Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Viewpoints

How to Interpret Resting-State fMRI: Ask Your Participants

Javier Gonzalez-Castillo, Julia W.Y. Kam, Colin W. Hoy and Peter A. Bandettini
Journal of Neuroscience 10 February 2021, 41 (6) 1130-1141; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1786-20.2020
Javier Gonzalez-Castillo
1Section on Functional Imaging Methods, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland, 20892
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Julia W.Y. Kam
2Department of Psychology, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada, T2N 1N4
3Hotchkiss Brain Institute, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada, T2N 4N1
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Colin W. Hoy
4Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California at Berkeley, Berkeley, California, 94720
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Peter A. Bandettini
1Section on Functional Imaging Methods, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland, 20892
5FMRI Core, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland, 20892
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Resting-state fMRI (rsfMRI) reveals brain dynamics in a task-unconstrained environment as subjects let their minds wander freely. Consequently, resting subjects navigate a rich space of cognitive and perceptual states (i.e., ongoing experience). How this ongoing experience shapes rsfMRI summary metrics (e.g., functional connectivity) is unknown, yet likely to contribute uniquely to within- and between-subject differences. Here we argue that understanding the role of ongoing experience in rsfMRI requires access to standardized, temporally resolved, scientifically validated first-person descriptions of those experiences. We suggest best practices for obtaining those descriptions via introspective methods appropriately adapted for use in fMRI research. We conclude with a set of guidelines for fusing these two data types to answer pressing questions about the etiology of rsfMRI.

  • fMRI
  • resting-state
  • introspection
  • coactivation patterns
  • ongoing experience
  • consciousness

Introduction

Resting-state fMRI (rsfMRI) refers to fMRI scans of subjects instructed to let their mind wander (i.e., think about whatever comes to mind) and fixate on a crosshair (or with eyes closed). No other motor, perceptual, or cognitive processes are explicitly demanded. This experimental simplicity makes rsfMRI data easy to collect and share, amenable for interspecies comparisons, and ideal for biomarker development. Unfortunately, the simplicity of rsfMRI also makes determining etiology of signals (e.g., neuronal, physiological, or artifactual) and establishing putative relationships to ongoing mental processes quite challenging. Nonrest imaging modalities can rely on task timing and cognitive constraints, as well as subjects' responses, to isolate neuronally driven fluctuations and interpret their cognitive correlates. Unfortunately, rsfMRI cannot resort to similar procedures because rsfMRI scans are rarely annotated with sufficiently detailed information about the cognitive, perceptual, and motor processes that unfold in subjects' minds and bodies during resting-state scans (i.e., a subject's ongoing experience during rest).

Examination of neuronal contributions to rsfMRI has been the focus of ample research (Box 1). Collectively, this research has cemented the view that low-frequency fluctuations in rsfMRI are primarily of neuronal origin and reflect meaningful aspects of the brain's functional organization. However, this “neurocentric” model is inconclusive (Lu et al., 2019), and effectively isolating and interpreting neuronally induced fluctuations remains challenging. In addition to the well-documented confounding effects of excessive head motion (Power et al., 2018), investigators have also demonstrated that network-like structures can arise in rsfMRI data from physiological phenomena: for example, voxelwise differences in vascular responses to respiration and cardiac events (Chen et al., 2020), and spread of other non-neural systemic oscillations1 of unknown origin through cerebral vasculature (Tong et al., 2015). Accordingly, basic network features, such as confinement to gray matter, interhemispheric symmetry, and overlap with functionally coherent cortical systems, although promising, become insufficient to claim neuronal origin. Additional evidence is needed to minimize interpretational uncertainty. Although desirable properties, such as echo-time dependence2 (Kundu et al., 2012) and conscious state dependence (Barttfeld et al., 2015), are sometimes explored, additional ways to substantiate interpretation are quite limited when concurrent electrophysiological recordings do not exist. Ideally, establishing how ongoing experience can be expected to regulate functional connectivity at regional and network levels could appreciably strengthen our confidence about how to interpret rsfMRI results. Unfortunately, as the role of ongoing experience in rsfMRI remains largely unexplored, little progress has been made on this front.

Box 1.

Neurocentric model of rsfMRI

In 1995, Barat Biswal and colleagues reported that low-frequency fluctuations in fMRI signals obtained from awake resting human subjects showed patterns of spatial correlation suggestive of a neurophysiological origin (Biswal et al., 1995). While their study focused on motor cortex, soon after other groups reported similar results for visual cortex (Hampson et al., 2004), auditory cortex (Lowe et al., 1997), language regions (Hampson et al., 2002), and eventually the default mode network (Greicius et al., 2003). In all these seminal studies, researchers took extreme precautions to ensure artifactual sources (e.g., system noise, physiological noise, head motion) could not account for the observed patterns. Still, evidence was not sufficiently conclusive to ensure the observed phenomena were purely neuronal.

In the 25 years that followed (for an excellent review of the first 15 years, see Lowe, 2010), dozens of studies aimed to answer this critical question: “To what extent is resting-state functional connectivity driven by underlying neuronal phenomena?” The diversity of approaches that researchers have adopted to test this idea is astonishing. For example, some have confirmed signal characteristics that fit expectations of a neuronally induced, BOLD-like phenomenon, including confinement to gray matter (Biswal et al., 1995), exponential dependence with echo time (Peltier and Noll, 2002), attenuation under hypercapnia (Biswal et al., 1997a), and manifestation in cerebral blood flow measures (Biswal et al., 1997b). Others have evaluated the effects of structural insults and found that compromised anatomic connectivity, such as in callosal agenesis (Lowe et al., 1997) and multiple sclerosis (Lowe et al., 2002), results in weakened or absent functional connectivity. In parallel, many have reported significant relationships between resting functional connectivity patterns and clinical symptoms (for review, see Lee et al., 2013), as well as with individual variability in behavioral traits, such as fluid intelligence (Finn et al., 2015), impulsivity (Li et al., 2013), neuroticism, and extraversion (Hsu et al., 2018). Another important line of research demonstrates that resting-state functional connectivity patterns mimic well-established principles of functional brain organization both at the whole-brain level (Smith et al., 2009), and at the finer scale of functional subdivisions in complex structures, such as the striatum (Di Martino et al., 2008), the anterior cingulate cortex (Margulies et al., 2007), and the thalamus (Zhang et al., 2008). Yet, perhaps the strongest evidence supporting a neuronal basis for resting-state functional connectivity comes from studies looking at concurrent hemodynamic and electrophysiological recordings. Key findings in this line of research include strong agreement between spatial distribution of low-frequency fluctuations and that of the following: (1) the amplitude of local field potentials in the γ band in anesthetized monkeys (Shmuel and Leopold, 2008), (2) neural firing in awake resting mice (Ma et al., 2016), (3) electrocortical recordings in anesthetized patients with epilepsy (Nir et al., 2008), and (4) electroencephalographic recordings in awake resting healthy humans (Laufs et al., 2003).

Here, we argue that resting subjects engage in rich mental experiences that correspond to bursts of short, spatially distributed activity, which in turn drive rsfMRI functional connectivity estimates. Moreover, we propose measuring these ongoing experiences with introspection to empirically test this hypothesis.

Resting ongoing experiences are rich and individually diverse

The first pillar to ensure steady progress in advancing our conception of the joint dynamics of ongoing experience and rsfMRI data are to explicitly acknowledge that resting state is not a single state of mind, but a succession of cognitive, emotional, perceptual, and motor processes, both conscious and nonconscious, unique to each scan (Fig. 1A). In other words, researchers should not assume that neural dynamics in the absence of experimentally imposed sensory stimulation or cognitive demands lack correspondence to meaningful experiences. Scanners hardly resemble sensory-deprived environments, and subjects' minds are unlikely idle. First, scanners generate loud pulsed acoustic noises during data acquisition, which can affect functional connectivity (Dionisio-Parra et al., 2020). Second, scanner room lighting conditions are often sufficiently lit for subjects to appreciate and direct their attention to different areas within their restricted field of view. Third, in addition to moving their heads, subjects often reposition themselves and voluntarily move their limbs (Tan et al., 2017). Fourth, subjects often report periods of heightened sensation (Delamillieure et al., 2010), including itches, physiological urges (e.g., urination, thirst), and muscular discomfort. Fifth, subjects engage in multiple spontaneous and goal-oriented cognitive processes (Diaz et al., 2013, 2014). Sixth, subjects' vigilance level fluctuates as scanning progresses (Tagliazucchi and Laufs, 2014). Despite the richness and diversity of cognitive, perceptual, and motor processes composing subjects' ongoing experience during rest, researchers rarely acknowledge these contributions, and instead use a single blanket term, such as “intrinsic” or “spontaneous,” to describe all neuronal activity observed during rsfMRI (Kucyi et al., 2018). Such an overly simplistic characterization reflects our limited understanding of how idiosyncratic ongoing experiences during resting state may shape functional connectivity. More importantly, it neglects converging evidence suggestive of a significant role for aspects of ongoing experience: for example, self-initiated motion (Petridou et al., 2013; Winder et al., 2017), content (Gorgolewski et al., 2014; Gonzalez-Castillo et al., 2019), form3 (Gorgolewski et al., 2014), spontaneity4 (Wang et al., 2018b), and temporal focus (Vatansever et al., 2020) of ongoing thoughts), other than shift in vigilance (Tagliazucchi and Laufs, 2014; Laumann et al., 2017), in modulating patterns of rsfMRI functional connectivity.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Schematic representation of the main premise of this work. A, Subjects have rich ongoing experiences characterized by a succession of cognitive, emotional, and perceptual processes unique to each scan. B, Those experiences manifest in rsfMRI data in the form of short bursts of distributed neuronal activity (i.e., coactivation events). C, Coactivation events, in turn, drive estimates of functional connectivity.

Moment-to-moment fluctuations in rsfMRI

The second pillar of the present argument is that a sound understanding of rsfMRI will emerge from exploration of its most dynamical aspects (Fig. 1B,C). For almost two decades, rsfMRI research focused almost exclusively on effects estimated using entire time series based on the assumption that functional connectivity remained constant for the entirety of individual scans. However, recent research demonstrates that rsfMRI connectivity is time-varying (for review, see Preti et al., 2017; Lurie et al., 2020); and functional networks can undergo several reconfigurations during individual rsfMRI scan, which often takes 5-15 min (Chang and Glover, 2010; Gonzalez-Castillo et al., 2014). Importantly, detailed explorations of moment-to-moment fluctuations in rsfMRI data have demonstrated that static functional connectivity estimates (e.g., intrinsic networks) are driven primarily by a small number of brief, strong, temporally sparse, spatially distributed bursts of activity, known as coactivation events (Fig. 1B) (Tagliazucchi et al., 2011, 2016; Petridou et al., 2013; Allan et al., 2015; Gutierrez-Barragan et al., 2019). Although these coactivation events only make up <10% of the data, they suffice to faithfully reproduce functional connectivity patterns estimated using full-time series in healthy individuals (Tagliazucchi et al., 2016; Gutierrez-Barragan et al., 2019), and to reproduce functional connectivity disruptions associated with sleep (Tagliazucchi et al., 2016) and disease (Tagliazucchi et al., 2011). Importantly, these findings cannot be reproduced with a similar number of randomly selected samples, suggesting that coactivation events are important drivers of functional connectivity in rsfMRI data. Mapping the ongoing experiences accompanying these important neuronal activations could hold the key to interpreting rsfMRI functional connectivity.

Four additional observations emphasize the prominent role of coactivation events in rsfMRI data. First, coactivation events follow the temporal signature of neuronally induced hemodynamic events; and as such, they can be reliably detected using hemodynamic deconvolution (Tagliazucchi et al., 2011; Allan et al., 2015). Second, concurrent hemodynamic and electrophysiological recordings demonstrate strong correspondence between hemodynamic and neuronal coactivation events (Matsui et al., 2019). Third, removal of coactivation events from rsfMRI data (via regression or scrubbing) significantly reduces the detectability of intrinsic functional connectivity networks (Allan et al., 2015). Fourth, coactivation events have been linked to self-initiated behavior (Petridou et al., 2013; Allan et al., 2015; Gonzalez-Castillo et al., 2019). Jointly, these observations not only indicate that rsfMRI coactivation events are neuronally driven, they also reveal that coactivation events are linked to observable behaviors: for example, human motion (Petridou et al., 2013) and mice whisking (Winder et al., 2017). However, the degree to which the relationship between coactivation events and behavior can be extended to mentation-related aspects of resting ongoing experiences remains unknown.

Given the above-mentioned role of coactivation events in shaping rsfMRI functional connectivity, their demonstrated neuronal origin, and their link to observable behaviors, we postulate that establishing the relationship between coactivation and mentation events is the most effective way to explore the cognitive correlates of rsfMRI. Importantly, such a scientific endeavor will require access to the timing, content, and modality (e.g., visual, auditory) of covert cognition unfolding during rest. Arguably, some of this information can be obtained via traditional, concurrent, nondisruptive third-person measures, such as those provided by skin conductance (e.g., to detect periods of elevated emotional valence) and eye tracking (e.g., to detect shifts in visuospatial attention). However, a detailed portrayal of ongoing mentation can only be achieved by reports from the first-person perspective as provided by introspection. We next briefly introduce the scientific methodology of introspection with an emphasis on its application to neuroimaging research.

Introspection: a window into the mind

Historically, introspection denotes “attention on the part of an individual to his own mental states and processes, as they occur, with a view of knowing more about them” (Baldwin, 1901). Here, we use this term differently to designate a set of scientific instruments by which experimenters gain access into one's ongoing experience. Over the years, introspection methods have consistently been met with strong skepticism over their reliance on subjective reports (Nisbett and Wilson, 1977; Skinner, 1977) (for our rebuttal, see Box 2). A critical issue for scientifically valid introspection is the distinction between content and interpretation. Just as scientists minimize bias by withholding theoretical interpretations when collecting and analyzing data, only introspective methods directed at gaining insights into the contents of our experience and not about the reasons for that experience (i.e., interpretation) can meet scientific criteria. Indeed, despite its critics, content-oriented introspection has played an important role in shaping the early years of experimental psychology (Box 3), and continues to be a vital instrument (Box 2) in cognitive neuroscience: for example, pain (Ploner et al., 2010), personality (Eisenberger et al., 2005), emotions (Northoff and Heinzel, 2006), dreaming (Horikawa et al., 2013), states of consciousness (Schooler, 2002), mind wandering (Martinon et al., 2019), and psychiatric diagnosis (Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, Ed 5, American Psychiatric Association DSM-5 Task Force, 2013; and International classification of diseases, Ed 11, World Health Organization, 2018).

Box 2.

Addressing subjectivity in introspection research

Many researchers object to measurements based on introspection, largely because they assume the subjective nature of such data is somehow contradictory to the scientific pursuit of objective truth. However, subjective reports are widely used in epidemiology, psychology, and other social sciences. These fields have developed extensive theory and methodology for establishing the necessary reliability, validity, and generalizability for any given psychometric instrument, just as is done for physical measurement systems (Kimberlin and Winterstein, 2008). Furthermore, the explanatory power of subjective reports is evident in their central role in several of the biggest success stories in neuroscience.

The clearest example is the field of psychophysics, which can be traced directly back to Wundt's pioneering use of introspection (Wundt and Wirth, 1902). Psychophysics investigates mechanisms of sensation and perception by obtaining subjective ratings of perceived stimulus intensity in response to precisely controlled presentation of physical stimuli. Converging evidence across numerous subjective measurement techniques established reliable and precise power law relationships between physical and perceived stimulus intensity for vision, audition, taste, and pain (Price et al., 2002). Importantly, these perceptual response curves were found to be directly proportional to the neural firing rates in afferent sensory nerves (Borg et al., 1967), and similar mappings between subjective ratings and activity in cortical and subcortical regions have been used to explain other phenomena in perception (e.g., multisensory integration) (Stein and Meredith, 1993). Notably, psychophysics methods have been adapted to cases in which experimenters cannot control physical stimuli by carefully operationalizing independent and dependent variables, ensuring participants understand those definitions, and obtaining ratings using well-validated scales (Price and Barrell, 2012).

Neuroeconomics also embraces subjectivity to improve our understanding of reward learning and decision-making. Behavioral economics applies psychology research on subjective biases (e.g., heuristics, loss aversion) to help explain irrational choice behavior. Neuroeconomics builds on this work by investigating the neural underpinnings of these value-based decisions by comparing subjective value ratings to neural recordings, which has revealed single neurons (Platt and Glimcher, 1999; Schultz, 2010) and cortical areas (Schultz, 2010) that encode value signals. These findings represent substantial progress toward one of the goals of neuroeconomics: redefining subjective value as objectively measured spikes per second in reward circuitry (Glimcher, 2009).

Combining measurements of subjective experience and neural activity has also driven advances in research on meta-cognition (Fleming and Dolan, 2012), placebo effects (Vase et al., 2011), and consciousness (Block, 2019), among others. Collectively, these diverse examples clearly demonstrate that first-person data are scientifically valid, widely used, complimentary to third-person data, and synergistic with neural recordings in the investigation of neural mechanisms of ongoing experience.

Box 3.

History of introspection in psychology

The influential work of several psychologists from the late 1800s to early 1900s demonstrates the utility of introspection as a scientific method in experimental psychology. Considered the father of introspection in psychology, Wilhelm Wundt adopted a highly controlled and systematic approach to investigating mental events (Wundt, 1907). He experimentally controlled stimulus presentation to elicit consistent and replicable percepts. Wundt referred to this approach as internal perception, which aimed to approximate reliability of data obtained in the natural sciences. Wundt also trained his observers to ensure that their passive observation happened quickly and accurately (Wundt, 1907). For instance, observers were systematically trained to report whether a tone is perceived as higher or lower in pitch compared with the last tone in a series of repeated presentations. The strict experimental conditions imposed by Wundt naturally limited his research to sensations and perceptions. Although Wundt's work fell short of providing a means to gain a comprehensive understanding of our inner experience, it highlighted the importance of training an observer to become experienced in introspection.

Another major proponent of introspection was Edward Titchener, a student of Wundt who also believed that accurate introspection was attainable by training (Titchener, 1912). In addition to internal perception, Titchener used retrospection in which the observers first have the experience, unattended and thus untainted, then recall the experience immediately after it occurs from memory. Although susceptible to memory distortion, retrospection was considered the only way in which the study of one's conscious experience is not interrupted by the act of actively observing one's experience (Boring, 1953; Danzinger, 1980). Titchener's work underscores the value of immediate retrospection in revealing an untainted inner experience of the observer.

In contrast to the experimental approach of Wundt, his contemporary, William James, investigated the stream of consciousness using qualitative retrospection. This approach involved direct, in-depth investigation of one's subjective experience beyond sensations and perceptions (James et al., 1981). James believed that introspection required retrospection, as this was the only way to not interfere with the actual experience itself. He did not conduct tightly controlled experiments to avoid reducing the stream of consciousness to sensory percepts. Rather, he was in favor of a more qualitative approach to examine the entirety of our conscious experience. Despite the qualitative nature of James' approach, his work set the foundation for the development of the most influential findings about human consciousness in psychology, attesting to the value that qualitative methodologies add to our understanding of the conscious experience.

Notably, introspection has also contributed to our overall understanding of ongoing experience in rsfMRI. Using self-report questionnaires, researchers have uncovered key dimensions of ongoing experience during rest (e.g., discontinuity of mind,5 theory of mind,6 planning) (Diaz et al., 2013, 2014; Gorgolewski et al., 2014), described considerable levels of intersubject variability in resting-state ongoing experiences (Delamillieure et al., 2010), identified patterns of thought (e.g., future planning, current concerns) that can explain interindividual differences in static resting-state functional connectivity (Vatansever et al., 2020), and established statistical relationships between the temporal persistence (dwell time) of certain functional connectivity configurations (brain states) and overall patterns of thinking (e.g., future planning) (Karapanagiotidis et al., 2020). To prevent disruption of the ongoing experience as it unfolds (i.e., observer effects), all those aforementioned studies required subjects to retrospectively (with hindsight) reflect and report summary characteristics of their experience immediately after scanning concluded (Fig. 2A). Although such reports can help establish statistical relationships between summary characteristics of ongoing experience and neuroimaging data (trait-level links), they lack temporal precision to reveal “one-to-one” links between individual instances of ongoing mentation and contemporaneous coactivation events (instance-level links). In addition, retrospection inherently relies on episodic memory, rendering it susceptible to distortion during encoding, storage, or recall (Lapping-Carr and Heavey, 2017). Finally, retrospective methods aggregate information across long epochs of time, blurring details in the content, modality, and quality of specific episodes of the resting ongoing experience occurring at any given instant (i.e., temporal blurring effects). In other words, although retrospective methods may play a key role when exploring the etiology of functional connectivity during resting state, they may not suffice when trying to explore the proposed link between specific coactivation events and aspects of ongoing experience. For this particular goal, retrospective approaches must be complemented with additional methods that can provide time-resolved information about ongoing experience (Fig. 2B). We discuss those next.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

A, Retrospective introspection relies on thought probes/surveys that take place at the conclusion of a scan and gather information about the overall ongoing experience of subjects during the entirety of the resting-state scan. B, In-the-moment introspection presents thought probes at pseudo-random times during the resting-state scan. Each of those probes is aimed at obtaining a characterization of the ongoing experience unfolding right before the probe was presented (e.g., it gathers information about a short, i.e., few seconds, period of time).

“In-the-moment” introspection

One method able to meet the requirement of time-resolved information is thought sampling (also known as experience sampling) (Martinon et al., 2019). This technique presents thought probes, namely, brief sets of force-choice or open-ended questions, at pseudorandom intervals that subject answer based on the characteristics of their ongoing experience at the moment immediately before the onset of the probe (e.g., Smallwood and Schooler, 2006). This “in-the-moment” introspection technique is common in mind wandering7 research (for review, see Martinon et al., 2019) a field in cognitive neuroscience with strong conceptual links to rsfMRI (Box 4) that has traditionally explored one's ongoing experience when attention is directed away from an external task (Smallwood and Schooler, 2006). Given that the depiction of mind wandering often involves gathering knowledge about the focus of attention and content of the wandering thoughts, introspection has always played an important role in mind-wandering research. Indeed, although it is possible to infer from behavioral performance that one's attention has wandered away from an experimental task, it is impossible to know where attention has drifted. Introspection provides the most direct measure of where the ongoing experience has wandered off.

Box 4.

Mind wandering and its relation to rsfMRI

The fields of mind wandering and rsfMRI present seemingly contrasting definitions of mind wandering. In mind-wandering studies, mind wandering is traditionally defined as attention away from an ongoing, externally oriented task (Christoff et al., 2009; Kam et al., 2011, 2012, 2013). In rsfMRI studies however, which generally instruct subjects to let their minds wander in a task-free environment, mind wandering simply implies that subjects think about whatever comes to mind. Indeed, the task here is to “mind wander.”

Despite such nuanced differences in procedural definitions, a recent shift in the mind-wandering field (away from the singular task-oriented definition) and the rsfMRI field (away from static metrics aggregating across entire time series) reveals a common interest in understanding the content and dynamics of ongoing experience. In particular, increased efforts in mind-wandering research to explore the content and dynamics of wandering thoughts (Christoff et al., 2016; Zanesco, 2020) are paralleled by a growing interest in rsfMRI research to understand the joint dynamics of signal fluctuations and covert cognition (Gonzalez-Castillo et al., 2019) (see Moment-to-Moment fluctuations in rsfMRI). This shared interest has converged on similar patterns of findings. For instance, both fields have independently established clinical correlates: abnormal patterns of mind wandering and rsfMRI have been identified in depression (Greicius, 2008) and Alzheimer's disease (Greicius, 2008; O'Callaghan et al., 2019).

The methodological approach and findings established in mind-wandering studies can therefore inform this new direction in rsfMRI. For instance, using various introspection techniques, mind-wandering studies have uncovered recurring dimensions of thought content (Sormaz et al., 2018; Turnbull et al., 2019), differentiated between thoughts engaged with and without intention (Seli et al., 2016), identified repeated patterns in the progression of thoughts (Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010) and emotions (Cowen and Keltner, 2017), and revealed individual differences in dominant modality of thoughts (Delamillieure et al., 2010). These separate lines of mind-wandering research provide unique insights into the characterization of our ongoing experience, but these experiences tend to be measured during an externally oriented task. The specific constraints imposed by each task likely alter the nature of ongoing experience in a manner similar to observer effects, potentially limiting the generalizability of their experiential and neural findings. Therefore, applying these methods in the task-free context of rsfMRI may provide more representative characterizations of the mental and neural patterns underlying naturalistic ongoing experience.

Although the controversy remains surrounding the definition of mind wandering in terms of thought dynamics (Christoff et al., 2016) versus relation to an ongoing task (Seli et al., 2018), this field shares complementary aims with rsfMRI studies to reveal the nature of our ongoing thoughts. Therefore, these two fields of mind wandering and rsfMRI are well positioned to share methods to gain a deeper understanding of how our minds wander during task performance, and how our ongoing experience unfolds in task-free environments, respectively.

While recent neuroimaging studies of mind wandering have adopted a multidimensional version of thought sampling, namely, multidimensional experience sampling (MDES) (Medea et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2018a), earlier work that strictly conceptualized mind wandering as attention away from the task at hand usually included one question per thought probe inquiring about whether or not attention was focused on the task (Christoff et al., 2009). MDES extends beyond this standard question to provide insights into the modality of ongoing experience (e.g., visual, auditory), temporal focus (e.g., past, present, future), and emotional valence (e.g., positive, negative). Responses to MDES probes can be entered into machine learning algorithms to discover groupings (e.g., instances of ongoing experience with similar characteristics) and latent axes (e.g., dimensions that vary in synchrony) (Sormaz et al., 2018; Turnbull et al., 2019, 2020). These latent dimensions can describe meaningful, coordinated aspects of ongoing experience that better explain neuroimaging findings. For example, using principal component analysis (PCA) on 13-dimensional MDES data, Sormaz et al. (2018) identified level-of-detail, task-relatedness, modality, and emotional content as important aspects of ongoing thoughts during active task states. Importantly, the level-of-detail dimension was significantly associated with activity in the default mode network, suggesting that this network's contribution to cognition is not restricted to task-unrelated processes (e.g., the resting state). What this study and others (Smallwood et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2020a) highlight is that, despite MDES' reliance on targeted survey items, it is still possible to combine MDES with advanced mathematical algorithms to uncover combinations of surveyed characteristics about ongoing experience that correlate with neuroimaging data. Nonetheless, it is important to note that such derived dimensions (e.g., the principal components of a PCA) are still initially limited to linear combinations of surveyed items. Therefore, the use of survey items in MDES is still susceptible to missing additional meaningful dimensions completely orthogonal to those initially hypothesized and therefore, included in the MDES probes. Although researchers could also rely on nonlinear methods (e.g., T-distributed stochastic neighbor embedding (T-SNE), neuronal networks), resulting representations might be hard to interpret. Further, survey items compared with open-ended questions may also overlook previously unknown dimensions. Perhaps a more beneficial approach, especially in initial exploratory studies, would be to combine MDES with less constrained forms of “in-the-moment” introspection (e.g., open-ended questions) to facilitate the discovery of novel, unrelated, meaningful dimensions of resting mental life.

One such candidate is an introspection technique known as descriptive experience sampling (DES), developed by Russell Hurlburt (Hurlburt and Akhter, 2006; Hurlburt and Heavy, 2006). The objective of DES is to attain high-fidelity, untainted descriptions of momentary instances of inner experience. In DES terms, inner experience denotes “ongoing naturally occurring thoughts, feelings, sensations, and so on that appear directly before the footlights of consciousness” (Hurlburt et al., 2017), a definition that broadly matches our conceptualization of ongoing experience. According to the DES protocol, subjects are presented with random beeps throughout the day, similar to the aforementioned thought probes. Upon hearing a beep, subjects are instructed to attend to their inner experience at the moment of the beep, and jot down a few notes about it. In contrast to MDES, subjects are free to annotate any aspect of their experience they consider relevant, and thereby are not restricted to characterize their experience based on a set of a predefined questions. Within 24 h from the time those notes were taken, the subject and experimenter would then meet to discuss the notes to uncover a more detailed and undistorted description of each annotated experience (Hurlburt and Akhter, 2006; Hurlburt and Heavy, 2006). During these meetings, subjects are iteratively trained on crucial “experience apprehending” skills to report undisturbed instances of the “raw” experience as it was experienced, untainted by interpretation or inferences. These skills include focusing on the experience right before the beep, reporting only what was actually experienced (as opposed to their interpretations of it), and avoiding coloring descriptions with presumptions, metaphors, or generalities. Once a sufficient number of high-fidelity descriptions have been accumulated, the experimenter extracts salient characteristics of inner experience for each individual, which are those that consistently permeate a given subject's inner experiences. These individual reports are then aggregated for group comparisons (Hurlburt and Heavy, 2006). Importantly, DES has revealed qualitative aspects of ongoing experience that were surprising to both experimenters and participants (e.g., the actual frequency of negative thoughts, the rare incidence of inner speech for some people) (Hurlburt and Heavy, 2006; Kuhn et al., 2014), highlighting how experience sampling can reveal new phenomena to explore.

Although DES was not originally designed for neuroimaging experimentation, this introspection method has been recently adapted and combined with fMRI to evaluate its neuroscientific value (Kuhn et al., 2014; Hurlburt et al., 2016, 2017). Subjects were first trained in DES procedures for 4 d outside of the scanner. Official data collection involved DES during nine 25-min-long rsfMRI scans, followed by meetings after each scan wherein the subject and experimenter discussed subjects' annotated experiences. The authors found that instances of inner speech, and not inner hearing, recruited the left inferior frontal gyrus, a key region involved in the production of overt speech (Kuhn et al., 2014). These results suggest that DES can help make important subtle distinctions about specific characteristics of ongoing experience in relation to their neural correlates.

Collectively, these separate lines of research converge on the scientific value of introspection in revealing details of individuals' ongoing experiences, and demonstrate that, when combined with neuroimaging data, introspection can deliver unique insights about the inner workings of the brain not attainable otherwise (Northoff and Heinzel, 2006; Horikawa et al., 2013; Martinon et al., 2019). Notably, devising an approach that optimally merges these two disciplines and maximizes their capacity for new discoveries is not a simple endeavor, but one determined by the research question at hand. For example, within the framework of mind-wandering research, in addition to the methods outlined above, researchers can also rely on behavioral methods (e.g., sustained attention response tasks) (Rosenberg et al., 2013) to detect and characterize ongoing thought away from the task at hand (for a review on the use of experience sampling methodologies to study mind wandering, see Martinon et al., 2019).

It is worth mentioning that the taxonomy of introspective methods presented here is not a comprehensive list of all scientifically valid introspective methodologies, but one that focuses on methods that could potentially benefit our understanding of resting-state functional connectivity. For example, in addition to retrospective methods and online methods (e.g., what we refer here as in-the-moment methods), Martinon et al. (2019) also discussed the use of dispositional methods (i.e., those that target traits linked to different types of experiences). Similarly, readers should be aware that the two dimensions highlighted here when describing introspective methodology, namely, retrospective versus in-the-moment and open-ended versus targeted survey items, can be manipulated independently (e.g., in-the-moment introspection methods can include open-ended or targeted survey items). As a case in point, while thought probes often tend to use targeted items to survey the ongoing experience, an MDES approach that involves thought probes using open-ended questions (as is the case with DES) can help reveal important dimensions of ongoing experience. For example, Baird et al. (2011) used such an approach to uncover temporal focus and self-relatedness as important characteristics of thoughts during mind wandering. These findings informed subsequent studies about these novel dimensions of thoughts, which have since been directly targeted by specific MDES items in studies of ongoing thoughts and mind wandering (Ruby et al., 2013; Sormaz et al., 2018). Finally, it is important to reiterate that no single introspective method discussed here is perfect for all empirical investigations of ongoing experience. The choice of the optimal introspective method depends heavily on the research question of interest. Here, our objective is to understand the role that ongoing mentation plays in shaping resting-state functional connectivity. This sets some specific demands on how to acquire, process, and merge introspective and neuroimaging data that we discuss next.

Introspection in rsfMRI research

So far, we argued that researchers interested in better interpreting rsfMRI data should explore the potential relationship between coactivation events and simultaneous instances of ongoing experience. In this section, we discuss considerations for implementation of this approach and potential outcomes for such a research agenda. We put greater emphasis on introspection methods relevant for rsfMRI, because a relatively extensive literature (Tagliazucchi et al., 2011; Karahanoğlu et al., 2013; Liu and Duyn, 2013; Liu et al., 2013, 2018; Petridou et al., 2013; Allan et al., 2015; Karahanoğlu and Van De Ville, 2015), with accompanying analytical tools (Caballero Gaudes et al., 2013, 2019; Bolton et al., 2020), already exists on the topic of fMRI coactivation events. Nonetheless, improvements in the sensitivity, reliability, and accuracy of coactivation event detection methodology should be considered as equally important for the advancement of the research agenda proposed here. For example, novel hemodynamic deconvolution methods that are able to account for inter-regional hemodynamic response differences (Wu et al., 2013) or to exploit echo-dependence profiles (Caballero-Gaudes et al., 2018, 2019) may greatly improve our ability to detect cognitively relevant coactivation events that may be strongly linked to experiential characteristics captured with introspective thought probes.

How to effectively annotate rsfMRI with meta-data about ongoing experience

In order to effectively gather and use rich introspection data, researchers will need to consider a few implementational factors specific to rsfMRI research. First, researchers must consider what the goal of their investigation is: to establish statistical relationships at the trait level, to uncover direct “one-to-one” links between contemporaneous instances of brain activity and ongoing experience, or both. This is important because no single introspective approach is optimally suited for all three scenarios. Retrospective techniques may be better suited for studies targeting trait-level relationships or in scenarios where observer effects are an important concern. Conversely, in-the-moment methods will be required when trying to establish instance-level links (e.g., do coactivation events with strong limbic and auditory components signals periods of ongoing experience characterized by inner speech and strong emotional valence?) and temporal blurring effects must be avoided. Despite these core differentiations, researchers should be aware that the undesirable effects of each method can be lessened via astute experimental designs. For example, the sparsity of coactivation events implies that a small number of concise, strategically placed introspective samples, which would minimize observer effects and conserve limited scan time, may suffice when seeking to establish a relationship between ongoing cognition and coactivation events. Notably, real time-fMRI could be used to trigger introspective probes only when specific coactivation events occur. This would maximize sampling efficiency relative to random sampling by ensuring all samples temporally align with coactivation events of interest. Importantly, probe triggers could be inhibited if subjects have been disturbed recently to ensure they can get back to rest in between probes. Similarly, administration of retrospective questionnaires inside the scanner following each individual resting scan will reduce the delay between when an ongoing experience occurs and when it is reported, which may help minimize temporal blurring effects. Ultimately, combining retrospective and in-the-moment approaches will be necessary to circumvent each method's limitations and converge on a detailed model of how ongoing experience shapes rsfMRI functional connectivity and behavior across timescales. For example, recent results in mind-wandering research highlight the complementary nature of different thought sampling techniques by showing that momentary regulation of off-task thought and trait-level propensity to engage in off-task thoughts when task demands are low are both associated with activity in the ventral attention network (Turnbull et al., 2019, 2020).

Second, researchers should account for the limitations imposed by the indirect nature of fMRI as a marker of neural activity. On average, there is a 5 s hemodynamic delay between the onset of neural activity and peak in fMRI response. Researchers should take this into account when fusing in-the-moment introspective information and neuroimaging data, and also when writing such probe questions. Additionally, researchers should be mindful that fMRI provides a systems-level overview of brain activity. Consequently, probes should focus on aspects of ongoing experience likely to be captured at this meso-scale level of detail, such as the modality of imagery (e.g., visual vs auditory) or the emotional valence of a thought, instead of questioning finer-scale details (e.g., whether a thought involved a sibling or colleague) that may not be accessible at current in vivo imaging resolutions.

Third, ongoing experience unfolds in a high dimensional space difficult to characterize via short, concise probes. Structured probes limited to previously established dimensions of mental life: for example, modality (Gorgolewski et al., 2014), content (Gorgolewski et al., 2014; Gonzalez-Castillo et al., 2019), time (Vatansever et al., 2020), and spontaneity (Wang et al., 2018a), may paint an incomplete picture and bias subjects to ignore previously overlooked, yet potentially relevant, aspects of their ongoing experiences. Past studies have successfully used this approach to describe robust patterns of ongoing experience that generalize across individuals (Smallwood et al., 2016), between the laboratory and real life (Ho et al., 2020), and correlate with state and trait-like neural patterns (Vatansever et al., 2020). Yet, as mentioned earlier, even advanced mathematical modeling techniques (e.g., PCA, canonical correlation analysis) (Wang et al., 2018b) cannot uncover additional meaningful but unanticipated dimensions that are completely orthogonal to the aspects of ongoing experience included in a given set of structured probes. Contrariwise, unstructured questioning may result in excessively heterogenous descriptions that are hard to quantify and aggregate, although modern text analysis and machine learning methods have great potential for augmenting traditional experimenter-dependent analysis strategies for extracting common factors from self-report data (Cowen and Keltner, 2017). Since no individual method is a panacea, initial studies may benefit from combining both structured (rating-scale) and unstructured questions (open-ended). To ensure brevity, survey items could be randomized across probes. As key dimensions emerge, subsequent studies can craft structured probe questions relevant to these key dimensions. In addition, efforts dedicated to probe standardization could considerably facilitate data aggregation across studies. That said, it is important to note that reports of fMRI combined with open-ended methodologies (e.g., DES) are much less common than reports in which fMRI data are annotated with information about ongoing experience gathered via targeted survey methods (e.g., MDES). This imbalance in the literature may signal how challenging it might be to correctly implement open-ended methods, so that they can reliably inform fMRI data interpretation. Future research should elucidate whether the proposed benefits of using open-ended methods to annotate and interpret resting-state fMRI are indeed attainable, and to what extent they do provide added value beyond that of targeted survey items.

Fourth, uncovering meaningful relationships between neuroimaging and introspective data will require advanced mathematical models: for example, canonical correlation analysis (Wang et al., 2020b) and spectral embeddings (Gonzalez-Castillo et al., 2019) that need quantitative data as inputs. fMRI data are inherently quantitative, yet introspection data are not and therefore need to be coded into numerical format. The rules governing those transformations may not always be obvious. As the coding approach can impact outcomes and interpretations, adoption of unambiguous and agreed-on coding practices for introspection data are an important part of standardization efforts. For example, during the design of quantitative probes, researchers should carefully consider, and explicitly acknowledge, whether a nominal scale (wherein order does not matter) or ordinal scale (wherein order is considered during modeling) is appropriate for a given observed variable/questionnaire item. This would ensure consistency across different studies using a given questionnaire item.

Finally, it is important to recognize that scientifically valid introspection requires substantial subject training to minimize the effort required to apprehend ongoing experience so as to avoid distorting the target experience with the apprehension process. Appropriate training is also essential to ensure subjects report their “raw” ongoing experience in the moment, without preconceptions, interpretations, and generalities (Price and Barrell, 2012; Hurlburt et al., 2016).

Expected outcomes and limitations

One key premise in our argument, initially supported by the evidence already discussed, is that ongoing conscious mentation plays an important role in the resting state. Should our hypothesis be confirmed, this would have broad implications for the field of rsfMRI. First, it would positively settle open debates regarding the value of time-varying functional connectivity to study brain function. Second, it would emphasize the need to account for systematic intergroup differences in ongoing mentation when interpreting rsfMRI results. Unaccounted for intersession and intersubject variability in ongoing experience during rest might explain why phenotypic prediction (e.g., fluid intelligence, clinical symptoms) is higher for structured experimental designs (e.g., task-based) (Greene et al., 2018) than for rsfMRI. It would also suggest that successful rsfMRI biomarker-based development must find ways to determine the clinical relevance of this source of variance, and either model or remove it accordingly. Third, it would provide an explanatory framework for the relationship between resting-state functional connectivity and task-based activity. Although this relation has been repeatedly observed empirically (Smith et al., 2009; Tavor et al., 2016), its mechanism remains elusive. One hypothesis is that task-evoked activity flows through intrinsic functional connectivity networks (Cole et al., 2016). An alternative explanation, based on our current working hypothesis, would be that such a strong relationship exists because subjects engage in many different “task-like states” when left to rest inside the scanner bore. Finally, a clearer understanding of the link between ongoing experience and rsfMRI data will likely come accompanied by increased knowledge about the functional role of spontaneous neuronal activity and could, ultimately, provide new avenues to explore the neuronal correlates of consciousness.

Alternatively, this proposed research using introspection might eventually establish that perception, cognition, emotion, or other aspects of ongoing experience have negligible, if any, contributions to rsfMRI. Such a negative outcome would still decrease interpretational uncertainty in rsfMRI by eliminating one plausible factor. Indeed, obviating all aspects of conscious mentation accessible to introspection as potential sources of rsfMRI variance would constrain rsfMRI interpretations to nonconscious processes, such as autonomic or homeostatic functions (e.g., breathing, heart rate, metabolic regulation), thereby shifting future research priorities toward basic physiology and invasive animal studies.

In conclusion, rsfMRI sits at the core of large neuroscientific research efforts, such as mapping the human functional connectome, understanding the neural correlates of clinical symptoms, or tracking brain development across the lifespan. Indeed, rsfMRI is a key component of large international neuroimaging initiatives, such as the Human Connectome Project (Van Essen et al., 2012) and the UK Biobank (Miller et al., 2016). Accordingly, the stakes are too high to accept the current levels of interpretational uncertainty that plagues the field. Here we argue that researchers can move the field forward by making first-person description of inner experience the center of our investigational efforts. Triangulation of first-person (i.e., inner experience) with third-person (i.e., neuroimaging) data to better understand the human brain has proven invaluable for other areas of neuroscientific investigation (e.g., emotions, perception). Likewise, to understand rsfMRI data, we recommend asking your participants.

Footnotes

  • J.G.-C. and P.A.B. were supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute of Mental Health (annual report ZIAMH002783). J.W.Y.K. was supported by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council Canada. C.W.H. was supported by National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke R37NS21135.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • ↵1 Systemic oscillations: signal oscillations present in both cerebral and peripheral vasculature. Their concurrent appearance in peripheral vasculature suggests a non-neuronal origin.

  • ↵2 Echo-time dependence: BOLD-like signal fluctuations in fMRI recordings show a linear dependence with echo time, namely, the lag between excitation and readout pulses, when examined in units of signal percent change. This linear dependence does not occur for non-BOLD-like fluctuations, such as those due to scanner instabilities, head motion, or inflow effects.

  • ↵3 Form: refers to the main modality in which a given experience is perceived by the individual. Examples of form include words, images, and speech.

  • ↵4 Spontaneity: characteristic of a given inner experience that describes the degree to which such experience was initiated without intention, in the absence of top-down control, and/or relationship to any external stimuli.

  • ↵5 Discontinuity of mind: one of the seven dimensions of resting-state cognition identified by Diaz et al. (2013) that refers to mental states described as feeling restless, having busy thoughts, or thoughts that rapidly shift from topic to topic.

  • ↵6 Theory of mind: one of the seven dimensions of resting-state cognition identified by Diaz et al. (2013) that refers to mental states that involve attributing beliefs, emotions, and intents to ourselves and others.

  • ↵7 The exact definition of mind wandering remains controversial, with the majority of studies characterizing mind wandering in relation to an ongoing externally oriented task, and not during resting state, which by definition occurs in absence of an external task. For an in-depth review of this issue, see Seli et al. (2018).

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Javier Gonzalez-Castillo at javier.gonzalez-castillo{at}nih.gov

SfN exclusive license.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Allan TW,
    2. Francis ST,
    3. Caballero-Gaudes C,
    4. Morris PG,
    5. Liddle EB,
    6. Liddle PF,
    7. Brookes MJ,
    8. Gowland PA
    (2015) Functional connectivity in MRI is driven by spontaneous BOLD events. PLoS One 10:e0124577. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0124577 pmid:25922945
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    American Psychiatric Association DSM-5 Task Force (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Ed 5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
  3. ↵
    1. Baird B,
    2. Smallwood J,
    3. Schooler JW
    (2011) Back to the future: autobiographical planning and the functionality of mind-wandering. Conscious Cogn 20:1604–1611. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.007 pmid:21917482
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Baldwin JM
    (1901) Dictionary of philosophy and psychology. New York: Macmillan.
  5. ↵
    1. Barttfeld P,
    2. Uhrig L,
    3. Sitt JD,
    4. Sigman M,
    5. Jarraya B,
    6. Dehaene S
    (2015) Signature of consciousness in the dynamics of resting-state brain activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 112:887–892. doi:10.1073/pnas.1418031112 pmid:25561541
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Biswal B,
    2. Yetkin FZ,
    3. Haughton VM,
    4. Hyde JS
    (1995) Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med 34:537–541. doi:10.1002/mrm.1910340409
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Biswal B,
    2. Hudetz AG,
    3. Yetkin FZ,
    4. Haughton VM,
    5. Hyde JS
    (1997a) Hypercapnia reversibly suppresses low-frequency fluctuations in the human motor cortex during rest using echo-planar MRI. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 17:301–308. doi:10.1097/00004647-199703000-00007
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Biswal BB,
    2. Van Kylen J,
    3. Hyde JS
    (1997b) Simultaneous assessment of flow and BOLD signals in resting-state functional connectivity maps. NMR Biomed 10:165–170. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-1492(199706/08)10:4/5<165::AID-NBM454>3.0.CO;2-7
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Block N
    (2019) What is wrong with the no-report paradigm and how to fix it. Trends Cogn Sci 23:1003–1013. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2019.10.001 pmid:31676213
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Bolton TA,
    2. Tuleasca C,
    3. Wotruba D,
    4. Rey G,
    5. Dhanis H,
    6. Gauthier B,
    7. Delavari F,
    8. Morgenroth E,
    9. Gaviria J,
    10. Blondiaux E,
    11. Smigielski L,
    12. Van De Ville D
    (2020) TbCAPs: a toolbox for coactivation pattern analysis. Neuroimage 211:116621. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116621 pmid:32058000
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Borg G,
    2. Diamant H,
    3. Strom L,
    4. Zotterman Y
    (1967) The relation between neural and perceptual intensity: a comparative study on the neural and psychophysical response to taste stimuli. J Physiol 192:13–20. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1967.sp008284 pmid:6051799
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Boring EG
    (1953) A history of introspection. Psychol Bull 50:169–189. doi:10.1037/h0090793 pmid:13056096
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Caballero Gaudes C,
    2. Petridou N,
    3. Francis ST,
    4. Dryden IL,
    5. Gowland PA
    (2013) Paradigm free mapping with sparse regression automatically detects single-trial functional magnetic resonance imaging blood oxygenation level dependent responses. Hum Brain Mapp 34:501–518. doi:10.1002/hbm.21452 pmid:22121048
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Caballero-Gaudes C,
    2. Moia S,
    3. Bandettini PA,
    4. Gonzalez-Castillo J
    (2018) Quantitative deconvolution of fMRI data with multi-echo sparse paradigm free mapping. Lect Notes Comput Sci 11072:311–319.
    OpenUrl
  15. ↵
    1. Caballero-Gaudes C,
    2. Moia S,
    3. Panwar P,
    4. Bandettini PA,
    5. Gonzalez-Castillo J
    (2019) A deconvolution algorithm for multi-echo functional MRI: multi-echo sparse paradigm free mapping. Neuroimage 202:116081. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116081 pmid:31419613
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Chang C,
    2. Glover GH
    (2010) Time-frequency dynamics of resting-state brain connectivity measured with fMRI. Neuroimage 50:81–98. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.011 pmid:20006716
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Chen JE,
    2. Lewis LD,
    3. Chang C,
    4. Tian Q,
    5. Fultz NE,
    6. Ohringer NA,
    7. Rosen BR,
    8. Polimeni JR
    (2020) Resting-state “physiological networks.” Neuroimage 213:116707. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116707 pmid:32145437
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Christoff K,
    2. Gordon AM,
    3. Smallwood J,
    4. Smith R,
    5. Schooler JW
    (2009) Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:8719–8724. doi:10.1073/pnas.0900234106 pmid:19433790
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Christoff K,
    2. Irving ZC,
    3. Fox KC,
    4. Spreng RN,
    5. Andrews-Hanna JR
    (2016) Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework. Nat Rev Neurosci 17:718–731. doi:10.1038/nrn.2016.113 pmid:27654862
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Cole MW,
    2. Ito T,
    3. Bassett DS,
    4. Schultz DH
    (2016) Activity flow over resting-state networks shapes cognitive task activations. Nat Neurosci 19:1718–1726. doi:10.1038/nn.4406 pmid:27723746
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Cowen AS,
    2. Keltner D
    (2017) Self-report captures 27 distinct categories of emotion bridged by continuous gradients. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 114:E7900–E7909. doi:10.1073/pnas.1702247114 pmid:28874542
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Danzinger K
    (1980) The history of introspection reconsidered. J Hist Behav Sci 16:241–262.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Delamillieure P,
    2. Doucet G,
    3. Mazoyer B,
    4. Turbelin MR,
    5. Delcroix N,
    6. Mellet E,
    7. Zago L,
    8. Crivello F,
    9. Petit L,
    10. Tzourio-Mazoyer N,
    11. Joliot M
    (2010) The resting state questionnaire: an introspective questionnaire for evaluation of inner experience during the conscious resting state. Brain Res Bull 81:565–573. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2009.11.014 pmid:20003916
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Di Martino A,
    2. Scheres A,
    3. Margulies DS,
    4. Kelly AM,
    5. Uddin LQ,
    6. Shehzad Z,
    7. Biswal B,
    8. Walters JR,
    9. Castellanos FX,
    10. Milham MP
    (2008) Functional connectivity of human striatum: a resting state FMRI study. Cereb Cortex 18:2735–2747. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn041 pmid:18400794
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Diaz BA,
    2. Van Der Sluis S,
    3. Moens S,
    4. Benjamins JS,
    5. Migliorati F,
    6. Stoffers D,
    7. Braber A,
    8. Poil SS,
    9. Hardstone R,
    10. Van't Ent D,
    11. Boomsma DI,
    12. De Geus E,
    13. Mansvelder HD,
    14. Van Someren EJ,
    15. Linkenkaer-Hansen K
    (2013) The Amsterdam Resting-State Questionnaire reveals multiple phenotypes of resting-state cognition. Front Hum Neurosci 7:446.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Diaz BA,
    2. Van Der Sluis S,
    3. Benjamins JS,
    4. Stoffers D,
    5. Hardstone R,
    6. Mansvelder HD,
    7. Van Someren EJ,
    8. Linkenkaer-Hansen K
    (2014) The ARSQ 2.0 reveals age and personality effects on mind-wandering experiences. Front Psychol 5:271. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00271 pmid:24772097
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Dionisio-Parra B,
    2. Wiesinger F,
    3. Samann PG,
    4. Czisch M,
    5. Solana AB
    (2020) Looping star fMRI in cognitive tasks and resting state. J Magn Reson Imaging 52:739–751. doi:10.1002/jmri.27073
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  28. ↵
    1. Eisenberger NI,
    2. Lieberman MD,
    3. Satpute AB
    (2005) Personality from a controlled processing perspective: an fMRI study of neuroticism, extraversion, and self-consciousness. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 5:169–181. doi:10.3758/cabn.5.2.169 pmid:16180623
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Finn ES,
    2. Shen X,
    3. Scheinost D,
    4. Rosenberg MD,
    5. Huang J,
    6. Chun MM,
    7. Papademetris X,
    8. Constable RT
    (2015) Functional connectome fingerprinting: identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nat Neurosci 18:1664–1671. doi:10.1038/nn.4135 pmid:26457551
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Fleming SM,
    2. Dolan RJ
    (2012) The neural basis of metacognitive ability. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 367:1338–1349. doi:10.1098/rstb.2011.0417 pmid:22492751
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Glimcher PW
    (2009) Choice: Towards a standard back-pocket model. In P. W. Glimcher CF, Camerer EF, Poldrack RA (Eds.), Neuroeconomics: Decision making and the brain. (p. 503–521). Elsevier Academic Press.
  32. ↵
    1. Gonzalez-Castillo J,
    2. Handwerker DA,
    3. Robinson ME,
    4. Hoy CW,
    5. Buchanan LC,
    6. Saad ZS,
    7. Bandettini PA
    (2014) The spatial structure of resting state connectivity stability on the scale of minutes. Front Neurosci 8:138. doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00138 pmid:24999315
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Gonzalez-Castillo J,
    2. Caballero-Gaudes C,
    3. Topolski N,
    4. Handwerker DA,
    5. Pereira F,
    6. Bandettini PA
    (2019) Imaging the spontaneous flow of thought: distinct periods of cognition contribute to dynamic functional connectivity during rest. Neuroimage 202:116129. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116129 pmid:31461679
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Gorgolewski KJ,
    2. Lurie D,
    3. Urchs S,
    4. Kipping JA,
    5. Craddock RC,
    6. Milham MP,
    7. Margulies DS,
    8. Smallwood J
    (2014) A correspondence between individual differences in the brain's intrinsic functional architecture and the content and form of self-generated thoughts. PLoS One 9:e97176. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0097176 pmid:24824880
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Greene AS,
    2. Gao S,
    3. Scheinost D,
    4. Constable RT
    (2018) Task-induced brain state manipulation improves prediction of individual traits. Nat Commun 9:2807. doi:10.1038/s41467-018-04920-3
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  36. ↵
    1. Greicius MD
    (2008) Resting-state functional connectivity in neuropsychiatric disorders. Curr Opin Neurol 21:424–430. doi:10.1097/WCO.0b013e328306f2c5 pmid:18607202
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Greicius MD,
    2. Krasnow B,
    3. Reiss AL,
    4. Menon V
    (2003) Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:253–258. doi:10.1073/pnas.0135058100 pmid:12506194
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Gutierrez-Barragan D,
    2. Basson MA,
    3. Panzeri S,
    4. Gozzi A
    (2019) Infraslow state fluctuations govern spontaneous fMRI network dynamics. Curr Biol 29:2295–2306.e2295. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2019.06.017 pmid:31303490
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Hampson M,
    2. Peterson BS,
    3. Skudlarski P,
    4. Gatenby JC,
    5. Gore JC
    (2002) Detection of functional connectivity using temporal correlations in MR images. Hum Brain Mapp 15:247–262. doi:10.1002/hbm.10022 pmid:11835612
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Hampson M,
    2. Olson IR,
    3. Leung HC,
    4. Skudlarski P,
    5. Gore JC
    (2004) Changes in functional connectivity of human MT/V5 with visual motion input. Neuroreport 15:1315–1319. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000129997.95055.15 pmid:15167557
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Ho NS,
    2. Poerio G,
    3. Konu D,
    4. Turnbull A,
    5. Sormaz M,
    6. Leech R,
    7. Bernhardt B,
    8. Jefferies E,
    9. Smallwood J
    (2020) Facing up to the wandering mind: patterns of off-task laboratory thought are associated with stronger neural recruitment of right fusiform cortex while processing facial stimuli. Neuroimage 214:116765. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116765 pmid:32213314
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Horikawa T,
    2. Tamaki M,
    3. Miyawaki Y,
    4. Kamitani Y
    (2013) Neural decoding of visual imagery during sleep. Science 340:639–642. doi:10.1126/science.1234330 pmid:23558170
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    1. Hsu WT,
    2. Rosenberg MD,
    3. Scheinost D,
    4. Constable RT,
    5. Chun MM
    (2018) Resting-state functional connectivity predicts neuroticism and extraversion in novel individuals. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 13:224–232. doi:10.1093/scan/nsy002 pmid:29373729
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Hurlburt RT,
    2. Akhter SA
    (2006) The descriptive experience sampling. Phenom Cogn Sci 5:271–301. doi:10.1007/s11097-006-9024-0
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  45. ↵
    1. Hurlburt RT,
    2. Heavy CL
    (2006) Exploring inner experience: the descriptive experience sampling method. In: Advances in consciousness research, Vol 64, p 276. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
    OpenUrl
  46. ↵
    1. Hurlburt RT,
    2. Alderson-Day B,
    3. Kühn S,
    4. Fernyhough C
    (2016) Exploring the ecological validity of thinking on demand: neural correlates of elicited vs. spontaneously occurring inner speech. PLoS One 11:e0147932. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0147932 pmid:26845028
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Hurlburt RT,
    2. Alderson-Day B,
    3. Fernyhough C,
    4. Kühn S
    (2017) Can inner experience be apprehended in high fidelity? Examining brain activation and experience from multiple perspectives. Front Psychol 8:43. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00043 pmid:28191000
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. James W,
    2. Burkhardt F,
    3. Bowers F,
    4. Skrupskelis IK
    (1981) The principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard UP.
  49. ↵
    1. Kam JW,
    2. Dao E,
    3. Farley J,
    4. Fitzpatrick K,
    5. Smallwood J,
    6. Schooler JW,
    7. Handy TC
    (2011) Slow fluctuations in attentional control of sensory cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 23:460–470. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21443 pmid:20146593
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Kam JW,
    2. Dao E,
    3. Blinn P,
    4. Krigolson OE,
    5. Boyd LA,
    6. Handy TC
    (2012) Mind wandering and motor control: off-task thinking disrupts the online adjustment of behavior. Front Hum Neurosci 6:329. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00329 pmid:23248596
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Kam JW,
    2. Dao E,
    3. Stanciulescu M,
    4. Tildesley H,
    5. Handy TC
    (2013) Mind wandering and the adaptive control of attentional resources. J Cogn Neurosci 25:952–960. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00375 pmid:23448525
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Karahanoğlu FI,
    2. Van De Ville D
    (2015) Transient brain activity disentangles fMRI resting-state dynamics in terms of spatially and temporally overlapping networks. Nat Commun 6:7751. doi:10.1038/ncomms8751 pmid:26178017
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Karahanoğlu FI,
    2. Caballero-Gaudes C,
    3. Lazeyras F,
    4. Van de Ville D
    (2013) Total activation: fMRI deconvolution through spatio-temporal regularization. Neuroimage 73:121–134. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.067 pmid:23384519
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Karapanagiotidis T,
    2. Vidaurre D,
    3. Quinn AJ,
    4. Vatansever D,
    5. Poerio GL,
    6. Turnbull A,
    7. Ho NS,
    8. Leech R,
    9. Bernhardt BC,
    10. Jefferies E,
    11. Margulies DS,
    12. Nichols TE,
    13. Woolrich MW,
    14. Smallwood J
    (2020) The psychological correlates of distinct neural states occurring during wakeful rest. Sci Rep 10:21121.
    OpenUrl
  55. ↵
    1. Killingsworth MA,
    2. Gilbert DT
    (2010) A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science 330:932. doi:10.1126/science.1192439 pmid:21071660
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. ↵
    1. Kimberlin CL,
    2. Winterstein AG
    (2008) Validity and reliability of measurement instruments used in research. Am J Health Syst Pharm 65:2276–2284. doi:10.2146/ajhp070364 pmid:19020196
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    1. Kucyi A,
    2. Tambini A,
    3. Sadaghiani S,
    4. Keilholz S,
    5. Cohen JR
    (2018) Spontaneous cognitive processes and the behavioral validation of time-varying brain connectivity. Netw Neurosci 2:397–417. doi:10.1162/netn_a_00037 pmid:30465033
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Kuhn S,
    2. Fernyhough C,
    3. Alderson-Day B,
    4. Hurlburt RT
    (2014) Inner experience in the scanner: can high fidelity apprehensions of inner experience be integrated with fMRI? Front Psychol 5:1393. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01393 pmid:25538649
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Kundu P,
    2. Inati SJ,
    3. Evans JW,
    4. Luh WM,
    5. Bandettini PA
    (2012) Differentiating BOLD and non-BOLD signals in fMRI time series using multi-echo EPI. Neuroimage 60:1759–1770. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.028 pmid:22209809
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Lapping-Carr LR,
    2. Heavey CL
    (2017) Pristine inner experience and descriptive experience sampling: implications for psychology. Front Psychol 8:2170. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02170 pmid:29312047
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Laufs H,
    2. Krakow K,
    3. Sterzer P,
    4. Eger E,
    5. Beyerle A,
    6. Salek-Haddadi A,
    7. Kleinschmidt A
    (2003) Electroencephalographic signatures of attentional and cognitive default modes in spontaneous brain activity fluctuations at rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:11053–11058. doi:10.1073/pnas.1831638100 pmid:12958209
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  62. ↵
    1. Laumann TO,
    2. Snyder AZ,
    3. Mitra A,
    4. Gordon EM,
    5. Gratton C,
    6. Adeyemo B,
    7. Gilmore AW,
    8. Nelson SM,
    9. Berg JJ,
    10. Greene DJ,
    11. McCarthy JE,
    12. Tagliazucchi E,
    13. Laufs H,
    14. Schlaggar BL,
    15. Dosenbach NU,
    16. Petersen SE
    (2017) On the stability of BOLD fMRI correlations. Cereb Cortex 27:4719–4732. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhw265 pmid:27591147
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Lee MH,
    2. Smyser CD,
    3. Shimony JS
    (2013) Resting-state fMRI: a review of methods and clinical applications. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 34:1866–1872. doi:10.3174/ajnr.A3263 pmid:22936095
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  64. ↵
    1. Li N,
    2. Ma N,
    3. Liu Y,
    4. He XS,
    5. Sun DL,
    6. Fu XM,
    7. Zhang X,
    8. Han S,
    9. Zhang DR
    (2013) Resting-state functional connectivity predicts impulsivity in economic decision-making. J Neurosci 33:4886–4895. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1342-12.2013 pmid:23486959
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  65. ↵
    1. Liu X,
    2. Duyn JH
    (2013) Time-varying functional network information extracted from brief instances of spontaneous brain activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110:4392–4397. doi:10.1073/pnas.1216856110 pmid:23440216
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  66. ↵
    1. Liu X,
    2. Chang C,
    3. Duyn JH
    (2013) Decomposition of spontaneous brain activity into distinct fMRI coactivation patterns. Front Syst Neurosci 7:101. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2013.00101 pmid:24550788
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Liu X,
    2. Zhang N,
    3. Chang C,
    4. Duyn JH
    (2018) Coactivation patterns in resting-state fMRI signals. Neuroimage 180:485–494. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.041 pmid:29355767
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Lowe MJ
    (2010) A historical perspective on the evolution of resting-state functional connectivity with MRI. MAGMA 23:279–288. doi:10.1007/s10334-010-0230-y pmid:21076991
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Lowe MJ,
    2. Rutecki P,
    3. Woodard A,
    4. Turski P,
    5. Sorenson JA
    (1997) Auditory cortex fMRI noise correlations in callosal agenesis. Human Brain Mapping 5:S194.
    OpenUrl
  70. ↵
    1. Lowe MJ,
    2. Phillips MD,
    3. Lurito JT,
    4. Mattson D,
    5. Dzemidzic M,
    6. Mathews VP
    (2002) Multiple sclerosis: low-frequency temporal blood oxygen level-dependent fluctuations indicate reduced functional connectivity initial results. Radiology 224:184–192. doi:10.1148/radiol.2241011005
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Lu H,
    2. Jaime S,
    3. Yang Y
    (2019) Origins of the resting-state functional MRI signal: potential limitations of the “neurocentric” model. Front Neurosci 13:1136. doi:10.3389/fnins.2019.01136 pmid:31708731
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Lurie DJ,
    2. Kessler D,
    3. Bassett DS,
    4. Betzel RF,
    5. Breakspear M,
    6. Kheilholz S,
    7. Kucyi A,
    8. Liégeois R,
    9. Lindquist MA,
    10. McIntosh AR,
    11. Poldrack RA,
    12. Shine JM,
    13. Thompson WH,
    14. Bielczyk NZ,
    15. Douw L,
    16. Kraft D,
    17. Miller RL,
    18. Muthuraman M,
    19. Pasquini L,
    20. Razi A, et al
    . (2020) Questions and controversies in the study of time-varying functional connectivity in resting fMRI. Netw Neurosci 4:30–69. doi:10.1162/netn_a_00116 pmid:32043043
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Ma Y,
    2. Shaik MA,
    3. Kozberg MG,
    4. Kim SH,
    5. Portes JP,
    6. Timerman D,
    7. Hillman EM
    (2016) Resting-state hemodynamics are spatiotemporally coupled to synchronized and symmetric neural activity in excitatory neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA USA 113:E8463–E8471. doi:10.1073/pnas.1525369113
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  74. ↵
    1. Margulies DS,
    2. Kelly AM,
    3. Uddin LQ,
    4. Biswal BB,
    5. Castellanos FX,
    6. Milham MP
    (2007) Mapping the functional connectivity of anterior cingulate cortex. Neuroimage 37:579–588. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.019 pmid:17604651
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Martinon LM,
    2. Smallwood J,
    3. McGann D,
    4. Hamilton C,
    5. Riby LM
    (2019) The disentanglement of the neural and experiential complexity of self-generated thoughts: a user's guide to combining experience sampling with neuroimaging data. Neuroimage 192:15–25. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.034 pmid:30802513
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Matsui T,
    2. Murakami T,
    3. Ohki K
    (2019) Neuronal origin of the temporal dynamics of spontaneous BOLD activity correlation. Cereb Cortex 29:1496–1508. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhy045 pmid:29522092
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Medea B,
    2. Karapanagiotidis T,
    3. Konishi M,
    4. Ottaviani C,
    5. Margulies D,
    6. Bernasconi A,
    7. Bernasconi N,
    8. Bernhardt BC,
    9. Jefferies E,
    10. Smallwood J
    (2018) How do we decide what to do? Resting-state connectivity patterns and components of self-generated thought linked to the development of more concrete personal goals. Exp Brain Res 236:2469–2481. doi:10.1007/s00221-016-4729-y pmid:27443852
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. Miller KL,
    2. Alfaro-Almagro F,
    3. Bangerter NK,
    4. Thomas DL,
    5. Yacoub E,
    6. Xu J,
    7. Bartsch AJ,
    8. Jbabdi S,
    9. Sotiropoulos SN,
    10. Andersson JL,
    11. Griffanti L,
    12. Douaud G,
    13. Okell TW,
    14. Weale P,
    15. Dragonu I,
    16. Garratt S,
    17. Hudson S,
    18. Collins R,
    19. Jenkinson M,
    20. Matthews PM, et al
    . (2016) Multimodal population brain imaging in the UK Biobank prospective epidemiological study. Nat Neurosci 19:1523–1536. doi:10.1038/nn.4393 pmid:27643430
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Nir Y,
    2. Mukamel R,
    3. Dinstein I,
    4. Privman E,
    5. Harel M,
    6. Fisch L,
    7. Gelbard-Sagiv H,
    8. Kipervasser S,
    9. Andelman F,
    10. Neufeld MY,
    11. Kramer U,
    12. Arieli A,
    13. Fried I,
    14. Malach R
    (2008) Interhemispheric correlations of slow spontaneous neuronal fluctuations revealed in human sensory cortex. Nat Neurosci 11:1100–1108. doi:10.1038/nn.2177
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Nisbett RE,
    2. Wilson TD
    (1977) Telling more than we can know: verbal reports on mental processes. Psychol Rev 84:231–259. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.231
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  81. ↵
    1. Northoff G,
    2. Heinzel A
    (2006) First-person neuroscience: a new methodological approach for linking mental and neuronal states. Philos Ethics Humanit Med 1:E3. doi:10.1186/1747-5341-1-3 pmid:16759399
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    1. O'Callaghan C,
    2. Shine JM,
    3. Hodges JR,
    4. Andrews-Hanna JR,
    5. Irish M
    (2019) Hippocampal atrophy and intrinsic brain network dysfunction relate to alterations in mind wandering in neurodegeneration. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 116:3316–3321.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  83. ↵
    1. Peltier SJ,
    2. Noll DC
    (2002) T(2)(*) dependence of low frequency functional connectivity. Neuroimage 16:985–992. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1141
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  84. ↵
    1. Petridou N,
    2. Gaudes CC,
    3. Dryden IL,
    4. Francis ST,
    5. Gowland PA
    (2013) Periods of rest in fMRI contain individual spontaneous events which are related to slowly fluctuating spontaneous activity. Hum Brain Mapp 34:1319–1329. doi:10.1002/hbm.21513
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  85. ↵
    1. Platt ML,
    2. Glimcher PW
    (1999) Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex. Nature 400:233–238. doi:10.1038/22268 pmid:10421364
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  86. ↵
    1. Ploner M,
    2. Lee MC,
    3. Wiech K,
    4. Bingel U,
    5. Tracey I
    (2010) Prestimulus functional connectivity determines pain perception in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:355–360. doi:10.1073/pnas.0906186106 pmid:19948949
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  87. ↵
    1. Power JD,
    2. Plitt M,
    3. Gotts SJ,
    4. Kundu P,
    5. Voon V,
    6. Bandettini PA,
    7. Martin A
    (2018) Ridding fMRI data of motion-related influences: removal of signals with distinct spatial and physical bases in multiecho data. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 115:E2105–E2114. doi:10.1073/pnas.1720985115 pmid:29440410
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  88. ↵
    1. Preti MG,
    2. Bolton TA,
    3. Van De Ville D
    (2017) The dynamic functional connectome: state-of-the-art and perspectives. Neuroimage 160:41–54. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.061 pmid:28034766
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  89. ↵
    1. Price DD,
    2. Barrell JJ
    (2012) Inner experience and neuroscience: merging both perspectives. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
  90. ↵
    1. Price DD,
    2. Barrell JJ,
    3. Rainville P
    (2002) Integrating experiential-phenomenological methods and neuroscience to study neural mechanisms of pain and consciousness. Conscious Cogn 11:593–608. doi:10.1016/s1053-8100(02)00018-1 pmid:12470625
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  91. ↵
    1. Rosenberg M,
    2. Noonan S,
    3. DeGutis J,
    4. Esterman M
    (2013) Sustaining visual attention in the face of distraction: a novel gradual-onset continuous performance task. Atten Percept Psychophys 75:426–439. doi:10.3758/s13414-012-0413-x pmid:23299180
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  92. ↵
    1. Ruby FJ,
    2. Smallwood J,
    3. Engen H,
    4. Singer T
    (2013) How self-generated thought shapes mood: the relation between mind-wandering and mood depends on the socio-temporal content of thoughts. PLoS One 8:e77554. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0077554 pmid:24194889
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  93. ↵
    1. Schooler JW
    (2002) Re-representing consciousness: dissociations between experience and meta-consciousness. Trends Cogn Sci 6:339–344. doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(02)01949-6 pmid:12140084
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  94. ↵
    1. Schultz W
    (2010) Subjective neuronal coding of reward: temporal value discounting and risk. Eur J Neurosci 31:2124–2135. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07282.x pmid:20497474
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  95. ↵
    1. Seli P,
    2. Risko EF,
    3. Smilek D,
    4. Schacter DL
    (2016) Mind-wandering with and without intention. Trends Cogn Sci 20:605–617. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.010 pmid:27318437
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  96. ↵
    1. Seli P,
    2. Kane MJ,
    3. Metzinger T,
    4. Smallwood J,
    5. Schacter DL,
    6. Maillet D,
    7. Schooler JW,
    8. Smilek D
    (2018) The family-resemblances framework for mind-wandering remains well clad. Trends Cogn Sci 22:959–961. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2018.07.007 pmid:30220475
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  97. ↵
    1. Shmuel A,
    2. Leopold DA
    (2008) Neuronal correlates of spontaneous fluctuations in fMRI signals in monkey visual cortex: implications for functional connectivity at rest. Hum Brain Mapp 29:751–761. doi:10.1002/hbm.20580 pmid:18465799
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  98. ↵
    1. Skinner BF
    (1977) Why I am not a cognitive psychologist. Behaviorism 5:1–10.
    OpenUrl
  99. ↵
    1. Smallwood J,
    2. Schooler JW
    (2006) The restless mind. Psychol Bull 132:946–958. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946 pmid:17073528
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  100. ↵
    1. Smallwood J,
    2. Karapanagiotidis T,
    3. Ruby F,
    4. Medea B,
    5. de Caso I,
    6. Konishi M,
    7. Wang HT,
    8. Hallam G,
    9. Margulies DS,
    10. Jefferies E
    (2016) Representing representation: integration between the temporal lobe and the posterior cingulate influences the content and form of spontaneous thought. PLoS One 11:e0152272. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0152272 pmid:27045292
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  101. ↵
    1. Smith SM,
    2. Fox PT,
    3. Miller KL,
    4. Glahn DC,
    5. Fox PM,
    6. Mackay CE,
    7. Filippini N,
    8. Watkins KE,
    9. Toro R,
    10. Laird AR,
    11. Beckmann CF
    (2009) Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:13040–13045. doi:10.1073/pnas.0905267106 pmid:19620724
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  102. ↵
    1. Sormaz M,
    2. Murphy C,
    3. Wang HT,
    4. Hymers M,
    5. Karapanagiotidis T,
    6. Poerio G,
    7. Margulies DS,
    8. Jefferies E,
    9. Smallwood J
    (2018) Default mode network can support the level of detail in experience during active task states. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 115:9318–9323. doi:10.1073/pnas.1721259115 pmid:30150393
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  103. ↵
    1. Stein BE,
    2. Meredith MA
    (1993) The merging of the senses. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
  104. ↵
    1. Tagliazucchi E,
    2. Laufs H
    (2014) Decoding wakefulness levels from typical fMRI resting-state data reveals reliable drifts between wakefulness and sleep. Neuron 82:695–708. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.03.020 pmid:24811386
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  105. ↵
    1. Tagliazucchi E,
    2. Balenzuela P,
    3. Fraiman D,
    4. Montoya P,
    5. Chialvo DR
    (2011) Spontaneous BOLD event triggered averages for estimating functional connectivity at resting state. Neurosci Lett 488:158–163. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.11.020 pmid:21078369
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  106. ↵
    1. Tagliazucchi E,
    2. Siniatchkin M,
    3. Laufs H,
    4. Chialvo DR
    (2016) The voxel-wise functional connectome can be efficiently derived from coactivations in a sparse spatio-temporal point-process. Front Neurosci 10:381.
    OpenUrl
  107. ↵
    1. Tan FM,
    2. Caballero-Gaudes C,
    3. Mullinger KJ,
    4. Cho SY,
    5. Zhang Y,
    6. Dryden IL,
    7. Francis ST,
    8. Gowland PA
    (2017) Decoding fMRI events in sensorimotor motor network using sparse paradigm free mapping and activation likelihood estimates. Hum Brain Mapp 38:5778–5794. doi:10.1002/hbm.23767 pmid:28815863
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  108. ↵
    1. Tavor I,
    2. Parker Jones O,
    3. Mars RB,
    4. Smith SM,
    5. Behrens TE,
    6. Jbabdi S
    (2016) Task-free MRI predicts individual differences in brain activity during task performance. Science 352:216–220. doi:10.1126/science.aad8127 pmid:27124457
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  109. ↵
    1. Titchener EB
    (1912) Prolegomena to a study of introspection. Am J Psychol 23:427–448. doi:10.2307/1413427
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  110. ↵
    1. Tong Y,
    2. Hocke LM,
    3. Fan X,
    4. Janes AC,
    5. Frederick B
    (2015) Can apparent resting state connectivity arise from systemic fluctuations? Front Hum Neurosci 9:285. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00285 pmid:26029095
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  111. ↵
    1. Turnbull A,
    2. Wang HT,
    3. Murphy C,
    4. Ho NS,
    5. Wang X,
    6. Sormaz M,
    7. Karapanagiotidis T,
    8. Leech RM,
    9. Bernhardt B,
    10. Margulies DS,
    11. Vatansever D,
    12. Jefferies E,
    13. Smallwood J
    (2019) Left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex supports context-dependent prioritisation of off-task thought. Nat Commun 10:3816. doi:10.1038/s41467-019-11764-y pmid:31444333
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  112. ↵
    1. Turnbull A,
    2. Karapanagiotidis T,
    3. Wang HT,
    4. Bernhardt BC,
    5. Leech R,
    6. Margulies D,
    7. Schooler J,
    8. Jefferies E,
    9. Smallwood J
    (2020) Reductions in task positive neural systems occur with the passage of time and are associated with changes in ongoing thought. Sci Rep 10:9912. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-66698-z pmid:32555212
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  113. ↵
    1. Van Essen DC,
    2. Ugurbil K,
    3. Auerbach E,
    4. Barch D,
    5. Behrens TE,
    6. Bucholz R,
    7. Chang A,
    8. Chen L,
    9. Corbetta M,
    10. Curtiss SW,
    11. Della Penna S,
    12. Feinberg D,
    13. Glasser MF,
    14. Harel N,
    15. Heath AC,
    16. Larson-Prior L,
    17. Marcus D,
    18. Michalareas G,
    19. Moeller S,
    20. Oostenveld R, et al
    . (2012) The Human Connectome Project: a data acquisition perspective. Neuroimage 62:2222–2231. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.018 pmid:22366334
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  114. ↵
    1. Vase L,
    2. Norskov KN,
    3. Petersen GL,
    4. Price DD
    (2011) Patients' direct experiences as central elements of placebo analgesia. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366:1913–1921. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0402 pmid:21576149
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  115. ↵
    1. Vatansever D,
    2. Karapanagiotidis T,
    3. Margulies DS,
    4. Jefferies E,
    5. Smallwood J
    (2020) Distinct patterns of thought mediate the link between brain functional connectomes and well-being. Netw Neurosci 4:637–657. doi:10.1162/netn_a_00137 pmid:32885119
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  116. ↵
    1. Wang HT,
    2. Poerio G,
    3. Murphy C,
    4. Bzdok D,
    5. Jefferies E,
    6. Smallwood J
    (2018a) Dimensions of experience: exploring the heterogeneity of the wandering mind. Psychol Sci 29:56–71. doi:10.1177/0956797617728727 pmid:29131720
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  117. ↵
    1. Wang HT,
    2. Bzdok D,
    3. Margulies D,
    4. Craddock C,
    5. Milham M,
    6. Jefferies E,
    7. Smallwood J
    (2018b) Patterns of thought: population variation in the associations between large-scale network organisation and self-reported experiences at rest. Neuroimage 176:518–527. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.04.064 pmid:29733956
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  118. ↵
    1. Wang HT,
    2. Ho NS,
    3. Bzdok D,
    4. Bernhardt BC,
    5. Margulies DS,
    6. Jefferies E,
    7. Smallwood J
    (2020a) Neurocognitive patterns dissociating semantic processing from executive control are linked to more detailed off-task mental time travel. Sci Rep 10:11904. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-67605-2 pmid:32681101
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  119. ↵
    1. Wang HT,
    2. Smallwood J,
    3. Mourao-Miranda J,
    4. Xia CH,
    5. Satterthwaite TD,
    6. Bassett DS,
    7. Bzdok D
    (2020b) Finding the needle in a high-dimensional haystack: canonical correlation analysis for neuroscientists. Neuroimage 216:116745. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116745 pmid:32278095
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  120. ↵
    1. Winder AT,
    2. Echagarruga C,
    3. Zhang Q,
    4. Drew PJ
    (2017) Weak correlations between hemodynamic signals and ongoing neural activity during the resting state. Nat Neurosci 20:1761–1769. doi:10.1038/s41593-017-0007-y pmid:29184204
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  121. ↵
    World Health Organization (2018) International classification of diseases for mortality and morbidity statistics (11th revision). Geneva: World Health Organization.
  122. ↵
    1. Wu GR,
    2. Liao W,
    3. Stramaglia S,
    4. Ding JR,
    5. Chen H,
    6. Marinazzo D
    (2013) A blind deconvolution approach to recover effective connectivity brain networks from resting state fMRI data. Med Image Anal 17:365–374. doi:10.1016/j.media.2013.01.003 pmid:23422254
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  123. ↵
    1. Wundt WM
    (1907) Über Ausfrageexperimente und über die Methoden zur Psychologie des Denkens. Psychologische Studien 3:301–360.
    OpenUrl
  124. ↵
    1. Wundt WM,
    2. Wirth W
    (1902) Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie, 5. völlig umgearb. Leipzig: Engelmann.
  125. ↵
    1. Zanesco AP
    (2020) Quantifying streams of thought during cognitive task performance using sequence analysis. Behav Res Methods Advance online publication. Retrieved May 18, 2020. doi: 10.3758/s13428-020-01416-1.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  126. ↵
    1. Zhang D,
    2. Snyder AZ,
    3. Fox MD,
    4. Sansbury MW,
    5. Shimony JS,
    6. Raichle ME
    (2008) Intrinsic functional relations between human cerebral cortex and thalamus. J Neurophysiol 100:1740–1748. doi:10.1152/jn.90463.2008 pmid:18701759
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 41 (6)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 41, Issue 6
10 Feb 2021
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
How to Interpret Resting-State fMRI: Ask Your Participants
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
How to Interpret Resting-State fMRI: Ask Your Participants
Javier Gonzalez-Castillo, Julia W.Y. Kam, Colin W. Hoy, Peter A. Bandettini
Journal of Neuroscience 10 February 2021, 41 (6) 1130-1141; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1786-20.2020

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
How to Interpret Resting-State fMRI: Ask Your Participants
Javier Gonzalez-Castillo, Julia W.Y. Kam, Colin W. Hoy, Peter A. Bandettini
Journal of Neuroscience 10 February 2021, 41 (6) 1130-1141; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1786-20.2020
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • fMRI
  • resting-state
  • introspection
  • coactivation patterns
  • ongoing experience
  • consciousness

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

  • RE: How to Interpret Resting-State fMRI: Ask Your Participants
    David A. Booth
    Published on: 11 February 2021
  • Published on: (11 February 2021)
    Page navigation anchor for RE: How to Interpret Resting-State fMRI: Ask Your Participants
    RE: How to Interpret Resting-State fMRI: Ask Your Participants
    • David A. Booth, Research Professor, University of Sussex, UK

    Asking what someone is thinking should not be thought of as gathering "introspection[s]". Thought is not in another world of private experiences. Thinking is a performance that can be made public by the thinker recounting the objective content of their thoughts (e.g., Ericsson KA & Simon HA, 1980, 1993; Lyons; WE, 1986). A person in fMRI, EEG, rTMS etc.should be asked what in the shared world each thought concerns. This is a claim that can be tested when it is made again by the scientific methods of experimental psychology. There is much more to interpreting any set of fMRI data than "fusing" it with spontaneous verbal data. Don't ask just, "What are you thinking?" Ask, "What are you thinking ABOUT?" When that person professes to have those memories, expectations, attributions and so on again, what they say can be measured against performance in a cognitive task on such material.

    Competing Interests: None declared.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • On the Role of Theory and Modeling in Neuroscience
  • The Protective Effect of Social Reward on Opioid and Psychostimulant Reward and Relapse: Behavior, Pharmacology, and Brain Regions
  • Amygdala Intercalated Cells: Gate Keepers and Conveyors of Internal State to the Circuits of Emotion
Show more Viewpoints
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.