Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Systems/Circuits

Maturation of Temporal Saccade Prediction from Childhood to Adulthood: Predictive Saccades, Reduced Pupil Size, and Blink Synchronization

Olivia G. Calancie, Donald C. Brien, Jeff Huang, Brian C. Coe, Linda Booij, Sarosh Khalid-Khan and Douglas P. Munoz
Journal of Neuroscience 5 January 2022, 42 (1) 69-80; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0837-21.2021
Olivia G. Calancie
1Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Donald C. Brien
1Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jeff Huang
1Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Brian C. Coe
1Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Linda Booij
2Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, Quebec H4B 1R6, Canada
3Department of Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3L3, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sarosh Khalid-Khan
1Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada
4Division of Child and Youth Mental Health, Kingston Health Sciences Centre, Kingston, Ontario K7L 5G2, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Douglas P. Munoz
1Centre for Neuroscience Studies, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada
5Department of Biomedical and Molecular Sciences, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

When presented with a periodic stimulus, humans spontaneously adjust their movements from reacting to predicting the timing of its arrival, but little is known about how this sensorimotor adaptation changes across development. To investigate this, we analyzed saccade behavior in 114 healthy humans (ages 6–24 years) performing the visual metronome task, who were instructed to move their eyes in time with a visual target that alternated between two known locations at a fixed rate, and we compared their behavior to performance in a random task, where target onsets were randomized across five interstimulus intervals (ISIs) and thus the timing of appearance was unknown. Saccades initiated before registration of the visual target, thus in anticipation of its appearance, were labeled predictive [saccade reaction time (SRT) < 90 ms] and saccades that were made in reaction to its appearance were labeled reactive (SRT > 90 ms). Eye-tracking behavior including saccadic metrics (e.g., peak velocity, amplitude), pupil size following saccade to target, and blink behavior all varied as a function of predicting or reacting to periodic targets. Compared with reactive saccades, predictive saccades had a lower peak velocity, a hypometric amplitude, smaller pupil size, and a reduced probability of blink occurrence before target appearance. The percentage of predictive and reactive saccades changed inversely from ages 8–16, at which they reached adult-levels of behavior. Differences in predictive saccades for fast and slow target rates are interpreted by differential maturation of cerebellar-thalamic-striatal pathways.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT From the first moments of life, humans are exposed to rhythm (i.e., mother's heartbeat in utero), but the timeline of brain development to promote the identification and anticipation of a rhythmic stimulus, known as temporal prediction, remains unknown. Here, we used saccade reaction time (SRT) in the visual metronome task to differentiate between temporally predictive and reactive responses to a target that alternated at a fixed rate in humans aged 6–24. Periods of age-related change varied little by target rate, with matured predictive performance evident by mid-adolescence for fast and slow rates. A strong correlation among saccade, pupil, and blink responses during target prediction provides evidence of oculomotor coordination and dampened noradrenergic neuronal activity when generating rhythmic motor responses.

  • blink rate
  • development
  • eye movements
  • pupil diameter
  • rhythm
  • timing

Introduction

From an early age, we are exposed to rhythms in our environment. In the womb, we listen to our mother's heartbeat, and as an infant, we find comfort in the swaying of a rocking chair. Attending to stimuli with periodicity has been shown to induce physiological relaxation (Johnson and Trawick, 1938; Brauchli et al., 1995). Identifying rhythmicity and shifting motor behavior from reacting to predicting an upcoming stimulus, known as temporal prediction, occurs spontaneously and is fundamental for adaptive sensorimotor behavior (Fitch, 2013; van der Steen and Keller, 2013). Neural maintenance of an interstimulus interval (ISI) and coordination of endogenously driven motor commands to anticipate its arrival are required to predict rhythmic stimuli (Repp and Su, 2013). Brain regions important for timing-related signals include the frontal cortex (Maimon and Assad, 2006; Jazayeri and Shadlen, 2015), basal ganglia (Lee and Assad, 2003; Turner and Anderson, 2005), cerebellum (Ashmore and Sommer, 2013), and thalamus (Matsuyama and Tanaka, 2021). Neuronal firing in these areas correlates with the ISI of a periodic target, and pharmacological silencing impairs predictive movements (Buhusi and Meck, 2005; Merchant et al., 2013; Matsuyama and Tanaka, 2021; Tanaka et al., 2021). Additionally, an impaired ability to anticipate rhythmic stimuli to respond predictively is evident in multiple brain-related disorders, including developmental coordination disorder (Debrabant et al., 2013), developmental dyslexia (Lukasova et al., 2016), Huntington's disease (Vaca-Palomares et al., 2019), and spinocerebellar atrophy (Bares et al., 2007). Despite the important role temporal prediction plays in essential sensorimotor learning, and its clinical relevance to various neurologic conditions, its developmental timeline in normal controls remains unknown.

One commonly used method of measuring temporal prediction is the visual metronome task (Stark et al., 1962), where participants are asked to move their eyes in time with a square-wave target that alternates at a fixed rate between two known locations. Saccades initiated before neural registration of the visual target, in anticipation of its upcoming temporal appearance, are categorized as predictive [saccade reaction time (SRT) < 90 ms; Fischer et al., 1997; Munoz et al., 1998; Lee et al., 2016). Saccades initiated in response to the visual target are considered reactive (SRT > 90 ms). Analysis of saccade behavior in this task is particularly useful for estimation of brain functioning of areas relevant to temporal prediction given that they overlap with brain regions required for saccade initiation (i.e., brainstem, cerebellum, superior colliculus (SC), thalamus, basal ganglia and frontal cortex). Based on a handful of studies using the visual metronome task in children [2–53 child participants per study (age range: 4–15 years)], it is clear that compared with adults, children make fewer predictive saccades and on average, have SRTs that are on average, 100–150 ms slower (Kowler and Martins, 1982; Ross and Ross, 1987; Ross et al., 1994; Lukasova et al., 2018). However, given the small sample sizes of these studies and paucity of adolescent participant data, normal predictive saccade performance from early childhood to young adulthood has yet to be characterized.

To investigate temporal predictive performance across developmental ages, we measured predictive saccades in 114 healthy participants, aged 6–24 years, performing the metronome task at five different target rates. Because pupil size has reliably been shown to correlate with noradrenergic modulation by the locus coeruleus norepinephrine (LC-NE) system (Alnæs et al., 2014; Murphy et al., 2014), we sought to test whether previous reports of autonomic relaxation while attending to rhythm (Johnson and Trawick, 1938; Brauchli et al., 1995) could be replicated based on evidence of a decreased pupil size when participants predict versus react to targets, indicative of dampened LC-NE signaling. Finally, we tested whether eyeblink timing varied based on participant prediction or reaction to targets. To ensure that age-related changes in performance were not driven by improvements in processing speed or oculomotor kinematics, participants also performed a random task, which is identical to the metronome task, except the timing of the target is randomly selected for each target step, making the target steps unpredictable.

Materials and Methods

Study participants

The research protocol was approved by Queen's University Faculty of Health Sciences (protocol ID: PHYS-007-97). A total of 118 participants completed the study (mean age: 14.9 ± 3.8 years; 55.1% female). Adults aged ≥18 years provided their written consent. Children aged <18 years gave their verbal assent and parents/legal guardians provided written informed consent. All participants were free of neurologic, psychiatric or ocular diagnoses and were not taking psychotropic medications, and participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.

Recording of eye movements

Participants were seated 60 cm away from a computer screen with their head position stabilized by a fixed head mount and chin rest at a viewing angle of 32 × 26°. Experimental stimuli were presented as 0.5° diameter sized circular red targets (luminance measured 44 cd/m2 with an optometer for LCD monitors) on a 17-inch LCD iiYama Prolite monitor at a screen resolution of 1280 × 1024 pixels with a 60-Hz refresh rate. Experiments were completed in the dark with the only light source being the experimentally controlled stimuli. Monocular eye tracking was conducted (right eye position was measured) using the video-based eye tracker EyeLink 1000 Plus in 83 participants and the EyeLink II in 35 participants (unpublished data previously collected in the lab that used the same experimental task; SR-Research Ltd.). Both cameras of the EyeLink systems had a sampling rate of 500 Hz and a mean eye position accuracy of ≤0.5°. Eye movements to a nine-point calibration grid were performed by each subject before each experiment, with eye position accuracy within 1° of the visual target considered acceptable. Eye position was reassessed for drift after every five trials of target presentation and calibration was performed again if needed.

Experimental design

Participants were cued with a red central fixation point (FP) on a blank screen that had a random offset (i.e., the FP disappeared) between 1 and 1.5 s. Coincident with the disappearance of the FP, a peripheral target appeared 10° right or left from the central FP, on the horizontal axis. The target alternated between the two fixed locations on the horizontal plane for a total of 12 target steps (i.e., six in each direction; see Fig. 1A for task design). In the visual metronome task, a square-wave target alternated consistently at one of the five target rates (0.66, 0.8, 1, 1.33, and 2 Hz). These five target rates correspond to the following ISIs: 1500, 1250, 1000, 750, and 500 ms, respectively. Participants were instructed to move their eyes in time with the jumping target given both the target location and timing of the next target were predictable after the first presentation. In the random task, one of the five ISIs used in the metronome task was randomly used for each target step, such that the target location was predictable, but the timing of the next target appearance was unpredictable. The randomization was set so that the same ISI could not appear more than three times in a row. Thus, the only difference between the metronome and random task was the temporal predictability of target appearance. Participants were offered a break in between the two saccade tasks to assure alertness. The order of the metronome and random tasks was counterbalanced across participants. All stimulus timings were verified independently with a photosensor.

Eye-tracking analysis

Offline analyses of eye-tracking data were performed using MATLAB version R2019b (MathWorks). Saccades were identified based on their instantaneous velocity which was calculated on a three-point moving average of x and y eye positions in degrees. When the instantaneous velocity exceeded 2 SD above the mean fixation velocity (defined as <50°/s) for at least five continuous points, a saccade was labeled.

SRT

SRT was computed by subtracting the time of peripheral target appearance from the time of primary saccade onset. Saccades were labeled as predictive, express, and regular based on their SRTs, being <90, 90–120, and >120 ms, respectively (Fig. 1B,C; Fischer and Ramsperger, 1984; Fischer and Weber, 1993; Dorris and Munoz, 1998; Munoz et al., 1998). The 90-ms cutoff for predictive saccades was based on data by Munoz et al. (1998) which demonstrated that pro-saccades to one of two potential target locations were correct only 50% of the time when initiated before 90 ms after target appearance, whereas those with a SRT >90 ms were correct >95% of the time. Therefore, 90 ms is the lower limit of time to allow visual processing and motor reaction to a peripheral external target, and saccades with SRTs below this cutoff are internally generated. Express and regular saccades are both reactive (e.g., made in response to a visual target), but have unique SRT distributions. Express saccades are the fastest visually evoked eye-movements and are generated when the incoming visual transient signal to the SC is transformed directly into a saccade motor command (Edelman and Keller, 1996; Dorris et al., 1997). Previous research supports that the express saccade epoch varies based on the specific parameters of an eye-tracking task, for example variations to target luminance can alter the timing of visual response latencies in the SC (Marino et al., 2015). Moreover, it is known that variables including target eccentricity, target predictability, previous training, and the presence or absence of a gap between fixation and target appearance all influence the probability of express saccades in a given behavioral task (Weber et al., 1992; Fischer and Weber, 1993; Paré and Munoz, 1996; Dorris and Munoz, 1998). Thereby we sought to quantify the SRT range of express saccades in the metronome and random task, respectively, and later assess the effect of target properties (e.g., rate) on their occurrence. We plotted the distribution of saccades with a SRT >90 ms made in the metronome and random task in Figure 1C. A distinct dip in the SRT distribution is seen at 120 ms for both experimental tasks, supported by a bimodality coefficient (BC) of 0.63 in the metronome task and 0.54 in the random task. Correspondingly, the express epoch range for saccades in the current paper was defined as 90–120 ms, inclusive, and regular saccades were defined as those with reaction times >120 ms. The percentage of predictive, express, and regular saccades were computed for each participant for comparison across trial conditions (metronome task's five target rate conditions and random task; six conditions in total), as well as analyzed across participant age.

Saccade metrics

Saccadic amplitude and peak velocity were analyzed for predictive, express, and regular saccades to all target steps made in the metronome task. To ensure quality of saccade data, we removed saccades with a maximum peak velocity value above 1000°/s (N = 81/34 497; 0.23% of saccades), while still including any remaining saccades in the trial if they had a peak velocity below 1000°/s. To compare the main sequence for predictive, express, and regular saccades, peak velocity versus amplitude were plotted for each saccade type. A square root model (y=Vx ) was used to fit the main sequence data, based on the results of a recent paper by Gibaldi and Sabatini (2021), which demonstrated that this model is highly robust for characterizing the main sequence of saccades with eccentricities between 5° and 20°. Metronome main sequence fits were derived at a group level (e.g., all saccades in the metronome task combined and categorized by saccade type) and at an individual subject level (e.g., an individual subject's model coefficients for predictive, express, and regular saccades generated in the metronome task). Square root model coefficients were then analyzed by saccade type and across participants' age to test whether (1) the model fit for the main sequence varied by saccade type, and (2) whether the main sequence for each saccade type varied with participant age.

Pupil size

To evaluate whether physiological arousal varied based on the reaction time to the alternating target, we analyzed pupil size in the metronome task. Pupil size, recorded every 2 ms, was averaged over a 200 ms epoch following the end of a saccade, provided the participant remained fixated on that location for a minimum of 200 ms. This window length was selected to avoid any pupil size changes induced by the pupillary light reflex if the target appeared postsaccade (i.e., a source of light stimulates pupil constriction ∼300 ms following its appearance on a dark background; Ellis, 1981; Wang et al., 2018). Pupil trials that met the minimum fixation length criterion were excluded from analysis if a blink or saccade occurred during peripheral target fixation, if the timing of fixation onset exceeded the minimum gap (100 ms) between fixation and next target appearance, and if pupil velocity fell outside the range of −5000°/s to 5000°/s. This left 19175 viable pupil trials in the metronome task to analyze (2 Hz: 4093 trials; 1.33 Hz: 3904; 1 Hz: 3845 trials; 0.8 Hz: 3794 trials; and 0.66 Hz: 3539 trials), and these trials were divided into three categories based on SRT: predictive (6094 trials), express (2384 trials), and regular (10697 trials). To query whether pupil size varied based on prediction or reaction to the rhythmic target, two analyses were performed. First, a one-way ANOVA was computed to test for a main effect of saccade type (predictive, express, and regular) on pupil size. Second, saccade types were combined to test whether SRT correlated with pupil size using a Spearman rank correlation. To test whether pupil size varied across the developmental age range, baseline pupil size for predictive, express, and regular saccades were analyzed per individual subject.

Blink metrics

Inspection of loss of eye-tracking revealed a consistent profile of blinks with a duration of 50–300 ms, which is supported by the literature (Caffier et al., 2003; Betke and Chau, 2005). Frequency of blinks during the presence of right and left peripheral targets in the visual metronome task were compared to examine whether participants exercised a spatial bias. No bias was observed (1876 and 1864 blinks made during right and left targets, respectively; χ2 test of proportions = 0.06, p = 0.80) and blinks made during either target direction were collapsed for analysis. Blink rate was recorded for every trial starting 1 s before the appearance of the first target, continuing to the trial's end. Blinks that were not yet completed by the end of the trial were excluded from analysis as their duration was unknown. The number of blinks was summed per trial and divided by the trial length to compute blink rate (blinks/min) per participant.

To evaluate the timing of blinks in the metronome task, blink reaction times were characterized relative to target appearance for each target rate. The probability that participants made a blink on a given trial was computed for three epochs relative to target appearance (0 ms): −1000 to 1000, −1000 to 0, and 0 to +1000 ms. Blink probabilities were then categorized by the type of saccade (predict; express; regular) generated within −1000 and +1000 ms of target appearance and analyzed to understand whether the timing of blinks on a given trial varied based on whether the participant predicted or reacted to the target.

Statistical analysis and model selection

Multivariate tests revealed no difference in parameters between the Eyelink tracking systems [multivariate test of participant mean saccade SRT, amplitude and peak velocity (F(18,94) = 1.19, Wilk's λ = 0.815, p = 0.288), participant mean pupil size for predictive, express, and regular saccades by system (F(3,108) = 0.90, Wilk's λ = 0.975, p = 0.442), and mean blink rate for the five target rate conditions by system (F(5,108) = 1.50, Wilk's λ = 0.935, p = 0.195)]. Therefore, eye-tracking data were collapsed for analysis. Generalized additive models (GAMs; Wood, 2017) were used to estimate the effect of age on eye-tracking parameters. GAMs were selected for their semiparametric nature, robustness to overfitting, and ability to query age-related effects across adolescence without assuming the shape of the developmental trajectory (Wierenga et al., 2019; Luna et al., 2021). Age was entered as a smooth function in the GAM model yi=β0+sλAgei+errori with β0 denoting the random intercepts, sλ as the smoothness parameter of the Age of the individual i and individual error as errori . Smoothing parameter estimation was performed with the restricted maximum likelihood method (REML), as it is less prone to undersmoothing than other criteria (e.g., GCV, AIC, and UBRE; Wood, 2011). From a Bayesian perspective, GAM's computed smoothing penalty λ (k = 9) acted as a prior for coefficients of the basis functions to improve the generalizability of the developmental curves. Gender was not entered as a variable in the GAM models given it was not evenly distributed across the adolescent age range (i.e., more females were recruited for participation in a separate eye-tracking study that was run in parallel), and previous studies with child participants have reported no differences in eye-tracking metrics (for review, see Salman et al., 2006).

To identify periods of statistically significant developmental change and age of maturation, the uncertainty of each estimated model was calculated using confidence intervals. Confidence intervals (95%) were computed via posterior simulation, a process described by Wood (2017) and previously implemented to identify periods of age-related changes across adolescence in white matter and brain functional connectivity (Simmonds et al., 2014; Calabro et al., 2020). During posterior simulation, 1000 random draws from a multivariate normal distribution whose vector of means and covariance corresponded to the fitted GAM parameters were taken, therefore each random draw represented a new trend that was compatible with the fitted trend but also reflected the uncertainty in the estimated trend (Simpson, 2018). The first derivative of the GAM fit was computed (at 0.1-year age intervals) for the 1000 random draws to identify time points xt equal to zero, consistent with the null hypothesis of no change. Significant periods of age-related change were identified when the (simultaneous) confidence interval of the first simulated derivative did not include zero (p < 0.05). Points of maturation were identified as the maximum age when the confidence intervals were nonzero. GAM fit parameters (adjusted r2, deviance explained, and p value) were reported for each statistically significant model, as well as the period of significant age-related change and maturation point. The Gaussian family of GAM models was used and modeling was performed with the mgcv package in R (Wood, 2009).

In general, statistical analyses were performed on population-level (i.e., averaged participant responses) data for saccade, blink, and pupil responses and on an individual trial level (i.e., trials with predictive saccades, for example). Before any group-level analysis was performed, normality of response parameters was assessed using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. Parametric tests were performed for normally distributed data and nonparametric tests were performed for skewed data. Means, SDs, and confidence intervals were reported for statistically significant main effects. All main effects were further assessed using t tests and reported significant p values were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni method. t tests and regression analyses were two-tailed.

Results

Data were excluded from three participants in the random task and four participants in the visual metronome task because of poor quality eye-tracking. Therefore, statistical analyses were performed on 115 participants in the random task and 114 participants in the metronome task (see Fig. 1D for the distribution of participants' age and gender).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

A, In the metronome task (first developed by Stark et al., 1962), participants were cued with a central FP with a random interval offset (1000–1500 ms) followed by 12 targets that alternate 10° right (R) and left (L) from center at a fixed target rate. Five target rates (0.66, 0.8, 1.0, 1.33, 2.0 Hz) were delivered over five blocks of trials with 12 targets each (60 targets per stimulus rate), with a pseudorandom trial order. These target rates correspond to an ISI of 1500, 1250, 1000, 750, and 500 ms, respectively. Participants were instructed to move their eyes in time with the targets. In the random task, target rates were randomized such that the participant could not anticipate the timing of the upcoming target while all other aspects of the task were held constant. B, Schematic of the characterization of saccades by SRT: predictive (SRT < 90 ms) in dark blue, express (SRT: 90–120 ms) in light blue, and regular (SRT > 120 ms) in red. C, Example eye position data collected from a single subject over a single trial. Colored bars correspond with the type of saccade generated according to its SRT toward the alternating target. D, The distribution of saccades with a SRT > 90 ms in the metronome and random task is bimodal, with an express saccade epoch from 90 to 120 ms (solid light blue line) and regular saccade SRTs being >120 ms in both task conditions. The BCs for the distribution of SRTs >90 ms in the metronome and random task were 0.63 (skewness = 4.7; kurtosis = 33.5) and 0.54 (skewness = 4.3; kurtosis = 33.6), respectively. E, Histogram of participants' ages in the metronome and random task that were included for experimental analysis (bin width = one year). White boxes represent female participants and dark blue boxes represent males.

Task metrics

SRT

Figure 2 shows all participants' SRTs to target steps 1–12 for each trial of the visual metronome task (∼7000 saccades per target frequency; 34497 total saccades) and the random task (35811 saccades). In the metronome task, the first three target steps largely consisted of regular and express saccades (SRT > 90 ms), whereas saccades to target steps 4–12 showed an increased incidence of predictive behavior (SRT < 90 ms). Participants therefore used the first approximately three target steps to identify the rate of the alternating target before launching predictive saccades to anticipate the timing of the next target's appearance, in agreement with previously published predictive saccade results (Joiner and Shelhamer, 2006; Zorn et al., 2007). Nonparametric kernel density curves of SRTs in Figure 2A (predictive in red; express in light blue; regular in dark blue) show the probability of a given reaction time in the three saccade categories. Although the SRT range for each category was unique, the kernel nature of the density curves displays an artificial overlap among distributions. As the rate of the alternating target slowed from 2 to 0.66 Hz, the probability peak of predictive saccades correspondingly decreased and became broader (Fig. 2, red traces).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

All subjects' SRTs in the metronome and random tasks. Individual subjects' SRT to each target step is plotted from the top to the bottom according to the trial number. For example, an individual subject's SRT to target step 1 in trial 1 is plotted on top of the figure, followed by that same individual's SRT to trial 2's target step 1 and so forth until trial 5's target step 1; then the next subject's SRT data to target step 1 is plotted. This is continued for all subjects across the five blocks of 12 target steps. Frequency of saccade type is visualized with a kernel density estimator. The bandwidth of the kernel smoothing window for SRTs was optimized by MATLAB's scatterhist function. SRT in ms. Predictive saccades (red; SRT < 90 ms), express saccade (light blue; SRT: 90–120 ms), regular saccade (dark blue; SRT > 120 ms).

A main effect of metronome target rate was observed for percentage of regular (F(4,565) = 25.70, p = 1.39e-19, effect size η2 = 0.154) and predictive saccades (F(4,565) = 22.16, p = 5.18e-17, effect size η2 = 0.136) but not express saccades (F(4,565) = 1.26, p = 0.283). t tests revealed that the percentage of predictive saccades significantly varied among the following frequencies: 2 Hz [mean percentage: 55.6% (SD = 22.3)] and 0.8 Hz [42.2% (20.5); p = 3.10e-05], 2 and 0.66 Hz [34.9% (20.0); p = 1.09e-11], 1.33 Hz [57.5% (22.9)] and 0.8 Hz (p = 1.00e-06), 1.33 and 0.66 Hz (p = 9.89e-14), and 1 Hz [50.1% (21.1)] and 0.66 Hz (p = 1.00e-06). Target rates that differed by the percentage of regular saccades included: 2 Hz [33.9% (19.1)] and 0.8 Hz [47.7% (21.3); p = 6.00e-06], 2 and 0.66 Hz [56.7% (21.0); p = 8.13e-15], 1.33 Hz [33.6% (21.3)] and 0.8 Hz (p = 4.00e-06), 1.33 and 0.66 Hz (p = 3.86e-15), and 1 and 0.66 Hz (p = 2.25e-08). In summary, as target rates slowed, participants generated fewer predictive saccades and more regular saccades, while the frequency of express saccades remained unchanged.

Age and SRT

GAM models, corrected for multiple comparisons (required p value for significance = 0.05/5 target rate conditions; p = 0.01), revealed a significant relationship among age and percentage of predictive saccades for the following target rates in the metronome task (Fig. 3, red data points and traces; adjusted r2 values and period of age-related changes are reported in the figure): 2 Hz (deviance explained = 15.5%, p = 0.0011), 1.33 Hz (22.5%, p = 2.29e-05), 1 Hz (10.1%, p = 0.00843), and 0.66 Hz (8.58%, p = 0.01). Age of maturation for predicting short and medium target rates (2, 1.33, 1 Hz) were highly similar (∼14 years), whereas maturation for the longest target rate (0.66 Hz) was delayed until age 16. There was no significant age × percentage of predictive saccade effect observed for 0.8 Hz (p = 0.10).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Age-related changes of temporal saccadic prediction according to each of the metronome task's target frequencies (A–E) and in the random task (F). GAMs were applied (% of predict, express, regular × age) for each target condition and corrected for multiple comparisons. Solid lines represent significant interactions between percentage of saccade type and age (p < 0.01). 95% confidence intervals are shown as semi-transparent banding along developmental curves of statistical significance. Lines without shaded confidence bands reflect models of nonsignificance. Adjusted r2 values and periods of significant age-related change are reported for each significant model in the upper right panel of the plots. The maturation point is the upper limit of the significant period of age-related change where task performance reached adult-level performance. Predictive saccades (red; SRT < 90 ms), express saccade (light blue; SRT: 90–120 ms), regular saccade (dark blue; SRT > 120 ms).

The paucity of predictive saccades to rhythmic targets in young children across all presentation rates coincided with a higher prevalence of regular saccades. GAM models revealed a significant inverse relationship between age and percentage of regular saccades in all target rates of the metronome task (Fig. 3, dark blue data points and traces). Significant periods of age-related change for percentage of regular saccades in the 2-Hz condition were observed from 7.5–14.1 years (22.8%, p = 1.87e-05), 7.6–13.8 years for 1.33 Hz (30.5%, p = 3.91e-07), and 7.4.−16.7 years for 1 Hz (14.0%, p = 5.12e-04), 6.5–23 years for 0.8 Hz (7.51%, p = 0.00319), and 7.8–17.3 years for 0.66 Hz (13.2%, p = 6.51e-04). The percentage of express saccades × age GAM models revealed a significant effect of age on express saccades for targets that alternated at 0.8 Hz (Fig. 3D, light blue data points and traces), with 8.98% deviance explained (p = 0.00121). In the random task, a significant interaction between age and the percentage of express saccades was observed (14.1%, p = 0.00914).

Metronome saccade metrics and age

A multivariate ANOVA was run with individuals' mean saccade amplitude and peak velocity toward all target steps entered as dependent variables and saccade type in the metronome task as the independent variable. A main effect of saccade type was observed on the saccade metrics [F(4,674) = 28.89 (p = 3.49e-22), partial η2 = 0.146]. As expected (Bronstein and Kennard, 1987; Evans et al., 1999; Shelhamer and Joiner, 2003; Wong and Shelhamer, 2011) predictive saccades were hypometric [mean = 17.7° (SE = 0.16)] relative to the 20° targets, likely because the target appearance was not yet visually processed when the saccade was initiated, resulting in increased neural noise in the brainstem for the saccadic command. Regular saccades were also hypometric [16.4° (0.16)], although we attribute this to the abundance of regular latency saccades to the first target step 10° away from FP (see Fig. 4D). Amplitudes differed among predictive and regular saccades (p = 8.70e-09), express [18.0° (0.16)] and regular saccades (p = 2.90e-12), but not among predictive and express saccades. Peak velocity differed among all saccade types (mean peak velocity for predictive saccades = 433.9°/s; express = 496.4°/s; regular = 459.1°/s; p < 0.001).

To evaluate the slope of the main sequence by saccade type, a square root model (y=Vx ) was fit to all saccades made in the metronome task with a peak velocity <1000°/s, resulting in 34416 saccades (Fig. 4A–D). As can be seen, compared with the express epoch (Fig. 4C), there are a significant number of saccades in the regular epoch (Fig. 4D) with amplitudes around 10°, contributing to the previously reported difference in amplitude among express and regular saccades. Given that peak velocity changes with saccadic amplitude, we evaluated whether the participants' mean peak velocity varied among saccade types for saccades with amplitudes that ranged between 18° and 21°, inclusive. Indeed a main effect of saccade type was still observed F(2,325) = 22.12 (p = 9.88e-10); however, post hoc t tests revealed that at these amplitudes, peak velocity for express and regular saccades did not differ (p = 0.67), whereas predictive and express (p < 0.001) and predictive and regular saccades (p < 0.001) did. Indeed, predictive saccades are known to have slower peak velocities compared with reactive saccades (Bronstein and Kennard, 1987; Smit and Van Gisbergen, 1989).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

A, Square root model (y=Vx ) was fit to estimate the main sequence of predictive (in red), express (in light blue), and regular saccades (in dark blue) made by all subjects across the five target rates in the metronome task. Model fits significantly varied among nonvisually triggered predictive and express saccades, and predictive and regular saccades, but not among express and regular saccades. Raw datapoints of saccadic peak velocity and amplitude used to calculate the main sequence model fits are shown for predictive (B), express (C), and regular (D) saccades. A kernel density estimate was applied to the scatterplot data in B–D to visualize the density of the datapoints, with warmer colors indicating increased density. E, Square root models were performed for predictive, express, and regular saccades made in the metronome task by each individual subject. Participants' main sequence model coefficients are plotted on the y-axis, and their age is plotted on the x-axis with lines representing linear polynomial curves. Spearman rank correlations revealed no significant association between age and main sequence model fits for participant age and predictive saccades (ρ = −0.07, p = 0.43), express saccades (ρ = −0.01, p = 0.90), or regular saccades (ρ = 0.02, p = 0.79).

Square root model fits were derived to estimate the main sequence of individual subjects' predictive, express, and regular saccades (including all amplitudes) in Figure 4B–D and a Kruskal–Wallis test was performed to test whether they shared the same underlying distribution. A main effect of saccade type was observed on the model fits (χ2(2,336) = 47.76, p =4.26e-11), where model coefficients varied between predictive and express saccades (p < 0.001) and predictive and regular saccades (p < 0.001), but not between express and regular saccades (p = 0.81; Fig. 4A). Spearman ρ correlations were run for age and the main sequence model fits for the three saccade types, and as expected (Fischer et al., 1997; Munoz et al., 1998; Luna et al., 2001), there was no observed association with age (Fig. 4E).

Pupil size, temporal prediction, and age

All participant pupil size values were combined (Fig. 5C) and categorized according to their saccade (e.g., predictive, express, regular) to target on given metronome trial (Fig. 5A). A Kruskal–Wallis test supported a main effect of saccade type on pupil size in the metronome task (χ2(2) = 369.82, p = 4.94e-81), with post hoc t tests supporting differences among all saccade types (p < 0.001; see Fig. 5A,B). In Figure 5C, participants' corresponding pupil size for all metronome trials that met the pupillary analysis criteria are plotted against the RT of the saccade to target, showing an increased density of bigger pupil sizes at around the time that the visual transient signal is converted to a saccade motor command (SRT > 90 ms). A significant positive Spearman's rank-order correlation was observed between pupil size and SRT (rs = 0.1409, p = 1.306e-85), with an increased pupil size for longer saccade latencies to periodic targets. Individual participant's pupil size was measured on each trial and averaged based on their SRT category (predictive, express, and regular). GAM models were applied to estimate how pupil size changed with age across the three saccade categories. Indeed, pupil size significantly varied with age for predictive saccades (adjusted r2 = 0.118, deviance explained = 12.6%, p = 0.000115), express saccades (adjusted r2 = 0.0985, deviance explained = 10.7%, p = 0.000421) and regular saccades (adjusted r2 = 0.12, deviance explained = 12.8%, p = 9.29e-05; see Fig. 5D). As can be seen with the GAM model fits, the difference among pupil size by saccade type is not observed at an individual-subject level. The SD for pupil size was compared across participants and no significant difference was observed across saccade types (χ2(2) = 4.44, p = 0.1084).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

On metronome trials when participants fixated for a minimum of 200 ms following the completion of saccade to target, pupil size was recorded at 2-ms intervals (Eyelink camera sampling rate: 500 Hz) to test whether sympathetic arousal, indicated by pupil size, varied based on the participant's reaction time to square-wave target. Metronome trials that met the criteria for pupillary analysis were combined across subjects and target frequencies are plotted in A–C. Mean pupil size (A), from the time of the saccade completion (time point: 0 ms) to 200 ms following the saccade, significantly differed according to saccade type (predictive in red; express in light blue; regular in dark blue), with smaller pupil sizes observed for predictive versus reactive saccades [mean pupil size (in pixels) for predictive saccades = 2.67e+03, express = 2.937e+03, and regular = 2.9702e+03]. B, Variability in pupil sizes averaged in A is shown in a cumulative probability plot. Analysis of the SD of participants' pupil size revealed that they did not significantly differ by saccade type. C, Participants' pupil sizes positively correlated with the reaction time of their saccade to target (rs = 0.1409, p = 1.306e-85), with the density of datapoints visualized on a jet color scale from cool to warm using a kernel density estimate. D, Age-related decreases in pupil size were observed for all three saccade types (p < 0.001) and were highly consistent in their slope (estimated using GAMs).

Blinks

Inspection of blink and saccade probability density plots in Figure 6A revealed an anti-correlated relationship between saccades and blinks. During visual fixation, participants show the highest rate of blinking, which is reduced during target steps 1 and 2, and increased again for target steps 3–12. Participants' mean blink rate did not vary according to the metronome task's target frequency (p = 0.323; Fig. 6B), nor did it vary among metronome target rates and the random task. On the other hand, the timing of blink onsets relative to target appearance in the metronome task significantly varied (χ2(4) = 203.73, p = 5.91e-43; see Fig. 6C). Multiple comparison tests showed differences in blink reaction times (relative to target appearance) between all target frequencies at the p < 0.001 level (participants' median blink reaction times: 262.2 ms for 2 Hz, 417.5 ms for 1.33 Hz, 501.3 for 1 Hz, 584.7 for 0.8 Hz, and 678.8 for 0.66 Hz), with the exception of frequencies 1.33 and 1 Hz (p = 0.0155), 1 and 0.8 Hz (p = 0.0108), and 0.8 and 0.66 Hz (p = 0.0569). The relationship among participants' age and their corresponding mean blink rate and average blink reaction time to target was queried tested using Spearman's rank correlation coefficient tests across the metronome target frequencies and corrected for multiple comparisons. No correlation was observed among participants' age and blink reaction time (range of ρ values: −0.005–0.20). Similarly, no relationship was observed among participants' age and mean blink rate with the exception of target frequency 0.66 Hz (ρ = 0.34; p = 5.15e-04).

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

A, Diagram of blinks (in purple) and saccades (in gray) made toward the alternating target of the metronome task at target frequency 0.66 Hz. Blinks and saccades are displayed such that each horizontal line marks a unique trial from a participant, making up 570 occurrences (114 participants × 5 trials of the alternating target at a rate of 0.66 Hz), and gray borders mark the timing of the target appearance. Histogram of blink probability (in purple) is scaled up by a factor of 5 so blink and saccade probabilities can be viewed on the same plot. B, Moving average of blink rate for each of the five metronome frequencies and the random task across trial lengths. C, A main effect of mean blink reaction time by target frequency was observed, with gray bars indicating the frequencies that were significantly different at the post hoc level. Crosses show outliers (±2 SD away from the mean). D–F, Probability of a blink or saccade 1000 ms before and after target appearance (0 ms) collapsed across all metronome trials. To visualize blink and saccade probabilities on the same graph, blink probabilities are multiplied by 20. Color of blink traces reflect the saccade type color scheme [predictive (red), early express (light blue), and regular (dark blue)]. G, Unscaled blink probability categorized by saccade type. A main effect of saccade type was observed on blink probability across the 2000-ms epoch (1000 ms before and 1000 ms after target appearance); χ2(2) = 18.99, p = 7.53e-05.

Mean probabilities of participants making a saccade or blink during the 1000 ms before and after target appearance are plotted in Figure 6D–G, categorized by saccade type. Participant mean blink probability differed by saccade type across the −1000- to +1000-ms epoch relative to target appearance at 0 ms (2000-ms epoch total; χ2(2) = 18.99, p = 7.53e-05]. Multiple comparisons showed that participants were more likely to make a blink during this epoch if they generated a regular saccade to target as opposed to a predictive (p = 0.0059) or express saccade (p = 0.0001). In the 1000-ms period before target appearance, blink probability significantly varied by saccade type (χ2(2) = 32.35, p = 9.43e-08), with post hoc differences among all saccade types (p < 0.001). In the 1000-ms period following target appearance, blink probability also significantly varied by saccade type (χ2(2) = 10.54, p = 0.005), with all saccade types differing at the post hoc level [predictive and express (p = 0.0135), predictive and regular (p < 0.001), and express and regular (p = 0.0135)]. These data support a novel finding in that when participants generated a predictive saccade, blinks were more likely to be inhibited in the 1000 ms prior to target appearance. Indeed, compared to visually triggered saccades, blinks on predictive saccade trials were more likely to occur in the 1000 ms following target appearance. Spearman's rank correlation coefficient tests revealed that blink probability across these tested epochs did not significantly vary with participant age in any of the saccade categories.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to better understand temporal prediction of visual metronome targets in typically developing children, adolescents, and young adults. Temporal prediction significantly correlated with age in four of the five metronome target rates with adult-level performance being reached by age 16. Smaller pupil sizes were observed following a predictive versus reactive saccade, supporting autonomic relaxation when anticipating rhythmic stimuli. Blink rate did not differ among metronome and random targets, however, blink timing varied with target frequency and saccade type; blinks occurred at the middle of the ISI and were suppressed before target appearance on trials when participants predicted the target. Together, these results support a strong age effect on the temporal prediction of periodic targets and a coordination among saccade, pupil, and blink metrics.

Maturation timeline of saccadic temporal prediction

Predictive saccades increased with age for nearly all target rates in the metronome task (Fig. 3A–E, red curves). This was not observed in the random task (Fig. 3F), indicating that changes in performance were specific to conditions that had rhythmic stimuli, rather than any changes in anticipatory saccades toward visual targets in general. Developmental curves for predictive and regular saccades across target rates were inversely related, illustrating a shared maturation timeline; while the percentage of express saccades changed only slightly with age (Fig. 3D,F). These data are in agreement with previous reports that young children (<12 years) made fewer predictive saccades than adults to target frequencies 0.5–1.33 Hz (Kowler and Martins, 1982; Ross and Ross, 1987; Lukasova et al., 2018). Similar to adult studies (Shelhamer and Joiner, 2003; Isotalo et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2016), we found that the highest rates of saccadic prediction occurred toward target frequencies of 1.33 and 2 Hz. Likewise, in subjects aged 4–38 years, the preferred motor tempi for spontaneous finger tapping was found to range from 1–2 Hz (McAuley et al., 2006). (McAuley et al., 2006). This frequency range closely aligns with the human heart rate (80 BPM = 1.33 Hz), raising interesting questions about whether individuals can more adeptly predict tempos that synch to their heart rate, or if tempo preference is shaped by exposure to a heartbeat in utero.

The prolonged period of age-related change for low-frequency (i.e., 0.66 Hz) versus high-frequency (i.e., 1–2 Hz) targets may reflect additional signaling of the striatum, thalamus and/or cerebral cortex during middle adolescence. The cerebellum and basal ganglia have unique temporal sensitivity for high and low frequencies (Ivry and Spencer, 2004; Buhusi and Meck, 2005; Allman et al., 2014). For example, when listening to auditory stimuli, patients with cerebellar lesions had reduced surface level EEG amplitude for beats with high frequencies, whereas patients with basal ganglia lesions demonstrated reduced EEG activity for low frequencies (Nozaradan et al., 2017). Additionally, pharmacological silencing of the cerebellar dentate in monkeys impaired self-timed SRTs to fast versus slow-paced periodic targets (Kunimatsu et al., 2018). In a combined eye-tracking and fMRI study, Debrabant et al. (2013) observed that compared with age-matched controls, children diagnosed with developmental coordination disorder made fewer predictive saccades to a periodic target (ISI: 1200 ms) and exhibited less BOLD activity in cerebellar Crus I. The maturational timelines of predictive saccades in our participants toward target frequencies ≥1 Hz were remarkably similar, varying by only 1.3 years from the onset of performance-related change to its conclusion (Fig. 3A–C). These data, combined with previous reports of the specificity of cerebellar signaling for coordinating motor movements toward target frequencies ≥1 Hz (see Ivry and Spencer, 2004), together imply that in healthy children, the period of age-related cerebellar maturation to support saccades toward fast-paced rhythmic tempos occurs from ages 8 to 14.

Previous nonhuman primate research has causally demonstrated that the caudate closely maintains neuronal activity to match the delay interval of an ISI, while, neuronal activity in the dentate is relatively constant for various target frequencies, showing characteristic ramping activity 500 ms before the self-timed saccade and peaked activity at saccade execution (Ohmae et al., 2017). These results may explain why we observed fewer predictive saccades at the lowest frequency of 0.66 Hz, given low frequencies demand prolonged neuronal firing in the striatum to accurately signal the timing of saccade execution. Longitudinal cohort studies support age-related change of caudate gray matter volume from early childhood into young adulthood (Wierenga et al., 2014; Larsen and Luna, 2015). Furthermore, gray matter atrophy in the putamen in Huntington's disease patients was negatively correlated with predictive saccade SRT (Vaca-Palomares et al., 2019). Other potential neural correlates for the prolonged maturation of predicting targets at low frequencies include the thalamus, which acts as a relay region for a disynaptic connection between the dentate and striatum (Hoshi et al., 2005; Bostan et al., 2013). The thalamus integrates timing-related subcortical signals with cortical signals (Matsuyama and Tanaka, 2021) and has strong buildup activity for self-timed saccades (Tanaka, 2006, 2007). Additional cortical regions that modulate their firing during timing-related tasks and may further contribute to young adolescents' delayed temporal performance at 0.66 Hz include the Frontal Eye Fields (FEF) (O'Driscoll et al., 2000; Gagnon et al., 2002) and parietal cortex (Maimon and Assad, 2006; Jazayeri and Shadlen, 2015).

Pupil size and temporal prediction across age

Previous studies have reported a physiological relaxation effect when subjects were presented with rhythmic stimuli (Johnson and Trawick, 1938; Brauchli et al., 1995; Berger, 2012). Indeed, Figure 5A,B shows that this relaxation effect was replicated using pupil size, which was smaller for predictive versus reactive saccade trials. NE has been shown to have a neuromodulatory effect on the parasympathetic oculomotor complex via LC inputs to SC (Edwards et al., 1979) and SC's corresponding projection through Edinger–Westphal nucleus (Harting et al., 1980) to the ciliary ganglion, which can be both excitatory and inhibitory (Barnerssoi et al., 2017), providing a potential pathway for a decreased pupil size and thereby arousal for predictive saccades. This pupil effect is complementary to a previous fMRI study by Lee et al. (2016) that demonstrated strong activation of the default mode network (DMN), a neural network that increases its metabolic activity during relaxation, when adults made predictive saccades in the metronome task. Pupil size has been shown to negatively correlate with BOLD activity of the posterior cingulate cortex and parahippocampal gyrus of the DMN and positively correlate with LC (Alnæs et al., 2014). Pupil size positively correlated with SRT (Fig. 5C), in accordance with a previous study that reported this coordination via shared signaling pathways in the SC (Wang and Munoz, 2021). Yet while differences in mean pupil size by saccade type were evident when all metronome trials were collapsed, this effect was not maintained when analyzed at the subject-level (Fig. 5D). This may be because of variability in the number of individual participants' pupil trials that successfully met criteria for analysis for each saccade type. To address this, the field may benefit from incorporating biometric sensors that are additional to pupil size recording (e.g., respiration rate; skin conductance; heart rate) to measure sympathetic tone while individuals predict or react to rhythmic stimuli.

Pupil size reliably decreased with age for all saccade types (Fig. 5D). Previous studies have described age-related pupil size declines starting in later adolescence (for a detailed description, see Loewenfeld, 1999, p 501); however, our results suggest that this phenomenon may be observed earlier, beginning in childhood (onset range 7–12 years) and declining in size until age 24, the oldest we tested. Presumably this reduction in pupil size with age is driven by a diminished constriction capacity of the dilator pupillae muscle (Loewenfeld, 1999).

Coordination of blinks and predictive saccades

Despite not being provided with instructions regarding when to blink, participants exhibited a clear pattern in the timing of their blinks relative to metronome target appearance, with median blink reaction times occurring at the halfway point of the ISI (Fig. 6C). Blink rate did not vary according to the rhythmicity of targets (Gagnon et al., 2002), but rather, blink timing varied according to frequency, and according to whether subjects predicted or reacted to targets (Fig. 6). It has been shown previously that blink reaction time variability decreases when participants view videos that have clear temporal event structures (Nakano et al., 2009). Plotting the blink probability leading up to target appearance allowed us to reasonably estimate the SRT category of the subsequent saccade (Fig. 6G), with increased blink probability for saccadic categories with slower RTs. Given that dopaminergic signaling within the basal ganglia has been shown to modulate blink excitability (Groman et al., 2014; Jongkees and Colzato, 2016) and adjust the precision of interval-based timing in mammals and humans (Kunimatsu et al., 2016; Soares et al., 2016; De Corte et al., 2019), the coordination we observed between blinks and temporal prediction may involve dopaminergic phasic activity. Kaminer et al. (2011) describe a neural mechanism whereby blinks are modified by striatal dopamine levels via substantia nigra pars reticulata inhibition of the SC, and the SC's excitation of the nucleus raphe magnus and the subsequent inhibition of the spinal trigeminal complex. However, more research is needed to elucidate the exact mechanisms underlying the coordination between saccade and blink reaction times toward periodic targets.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the Southeastern Ontario Academic Medical Organization AFP Innovation Fund Award SEA-17-004 (to O.G.C., L.B., S.K.-K., and D.P.M.) and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research Grant MOP-FDN-148418 (to D.P.M.). O.G.C. is supported by an Ontario Graduate Scholarship. D.P.M. is supported by the Canada Research Chair Program. We thank E. Robertson, H. Riek, and M. Kilmade for help with data collection. We also thank A. Lablans and M. Lewis for their outstanding technical assistance and Dr. B. Calancie for his helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Douglas P. Munoz at doug.munoz{at}queensu.ca or Olivia G. Calancie at olivia.calancie{at}queensu.ca

SfN exclusive license.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Allman MJ,
    2. Teki S,
    3. Griffiths TD,
    4. Meck WH
    (2014) Properties of the internal clock: first- and second-order principles of subjective time. Annu Rev Psychol 65:743–771. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115117 pmid:24050187
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Alnæs D,
    2. Sneve MH,
    3. Espeseth T,
    4. Endestad T,
    5. van de Pavert SHP,
    6. Laeng B
    (2014) Pupil size signals mental effort deployed during multiple object tracking and predicts brain activity in the dorsal attention network and the locus coeruleus. J Vis 14:1–20. doi:10.1167/14.4.1
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    1. Ashmore RC,
    2. Sommer MA
    (2013) Delay activity of saccade-related neurons in the caudal dentate nucleus of the macaque cerebellum. J Neurophysiol 109:2129–2144. doi:10.1152/jn.00906.2011 pmid:23365182
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Bares M,
    2. Lungu O,
    3. Liu T,
    4. Waechter T,
    5. Gomez CM,
    6. Ashe J
    (2007) Impaired predictive motor timing in patients with cerebellar disorders. Exp Brain Res 180:355–365. doi:10.1007/s00221-007-0857-8 pmid:17256160
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Barnerssoi M,
    2. May PJ,
    3. Horn AKE
    (2017) GABAergic innervation of the ciliary ganglion in macaque monkeys – a light and electron microscopic study. J Comp Neurol 525:1517–1531. doi:10.1002/cne.24145 pmid:27864939
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Berger DS
    (2012) Pilot study investigating the efficacy of tempo-specific rhythm interventions in music-based treatment addressing hyper-arousal, anxiety, system pacing, and redirection of fight-or-flight fear behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). J Biomusical Eng 2:1–15. doi:10.4303/jbe/M110902
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  7. ↵
    1. Betke M,
    2. Chau M
    (2005) Real time eye tracking and blink detection with USB cameras. Bost Univ Comput Sci 2215:1–10.
    OpenUrl
  8. ↵
    1. Bostan AC,
    2. Dum RP,
    3. Strick PL
    (2013) Cerebellar networks with the cerebral cortex and basal ganglia. Trends Cogn Sci 17:241–254. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.003 pmid:23579055
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Brauchli P,
    2. Michel CM,
    3. Zeier H
    (1995) Electrocortical, autonomic, and subjective responses to rhythmic audio-visual stimulation. Int J Psychophysiol 19:53–66. doi:10.1016/0167-8760(94)00074-O
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Bronstein AM,
    2. Kennard C
    (1987) Predictive eye saccades are different from visually triggered saccades. Vision Res 27:517–520. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(87)90037-x pmid:3660613
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Buhusi CV,
    2. Meck WH
    (2005) What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:755–765. doi:10.1038/nrn1764 pmid:16163383
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Caffier PP,
    2. Erdmann U,
    3. Ullsperger P
    (2003) Experimental evaluation of eye-blink parameters as a drowsiness measure. Eur J Appl Physiol 89:319–325. doi:10.1007/s00421-003-0807-5 pmid:12736840
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Calabro FJ,
    2. Murty VP,
    3. Jalbrzikowski M,
    4. Tervo-Clemmens B,
    5. Luna B
    (2020) Development of hippocampal-prefrontal cortex interactions through adolescence. Cereb Cortex 30:1548–1558. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhz186 pmid:31670797
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Debrabant J,
    2. Gheysen F,
    3. Caeyenberghs K,
    4. Van Waelvelde H,
    5. Vingerhoets G
    (2013) Neural underpinnings of impaired predictive motor timing in children with developmental coordination disorder. Res Dev Disabil 34:1478–1487. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.02.008 pmid:23474999
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. De Corte BJ,
    2. Wagner LM,
    3. Matell MS,
    4. Narayanan NS
    (2019) Striatal dopamine and the temporal control of behavior. Behav Brain Res 356:375–379. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2018.08.030 pmid:30213664
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Dorris MC,
    2. Munoz DP
    (1998) Saccadic probability influences motor preparation signals and time to saccadic initiation. J Neurosci 18:7015–7026. pmid:9712670
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    1. Dorris MC,
    2. Paré M,
    3. Munoz DP
    (1997) Neuronal activity in monkey superior colliculus related to the initiation of saccadic eye movements. J Neurosci 17:8566–8579. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-21-08566.1997 pmid:9334428
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Edelman JA,
    2. Keller EL
    (1996) Activity of visuomotor burst neurons in the superior colliculus accompanying express saccades. J Neurophysiol 76:908–926. doi:10.1152/jn.1996.76.2.908 pmid:8871208
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Edwards SB,
    2. Ginsburgh CL,
    3. Henkel CK,
    4. Stein BE
    (1979) Sources of subcortical projections to the superior colliculus in the cat. J Comp Neurol 184:309–330. doi:10.1002/cne.901840207 pmid:762286
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Ellis CJK
    (1981) The pupillary light reflex in normal subjects. Br J Ophthalmol 65:754–759. doi:10.1136/bjo.65.11.754 pmid:7326222
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Evans A,
    2. McDonald D,
    3. Hurwitz AS,
    4. Toga A,
    5. Schmahmann JD,
    6. Holmes C,
    7. Petrides M,
    8. Doyon J,
    9. Lavoie K,
    10. Kabani N
    (1999) Three-dimensional MRI atlas of the human cerebellum in proportional stereotaxic space. Neuroimage 10:233–260. doi:10.1006/nimg.1999.0459 pmid:10458940
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Fischer B,
    2. Ramsperger E
    (1984) Human express saccades: extremely short reaction times of goal directed eye movements. Exp Brain Res 57:191–195. doi:10.1007/BF00231145 pmid:6519226
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Fischer B,
    2. Weber H
    (1993) Express saccades and visual attention. Behav Brain Sci 16:553–567. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00031575
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  24. ↵
    1. Fischer B,
    2. Biscaldi M,
    3. Gezeck S
    (1997) On the development of voluntary and reflexive components in human saccade generation. Brain Res 754:285–297. doi:10.1016/s0006-8993(97)00094-2 pmid:9134986
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Fitch WT
    (2013) Rhythmic cognition in humans and animals: distinguishing meter and pulse perception. Front Syst Neurosci 7:1–16.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Gagnon D,
    2. O'Driscoll GA,
    3. Petrides M,
    4. Pike GB
    (2002) The effect of spatial and temporal information on saccades and neural activity in oculomotor structures. Brain 125:123–139. doi:10.1093/brain/awf005 pmid:11834598
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Gibaldi A,
    2. Sabatini SP
    (2021) The saccade main sequence revised: a fast and repeatable tool for oculomotor analysis. Behav Res Methods 53:167–187. doi:10.3758/s13428-020-01388-2 pmid:32643061
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Groman SM,
    2. James AS,
    3. Seu E,
    4. Tran S,
    5. Clark TA,
    6. Harpster SN,
    7. Crawford M,
    8. Burtner JL,
    9. Feiler K,
    10. Roth RH,
    11. Elsworth JD,
    12. London ED,
    13. Jentsch JD
    (2014) In the blink of an eye: relating positive-feedback sensitivity to striatal dopamine d2-like receptors through blink rate. J Neurosci 34:14443–14454. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3037-14.2014 pmid:25339755
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Harting JK,
    2. Huerta MF,
    3. Frankfurter AJ,
    4. Strominger NL,
    5. Royce GJ
    (1980) Ascending pathways from the monkey superior colliculus: an autoradiographic analysis. J Comp Neurol 192:853–882. doi:10.1002/cne.901920414 pmid:7419758
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Hoshi E,
    2. Tremblay L,
    3. Féger J,
    4. Carras PL,
    5. Strick PL
    (2005) The cerebellum communicates with the basal ganglia. Nat Neurosci 8:1491–1493. doi:10.1038/nn1544 pmid:16205719
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Isotalo E,
    2. Lasker AG,
    3. Zee DS
    (2005) Cognitive influences on predictive saccadic tracking. Exp Brain Res 165:461–469. doi:10.1007/s00221-005-2317-7
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Ivry RB,
    2. Spencer RMC
    (2004) The neural representation of time. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14:225–232. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.013 pmid:15082329
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Jazayeri M,
    2. Shadlen MN
    (2015) A neural mechanism for sensing and reproducing a time interval. Curr Biol 25:2599–2609. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.08.038 pmid:26455307
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Johnson DM,
    2. Trawick M
    (1938) Influence of rhythmic sensory stimuli upon the heart-rate. J Psychol 6:303–310. doi:10.1080/00223980.1938.9917608
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  35. ↵
    1. Joiner WM,
    2. Shelhamer M
    (2006) An internal clock generates repetitive predictive saccades. Exp Brain Res 175:305–320. doi:10.1007/s00221-006-0554-z pmid:16964491
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Jongkees BJ,
    2. Colzato LS
    (2016) Spontaneous eye blink rate as predictor of dopamine-related cognitive function—a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 71:58–82. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.020 pmid:27555290
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Kaminer J,
    2. Powers AS,
    3. Horn KG,
    4. Hui C,
    5. Evinger C
    (2011) Characterizing the spontaneous blink generator: an animal model. J Neurosci 31:11256–11267. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6218-10.2011 pmid:21813686
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Kowler E,
    2. Martins AJ
    (1982) Eye movements of preschool children. Science 215:997–999. doi:10.1126/science.7156979 pmid:7156979
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. ↵
    1. Kunimatsu J,
    2. Suzuki TW,
    3. Tanaka M
    (2016) Implications of lateral cerebellum in proactive control of saccades. J Neurosci 36:7066–7074. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0733-16.2016 pmid:27358462
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    1. Kunimatsu J,
    2. Suzuki TW,
    3. Ohmae S,
    4. Tanaka M
    (2018) Different contributions of preparatory activity in the basal ganglia and cerebellum for self-timing. Elife 7:e35676. doi:10.7554/eLife.35676
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Larsen B,
    2. Luna B
    (2015) In vivo evidence of neurophysiological maturation of the human adolescent striatum. Dev Cogn Neurosci 12:74–85. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2014.12.003 pmid:25594607
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Lee IH,
    2. Assad JA
    (2003) Putaminal activity for simple reactions or self-timed movements. J Neurophysiol 89:2528–2537. doi:10.1152/jn.01055.2002 pmid:12611988
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Lee SM,
    2. Peltsch A,
    3. Kilmade M,
    4. Brien DC,
    5. Coe BC,
    6. Johnsrude IS,
    7. Munoz DP
    (2016) Neural correlates of predictive saccades. J Cogn Neurosci 28:1210–1227. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00968 pmid:27054397
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Loewenfeld IE
    (1999) The pupil: anatomy, physiology, and clinical applications. Boston: Butterworth-Heinemann.
  45. ↵
    1. Lukasova K,
    2. Silva IP,
    3. Macedo EC
    (2016) Impaired oculomotor behavior of children with developmental dyslexia in antisaccades and predictive saccades tasks. Front Psychol 7:1–9.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  46. ↵
    1. Lukasova K,
    2. Nucci MP,
    3. Machado de Azevedo Neto R,
    4. Vieira G,
    5. Sato JR,
    6. Amaro E
    (2018) Predictive saccades in children and adults: a combined fMRI and eye tracking study. PLoS One 13:e0196000. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0196000
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  47. ↵
    1. Luna B,
    2. Thulborn KR,
    3. Munoz DP,
    4. Merriam EP,
    5. Garver KE,
    6. Minshew NJ,
    7. Keshavan MS,
    8. Genovese CR,
    9. Eddy WF,
    10. Sweeney JA
    (2001) Maturation of widely distributed brain function subserves cognitive development. Neuroimage 13:786–793. doi:10.1006/nimg.2000.0743 pmid:11304075
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Luna B,
    2. Tervo-Clemmens B,
    3. Calabro FJ
    (2021) Considerations when characterizing adolescent neurocognitive development. Biol Psychiatry 89:96–98. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.04.026 pmid:32507392
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Maimon G,
    2. Assad JA
    (2006) A cognitive signal for the proactive timing of action in macaque LIP. Nat Neurosci 9:948–955. doi:10.1038/nn1716 pmid:16751764
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Marino RA,
    2. Levy R,
    3. Munoz DP
    (2015) Linking express saccade occurance to stimulus properties and sensorimotor integration in the superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 114:879–892. doi:10.1152/jn.00047.2015 pmid:26063770
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Matsuyama K,
    2. Tanaka M
    (2021) Temporal prediction signals for periodic sensory events in the primate central thalamus. J Neurosci 41:2151–2120.
    OpenUrl
  52. ↵
    1. McAuley JD,
    2. Jones MR,
    3. Holub S,
    4. Johnston HM,
    5. Miller NS
    (2006) The time of our lives: life span development of timing and event tracking. J Exp Psychol Gen 135:348–367. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.135.3.348 pmid:16846269
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Merchant H,
    2. Harrington DL,
    3. Meck WH
    (2013) Neural basis of the perception and estimation of time. Annu Rev Neurosci 36:313–336. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170349 pmid:23725000
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Munoz DP,
    2. Broughton JR,
    3. Goldring JE,
    4. Armstrong IT
    (1998) Age-related performance of human subjects on saccadic eye movement tasks. Exp Brain Res 121:391–400. doi:10.1007/s002210050473 pmid:9746145
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Murphy PR,
    2. O'Connell RG,
    3. O'Sullivan M,
    4. Robertson IH,
    5. Balsters JH
    (2014) Pupil diameter covaries with BOLD activity in human locus coeruleus. Hum Brain Mapp 35:4140–4154. doi:10.1002/hbm.22466 pmid:24510607
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Nakano T,
    2. Yamamoto Y,
    3. Kitajo K,
    4. Takahashi T,
    5. Kitazawa S
    (2009) Synchronization of spontaneous eyeblinks while viewing video stories. Proc Biol Sci 276:3635–3644. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.0828 pmid:19640888
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Nozaradan S,
    2. Schwartze M,
    3. Obermeier C,
    4. Kotz SA
    (2017) Specific contributions of basal ganglia and cerebellum to the neural tracking of rhythm. Cortex 95:156–168. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2017.08.015 pmid:28910668
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. O'Driscoll G a,
    2. Wolff a L,
    3. Benkelfat C,
    4. Florencio PS,
    5. Lal S,
    6. Evans a C
    (2000) Functional neuroanatomy of smooth pursuit and predictive saccades. Neuroreport 11:1335–1340.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Ohmae S,
    2. Kunimatsu J,
    3. Tanaka M
    (2017) Cerebellar roles in self-timing for sub- and supra-second intervals. J Neurosci 37:3511–3522. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2221-16.2017 pmid:28242799
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  60. ↵
    1. Paré M,
    2. Munoz DP
    (1996) Saccadic reaction time in the monkey: advanced preparation of oculomotor programs is primarily responsible for express saccade occurrence. J Neurophysiol 76:3666–3681. doi:10.1152/jn.1996.76.6.3666
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Repp BH,
    2. Su YH
    (2013) Sensorimotor synchronization: a review of recent research (2006-2012). Psychon Bull Rev 20:403–452. doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0371-2 pmid:23397235
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Ross RG,
    2. Radant AD,
    3. Hommer DW,
    4. Young DA
    (1994) Saccadic eye movements in normal children from 8 to 15 years of age: a developmental study of visuospatial attention. J Autism Dev Disord 24:413–431. doi:10.1007/BF02172126 pmid:7961328
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Ross SM,
    2. Ross LE
    (1987) Children's and adults' predictive saccades to square-wave targets. Vision Res 27:2177–2180. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(87)90131-3 pmid:3447365
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Salman MS,
    2. Sharpe JA,
    3. Eizenman M,
    4. Lillakas L,
    5. Westall C,
    6. To T,
    7. Dennis M,
    8. Steinbach MJ
    (2006) Saccades in children. Vision Res 46:1432–1439. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2005.06.011 pmid:16051306
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Shelhamer M,
    2. Joiner WM
    (2003) Saccades exhibit abrupt transition between reactive and predictive, predictive saccade sequences have long-term correlations. J Neurophysiol 90:2763–2769. doi:10.1152/jn.00478.2003 pmid:14534279
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Simmonds DJ,
    2. Hallquist MN,
    3. Asato M,
    4. Luna B
    (2014) Developmental stages and sex differences of white matter and behavioral development through adolescence: a longitudinal diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) study. Neuroimage 92:356–368. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.044 pmid:24384150
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Simpson GL
    (2018) Modelling palaeoecological time series using generalised additive models. Front Ecol Evol 6:1–21.
    OpenUrl
  68. ↵
    1. Smit AC,
    2. Van Gisbergen JAM
    (1989) A short-latency transition in saccade dynamics during square-wave tracking and its significance for the differentiation of visually-guided and predictive saccades. Exp Brain Res 76:64–74. doi:10.1007/BF00253624 pmid:2753110
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Soares S,
    2. Atallah BV,
    3. Paton JJ
    (2016) Midbrain dopamine neurons control judgment of time. Science 354:1273–1277. doi:10.1126/science.aah5234 pmid:27940870
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  70. ↵
    1. Stark L,
    2. Vossius G,
    3. Young LR
    (1962) Predictive control of eye tracking movements. IRE Trans Hum Factors Electron HFE-3:52–57. doi:10.1109/THFE2.1962.4503342
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  71. ↵
    1. Tanaka M
    (2006) Inactivation of the central thalamus delays self-timed saccades. Nat Neurosci 9:20–22. doi:10.1038/nn1617 pmid:16341209
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Tanaka M
    (2007) Cognitive signals in the primate motor thalamus predict saccade timing. J Neurosci 27:12109–12118. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1873-07.2007 pmid:17978052
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  73. ↵
    1. Tanaka M,
    2. Kunimatsu J,
    3. Suzuki TW,
    4. Kameda M,
    5. Ohmae S,
    6. Uematsu A,
    7. Takeya R
    (2021) Roles of the cerebellum in motor preparation and prediction of timing. Neuroscience 462:220–234. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.04.039
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  74. ↵
    1. Turner RS,
    2. Anderson ME
    (2005) Context-dependent modulation of movement-related discharge in the primate globus pallidus. J Neurosci 25:2965–2976. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4036-04.2005 pmid:15772356
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  75. ↵
    1. Vaca-Palomares I,
    2. Brien DC,
    3. Coe BC,
    4. Ochoa-Morales A,
    5. Martínez-Ruano L,
    6. Munoz DP,
    7. Fernandez-Ruiz J
    (2019) Implicit learning impairment identified via predictive saccades in Huntington's disease correlates with extended cortico-striatal atrophy. Cortex 121:89–103. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2019.06.013 pmid:31550618
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. van der Steen MC,
    2. Keller PE
    (2013) The adaptation and anticipation model (ADAM) of sensorimotor synchronization. Front Hum Neurosci 7:1–15.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Wang CA,
    2. Munoz DP
    (2021) Coordination of pupil and saccade responses by the superior colliculus. J Cogn Neurosci 33:919–932. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_01688 pmid:34449845
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. Wang CA,
    2. Huang J,
    3. Yep R,
    4. Munoz DP
    (2018) Comparing pupil light response modulation between saccade planning and working memory. J Cogn 1:33. doi:10.5334/joc.33 pmid:31517206
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Weber H,
    2. Aiple F,
    3. Fischer B,
    4. Latanov A
    (1992) Dead zone for express saccades. Exp Brain Res 89:214–222. doi:10.1007/BF00229018 pmid:1601099
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Wierenga L,
    2. Langen M,
    3. Ambrosino S,
    4. van Dijk S,
    5. Oranje B,
    6. Durston S
    (2014) Typical development of basal ganglia, hippocampus, amygdala and cerebellum from age 7 to 24. Neuroimage 96:67–72. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.072 pmid:24705201
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    1. Wierenga LM,
    2. Bos MGN,
    3. van Rossenberg F,
    4. Crone EA
    (2019) Sex effects on development of brain structure and executive functions: greater variance than mean effects. J Cogn Neurosci 31:730–753. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_01375 pmid:30726177
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    1. Wong AL,
    2. Shelhamer M
    (2011) Exploring the fundamental dynamics of error-based motor learning using a stationary predictive-saccade task. PLoS One 6:e25225. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0025225
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  83. ↵
    1. Wood SN
    (2009) mgcv. R Package version 1.6-0. Available at http://cran.r-project.org/package=mgcv.
  84. ↵
    1. Wood SN
    (2011) Fast stable restricted maximum likelihood and marginal likelihood estimation of semiparametric generalized linear models. J R Stat Soc Ser B Stat Methodol 73:3–36. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9868.2010.00749.x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  85. ↵
    1. Wood SN
    (2017) Generalized additive models: an introduction with R. Boca Raton: CRC Press.
  86. ↵
    1. Zorn A,
    2. Joiner WM,
    3. Lasker AG,
    4. Shelhamer M
    (2007) Sensory versus motor information in the control of predictive saccade timing. Exp Brain Res 179:505–515. doi:10.1007/s00221-006-0806-y pmid:17216153
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 42 (1)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 42, Issue 1
5 Jan 2022
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Maturation of Temporal Saccade Prediction from Childhood to Adulthood: Predictive Saccades, Reduced Pupil Size, and Blink Synchronization
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Maturation of Temporal Saccade Prediction from Childhood to Adulthood: Predictive Saccades, Reduced Pupil Size, and Blink Synchronization
Olivia G. Calancie, Donald C. Brien, Jeff Huang, Brian C. Coe, Linda Booij, Sarosh Khalid-Khan, Douglas P. Munoz
Journal of Neuroscience 5 January 2022, 42 (1) 69-80; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0837-21.2021

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Maturation of Temporal Saccade Prediction from Childhood to Adulthood: Predictive Saccades, Reduced Pupil Size, and Blink Synchronization
Olivia G. Calancie, Donald C. Brien, Jeff Huang, Brian C. Coe, Linda Booij, Sarosh Khalid-Khan, Douglas P. Munoz
Journal of Neuroscience 5 January 2022, 42 (1) 69-80; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0837-21.2021
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • blink rate
  • development
  • eye movements
  • pupil diameter
  • rhythm
  • timing

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Rhythmic Entrainment Echoes in Auditory Perception
  • Multimodal Imaging for Validation and Optimization of Ion Channel-Based Chemogenetics in Nonhuman Primates
  • Cleavage of VAMP2/3 Affects Oligodendrocyte Lineage Development in the Developing Mouse Spinal Cord
Show more Research Articles

Systems/Circuits

  • Rhythmic Entrainment Echoes in Auditory Perception
  • Multimodal Imaging for Validation and Optimization of Ion Channel-Based Chemogenetics in Nonhuman Primates
  • Cleavage of VAMP2/3 Affects Oligodendrocyte Lineage Development in the Developing Mouse Spinal Cord
Show more Systems/Circuits
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.