changes in cognitive development contributes to mounting evidence that early exposure to prenatal immune challenge triggers a pattern of divergent neurodevelopmental trajectories in MIA-treated offspring. Data collected from this unique NHP cohort will allow us next to explore behavioral, transcriptional, brain network, and immunologic profiles of MIA-treated offspring to better understand resilience and susceptibility to prenatal immune activation.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the University of California Davis Conte Center (National Institute of Mental Health P50MH106438) to C.S.C. Development of the nonhuman primate model and behavioral characterization of the offspring were supported by P50MH106438-6618 (National Institute of Mental Health) to M.D.B. Neuroimaging studies were supported by P50MH106438-6616 (National Institute of Mental Health) to D.G.A. Cytokine analysis was supported by Biological and Molecular Analysis Core of the MIND Institute Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center P50HD103526. A.M.R. was supported by University of California Davis Autism Research Training Program T32MH073124. L.H.F. was supported by the University of California Davis Department of Psychiatry Resident Research Track Program. Additional support was provided by California National Primate Research Center base Grant RR00169. We thank Dr. Andres Salazar (Washington, DC) for providing Poly ICLC (Oncovir); the veterinary and animal services staff of the California National Primate Research Center for care of the animals; Emma Connolly and Kyle Bone for collecting behavioral data; and Matthew Matson and Anurupa Kar for assistance with manuscript preparation and editing.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to David G. Amaral at dgamaral{at}ucdavis.edu or Melissa D. Bauman at mdbauman{at}ucdavis.edu