Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

The Spatiotemporal Link of Temporal Expectations: Contextual Temporal Expectation Is Independent of Spatial Attention

Noam Tal-Perry and Shlomit Yuval-Greenberg
Journal of Neuroscience 23 March 2022, 42 (12) 2516-2523; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1555-21.2022
Noam Tal-Perry
1School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Shlomit Yuval-Greenberg
1School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel
2Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Temporal expectation is the ability to construct predictions regarding the timing of events, based on previously experienced temporal regularities of different types. For example, cue-based expectations are constructed when a cue validly indicates when a target is expected to occur. However, in the absence of such cues, expectations can be constructed based on contextual temporal information, including the onset distribution of the event and recent prior experiences, both providing implicit probabilistic information regarding the timing of the event. It was previously suggested that cue-based temporal expectation is exerted via synchronization of spatially specific neural activity at a predictable time of a target, within receptive fields corresponding to the expected location of the target. Here, we tested whether the same theoretical model holds for contextual temporal effects. Participants (n = 40, 25 females) performed a speeded spatial-cuing detection task with two-thirds valid spatial cues. The hazard-rate function of the target was modulated by varying the foreperiod—the interval between the spatial cue and the target—among trials and was manipulated between groups by changing the interval distribution. Reaction times were analyzed using both frequentist and Bayesian generalized linear mixed models, accounting for hazard and sequential effects. Results showed that the effects of contextual temporal structures on reaction times were independent of spatial attention. This suggests that the spatiotemporal mechanisms, thought to account for cue-based expectation, cannot explain other sources of temporal expectations. We conclude that expectations based on contextual structures have different characteristics than cue-based temporal expectation, suggesting reliance on distinct neural mechanisms.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Temporal expectation is the ability to predict an event onset based on temporal regularities. A neurophysiological model suggested that temporal expectation relies on the synchronization of spatially specific neurons whose receptive fields represent the attended location. This model predicts that temporal expectation would be evident solely within the locus of spatial attention. Existing evidence supported this model for expectation based on associations between a temporal cue and a target, but here we show that it cannot account for temporal expectation that is based on contextual information, that is, the distribution of intervals and recent priors. These findings reveal the existence of different predictive mechanisms for cued and contextual temporal predictions, with the former depending on spatial attention and the latter nonspatially specific.

  • FP-RT slope
  • hazard-rate function
  • reaction time
  • sequential effect
  • temporal attention
  • variable foreperiod effect

Introduction

Temporal expectation is the ability to construct predictions regarding the timing of events, based on temporal regularities. These regularities come in multiple forms, including contextual information, when statistical inferences regarding distributions of events and recent priors are used to predict timings of future events, and cued associations, when events are preceded by informative temporal cues (Coull and Nobre, 1998). Expectations of all these sources were associated with enhanced motor and perceptual performance (Niemi and Näätänen, 1981; Nobre and van Ede, 2018).

Despite abundant evidence on behavioral effects of temporal expectation, relatively little is known regarding its neurophysiological correlates. One framework suggests that temporal expectation results from neural synchronization at expected target time. It was suggested synchronized neuronal populations are spatially specific, that their receptive fields correspond to the expected target location (Rohenkohl et al., 2014; Nobre and van Ede, 2018). According to this view, temporal and spatial expectations are tightly linked as temporal expectation is restricted to the locus of spatial attention; to gain from knowing when a target will occur, one must know where it would occur. However, evidence for this spatiotemporal framework is limited to studies that manipulated cue-based temporal expectation (Doherty et al., 2005; Rohenkohl et al., 2014; Seibold et al., 2020). It remains unknown whether the same spatiotemporal mechanism accounts for temporal expectation based on other sources of regularities, such as those based on the overall distribution of intervals, and recent previous trials.

The distribution of intervals is linked to a frequently observed finding—the variable-foreperiod effect (VFE). When participants are presented with a warning signal (WS) followed by a target after a varying time interval (foreperiod; FP), and the foreperiods are randomly sampled from a uniform distribution, performance for targets appearing after long foreperiods is enhanced relative to short ones (Niemi and Näätänen, 1981). According to one interpretation, this effect depends on the conditional probability of the event, or the likelihood of an event to occur, given that it has yet to occur (Vallesi and Shallice, 2007; Nobre and van Ede, 2018). Conditional probabilities can be described as a function of time (hazard-rate function), which monotonically increases when foreperiods are uniformly distributed; but other distributions could lead to different hazard-rate functions (Luce, 1986). Another interpretation explains VFE as the result of memory activation. The WS acts as a memory retrieval cue, which activates traces of prior trials and results in reaction time (RT) modulations based on the frequencies of previous foreperiods (Los et al., 2017, 2021). Regardless of its interpretation, VFE reflects how the distribution of prior foreperiods modulates temporal expectation.

Another source of contextual information are previous trials. When a target appears following a foreperiod that is shorter than that of the previous trial, performance is reduced, relative to trials that were preceded by an identical foreperiod. This sequential effect (SE) is asymmetrical; performance remains unchanged when a target appears following a foreperiod that is longer than that of the previous trial (Bertelson, 1961; Possamai et al., 1973). There are various accounts for the SE, attributing this effect to a dual process of arousal and controlled monitoring (Vallesi and Shallice, 2007; Capizzi et al., 2015), to trace conditioning (Los et al., 2001) or, more recently, to retrieval of memory traces (Los et al., 2014, 2017, 2021). Regardless of their interpretations, VFE and SE both demonstrate how manipulating contextual priors changes the behavioral outcome of temporal expectations.

There is surprisingly little evidence regarding the interaction of spatial attention and contextual temporal expectation effects. Only one study examined a related question, focusing on the link between SE and inhibition of return, an exogenous attention effect (Los, 2004), but no study to date manipulated endogenous attention to examine its link with contextual temporal expectation. Here, we investigated this link by simultaneously manipulating attention and the distribution of foreperiods. In each trial, participants were presented with a spatial cue that was either congruent, incongruent, or neutral in respect to the location of the target that appeared after a varying foreperiod. The distribution of the foreperiods varied between participants to create two different hazard-rate functions. Finding that VFE and SE are independent of spatial attention would indicate that the spatiotemporal framework suggested to account for temporal expectation does not account for these processes; finding otherwise would support the link between cue-based and contextual temporal expectations.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 40 participants were included in this study, 20 in the uniform distribution group (12 females, 2 left-handed, mean age 25.35 ± 3.5 SDs) and 20 in the inverse-U-shaped distribution group (13 females, one left-handed; mean age 24.55 ± 4.0 SD). There were no exclusions of participants. Sample size was determined according to a pilot study consisting of 20 participants. A power simulation based on this pilot study showed that with only n = 8, these three effects are observed with 1−β = 0.84–.91. Because the pilot study used a higher valid to invalid ratio (75%) and did not include an uninformative condition, we have decided to opt for a larger sample size of 20 participants, as we did in previous studies on temporal expectations (Amit et al., 2019; Abeles et al., 2020; Tal-Perry and Yuval-Greenberg, 2020, 2021). A description of the pilot study and the power simulation is available from the Open Science Foundation (OSF; see below, Data availability). Participants received payment or course credit for their participation. All participants were healthy, reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and no history of neurologic disorders. The experimental protocols were approved by the ethical committees of Tel Aviv University and the School of Psychological Sciences. Before participation, participants signed informed consent forms.

Stimuli

The fixation object consisted of a dot (0.075° radius) within a ring (0.15° radius), embedded within a diamond shape (0.4 × 0.4°). The edges of the diamond changed color from black to white, cuing attention to the left side (two left edges became white) or right side (two right edges became white) of fixation object, or remaining neutral in respect to target location (all four edges became white; Fig. 1). The target was a black asterisk (0.4 × 0.4°) presented at 4° eccentricity to the right or left of fixation object. A 1000 Hz pure tone was sounded for 60 ms as negative feedback following errors. Fixation object and target were presented on a midgray background.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Trial progression. Each trial started with the presentation of a fixation stimulus that was presented until online eye tracking confirmed a continuous 1 s of steady fixation. This was followed by a spatial cue that was invalid in respect to target location in 25% of trials (as depicted), valid in 50% of trials, and uninformative in 25% of trials. In two groups, foreperiods were sampled from either a uniform or an inverse-U distribution. Participants were asked to make a single-button speeded response within 1000 ms of target onset. An error tone was played when participants responded before target onset or failed to respond within the time limit. For display purposes, stimuli are presented in this figure as larger and more eccentric than they appeared in the experiment.

Experimental design

Participants were seated in a dimly lit room, with a computer monitor placed 100 cm in front of them (24 inch LCD ASUS VG248QE, 1920 × 1080 pixels resolution, 120 Hz refresh rate, midgray luminance measured to be 110 cd/m2). During the experiment, participants rested their heads on a chinrest. MATLAB R2015a (MathWorks) was used to code and control the experiment, with stimuli displayed using Psychophysics Toolbox version 3 (Brainard, 1997). Gaze position was monitored binocularly using the EyeLink 1000 Plus infrared video-oculographic desktop-mounted system (SR Research) throughout the experiment, at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz. This system has 0.01° spatial resolution and an average accuracy of 0.25–0.5° when a chinrest is used, according to the manufacturer. A nine-point calibration of the eye-tracker was performed before each block and whenever necessary.

Each trial started with a central black fixation object, presented until an online gaze-contingent procedure verified 1000 ms of stable fixation (gaze was placed within a radius of 1.5° of screen center). Following this, the edges of the fixation object changed color for 200 ms to represent a spatial informative or uninformative cue. After a varying foreperiod (500/900/1300/1700/2100 ms) the target was briefly (33 ms) presented at 4° to the left or right of center, with target being congruent to a spatially informative cue direction in 50% of trials (valid condition), incongruent in 25% of trials (invalid condition), or neutral with respect to a spatially uninformative cue in the remaining 25% of trials (uninformative condition). Participants were requested to press a key with their dominant hand as quickly as possible, and after no longer than 1000 ms, on target detection. Between groups, participants were presented with the five foreperiods in either a uniform distribution (20% probability for each foreperiod) or an inverse-U-shaped distribution (a ratio of 1:2:3:2:1 among the five foreperiods, leading to trial percentages of ∼11, 22, 33, 22, and 11%, respectively). These prior distributions resulted in different time-dependent conditional probabilities, that is, different hazard-rate functions, as depicted in Figure 2. The manipulation of hazard rate was required to differentiate its effect from other foreperiod effects related to the WS, such as arousal (Steinborn and Langner, 2012; Weinbach and Henik, 2012). The different distributions were examined in separate participant groups to avoid carry-over effects of distribution learning (Mattiesing et al., 2017). Fixation was monitored throughout the foreperiod, using an online gaze-contingent procedure, and trials that included ≥1.5° gaze-shift for >10 ms during this period were aborted and repeated at a later stage of the session. An error feedback tone was sounded when participants responded before target onset or did not respond within 1000 ms following target onset. These trials were not included in the analysis. Trials in which participants responded before target onset were reshuffled into the trial pool and repeated at a later stage of the block. The trial procedure is depicted in Figure 1.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Target probability (bars) and hazard rate (conditional probability, dashed line) for the uniform and inverse-U foreperiod distributions.

Participants of the uniform distribution group performed 10 blocks of 160 trials each, divided into two sessions of ∼1.25 h each. Participants of the inverse-U-shaped distribution group performed 18 blocks of 144 trials each, divided into three sessions of ∼1.25 h each. This number of repetitions guaranteed that we have a minimum of 50 trials in all conditions and for all foreperiods in each of the two distributions, and a large enough number of trials to conduct a sequential analysis on pairs of consecutive trials. A short break was given after each block. Feedback on performance in each block was provided at the end of each experimental block and included mean RT and number of error trials (including both missed trials or premature responses). Starting from the second experimental block, participants were also presented with a message that encouraged them to perform faster if the mean RT of the current block fell below their global mean RT of the entire session. A practice block of 10 trials with random conditions was administered at the beginning of each session.

Statistical analysis

A negligible number of trials with no response (error trials; <1% of all trials; mean 0.7% of trials per participant, range 0–2.16% of trials) were discarded from analysis. Additionally, trials with response time below 150 ms were considered unlikely to represent genuine target-related responses (Keele and Posner, 1968; McLeod, 1987) and were likewise discarded from analysis (<1% of all remaining trials; mean 0.3% of trials per participant, range 0–2.2% of trials).

The RTs of the remaining trials were modeled using a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM), assuming a gamma family of responses with an identity link (see explanation below; Baayen and Milin, 2010; Lo and Andrews, 2015). Unlike ANOVA, GLMM is suited for non-normally distributed variables, like the positively skewed RT distribution, while also allowing to model trial-level covariates, thus increasing the power of the analysis (Baayen and Milin, 2010). Hierarchical models are also well suited for unevenly distributed trial numbers among conditions, as is the case with the inverse-U-shaped distribution and the relation to the previous trial in the current study, by weighting the population-level mean according to the number of samples included in the subject-level means for each condition. An assumption of this analysis is that the RTs follow gamma distribution. Gamma distributions are suited to describe continuous responses that are zero bounded and have a unimodal and rightward-skewed distribution (e.g., RTs). We further assumed that the predictors are linearly related to the predicted RT, thus an identity link was used (i.e., no transformation was made on the value produced by the predictors; Lo and Andrews, 2015).

The following fixed effects were modeled: (1) linear and quadratic terms for foreperiod duration to model the VFE; (2) cue (valid/invalid/uninformative) to model the effect of spatial attention; (3) the FP-distribution (uniform/inverse-U-shaped) to model the effect of the hazard-rate function; (4) linear and quadratic terms for the relation to previous trial to model SE, calculated as the difference between the current trial foreperiod and the previous trial foreperiod, so that positive values indicate the previous trial was shorter than the current trial and vice versa for negative values; and (5) the interaction terms between foreperiod duration, cue, and FP-distribution and between relation to previous trial, cue, and FP-distribution. For the purpose of examining the relation between spatial attention and each of the temporal structures, we assumed no interaction between the relation to previous trial and foreperiod duration, for example, we assumed that the cost in performance for a current trial of 500 ms and previous trial of 900 ms equals the cost of a 900 and 1300 ms pair of trials. It is acknowledged that this assumption is a simplification and does not strictly hold in all cases (Possamai et al., 1973). To reduce computational complexity, all continuous factors were z-scaled. To allow the computation of the relation to previous trial, the first trial of each session for each participant was discarded from analysis (total of 100 trials). Treatment contrasts coding scheme was used for cue, with the uninformative condition set as the reference level, and sum contrasts coding scheme was used for FP-distribution. Statistical significance for main effects and interactions was determined via a likelihood ratio (LR) test against a reduced nested model excluding the fixed term (i.e., type II sum of squares). Statistical significance for parameter coefficients was determined according to the Wald z test (Fox, 2016).

In addition to the fixed effects, we considered the z-scaled current trial number (i.e., the running trial identifier for the given session) as a covariate to capture effects of fatigue and training along the experiment (Baayen and Milin, 2010). Because the different experimental groups may have experienced different fatigue or training effects, we additionally considered the interaction between FP-Distribution and trial number. Covariates were added to the model if the extended model converged and was found to significantly improve fit (p < 0.05) in an LR test against the model without the covariate (Bates et al., 2015a).

The random effect structure of the model was selected according to the model that was found to be most parsimonious with the data, that is, the fullest model that the data permits while still converging with no singular estimates (Bates et al., 2015a), to balance between type I error and statistical power (Matuschek et al., 2017). To do this, we followed a procedure suggested by Bates et al. (2015a). We started with the simplest model, a random intercept-by-subject-only model, and then added random slopes to it, first, random slopes for fixed terms by subject and their correlation parameters, and then random interaction slopes by subject. In each iteration, we examined whether the new model converged and then used likelihood ratio test (using α=.05) between the new and the old model, to examine whether there is an improved fit over the previous model and avoid overfitting. Models that failed to converge were trimmed by the random slope with the least explained variance and were retested. Finally, we tested whether the model supports random slopes for the aforementioned covariates, using the same process. The full model selection process is described in the R markdown file in the project OSF repository (see below, Data availability).

To provide support for null results (p < 0.05), we additionally modeled the data using a Bayesian GLMM with weakly informative priors (Gelman et al., 2017) on the fixed and random effects of the model (N(0,10)) and correlation (LKJ(2)) parameters, using the default mean for the intercept (298), and using informative shape parameters (γ(0.02, 12.0)) according to (Lo and Andrews, 2015). Posterior distributions were constructed using four Markov chain Monte Carlo chains and 20,000 iterations per chain, with the first 2000 samples used as warmup. The large number of iterations was required to calculate a stable Bayes Factor (BF). BFs were calculated by comparing the marginal likelihood between the full model and a nested null model, with marginal likelihood estimated by 100 repetitions of bridge sampling (Gronau et al., 2017). BFs are reported with the null results in the nominator (BF01 or logBF01 for BF01>100), representing how much the data are supported by the null model relative to the full model, along with range and the proportional estimation error as in Morey and Rouder (2018).

Analyses were performed in R version 4.0.3 using RStudio version 1.3.959 (https://www.r-project.org/). Frequentist modeling was performed using the lme4 (Bates et al., 2015b) package, Bayesian modeling was performed using the brms package (Bürkner, 2017), and additional model diagnostics were performed using the performance package (Lüdecke et al., 2021). An R markdown file describing all the model fitting steps and diagnostic checks on the final model is available at the OSF repository for the project (see below, Data availability).

Data Availability

The datasets generated by this study and an R markdown file that reproduces all the reported modeling, statistical analyses, and graphs in the article are uploaded to the Open Science Foundation repository and are available at https://osf.io/25gzj.

Results

RTs were modeled using a GLMM with FP-distribution (uniform/inverse-U-shaped) as a between-subject fixed term and FP-duration (continuous), relation to previous trial (continuous), and cue (valid/invalid/uninformative) as within-subject fixed terms, as well as the full interaction terms between FP-duration, FP-distribution, and cue, and between relation to previous trial, FP-distribution, and cue. Trial number and the interaction between trial number and FP-distribution were added as covariates, and we allowed for a random intercept and a random slope for the linear term of FP-duration and cue by participant.

Effects of foreperiod and spatial attention

Results showed that the VFE, the decrease in RT as foreperiod increases, changed with distribution, for each of the cues (Fig. 3). We observed a significant main effect for FP-duration (χ2(2)=864.59,p<0.001), with negative linear and positive quadratic terms, consistent with the classic effect of foreperiod on RT and its expected shape. We additionally observed a main effect for cue (χ2(2)=19.90,p<0.001), indicating the expected effect of spatial attention on RT. This effect was reflected by a large benefit in RT for valid versus uninformative cues (β=−10.146,t=−12.582,p<0.001) as well as a smaller but significant cost for invalid versus uninformative trials (β=2.666,t=2.530,p=0.011). Most importantly, for the purpose of this study, we found no significant interaction between cue and FP-duration (χ2(4)=5.862,p=0.210), indicating that the effect for cue did not vary with foreperiod and supporting the hypothesis that spatial attention does not affect the VFE.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Effect of hazard-rate function on RTs. Mean RT for the uniform (left) and inverse-U-shaped (right) distributions. Each graph depicts group-averaged mean reaction time (colored dots) with second degree polynomial fit (colored lines). Error bars indicate SE ± 1 from the group mean, corrected to within-subject variability (Cousineau and O'Brien, 2014). N = 20 for each group.

Effects of the distribution shape

The between-group variable of FP-distribution (uniform/inverse-U-shaped) was used to assess the involvement of expectations based on the prior and posterior foreperiod distribution on the VFE and the relation of this effect to spatial attention. Findings showed no main group effect of FP-distribution on RT (χ2(1)=0.601,p=0.435), indicating that both groups had similar overall RT. However, there was a significant interaction between FP-distribution and FP-duration (χ2(2)=102.68,p<0.001), indicating that consistently with previous findings (Trillenberg et al., 2000; Cravo et al., 2011), the effect of foreperiod on RT was modulated by the prior distribution from which they originated. Importantly for the goal of this study, there was no evidence that this effect of FP-distribution on FP-duration was modulated by the validity of the cue as reflected by an insignificant interaction among cue, FP-distribution, and FP-duration (χ2(4)=4.699,p=0.320). This suggests that the effect of the FP distribution on the VFE was independent of spatial attention. As expected, no significant interaction was found between cue and FP-distribution (χ2(2)=0.050,p=0.975).

Sequential effects

To test for the existence of SEs, we calculated the difference between the FP-duration of one trial and the FP-duration of the previous trial (FPcurrent−FPprevious). Consistent with previous studies (Alegria and Delhaye-Rembaux, 1975; Niemi and Näätänen, 1981), results showed an asymmetrical sequential effect on RTs in that RTs were slower when the current trial was shorter than the previous trial (Fig. 4, negative values) but were not affected when the opposite was true (Fig. 4, positive values), leading to a quadratic relation with RT (χ2(2)=1644.5,p<0.001). The lack of effect when a trial is longer was explained by dual-process models of temporal preparation to result from the combined contribution of sequential and conditional probabilities effects. Sequential effects erroneously guide expectations toward an early timing leading to lower performance, but given that the target has not appeared at the earlier time, the conditional probability increases and expectation grows following the hazard-rate function, leading to higher performance (Vallesi and Shallice, 2007; Vallesi et al., 2013). Alternatively, this asymmetry could be explained by the aggregated activity of several previous memory traces and the slow dissipation of this activity over the trials (Los et al., 2014, 2021). Combined, the result is no enhancement or decrement of performance at late time points. Additionally, results revealed that this effect was significantly modulated by the FP-distribution (χ2(2)=28.924,p<0.001), with linear component being more negative for the inverse-U compared with the uniform distribution. This finding, also consistent with previous findings (Niemi and Näätänen, 1981), can be interpreted as reflecting the involvement of the hazard-rate function in this effect. Alternatively, it could be explained as reflecting the combined effect of multiple preceding trials (Los et al., 2001; Steinborn and Langner, 2012). Generally, these findings demonstrate that expectations based on the distribution shape and effects of previous trials each had a unique contribution to the resulting RTs, along with a synergetic effect between them.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Sequential effect on RTs. Mean RT for the uniform (left) and inverse-U-shaped (right) distributions, with x-axis depicting the sequential effect (difference between current (FPn) and previous (FPn-1) trial foreperiod). Each graph depicts group-averaged mean reaction time (colored dots) with second-degree polynomial fit (colored lines). Error bars indicate SE ± 1 from the group mean, corrected to within-subject variability (Cousineau and O'Brien, 2014). N = 20 for each group.

We next tested whether these effects were modulated by spatial attention by examining the interaction between them and cue. Results showed no significant interaction between relation to previous trial and cue (χ2(2)=1.177,p=0.882), nor a significant three-way interaction among relation to previous trial, FP-distribution, and cue (χ2(4)=2.585,p=0.630). Both results suggest that as the VFE, sequential effects are independent of the spatial locus of attention.

Model estimates for all fixed factors described are depicted in Figure 5. Model estimates for covariates and additional model information can be found online in the project OSF repository (see above, Data availability).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Model estimates. Forest plot of fixed factors estimates, modeled using a GLMM, assuming a gamma response family and identity link function (estimates are given in ms units) and depicting mean in respect to the reference level (uninformative cue type). All continuous factors were scaled and centered. Positive values are depicted in blue and negative values in red. Horizontal lines depict 95% Wald confidence intervals. Dashed vertical line centered at zero-sized estimate. Valid and invalid terms are relative to uninformative cue condition. FPL, Linear component of Foreperiod duration; FPQ, quadratic component of Foreperiod duration; Dist, FP-Distribution; SeqL, linear component of Sequential effect; SeqQ, quadratic component of Sequential effect. Cov, covariate. Interaction terms denoted by X.

Bayesian modeling

Our results indicated that there was no evidence for a three-way interaction among cue, FP-distribution, and FP-duration, as well as no three-way interaction among cue, FP-distribution, and relation to previous trial. To examine whether the evidence supports these null results, we constructed a Bayesian GLMM using the same model terms. Model estimates closely resembled the coefficients found in the frequentists model. We compared the resulting Bayesian model with two nested models, each lacking the corresponding three-way interaction term. Results showed large support for the null model lacking the FP duration three-way interaction term compared with the full model (mean logBF01 = 8.483 ± 0.002%, range 8.289–8.681), and similarly large support was observed for the null model lacking the relation to the previous trial three-way interaction term compared with the full model (mean logBF01=9.969±<0.001%, range 9.731–10.146). Both results support the conclusion that temporal expectations based on VFE and SE are independent of spatial attention. Additional modeling information can be found online (see above, Data availability).

Discussion

In this study we examined whether the spatiotemporal model that was proposed to account for cue-based temporal expectation also carries for temporal expectation based on contextual information, that is, the VFE and SE. As expected, findings showed the VFE with RT decreasing as the foreperiod increases. This VFE changed according to foreperiod distribution. In addition, we found the expected asymmetrical SE, slower RTs for trials in which the foreperiod was longer than their previous trial, and no opposite effect for trials in which the foreperiod was shorter than the previous trial. Critically, all these effects were unaffected by spatial attention. Similar modulations of expectations were found in both attended and unattended spatial locations. This indicates that temporal expectations based on contextual information—the VFE and SE—are independent of spatial attention.

The spatiotemporal model of temporal expectation

Doherty et al. (2005) were the first to demonstrate an interaction between cue-based temporal and spatial attention in early visual event-related potentials (ERPs) components. They presented participants with moving objects that disappeared behind an occluder and reappeared in an expected or unexpected location and/or time. Participants were requested to indicate whether a target was presented on the reappearing object. Findings showed that when a target appeared at an expected location, the early visual P1 component was increased relative to an unexpected location, with enhanced effect when the target also appeared at the expected time. However, when a target appeared at the expected time but not the expected location, there was no enhancement relative to a neutral condition, suggesting that early perceptual benefits of temporal attention depend on the allocation of spatial attention. This spatiotemporal synergism was not found in later ERP components, such as the P3, considered to be less affected by perceptual processes and more by response requirements, and not in RTs.

In a later study by Rohenkohl et al. (2014), symbolic spatial and/or temporal cues predicted with 80% validity the time and location of a grating-patch target, for which participants were requested to perform a nonspeeded orientation discrimination task. Findings showed that valid temporal cues improved both RT and perceptual sensitivity relative to invalid cues but that this effect was limited to trials where spatial attention was focused at the location of the target. These findings provided, again, evidence for a strong synergistic interaction between temporal and spatial expectations in a discrimination task. Consistently, evidence by Seibold et al. (2020) showed that temporal attention boosts the effect of spatial attention on early ERP components in a visual search task.

This evidence of a tight link between spatial attention and cue-based temporal expectation led Nobre and van Ede (2018) to propose their spatiotemporal neurophysiological model, according to which the interaction between spatial and temporal processes stems from time-specific synchronization of spatially specific neural populations at the attended retinotopic receptive fields. These neurons, coding the attended location and relevant features, acquire a temporal structure from repeated exposure to the temporal cues, which affects them but not populations outside the receptive field (Nobre and van Ede, 2018). This model was developed based on evidence on cue-based expectation and was never before examined for other sources of temporal expectations. The present evidence indicates that expectation effects based on contextual information (VFE and SE) do not depend on spatial attention, suggesting that these forms of expectation cannot be explained by this spatiotemporal mechanism.

This further suggests that cue-based temporal expectation and temporal expectation which are driven by contextual information and are often described as two manifestations of the same expectation process, likely rely on distinct neural mechanisms. This evidence is consistent with studies that dissociated VFE and cue-based temporal expectation and found that these two sources of expectations share some, but not all, of their underlying brain networks (Lima et al., 2011; Coull et al., 2016; Amit et al., 2019). More generally, this conclusion is compatible with the increasing recognition that there is no single unified expectation mechanism but that distinct sources of temporal expectations facilitate performance via distinct neural mechanisms (van Ede et al., 2020).

Spatiotemporal synergism and cue-based expectations

It is important to note, however, that evidence regarding the dependency, or lack thereof, of cue-based temporal expectation on spatial attention is ambivalent. In addition to the supporting evidence described above, a few studies provided evidence challenging this interaction. For example, one study investigated the combined influence of temporal, spatial, and feature-based attention and found no synergetic effects between spatial and temporal attention (Rolke et al., 2016). Another study (MacKay and Juola, 2007) used a visual search task in a rapid stimulus visual presentation stream of letters and showed that both spatial and temporal types of cues were effective on their own and that their combined effect was additive, indicating there was no interaction between temporal and spatial attention. In a later study, Weinbach et al. (2015) used a spatiotemporal cuing paradigm and showed that temporal cuing improves RT even when coupled with an invalid spatial cue. Moreover, there was no interaction between the effect of the temporal and the spatial cues, indicating that enhancement resulting from temporal attention was not affected by spatial attention. The authors noted that the discrepancy between their findings and previous findings may have stemmed from differences in task demands, and whereas most previous studies used demanding perceptual-discrimination tasks, Weinbach et al. (2015) used a speeded-RT detection task. In the present study, we have also used a speeded detection task and therefore cannot rule out the possibility that the effect we have observed is specific to speeded easy tasks. However, we note that all previous studies that manipulated the foreperiod distributions to study VFE have either used very easy tasks (Trillenberg et al., 2000; Cravo et al., 2011; Mattiesing et al., 2017; Grabenhorst et al., 2019; Los et al., 2021) or have not reported accuracy rates (Herbst et al., 2018), with the exception of one study that has measured the effect of foreperiod distribution on perceptual speed (Vangkilde et al., 2013). This raises the hypothesis that the VFE depends on a speeded response to an easy task. If this is the case, it would constitute another inherent dissociation between cue-based and contextual temporal expectation. However, with the present data in our hands, we cannot confirm or rule out this hypothesis or determine the link between task difficulty and independency of spatial attention.

Temporal attention and temporal expectation

The apparent discrepancies among different findings on spatiotemporal dependency could be accounted for by the dissociation between attention and expectation processes. According to one view, described in Summerfield and Egner (2009), expectation reflects the narrowing down of the probability space of possibilities constructed according to prior knowledge, whereas attention is the selection of specific, goal-relevant information that should be prioritized. Both attention and expectation coexist and are often entangled; for example, cuing to the left visual field increases our expectation of encountering a target at that location and induces a shift of attention that prioritizes information on that particular visual space. Tailored experimental designs can dissociate attention and expectation, as was demonstrated in visual spatial attention and feature attention studies (Summerfield and Egner, 2009, 2016; Kok et al., 2012).

Similar to spatial cues, temporal cuing paradigms often create a symbolic association between a certain cue and a specific target onset time. Thus, the onset of the cue induces an attentional shift that prioritizes information processing around the cued time interval. In addition, in these designs, the repeated exposure to target onset after a cue changes the probability space and induces temporal expectation, which is independent of attention according to the definition described above (Summerfield and Egner, 2009). Therefore, according to this view, in these designs, temporal attention often coincides with temporal expectation, although specific experimental designs can dissociate these functions (Denison et al., 2019, 2021). Contextual information narrows down the probability space and therefore can be viewed as a source of temporal expectation. This narrowing down of the probability space can be based on exposure to recent previous trials or on the activation of a memory trace of prior, even not so recent experiences (Mattiesing et al., 2017). In a series of studies, Los et al. (2001, 2014, 2017, 2021) presented the multiple trace theory of temporal preparation, suggesting that the basis for the VFE is not conditional probabilities but the activation of memory traces of previous trials. Furthermore, Los et al. (2021) showed that different cues can be associated with memory traces of different temporal distributions, thereby modifying temporal preparation according to cue identity. Arguably, regardless of whether the probability space is gathered from very recent experiences or based on a memory trace associated with a cue, both the VFE and SE could be the result of redefining a probability space rather than focusing attention on a specific time point.

We hypothesize that this proposed dissociation between expectation and attention could account for the discrepancies among previous studies on the spatiotemporal dependency, with temporal attention being spatially specific and temporal expectation remaining independent of the spatial locus of attention. This, in turn, could explain the results observed here, that is, because the hazard rate and sequential manipulations affect only temporal expectation and not attention, their manifestations were free of spatial constraints.

Conclusions

We conclude that the benefits of contextual temporal expectation are not spatially specific but rather reflect a general nonspecific enhancement that is not accompanied by shifts of attention. Furthermore, we suggest that the spatiotemporal neurophysiological model proposed by Nobre and van Ede (2018) to explain cue-based expectation cannot account for the VFE and SE and their link with temporal expectation. Future studies are encouraged to examine the dissociation between different mechanisms of temporal expectation and to refine the terminology to reflect this dissociation.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by Israel Science Foundation Grant 1960/19 to S.Y-G. We thank Danielle Allon and Keren Nistor for assistance in running the experiment.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Noam Tal-Perry at noamtalperry{at}gmail.com

SfN exclusive license.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Abeles D,
    2. Amit R,
    3. Tal-Perry N,
    4. Carrasco M,
    5. Yuval-Greenberg S
    (2020) Oculomotor inhibition precedes temporally expected auditory targets. Nat Commun 11:3524. doi:10.1038/s41467-020-17158-9 pmid:32665559
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Alegria J,
    2. Delhaye-Rembaux M
    (1975) Sequential effects of foreperiod duration and conditional probability of the signal in a choice reaction time task. Acta Psychologica 39:321–328. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(75)90024-4
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  3. ↵
    1. Amit R,
    2. Abeles D,
    3. Carrasco M,
    4. Yuval-Greenberg S
    (2019) Oculomotor inhibition reflects temporal expectations. Neuroimage 184:279–292. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.026 pmid:30223059
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Baayen HR,
    2. Milin P
    (2010) Analyzing reaction times. Int J Psycholog Res 3:12–28. doi:10.21500/20112084.807
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  5. ↵
    1. Bates D,
    2. Kliegl R,
    3. Vasishth S,
    4. Baayen HR
    (2015a) Parsimonious mixed models. arXiv:1506.04967.
  6. ↵
    1. Bates D,
    2. Mächler M,
    3. Bolker B,
    4. Walker S
    (2015b) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw 67:1–48. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Bertelson P
    (1961) Sequential redundancy and speed in a serial two-choice responding task. Q J Exp Psychol 13:90–102. doi:10.1080/17470216108416478
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  8. ↵
    1. Brainard DH
    (1997) The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spat Vis 10:433–436. pmid:9176952
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Bürkner P-C
    (2017) brms: an R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. J Stat Softw 80:28. doi:10.18637/jss.v080.i01
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  10. ↵
    1. Capizzi M,
    2. Correa Á,
    3. Wojtowicz A,
    4. Rafal RD
    (2015) Foreperiod priming in temporal preparation: testing current models of sequential effects. Cognition 134:39–49. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.09.002 pmid:25460377
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Coull JT,
    2. Nobre AC
    (1998) Where and when to pay attention: the neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. J Neurosci 18:7426–7435. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-18-07426.1998
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. Coull JT,
    2. Cotti J,
    3. Vidal F
    (2016) Differential roles for parietal and frontal cortices in fixed versus evolving temporal expectations: dissociating prior from posterior temporal probabilities with fMRI. Neuroimage 141:40–51. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.036 pmid:27431757
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Cousineau D,
    2. O'Brien F
    (2014) Error bars in within-subject designs: a comment on Baguley (2012). Behav Res Methods 46:1149–1151. doi:10.3758/s13428-013-0441-z pmid:24477859
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Cravo AM,
    2. Rohenkohl G,
    3. Wyart V,
    4. Nobre AC
    (2011) Endogenous modulation of low frequency oscillations by temporal expectations. J Neurophysiol 106:2964–2972. doi:10.1152/jn.00157.2011 pmid:21900508
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Denison RN,
    2. Yuval-Greenberg S,
    3. Carrasco M
    (2019) Directing voluntary temporal attention increases fixational stability. J Neurosci 39:353–363. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1926-18.2018 pmid:30459223
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Denison RN,
    2. Carrasco M,
    3. Heeger DJ
    (2021) A dynamic normalization model of temporal attention. Nat Hum Behav 5:1674–1685. doi:10.1038/s41562-021-01129-1
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  17. ↵
    1. Doherty JR,
    2. Rao A,
    3. Mesulam MM,
    4. Nobre AC
    (2005) Synergistic effect of combined temporal and spatial expectations on visual attention. J Neurosci 25:8259–8266. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1821-05.2005 pmid:16148233
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Fox J
    (2016) Applied regression analysis and genealized linear models. London, UK: Sage Publications.
  19. ↵
    1. Gelman A,
    2. Simpson D,
    3. Betancourt M
    (2017) The prior can often only be understood in the context of the likelihood. Entropy 19:555. doi:10.3390/e19100555
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  20. ↵
    1. Grabenhorst M,
    2. Georgios M,
    3. Maloney LT,
    4. Poeppel D
    (2019) The anticipation of events in time. Nat Commun 10:5802. doi:10.1038/s41467-019-13849-0 pmid:31862912
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Gronau QF,
    2. Sarafoglou A,
    3. Matzke D,
    4. Ly A,
    5. Boehm U,
    6. Marsman M,
    7. Leslie DS,
    8. Forster JJ,
    9. Wagenmakers EJ,
    10. Steingroever H
    (2017) A tutorial on bridge sampling. J Math Psychol 81:80–97. doi:10.1016/j.jmp.2017.09.005 pmid:29200501
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Herbst SK,
    2. Fiedler L,
    3. Obleser J
    (2018) Tracking temporal hazard in the human electroencephalogram using a forward encoding model. eNeuro 5:ENEURO.0017-18.2018–17. doi:10.1523/ENEURO.0017-18.2018
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  23. ↵
    1. Keele SW,
    2. Posner MI
    (1968) Processing of visual feedback in rapid movements. J Exp Psychol 77:155–158. doi:10.1037/h0025754
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Kok P,
    2. Rahnev D,
    3. Jehee JFM,
    4. Lau HC,
    5. De Lange FP
    (2012) Attention reverses the effect of prediction in silencing sensory signals. Cereb Cortex 22:2197–2206. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr310 pmid:22047964
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Lima B,
    2. Singer W,
    3. Neuenschwander S
    (2011) Gamma responses correlate with temporal expectation in monkey primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 31:15919–15931. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0957-11.2011 pmid:22049435
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    1. Lo S,
    2. Andrews S
    (2015) To transform or not to transform: using generalized linear mixed models to analyse reaction time data. Front Psychol 6:1–16. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01171
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Los SA
    (2004) Inhibition of return and nonspecific preparation: separable inhibitory control mechanisms in space and time. Percept Psychophys 66:119–130. doi:10.3758/bf03194866 pmid:15095945
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Los SA,
    2. Heuvel CE,
    3. Van Den Van Den Heuvel CE
    (2001) Intentional and unintentional contributions to nonspecific preparation during reaction time foreperiods. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27:370–386. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.27.2.370 pmid:11318053
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Los SA,
    2. Kruijne W,
    3. Meeter M
    (2014) Outlines of a multiple trace theory of temporal preparation. Front Psychol 5:1058. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01058 pmid:25285088
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Los SA,
    2. Kruijne W,
    3. Meeter M
    (2017) Hazard versus history: temporal preparation is driven by past experience. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 43:78–88. doi:10.1037/xhp0000279 pmid:27808547
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Los SA,
    2. Nieuwenstein J,
    3. Bouharab A,
    4. Stephens DJ,
    5. Meeter M,
    6. Kruijne W
    (2021) The warning stimulus as retrieval cue: the role of associative memory in temporal preparation. Cogn Psychol 125:101378. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2021.101378 pmid:33524889
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Luce RD
    (1986) Response times: their role in inferring elementary mental organization. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  33. ↵
    1. Lüdecke D,
    2. Ben-Shachar MS,
    3. Patil I,
    4. Waggoner P,
    5. Makowski D
    (2021) performance: An R package for assessment, comparison and testing of statistical models. J Open Source Softw 6:3139.
    OpenUrl
  34. ↵
    1. MacKay A,
    2. Juola JF
    (2007) Are spatial and temporal attention independent? Percept Psychophys 69:972–979. doi:10.3758/bf03193935 pmid:18018979
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Mattiesing RM,
    2. Kruijne W,
    3. Meeter M,
    4. Los SA
    (2017) Timing a week later: the role of long-term memory in temporal preparation. Psychon Bull Rev 24:1900–1905. doi:10.3758/s13423-017-1270-3 pmid:28439808
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Matuschek H,
    2. Kliegl R,
    3. Vasishth S,
    4. Baayen HR,
    5. Bates D
    (2017) Balancing type I error and power in linear mixed models. J Mem Lang 94:305–315. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2017.01.001
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  37. ↵
    1. McLeod P
    (1987) Visual reaction time and high-speed ball games. Perception 16:49–59. doi:10.1068/p160049 pmid:3671040
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Morey RD,
    2. Rouder JN
    (2018) BayesFactor: computation of bayes factors for common designs. R package version 0.9.12-4.3.
  39. ↵
    1. Niemi P,
    2. Näätänen R
    (1981) Foreperiod and simple reaction time. Psychol Bull 89:133–162. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.89.1.133
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  40. ↵
    1. Nobre AC,
    2. van Ede F
    (2018) Anticipated moments: temporal structure in attention. Nat Rev Neurosci 19:34–48. doi:10.1038/nrn.2017.141 pmid:29213134
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Possamai CA,
    2. Granjon M,
    3. Requin J,
    4. Reynard G
    (1973) Sequential effects related to foreperiod duration in simple reaction time. Percept Motor Skill 36:1185–1186. doi:10.2466/pms.1973.36.3c.1185
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  42. ↵
    1. Rohenkohl G,
    2. Gould IC,
    3. Pessoa J,
    4. Nobre AC
    (2014) Combining spatial and temporal expectations to improve visual perception. J Vis. 14(4):8. doi:10.1167/14.4.8
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    1. Rolke B,
    2. Festl F,
    3. Seibold VC
    (2016) Toward the influence of temporal attention on the selection of targets in a visual search task: an ERP study. Psychophysiology 53:1690–1701. doi:10.1111/psyp.12734 pmid:27479494
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Seibold VC,
    2. Stepper MY,
    3. Rolke B
    (2020) Temporal attention boosts perceptual effects of spatial attention and feature-based attention. Brain Cogn 142:105570. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2020.105570 pmid:32447188
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Steinborn MB,
    2. Langner R
    (2012) Arousal modulates temporal preparation under increased time uncertainty: evidence from higher-order sequential foreperiod effects. Acta Psychologica 139:65–76. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.10.010 pmid:22088963
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Summerfield C,
    2. Egner T
    (2009) Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 13:403–409. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.003 pmid:19716752
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Summerfield C,
    2. Egner T
    (2016) Feature-based attention and feature-based expectation. Trends Cogn Sci 20:401–404. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.03.008 pmid:27079632
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Tal-Perry N,
    2. Yuval-Greenberg S
    (2020) Pre-target oculomotor inhibition reflects temporal orienting rather than certainty. Scientific Reports 10:1–9. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-78189-2
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  49. ↵
    1. Tal-Perry N,
    2. Yuval-Greenberg S
    (2021) Prestimulus inhibition of eye movements reflects temporal expectation rather than time estimation. Atten Percept Psychophys 83:2473–2485.
    OpenUrl
  50. ↵
    1. Trillenberg P,
    2. Verleger R,
    3. Wascher E,
    4. Wauschkuhn B,
    5. Wessel K
    (2000) CNV and temporal uncertainty with “ageing” and “non-ageing” S1-S2 intervals. Clin Neurophysiol 111:1216–1226. doi:10.1016/s1388-2457(00)00274-1 pmid:10880797
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Vallesi A,
    2. Shallice T
    (2007) Developmental dissociations of preparation over time: deconstructing the variable foreperiod phenomena. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 33:1377–1388. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.33.6.1377 pmid:18085950
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Vallesi A,
    2. Lozano VN,
    3. Correa Á
    (2013) Dissociating temporal preparation processes as a function of the inter-trial interval duration. Cognition 127:22–30. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.011 pmid:23318351
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. van Ede F,
    2. Rohenkohl G,
    3. Gould I,
    4. Nobre AC
    (2020) Purpose-dependent consequences of temporal expectations serving perception and action. J Neurosci 40:7877–7886. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1134-20.2020
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  54. ↵
    1. Vangkilde S,
    2. Petersen A,
    3. Bundesen C
    (2013) Temporal expectancy in the context of a theory of visual attention. Philos Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci 368:20130054. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0054 pmid:24018716
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Weinbach N,
    2. Henik A
    (2012) Temporal orienting and alerting—the same or different? Front Psychol 3:236. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00236
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Weinbach N,
    2. Shofty I,
    3. Gabay S,
    4. Henik A
    (2015) Endogenous temporal and spatial orienting: evidence for two distinct attentional mechanisms. Psychon Bull Rev 22:967–973. doi:10.3758/s13423-014-0750-y pmid:25338657
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 42 (12)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 42, Issue 12
23 Mar 2022
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
The Spatiotemporal Link of Temporal Expectations: Contextual Temporal Expectation Is Independent of Spatial Attention
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
The Spatiotemporal Link of Temporal Expectations: Contextual Temporal Expectation Is Independent of Spatial Attention
Noam Tal-Perry, Shlomit Yuval-Greenberg
Journal of Neuroscience 23 March 2022, 42 (12) 2516-2523; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1555-21.2022

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
The Spatiotemporal Link of Temporal Expectations: Contextual Temporal Expectation Is Independent of Spatial Attention
Noam Tal-Perry, Shlomit Yuval-Greenberg
Journal of Neuroscience 23 March 2022, 42 (12) 2516-2523; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1555-21.2022
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • FP-RT slope
  • hazard-rate function
  • reaction time
  • sequential effect
  • temporal attention
  • variable foreperiod effect

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Regional Excitatory–Inhibitory Balance Relates to Self-Reference Effect on Recollection via the Precuneus/Posterior Cingulate Cortex–Medial Prefrontal Cortex Connectivity
  • Attention Alters Population Spatial Frequency Tuning
  • Sequence Diversity and Encoded Enzymatic Differences of Monocistronic L1 ORF2 mRNA Variants in the Aged Normal and Alzheimer's Disease Brain
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Attention Alters Population Spatial Frequency Tuning
  • Complex Impact of Stimulus Envelope on Motor Synchronization to Sound
  • The Molecular Substrates of Second-Order Conditioned Fear in the Basolateral Amygdala Complex
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.