Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Selective Prefrontal–Amygdala Circuit Interactions Underlie Social and Nonsocial Valuation in Rhesus Macaques

Maia S. Pujara, Nicole K. Ciesinski, Joseph F. Reyelts, Sarah E.V. Rhodes and Elisabeth A. Murray
Journal of Neuroscience 13 July 2022, 42 (28) 5593-5604; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0794-21.2022
Maia S. Pujara
Section on the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, Laboratory of Neuropsychology, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Maia S. Pujara
Nicole K. Ciesinski
Section on the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, Laboratory of Neuropsychology, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Joseph F. Reyelts
Section on the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, Laboratory of Neuropsychology, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sarah E.V. Rhodes
Section on the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, Laboratory of Neuropsychology, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Elisabeth A. Murray
Section on the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, Laboratory of Neuropsychology, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Elisabeth A. Murray
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Lesion studies in macaques suggest dissociable functions of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and medial frontal cortex (MFC), with OFC being essential for goal-directed decision-making and MFC supporting social cognition. Bilateral amygdala damage results in impairments in both of these domains. There are extensive reciprocal connections between these prefrontal areas and the amygdala; however, it is not known whether the dissociable roles of OFC and MFC depend on functional interactions with the amygdala. To test this possibility, we compared the performance of male rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) with crossed surgical disconnection of the amygdala and either MFC (MFC × AMY, n = 4) or OFC (OFC × AMY, n = 4) to a group of unoperated controls (CON, n = 5). All monkeys were assessed for their performance on two tasks to measure the following: (1) food-retrieval latencies while viewing videos of social and nonsocial stimuli in a test of social interest and (2) object choices based on current food value using reinforcer devaluation in a test of goal-directed decision-making. Compared with the CON group, the MFC × AMY group, but not the OFC × AMY group, showed significantly reduced food-retrieval latencies while viewing videos of conspecifics, indicating reduced social valuation and/or interest. By contrast, on the devaluation task, group OFC × AMY, but not group MFC × AMY, displayed deficits on object choices following changes in food value. These data indicate that the MFC and OFC must functionally interact with the amygdala to support normative social and nonsocial valuation, respectively.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Ascribing value to conspecifics (social) versus objects (nonsocial) may be supported by distinct but overlapping brain networks. Here, we test whether two nonoverlapping regions of the prefrontal cortex, the medial frontal cortex and the orbitofrontal cortex, must causally interact with the amygdala to sustain social valuation and goal-directed decision-making, respectively. We found that these prefrontal-amygdala circuits are functionally dissociable, lending support for the idea that medial frontal and orbital frontal cortex make independent contributions to cognitive appraisals of the environment. These data provide a neural framework for distinct value assignment processes and may enhance our understanding of the cognitive deficits observed following brain injury or in the development of mental health disorders.

  • amygdala
  • decision-making
  • lesions
  • medial frontal cortex
  • orbitofrontal cortex
  • social cognition

Introduction

Both individual and collective fitness contribute to the evolutionary success of anthropoid primates, including macaques. In their natural habitats, macaques gain advantages for survival by optimizing behaviors that lead to beneficial outcomes both for themselves and their group. In a laboratory setting, monkeys, like humans, develop and express transitive subjective preferences for different foodstuffs (Tremblay and Schultz, 1999; Padoa-Schioppa et al., 2006; Martin et al., 2018) and juices (Tremblay and Schultz, 1999; Padoa-Schioppa and Assad, 2006). Macaques also forego juice rewards to view images of female perinea (i.e., sex skin) and dominant male monkey faces (Deaner and Platt, 2003; Deaner et al., 2005), which suggests that social cues have intrinsic value. Although assigning value to foods and/or conspecifics and the stimuli that predict them is a central feature of cognition, evidence suggests that these processes are supported by distinct neural networks. In this study, we examined the neuroanatomical basis of two aspects of biological value in macaques: nonsocial objects associated with food rewards and social signals conveyed by conspecifics.

Neuropsychological research in humans points to a causal role for the amygdala and ventromedial frontal cortex (VMF) in carrying out these essential higher order functions. Patients with Urbach-Wiethe disease who experience selective bilateral amygdala damage are profoundly impaired both in their ability to learn from social information (Rosenberger et al., 2019) and in making rational decisions (Brand et al., 2007; Talmi et al., 2010). Similarly, patients with lesions of the VMF, which typically compromises substantial portions of both orbital and medial sectors of the frontal cortex, are impaired in both social cognition (Eslinger and Damasio, 1985; Barrash et al., 2000; Hornak et al., 2003; Mah et al., 2004; Beer et al., 2006; Ciaramelli et al., 2007; Jenkins et al., 2014; Hiser and Koenigs, 2018) and rational or goal-directed decision-making (Eslinger and Damasio, 1985; Anderson et al., 1999; Fellows and Farah, 2003; Camille et al., 2004; Koenigs and Tranel, 2007; Wheeler and Fellows, 2008; Pujara et al., 2015; Reber et al., 2017; Barrash et al., 2018; Hiser and Koenigs, 2018).

Because brain damage in humans rarely respects boundaries of individual structures, teasing apart the contributions of the medial frontal cortex (MFC) and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and their independent networks depends on studies in nonhuman primates, where targeting these areas individually and testing circuit interactions is more tractable (Vaidya et al., 2019). Studies in macaques suggest specializations for MFC and OFC in social and nonsocial valuation, respectively. Whereas the OFC is involved in value coding, updating stimulus–outcome associations, and value-based decision-making (Padoa-Schioppa and Assad, 2006; Padoa-Schioppa, 2011; Wallis, 2011; Murray and Rudebeck, 2018), the MFC has been implicated in social cognition (Rudebeck et al., 2006; Noonan et al., 2010a; Lockwood et al., 2020) and encoding of rewards for the self and others (Chang et al., 2013). Given the known reciprocal anatomic connections between the amygdala and both the MFC and OFC (Carmichael and Price, 1995; Ghashghaei and Barbas, 2002; Saleem et al., 2008), it is possible that the MFC and OFC must functionally interact with the amygdala to support these processes (Gangopadhyay et al., 2021). If so, these prefrontal–amygdala circuits may be separable in empirical tests of their functions.

Although there is evidence that OFC–amygdala interactions support value-based decision-making (Baxter et al., 2000), the aspiration OFC lesions in that study may have involved damage to fibers of passage (Rudebeck et al., 2013a); therefore, using selective, excitotoxic lesions to update the interpretation of the function of this circuit would be timely. No macaque lesion studies to date have examined OFC– and MFC–amygdala interactions in social cognition. We predicted that relative to intact controls, monkeys with crossed surgical disconnection of the MFC and the amygdala would be impaired in evaluating naturalistic videos of conspecifics but not goal-directed decision-making, whereas monkeys with crossed surgical disconnection of the OFC and the amygdala would show the opposite pattern of results.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Thirteen adult male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) served as subjects. All monkeys were obtained from a domestic breeding colony. Macaques were born and raised in outdoor corrals, with access to shelter from the elements, in groups ranging from ∼10–50 individuals. When macaques were ∼3–5 years of age, they were transported to the National Institutes of Health Animal Center to undergo quarantine and were then ultimately brought to our animal facility. Veterinarians and technicians trained in macaque husbandry were on site at the breeding colony to monitor the health and well-being of the monkeys throughout the rearing period. After transfer to our facility, animals were housed and enriched in a manner consistent with the Guide for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

Four monkeys sustained unilateral aspiration lesions of the MFC targeting area 32 (Carmichael and Price, 1994), and four monkeys sustained unilateral excitotoxic lesions of the OFC targeting Walker's areas 11, 13, and 14 (Walker, 1940). All operated monkeys sustained excitotoxic amygdala lesions in the hemisphere contralateral to their cortical lesions (i.e., crossed lesions). The crossed-surgical disconnection eliminates the direct functional interaction of the amygdala with either the MFC (MFC × AMY group) or the OFC (OFC × AMY group). If direct functional interaction of the amygdala with either prefrontal cortex region is necessary for the behaviors assessed in our tasks, then monkeys in those surgical groups should be impaired relative to unoperated controls. Our study design also controls for the effects, if any, of unilateral amygdala damage and two-stage surgery. The remaining five monkeys were retained as unoperated controls (CON). Monkeys weighed between 7.1 kg and 15.2 kg (mean, 9.5 kg) and all were at least 5 years old at the start of testing. All training and testing was conducted postoperatively, starting with an action–outcome reinforcer devaluation task previously described in Fiuzat et al. (2017), followed by a social valuation task and an object–outcome reinforcer devaluation task described below. Importantly, all monkeys had the same training history.

Removal of certain brain regions (e.g., amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex, medial frontal cortex) has been reported to result in alterations in emotional and social behavior (Dicks et al., 1968; Kling, 1972; Emery et al., 2001; Hadland et al., 2003; Izquierdo et al., 2005). Accordingly, monkeys were singly housed out of concern for their safety per historic convention. To control for housing conditions, the unoperated controls were likewise housed singly. All macaques had visual and auditory contact with several other conspecifics in the animal housing room, were kept on a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 A.M.), maintained on controlled access to primate chow supplemented with fruit to ensure sufficient motivation to respond in the test apparatus, and had ad libitum access to water. Testing occurred during the light period. All procedures were reviewed and approved by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Animal Care and Use Committee.

Surgery

Eight monkeys received surgery in two stages to produce crossed surgical disconnection of the amygdala and either the MFC or OFC. All operated monkeys sustained amygdala damage in one hemisphere and damage of either MFC (MFC × AMY) or OFC (OFC × AMY) in the other hemisphere; groups were balanced for site (frontal cortex, amygdala) and hemisphere (left, right) of first surgery. For the purpose of relating the location of our intended lesions to other commonly used anatomic frameworks, we note that the intended MFC lesion in our study corresponds approximately to area 32, also known as prelimbic cortex or pregenual cortex (Barbas and Pandya, 1989; Carmichael and Price, 1994), and the intended OFC lesion corresponds approximately to areas 11l, 11m, 13l, 13m, 13b, 14r, 14c, and 10m (Carmichael and Price, 1994). The intended amygdala lesion encompassed both the basolateral and centromedial nuclear groups.

At the time of surgery, anesthesia was induced with ketamine hydrochloride (10 mg/kg, i.m.) and maintained with isoflurane (1.0–3.0%, to effect). Heart rate, respiration rate, blood pressure, expired CO2, and body temperature were monitored during surgery, and isotonic fluids were given throughout. Aseptic procedures were used. After completing the series of ibotenate (ibotenic acid) injections (excitotoxic) or combination of suction and cautery (aspiration), the surgical site was closed in anatomic layers with sutures. The preoperative and postoperative treatment regimen consisted of dexamethasone sodium phosphate (0.4 mg/kg, i.m.) and cefazolin antibiotic (15 mg/kg, i.m.) for 1 d before surgery and 1 week after surgery to reduce swelling and prevent infection, respectively. At the end of surgery and for 2 additional days, the monkeys received the analgesic ketoprofen (10–15 mg, i.m.), followed by ibuprofen (100 mg) for the following 5 d. Operations were separated by a minimum of 2 weeks. Postoperative behavioral testing was initiated 10–14 d following the second stage of surgery.

Monkeys receiving an amygdala lesion were anesthetized and then placed in a stereotaxic frame. A large bone flap was turned over the appropriate portion of the cranium. The injection sites were calculated based on landmarks that were visible in the MRI scans obtained before surgery. The sagittal sinus served as a landmark for the mediolateral coordinates, and the interaural plane (ear bars) served as a landmark for the anteroposterior and dorsoventral coordinates. The monkeys received between 16 and 23 unilateral injections to sites located ∼2 mm apart in each plane. Each injection consisted of 1.0 µl of ibotenate (10–15 µg/µl; 0.2 µl/min; Sigma-Aldrich) administered via a 30-gauge needle of a Hamilton syringe held in a David Kopf Instruments manipulator. Before lowering the needle, small slits were made in the dura to allow the needle to pass unobstructed into the brain. The needle remained in place 2–3 min after each injection to limit diffusion of the toxin up the needle track. On closing, 30 ml of mannitol (25%, 1 ml/min, i.v.) was administered to control edema.

Monkeys receiving either MFC or OFC lesions were anesthetized and then placed in a custom head holder. At the beginning of surgery, monkeys were given 30 ml of mannitol (25%, 1 ml/min, i.v.) to increase access to the orbital and medial surfaces and to control edema. For the MFC lesion, a large, bilaterally symmetrical bone flap was turned over the frontal cortex, and the dura was reflected toward the midline. Sulcal landmarks on the medial surface were identified with the aid of an operating microscope, and the boundaries of the lesion were marked with a line of electrocautery. Then, using a combination of suction and electrocautery, the MFC was removed by subpial aspiration through a fine-gauge metal sucker that was insulated except at the tip. The intended MFC lesion was approximately rectangular in shape and extended rostrocaudally along the length of the rostral sulcus from the genu of the corpus callosum, caudally, to the rostral tip of the cingulate sulcus, rostrally. The dorsal boundary of the lesion was ∼2 mm ventral to the cingulate sulcus (at the midlevel of the genu of the corpus callosum), and the ventral boundary of the lesion was the fundus of the rostral sulcus.

For the OFC lesion, a large bone flap was turned over the dorsal frontal cortex. The dura was opened with a crescent-shaped cut and then reflected toward the orbit. Sulcal landmarks on the orbital surface were identified with the aid of an operating microscope. Unilateral injections of ibotenate were made into sites located ∼2 mm apart. At each site, 1.0 µl of ibotenate (10–15 µg/µl; Sigma-Aldrich) was injected as a bolus via a hand-held Hamilton syringe with a 30 gauge needle. Monkeys received between 87 and 99 injections. The intended OFC lesion extended from the fundus of the lateral orbital sulcus, laterally, to the rostral sulcus on the medial surface of the hemisphere, medially. The rostral boundary of the intended lesion was an imaginary line joining the rostral tips of the medial and lateral orbital sulci. The caudal boundary of the intended lesion was an imaginary line joining the most caudal points of the medial and lateral orbital sulci.

Lesion assessment

For the OFC and the amygdala excitotoxic lesions, the estimated damage for each monkey was assessed using a T2-weighted MRI scan obtained 4–8 d after surgery. The location and extent of excitotoxic lesions in the hippocampus are reliably indicated by the white hypersignal present on the T2-weighted postoperative scans (Málková et al., 2001). For the amygdala and surrounding structures, however, the hypersignal may represent an overestimation of damage (Basile et al., 2017). For the MFC aspiration lesions, the estimated damage for each monkey was assessed using a T1-weighted scan obtained postoperatively. Two of the four monkeys with MFC aspiration lesions (cases 3 and 4) had scans that were acquired approximately 3–4 months after surgery, whereas the remaining two (cases 1 and 2) had scans that were acquired 5 to 7 years after the surgery.

We developed a semiautomated lesion mapping procedure to assess the location and extent of the lesions. For all T2-weighted scans, volume estimates were taken by performing rigid (six-parameter rigid body transformation) and affine (diffeomorphic, allowing for 12-parameter local warps in structure) warps on each scan to the NIMH Macaque Template (NMT) version 2.0 (0.5 mm3 resolution; Seidlitz et al., 2018; Jung et al., 2020) using the Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI) 3dAllineate function for the rigid transform (Cox, 1996; Saad et al., 2009) and antsRegistrationSyN in Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs) for the affine transform (Avants et al., 2011). We then applied thresholding to identify the area of hyperintensity on the transformed T2-weighted scans to generate a binary mask that corresponded to the area of damage. These masks were visually inspected and manually edited to ensure that they fully captured the areas of hyperintensity.

For the T1 scans showing MFC damage, we used the AFNI @animal_warper command (Saad et al., 2009; Jung et al., 2020) to perform a rigid and affine alignment for each subject to the standard NMT version 2.0. For MFC damage, because the aspiration lesion resulted in collapse of nearby tissue into the space created by the lesion, we interpreted the extent of the damage in the transformed T1 scans by comparing the area of damage with the comparable region in the intact contralateral hemisphere, using anatomic and sulcal landmarks of the NMT as references. This method therefore ensured a more accurate estimation of damage than drawing the boundaries of the hypointense areas on the lesioned cortical surface.

A lesion overlap map for each region of interest, distinguished by group for the amygdala, was created by summing the binary masks for each hemisphere and displaying the outputs on NMT version 2.0 (Fig. 1A, OFC × AMY; Fig. 1B, MFC × AMY). For each subject, the extent of the damage was estimated by taking the percentage overlap of the lesion mask with the intended lesion mask for each region of interest (MFC, OFC, and amygdala) in its respective hemisphere (Table 1, OFC × AMY; Table 2, MFC × AMY).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Percentage estimated damage: OFC × AMY group

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Percentage estimated damage: MFC × AMY group

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Lesion overlap maps. A, Damage sustained by the four subjects in the OFC × AMY crossed lesion group. B, Damage sustained by the four subjects in the MFC × AMY crossed lesion group. For visualization purposes, cortical lesions are displayed on the right hemisphere, and amygdala lesions are displayed on the left hemisphere. Top two rows, Four sections of the intended cortical lesion mask (1 sagittal and 3 coronal views) with corresponding views of the extent of cortical lesion (A, B). Bottom two rows, Four sections of the intended amygdala lesion mask (1 sagittal and three coronal views) with corresponding views of the extent of amygdala lesion overlap (A, B). All coordinates for the NMT version 2.0 are displayed for reference.

In general, the lesions were as intended. Estimated damage to the amygdala ranged from 84 to 97%. Estimated OFC damage ranged from 67 to 91%. Estimated damage to the MFC ranged from 62 to 96%. One monkey in the OFC × AMY group (case 4) and another monkey in the MFC × AMY group (case 1) sustained inadvertent damage to the head of the caudate nucleus and putamen, apparently because of an infarction associated with the amygdala injections. Of note, the lesion extents reported for the OFC × AMY group in this report differ from the values reported in Fiuzat et al. (2017) because of the difference in the methodology used to estimate damage.

Experiment 1: Social valuation task

Apparatus

Briefly, monkeys were trained in a modified Wisconsin General Testing Apparatus (WGTA) located in a darkened room. Monkeys occupied a wheeled transport cage in the animal compartment of the WGTA. A clear Plexiglas box measuring 11.4 cm (width) × 71.1 cm (length) × 11.4 cm (height) with a hinged back was placed in front of the transport cage in the test compartment of the WGTA. This is the same apparatus used in other studies from this laboratory that have examined responses to objects placed inside the Plexiglas box (Pujara et al., 2019). In this study, however, we modified the apparatus to allow for the presentation of videographic materials, as in Rudebeck et al. (2006). Behind the Plexiglas box, a monitor was placed facing the front of the transport cage. On each trial, stimuli presented on the screen were 30-s video clips of one of the following four social contexts: a large monkey staring, a monkey exhibiting affiliative behaviors (lip smacking), a small monkey with food in its cheek pouches, and a female macaque with prominent perineal swelling. For further comparison, we used either moving or static neutral images (i.e., objects, scenes) and non-neutral (i.e., emotionally provocative) predator videos of a slithering snake and a moving toy crocodile. A camera (camera 1) mounted on top of the WGTA recorded movements of each monkey throughout the trial from a top-down view to track latencies of the monkeys to take food rewards placed on top of the Plexiglas box. Another camera (camera 2) was mounted on top of the monitor and faced the transport cage to record gaze information for subsequent analyses of visual attention to the videos.

Procedure

All monkeys were first required to retrieve food rewards that were located on top of the Plexiglas box while the box was empty for 20 trials. By the end of pretraining, monkeys were familiar with the Plexiglas box and the procedure of reaching for rewards overlaying the box. Stimuli were shown in a fixed order of presentation across two separate test sessions for each monkey. The first test included presentations of the lip-smacking monkey, the female perinea, and the snake videos among seven neutral stimuli for a total of 10 trials. The second test included presentations of the small monkey, the staring monkey, and the crocodile videos among seven neutral stimuli for a total of 10 trials.

On each trial, the opaque moveable screen separating the animal compartment and the test compartment of the WGTA was raised to reveal the video playing on the monitor and a piece of food on the Plexiglas box simultaneously. Therefore, valuation of the content of the video was pitted against the incentive value of the food. Monkeys were given 30 s to reach for the piece of food and view the video. After 30 s, the screen was lowered to signify the end of a trial. Trials were separated by 30 s. Social valuation was operationalized as the difference in the latency to reach for the food reward in the presence of the social videos compared with the neutral stimuli (see below in the Statistical analysis section for the method used to code food-retrieval latencies). Note that use of the term “social valuation” is shorthand for a collection of phenomena that reflect either learned or innate tendencies to attend to and process social signals. In using the term social valuation, we include related aspects of social cognition, including the orientation of selective attention to socially relevant visual stimuli, affective reactions and responses to those stimuli, subjective valuations and assessments of those stimuli, and inherent interest in social signals. In this sense, our measure reflects social valuation or social interest.

Experiment 2: Reinforcer devaluation task

Apparatus

As in experiment 1, all testing was conducted in the WGTA environment, and monkeys were tested inside a wheeled transport cage in the animal compartment of the WGTA. The test compartment of the WGTA held a test tray with two food wells spaced 235 mm apart. Test material for reinforcer devaluation consisted of 120 objects that varied in size, shape, color, and texture. Food rewards for the devaluation task consisted of two of the following six foods: M&Ms (Mars Candies), half peanuts, raisins, craisins (Ocean Spray), banana-flavored pellets (Noyes) and fruit snacks (Giant Foods).

Procedure

Procedures for the reinforcer devaluation task have been previously described (Baxter et al., 2000; Rudebeck et al., 2017a). First, the preference of each monkey for six different foods was assessed over a 15 d period. Each day, monkeys received 30 trials consisting of pairwise presentations of the six different foods, one each in the left and right wells of the test tray (e.g., half peanut on the left vs craisin on the right). The left-right position of the foods was counterbalanced. Preferences were determined by analyzing choices within each of the 15 possible pairs of foods over the final 5 d of testing. Two foods that the monkey valued highly and that were roughly equally palatable as judged by choices in the food preference test were selected for subsequent behavioral shaping and testing.

Object discrimination learning was used to set up unique object–outcome associations, followed by reinforcer devaluation tests, in which probe trials gauged abilities of monkeys to link objects with current food value. For object discrimination, monkeys were trained to discriminate 60 pairs of novel objects. For each pair, one object was randomly designated as the positive object (S+, rewarded) and the other was designated as negative (S−, unrewarded). Half of the positive objects were baited with food 1, and the other half were baited with food 2. For each monkey, the identity of foods 1 and 2 was based on the previously determined food preferences of the monkey. On each trial of discrimination training, monkeys were presented with a pair of objects, each covering a food well, and were allowed to displace one of the two objects. If they displaced the S+ object, they were allowed to retrieve the food, which led to termination of the trial. If they displaced the S− object, no food was available, and the trial was terminated. The left-right position of the S+ followed a pseudorandom order. Training continued until the monkeys attained the criterion of a mean of 90% correct responses over 5 consecutive days (i.e., 270 correct responses or greater in 300 trials).

The object choices of the monkeys were then assessed under two conditions for the reinforcer devaluation task: (1) under normal (baseline) conditions and (2) after one of the foods was devalued via selective satiety. On separate days, four test sessions were conducted, each consisting of 30 trials. Only the positive (S+) objects were used, so that on each trial, a food-1 object and a food-2 object were presented together for choice. Each object covered a well baited with the corresponding food. The object pairs were generated randomly for each session, with the constraint that a food-1 object was always paired with a food-2 object. Preceding two of the test sessions, a selective satiation procedure, intended to diminish the value of one of the foods, was conducted. For the other two test sessions, which provided baseline scores, monkeys were not sated on either food before being tested. The order in which the test sessions occurred was the same for all monkeys and was as follows: (1) baseline test 1, (2) food 1 devalued by selective satiation before test session, (3) baseline test 2, and (4) food 2 devalued by selective satiation before test session. A second test of object choice, identical to the first, was conducted between 3 and 4 months after the first object choice test. Monkeys were retrained on the same 60 object pairs to the same criterion as before. After relearning, the object choice test was conducted in the same manner as the first test.

For the selective satiation procedure, a food hopper was filled with a preweighed quantity of either food 1 or food 2 and was attached to the front of the home cage of the monkey. The monkey was given a total of 30 min to consume as much of the food as it wanted, at which point the experimenter began to observe the behavior of the monkey. Additional food was provided if necessary. The selective satiation procedure was deemed to be complete when the monkey refrained from retrieving food from the hopper for 5 min. The amount of time taken in the selective satiation procedure and the total amount of food consumed by each monkey was noted. The monkey was then taken to the WGTA within 10 min, and the test session was conducted.

We assessed the effect of selective satiation on the monkeys choices of the foods alone between 30 and 60 d after the second test of object choice. This food-only test was conducted to evaluate whether satiety transferred from the home cage to the WGTA, and whether behavioral effects of the lesion (if any) were because of an inability to link objects with food value as opposed to an inability to discriminate the foods. This test was identical to both reinforcer devaluation tests 1 and 2, with the important difference that no objects were presented over the two wells where foods were placed. On each trial of the 30-trial sessions, monkeys could see the two foods and were allowed to choose between them. As was the case for object choice tests 1 and 2, there were four critical test sessions, in which two were preceded by selective satiation and two were not.

Statistical analysis

Social valuation

Because of the small sample sizes of our experimental groups, we used nonparametric tests for all our within-test analyses. Food-retrieval latencies were derived from analyses of the video recordings from camera 1, which provided a top-down view of the compartment. Latencies were scored to the nearest frame and had a resolution of ∼4 ms. Time for the latency measure was initiated when the opaque screen was raised ∼15 cm above the test tray. This could be discerned in the videotape by a mark on the cage, visible in the view of camera 2, which provided a frontal view of the behavioral responses of the monkeys to the videos presented on the monitor. The response was considered complete when the monkey grasped the food reward just before withdrawing its arm. If no response was made within the trial limit of 30 s, a score of 30 s was recorded. To observe group differences in duration of time spent viewing the videos, we used the front view from camera 2 to measure the length of time the monkeys looked at the monitor screen that was used to display the stimuli (look-at duration, in seconds) for the full 30 s of the trial. All measurements were taken by an observer who was naive to group assignment.

For each monkey, food-retrieval latencies were averaged across both tests for stimuli in the neutral category, nonsocial affective category (nonsocial. predator), and social category. We first tested whether social videos elicited longer looking times compared with neutral stimuli in controls, as shown previously (Rudebeck et al., 2006; Noonan et al., 2010a), by running a within-subject signed-rank Wilcoxon test. To test the effects of crossed surgical disconnections of either the MFC or OFC with the amygdala on social valuation, we then ran separate two-tailed Kruskal–Wallis tests for the retrieval latency difference between the social and neutral stimulus categories, followed by post hoc Mann–Whitney U tests to compare the experimental lesion groups with the intact control group and with each other.

As a control, to determine whether the groups differed in the amount of time spent viewing the videos, we ran separate two-tailed Kruskal–Wallis tests for the social and neutral stimulus categories on the stimulus look-at durations, followed by post hoc Mann–Whitney U tests to compare the experimental lesion groups with each other and with the intact control group. Finally, we compared the two lesion groups and controls on their food-retrieval latencies for the nonsocial predator videos (i.e., snake and alligator) to test whether the lesion groups showed altered responses to this category of stimuli.

Reinforcer devaluation

Performance on reinforcer devaluation was measured by calculating a proportion shifted score (Murray et al., 2015) for object choice test 1, object choice test 2, and the food choice test. The proportion shifted score is defined as a shift in the number of food-1- and food-2-associated objects (object test) or food 1 and food 2 (food only test) choices after selective satiation relative to baseline choices, combined across probe tests for food 1 and food 2. The proportion shifted scores obtained from object choice test 1 and object choice test 2 were averaged to obtain a final object test proportion shifted score for each subject. A higher proportion shifted score indicates a greater shift away from choices associated with the devalued food.

To test the effects of crossed surgical disconnections of either the MFC or OFC with the amygdala on value updating, we ran separate two-tailed Kruskal–Wallis tests for proportion shifted scores obtained from object choices, followed by post hoc Mann–Whitney U tests to compare the experimental lesion groups with the intact control group and with each other. As a control, to demonstrate that the groups did not differ in their ability to update choices for the foods themselves following selective satiation (i.e., that impairments were specific to updating stimulus–outcome associations), we ran a separate two-tailed Kruskal–Wallis test for proportion shifted scores for food choices.

Double dissociation

To determine whether there was a double dissociation between the lesion groups, we normalized (z scored) the food-retrieval latencies from the social valuation task (social condition minus neutral condition) and the average proportion shifted scores from the object choice tests to better approximate normality across tasks. Because there is not an appropriate nonparametric test to compare test performance on two independent tests from two independent samples, we ran a 2 × 2 ANOVA to test for a significant group (OFC × AMY, MFC × AMY) by task (social, nonsocial) interaction, which would indicate a double dissociation between the two groups on task performance.

Results

Social valuation

We first evaluated whether social videos attracted the interest of unoperated control monkeys, as shown previously (Rudebeck et al., 2006; Noonan et al., 2010a), indexed by longer food-retrieval latencies in the presence of the social videos compared with neutral stimuli. We found that controls were slower to reach for food rewards when presented with social compared to neutral stimuli (Wilcoxon signed-rank paired test, testing whether the median latency in the neutral condition is less than the median latency in the social condition: V = 0, p = 0.03; Fig. 2). Thus, in line with previous work, social videos slowed food reward retrieval within the unoperated control group.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Social valuation task, food retrieval. A, Mean food-retrieval latencies when monkeys viewed videos in the neutral category. B, Mean food-retrieval latencies when monkeys viewed videos in the social category. C, Mean food-retrieval latencies when monkeys viewed videos in the nonsocial predator category. Error bars represent SEM. Individual cases for each group are plotted on the x-axis, and food-retrieval latencies (in seconds) are plotted on the y-axis; *p < 0.05. C-1 to C-5, Unoperated controls; M-1 to M-4, monkeys with crossed surgical disconnection of the MFC and amygdala; O-1 to O-4, monkeys with crossed surgical disconnection of the OFC and amygdala.

We then ran a Kruskal–Wallis test for latency differences between the social and neutral conditions across groups to test for a main effect of group. We found a nonsignificant effect of group (χ2=4.45,p=0.1,df=2). Given the presence of an outlier in the control group for the neutral stimulus category (case 3, latency, 22.2 s; 1 SD above the mean), we reran this analysis without this subject and report a significant effect of group on the social minus neutral contrast (χ2=6.04,p=0.05,df=2). Pairwise post hoc tests between groups revealed a significant difference between the MFC × AMY group and the control group so that the MFC × AMY group was faster to reach for food rewards than the control subjects in the presence of the social videos compared with the neutral videos (W = 16, p = 0.03). The OFC × AMY group did not differ from the control group (W = 12, p = 0.34), nor did the lesion groups differ from each other (W = 13, p = 0.2).

We also ran the analysis separately for the social condition and the neutral condition including the outlier and found a main effect of group for the social condition (χ2=7.78, p = 0.02, df=2; Fig. 2B) but not the neutral condition (χ2=3.22, p = 0.20, df=2; Fig. 2A). Specifically, we found that the MFC × AMY group was significantly faster to reach for food rewards than the control subjects in the presence of the social videos (unpaired Mann–Whitney U test, W = 20, p = 0.02) but not the neutral stimuli (unpaired Mann–Whitney U test, W = 16, p = 0.19). The OFC × AMY group did not differ from either group in the social condition (unpaired Mann–Whitney U test for OFC × AMY vs MFC × AMY, W = 12, p = 0.34; OFC × AMY vs CON, W = 18, p = 0.06, trending toward significance) or the neutral condition (unpaired Mann–Whitney U test for OFC × AMY vs MFC × AMY, W = 10, p = 0.67; OFC × AMY vs CON, W = 16, p = 0.19).

Although these data suggest that an MFC × AMY circuit is necessary for social valuation, to validate this interpretation, we attempted to rule out the possibility that subjects with MFC × AMY lesions were simply ignoring the stimuli, which might indicate a global disruption of visual attention. All groups spent more time viewing the social videos (Fig. 3B) compared with the neutral stimuli (Fig. 3A), as indicated by a significant main effect of stimulus category for viewing durations (χ2=14.21, p = 0.0002, df=1). Importantly, we found no significant main effect of group on the duration of time spent looking at the social videos (χ2=0.10, p = 0.95, df=2), suggesting intact visual attention across groups.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Social valuation task, viewing time. A, Mean time spent viewing the videos in the neutral category. B, Mean time spent viewing videos in the social category. C, Mean time spent viewing the videos in the nonsocial: predator category. Error bars represent SEM. Individual cases for each group are plotted on the x-axis and mean viewing times (in seconds) are plotted on the y-axis. C-1 to C-5, Unoperated controls; M-1 to M-4, monkeys with crossed surgical disconnection of the MFC and amygdala; O-1 to O-4, monkeys with crossed surgical disconnection of the OFC and amygdala.

Finally, we predicted that both operated groups would show reduced food-retrieval latencies to emotionally relevant stimuli given observed reductions in affective reactivity to predator stimuli following either unilateral (Izquierdo and Murray, 2004) or bilateral amygdala damage (Meunier et al., 1999; Izquierdo et al., 2005; Chudasama et al., 2009; Machado et al., 2009; Bliss-Moreau et al., 2011; Medina et al., 2020). Contrary to this prediction, there was no main effect of group for the difference in food-retrieval latencies for the predator videos compared with the neutral videos (χ2=0.90, p = 0.64, df=2; Fig. 2), nor did we observe a significant effect of group for the total video viewing time for the predator videos (χ2=0.30; p = 0.86, df=2; Fig. 3). We identified case 2 in the control group as an outlier (latency, 3.77 s; 1 SD below the mean). We therefore reran the Kruskal–Wallis analysis without this subject and did not observe a significant main effect of group on the difference in food-retrieval latencies in the presence of the predator videos compared with the neutral videos (χ2=0.03,p=0.22,df=2).

Reinforcer devaluation

We calculated a proportion shifted score for each subject and each test, with a higher proportion shifted score indicating a greater shift away from the object associated with the sated food. We found a significant effect of group on the proportion shifted score for object choices (χ2=6.50, p = 0.04, df=2; Fig. 4A). Whereas the MFC × AMY group did not differ from controls (unpaired Mann–Whitney U test, W = 12.5, p = 0.62), the OFC × AMY group showed significantly smaller proportion shifted scores for object choices compared with the control group (unpaired Mann–Whitney U test, W = 20, p = 0.02) but not compared with the MFC × AMY group (unpaired Mann–Whitney U test, W = 2, p = 0.11). As predicted, groups did not differ with respect to proportion shifted scores when presented with the actual foods for choice following selective satiation (df=2, p = 0.16, df=2; Fig. 4B). Therefore, all monkeys were able to identify foods by vision and had intact satiety mechanisms. Only monkeys with OFC × AMY lesions, however, were unable to link objects with the current value of the associated foods.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Reinforcer devaluation task. A, Mean proportion shifted scores for object choices. B, Mean proportion shifted scores for food choices. Individual cases for each group are plotted on the x-axis, and proportion shifted scores are plotted on the y-axis. Error bars represent SEM; *p < 0.05. C-1 to C-5, Unoperated controls; M-1 to M-4, monkeys with crossed surgical disconnection of the MFC and amygdala; O-1 to O-4, monkeys with crossed surgical disconnection of the OFC and amygdala.

Double dissociation of functional interactions in prefrontal–amygdala circuits

Experiments 1 and 2 together show that MFC interaction with the amygdala is necessary for the expression of social value, whereas OFC interaction with the amygdala is essential for updating information about object and/or food value. To demonstrate that these circuits are dissociable in terms of their designated functions, we directly compared lesion group performances on both tasks. We normalized the food-retrieval latencies from the social valuation task (social condition minus neutral condition) and the average proportion shifted scores from the object choice tests. We ran a 2 × 2 ANOVA and found a significant group-by-task interaction, (F(1,15) = 5.65, p = 0.03; Fig. 5), indicating that these circuits make selective contributions to social cognition and value-based decision-making.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Double dissociation between groups on social valuation task performance versus reinforcer devaluation task performance. The x-axis displays each task, and y-axis displays normalized z scores. Data for each group are plotted in a separate line. Error bars represent SEM. M-1 to M-4, monkeys with crossed surgical disconnection of the MFC and amygdala; O-1 to O-4, monkeys with crossed surgical disconnection of the OFC and amygdala.

Discussion

In support of our hypothesis, we showed that MFC interaction with the amygdala is necessary for the expression of social value, whereas OFC interaction with the amygdala is essential for linking objects with current food value. In a direct comparison of task performances, we found that these prefrontal–amygdala circuits make selective and dissociable contributions to behavior.

Mechanisms of MFC–amygdala interaction during social cognition

A wealth of evidence indicates that the MFC supports social cognition. For example, MFC damage in macaques yields reduced sensitivity to social information (Hadland et al., 2003; Rudebeck et al., 2006), and single-unit recordings reveal populations of neurons in the MFC that encode information about rewards for other versus self (Chang et al., 2013) and preferentially encode conspecific faces (Dinh et al., 2018). Decades of human neuroimaging work has resulted in the characterization of a so-called social brain network, which includes the amygdala, MFC, and superior temporal sulcus/temporoparietal junction (Blakemore, 2008; Apps et al., 2016; Noonan et al., 2017; Burgos-Robles et al., 2019; Lockwood et al., 2020; Gangopadhyay et al., 2021). Recently, it was found that monkeys with bilateral, excitotoxic MFC lesions, unlike controls, failed to develop prosocial tendencies in a reward allocation learning task involving the selection of visual cues that predicted reward delivery to the self, another monkey, or no one (Basile et al., 2020). In a recent multisite neural recording study in monkeys using a variant of the same task, oscillatory interactions between the MFC (specifically the ACC, including areas 32, 24b, and 24a) and amygdala were enhanced when monkeys chose to give juice to another monkey but suppressed when a monkey acted antisocially (Dal Monte et al., 2020). Although our experiment did not require associative learning in a social context, these and our results warrant future studies to examine whether learned cue–outcome associations from socially derived information are disrupted following transient or chronic interruption of MFC–amygdala activity.

One possible interpretation of our findings from the social valuation task is that social information may be modifying the value of the food rewards themselves. We think it is unlikely that the videos with social content literally alter the value of the food. The food on offer is the same food across trials and the internal state of the monkey changes little across the 10 trials in the test session. Rather than social information modifying food value, we suggest instead that the social valuation task involves two stimuli of value (the video clips and the food items) that compete for attention, for example. This is consistent with the idea that when given a choice, monkeys choose to collect more information about conspecifics and will forego juice reward to do so (Deaner and Platt, 2003; Deaner et al., 2005). Against this idea, all groups showed similar increases in viewing time of social relative to neutral videos. A closely related possibility, not mutually exclusive with the first, is that monkeys with MFC × AMY lesions have altered prioritization of acting on social and food signals. Acting on social signals would bias monkeys to view videos longer now to collect more information about conspecifics, whereas acting on food cues would bias monkeys to approach and retrieve the food. It is possible that it is this prioritization that is altered by the MFC × AMY crossed disconnection as opposed to the appreciation of the social signals (social valuation or social interest) per se. We favor these latter two interpretations because our task is designed to pit social signals against food, just as the original version of this task (Izquierdo and Murray, 2004) pits predator avoidance against approach to food. Indeed, we believe the task is a relatively sensitive assay in both cases precisely because the default action is approach food. Additional evidence from Hadland et al. (2003), in particular, demonstrates that damage of the MFC has an impact on social behaviors toward conspecifics independent of food value. In that study, which involved no food manipulation, monkeys with MFC damage chose to sit farther away from a conspecific compared with control monkeys. Further, given the adaptive food choices made by the MFC × AMY group in the reinforcer devaluation task, we maintain that food value was not significantly altered in the MFC × AMY group in a way that would preclude an interpretation of dissociable performance on the social valuation task. Indeed, the use of food as a way to assess relative value in both tasks represents a strength of the current study; any alterations in food value would be observed as nonspecific impairments in task performance in both tasks regardless of lesion group.

Although at least two studies have found no effects of OFC damage on social cognition in macaques (Rudebeck et al., 2006; Noonan et al., 2010b), there is some evidence in support of OFC involvement in social cognition as well (Watson and Platt, 2012; Goursaud and Bachevalier, 2020). One line of evidence comes from the discovery of face patches in the lateral orbital sulcus of the OFC (Tsao et al., 2008; Troiani et al., 2016). Recording studies suggest that these patches contain face-selective neurons that categorize social information from faces (Barat et al., 2018). In our own data, the OFC × AMY group shows a trend-level difference from controls on their food-retrieval latencies in the presence of the social videos. This raises the possibility that the division of labor between MFC and OFC in social cognition may not be so clear cut and that perhaps an intra-PFC network involving subregions of MFC and OFC encodes information that is relevant to social value and learning from social contexts. Another possibility, not mutually exclusive with the first, is that unilateral amygdala damage produces a mild disruption of social evaluation.

The amygdala has ubiquitously been implicated in social cognition (Adolphs et al., 1998; Amaral, 2002; Adolphs, 2010). Grabenhorst et al. (2019) reported that a population of neurons in the macaque amygdala simulate decisions of a conspecific before its choice during an observational learning task involving juice rewards. An intracranial recording study in humans also presents the possibility that there are two populations of neurons in the amygdala that encode cue–outcome associations from experienced (self) and observed (other) outcomes (Aquino et al., 2020). Another study in macaques found a population of amygdala neurons that code for both juice reward value and conspecific social rank, whereas the OFC and ACC coded for reward values but not hierarchical rank (Munuera et al., 2018). Together, and consistent with a role for MFC–amygdala interactions in social cognition, these data suggest that the amygdala may be a site where stimulus–outcome value associations and social information are integrated.

Mechanisms of OFC–amygdala interactions during value updating

Individually, the OFC (Izquierdo et al., 2004; Machado and Bachevalier, 2007; Noonan et al., 2010b; Rudebeck et al., 2013a; 2017a) and the amygdala (Málková et al., 1997; Izquierdo and Murray, 2007), but not the MFC (Chudasama et al., 2013; Rhodes and Murray, 2013), are necessary for nonsocial, value-based decisions in macaques. The present study confirms the results of an earlier study examining OFC–amygdala disconnection (Baxter et al., 2000) and extends those findings in two ways. First, the present study, unlike the earlier one, used selective, excitotoxic lesions of OFC. Thus, the results confirm an essential role in decision-making for amygdala projections to or from neurons in OFC. Second, the present study, unlike the earlier one, did not include a section of the forebrain commissures as part of the surgical disconnection. That the impairments in value updating follow from crossed surgical disconnection with the forebrain commissures intact indicate that the behavior depends on intrahemispheric interactions of the amygdala and OFC. In addition, previous work in macaques demonstrates a critical role for amygdala inputs to OFC but not MFC in sustaining reward value coding for well-learned stimulus–reward amounts (Rudebeck et al., 2013b) and in acquiring value coding in terms of both the information signaled and the proportion of neurons encoding stimulus–reward amounts during learning (Rudebeck et al., 2017b).

The possible mechanisms by which OFC and amygdala interactions subserve value updating is also starting to be unraveled. In macaques, pharmacological inactivation of posterior OFC (Murray et al., 2015) or the basolateral amygdala (Wellman et al., 2005) during but not after selective satiation impairs adaptive shifts in object choices on the devaluation task. Together, these studies imply that the OFC critically interacts with the amygdala to register changes in food value during the selective satiation procedure, and that this updated information is used to guide subsequent goal-directed choices. A mechanistic account has been proposed wherein the basolateral amygdala is involved in updating stimulus–outcome associations and the OFC maintains representations of past and present associations that are retrieved by the basolateral amygdala in the updating process (Sharpe and Schoenbaum, 2016; Lichtenberg et al., 2017). Thus, communication between the amygdala and the OFC appears to be critical for accurate value representations and cue–outcome associations to allay maladaptive choices.

Limitations and future directions

There are a number of limitations that may be addressed by future work. First, our social valuation task constitutes just one of many approaches for probing social cognition. Because only four discrete stimuli of conspecifics were used, we were unable to reveal potentially interesting differences in reactivity among different classes of social stimuli (e.g., submissive vs dominant, female vs male). The small number of subjects in each group also precluded our ability to determine whether dominance status of the monkeys changed following the lesions and whether dominance status at the time of testing was a contributing factor in video evaluation. Additionally, our measure of gaze duration for the stimuli was a crude estimation of video fixation duration. A follow-up study using eye-tracking measures for multiple social stimulus categories would isolate the effects of attention from social valuation by allowing the detection of subtle differences in gaze-fixation patterns between social and nonsocial stimuli among lesion and control groups, if any, as in Deaner et al. (2005). In addition, more direct indices of social behaviors and physiological measures such as pupil diameter and cardiovascular responses might provide a more detailed explanation of the effects of the MFC × AMY lesion on monkeys exposed to stimuli depicting conspecifics.

Another limitation in our study is the use of aspiration lesions to target the MFC. We cannot rule out the possibility that the behavioral effects of the MFC lesions were because of unintended damage to fibers of passage. Consistent with a role for MFC in social behavior, a recent study using bilateral excitotoxic lesions of the MFC in a region that overlaps with that studied here reported a reduction in prosocial preferences in monkeys (Basile et al., 2020). Thus, aspiration lesions (as opposed to excitotoxic lesions) of MFC are not essential to observing effects on social behavior. In addition, the area of the MFC targeted in our study may not have covered the full extent of the medial wall within the boundaries of the ACC thought to be involved in social cognition (Apps et al., 2016). Importantly, however, the MFC cortex lesion in our study included the gyrus of the ACC, which was found to be critical in supporting prosocial behaviors, relative to the sulcus of the ACC, which has not been implicated in these behaviors (Rudebeck et al., 2006; Chang et al., 2013). This region also contains neurons responsive to conspecific faces (Dinh et al., 2018).

Finally, although our subjects were reared in social groups, recent preliminary findings indicate that adult housing conditions can influence affective responding of macaques, with singly housed monkeys showing reduced reactivity to threatening stimuli compared with socially housed monkeys (Charbonneau et al., 2022). Thus, the present findings should be considered in that context. Notably, however, if a reduction in affective responding held for macaques in the present study, we would expect that had monkeys not been housed singly we may have observed longer food-retrieval latencies in the presence of the social videos for controls, thus making a double dissociation effect more prominent.

Conclusion

In this study, we show that the MFC and amygdala must interact to support the ability to ascribe value to others. Dysfunction in this circuitry may underlie social affiliation deficits, that is, marked reductions in an interest in and/or empathy for others, observed in psychopathy (Viding and McCrory, 2019) and autism spectrum disorder (Thakkar et al., 2008; DeMayo et al., 2019). We further show that the OFC and amygdala form a functional circuit that is essential for sustaining adaptive stimulus choice behaviors following changes in the incentive value of food rewards. Dysregulation in this circuit may underlie disorders in which compulsive and perseverative behaviors are observed, such as substance use disorder (Koob and Volkow, 2016; Bechara et al., 2019) and obsessive-compulsive disorder (Gillan and Robbins, 2014). The framework presented in the current study regarding the separable functions of these cortico-amygdala circuits will require additional testing to determine the anatomic specificity, molecular substrates, and temporal dynamics that give rise to these complex behaviors.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute of Mental Health (Grant ZIAMH002887 to E.A.M.). M.S.P. was supported by a National Institutes of Health Center for Compulsive Behavior Postdoctoral Fellowship. We thank Dawn Lundgren, Emily Moylan, and Emily Fiuzat for assistance with data collection; Richard Saunders and Emily Moylan for help performing surgery; and Ben Jung, Jakob Seidlitz, and Adam Messinger for assistance with the lesion assessments. We also thank the staff of the Nuclear Magnetic Resonance Facility, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, and the Laboratory of Diagnostic and Radiology Research.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Elisabeth A. Murray at murraye{at}mail.nih.gov

SfN exclusive license.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Adolphs R
    (2010) What does the amygdala contribute to social cognition? Ann N Y Acad Sci 1191:42–61. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05445.x pmid:20392275
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Adolphs R,
    2. Tranel D,
    3. Damasio AR
    (1998) The human amygdala in social judgment. Nature 393:470–474. doi:10.1038/30982 pmid:9624002
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Amaral DG
    (2002) The primate amygdala and the neurobiology of social behavior: implications for understanding social anxiety. Biol Psychiatry 51:11–17. doi:10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01307-5 pmid:11801227
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Anderson SW,
    2. Bechara A,
    3. Damasio H,
    4. Tranel D,
    5. Damasio AR
    (1999) Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 2:1032–1037. doi:10.1038/14833 pmid:10526345
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Apps MA,
    2. Rushworth MF,
    3. Chang SW
    (2016) The anterior cingulate gyrus and social cognition: tracking the motivation of others. Neuron 90:692–707. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2016.04.018 pmid:27196973
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Aquino TG,
    2. Minxha J,
    3. Dunne S,
    4. Ross IB,
    5. Mamelak AN,
    6. Rutishauser U,
    7. O'Doherty JP
    (2020) Value-related neuronal responses in the human amygdala during observational learning. J Neurosci 40:4761–4772.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    1. Avants BB,
    2. Tustison NJ,
    3. Song G,
    4. Cook PA,
    5. Klein A,
    6. Gee JC
    (2011) A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. Neuroimage 54:2033–2044. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025 pmid:20851191
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Barat E,
    2. Wirth S,
    3. Duhamel JR
    (2018) Face cells in orbitofrontal cortex represent social categories. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 115:E11158–E11167. doi:10.1073/pnas.1806165115 pmid:30397122
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Barbas H,
    2. Pandya DN
    (1989) Architecture and intrinsic connections of the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol 286:353–375. doi:10.1002/cne.902860306 pmid:2768563
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Barrash J,
    2. Tranel D,
    3. Anderson SW
    (2000) Acquired personality disturbances associated with bilateral damage to the ventromedial prefrontal region. Dev Neuropsychol 18:355–381. doi:10.1207/S1532694205Barrash pmid:11385830
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Barrash J,
    2. Stuss DT,
    3. Aksan N,
    4. Anderson SW,
    5. Jones RD,
    6. Manzel K,
    7. Tranel D
    (2018) “Frontal lobe syndrome”? Subtypes of acquired personality disturbances in patients with focal brain damage. Cortex 106:65–80. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2018.05.007 pmid:29883878
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Basile BM,
    2. Karaskiewicz CL,
    3. Fiuzat EC,
    4. Malkova L,
    5. Murray EA
    (2017) MRI overestimates excitotoxic amygdala lesion damage in rhesus monkeys. Front Integr Neurosci 11:12. doi:10.3389/fnint.2017.00012 pmid:28642691
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Basile BM,
    2. Schafroth JL,
    3. Karaskiewicz CL,
    4. Chang SWC,
    5. Murray EA
    (2020) The anterior cingulate cortex is necessary for forming prosocial preferences from vicarious reinforcement in monkeys. PLoS Biol 18:e3000677. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.3000677 pmid:32530910
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Baxter MG,
    2. Parker A,
    3. Lindner CC,
    4. Izquierdo AD,
    5. Murray EA
    (2000) Control of response selection by reinforcer value requires interaction of amygdala and orbital prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 20:4311–4319. pmid:10818166
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Bechara A,
    2. Berridge KC,
    3. Bickel WK,
    4. Morón JA,
    5. Williams SB,
    6. Stein JS
    (2019) A neurobehavioral approach to addiction: implications for the opioid epidemic and the psychology of addiction. Psychol Sci Public Interest 20:96–127. doi:10.1177/1529100619860513 pmid:31591935
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Beer JS,
    2. John OP,
    3. Scabini D,
    4. Knight RT
    (2006) Orbitofrontal cortex and social behavior: integrating self-monitoring and emotion-cognition interactions. J Cogn Neurosci 18:871–879. doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.6.871 pmid:16839295
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Blakemore SJ
    (2008) The social brain in adolescence. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:267–277. doi:10.1038/nrn2353 pmid:18354399
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Bliss-Moreau E,
    2. Bauman MD,
    3. Amaral DG
    (2011) Neonatal amygdala lesions result in globally blunted affect in adult rhesus macaques. Behav Neurosci 125:848–858. doi:10.1037/a0025757 pmid:21988521
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Brand M,
    2. Grabenhorst F,
    3. Starcke K,
    4. Vandekerckhove MM,
    5. Markowitsch HJ
    (2007) Role of the amygdala in decisions under ambiguity and decisions under risk: evidence from patients with Urbach-Wiethe disease. Neuropsychologia 45:1305–1317. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.021 pmid:17070876
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Burgos-Robles A,
    2. Gothard KM,
    3. Monfils MH,
    4. Morozov A,
    5. Vicentic A
    (2019) Conserved features of anterior cingulate networks support observational learning across species. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 107:215–228. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.09.009 pmid:31509768
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Camille N,
    2. Coricelli G,
    3. Sallet J,
    4. Pradat-Diehl P,
    5. Duhamel JR,
    6. Sirigu A
    (2004) The involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex in the experience of regret. Science 304:1167–1170. doi:10.1126/science.1094550 pmid:15155951
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Carmichael ST,
    2. Price JL
    (1994) Architectonic subdivision of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol 346:366–402. doi:10.1002/cne.903460305 pmid:7527805
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Carmichael ST,
    2. Price JL
    (1995) Limbic connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 363:615–641. doi:10.1002/cne.903630408 pmid:8847421
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Chang SW,
    2. Gariépy JF,
    3. Platt ML
    (2013) Neuronal reference frames for social decisions in primate frontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 16:243–250. doi:10.1038/nn.3287 pmid:23263442
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Charbonneau JA,
    2. Amaral DG,
    3. Bliss-Moreau E
    (2022) Social housing status impacts rhesus monkeys' affective responding in classic threat processing tasks. Scientific reports 12:4140.
    OpenUrl
  26. ↵
    1. Chudasama Y,
    2. Izquierdo A,
    3. Murray EA
    (2009) Distinct contributions of the amygdala and hippocampus to fear expression. Eur J Neurosci 30:2327–2337. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.07012.x pmid:20092575
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Chudasama Y,
    2. Daniels TE,
    3. Gorrin DP,
    4. Rhodes SE,
    5. Rudebeck PH,
    6. Murray EA
    (2013) The role of the anterior cingulate cortex in choices based on reward value and reward contingency. Cereb Cortex 23:2884–2898. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs266 pmid:22944530
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Ciaramelli E,
    2. Muccioli M,
    3. Làdavas E,
    4. di Pellegrino G
    (2007) Selective deficit in personal moral judgment following damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2:84–92. doi:10.1093/scan/nsm001 pmid:18985127
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Cox RW
    (1996) AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res 29:162–173. doi:10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014 pmid:8812068
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Dal Monte O,
    2. Chu CCJ,
    3. Fagan NA,
    4. Chang SWC
    (2020) Specialized medial prefrontal-amygdala coordination in other-regarding decision preference. Nat Neurosci 23:565–574. doi:10.1038/s41593-020-0593-y pmid:32094970
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Deaner RO,
    2. Platt ML
    (2003) Reflexive social attention in monkeys and humans. Curr Biol 13:1609–1613. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2003.08.025 pmid:13678591
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Deaner RO,
    2. Khera AV,
    3. Platt ML
    (2005) Monkeys pay per view: adaptive valuation of social images by rhesus macaques. Curr Biol 15:543–548. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.044 pmid:15797023
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. DeMayo MM,
    2. Young LJ,
    3. Hickie IB,
    4. Song YJC,
    5. Guastella AJ
    (2019) Circuits for social learning: a unified model and application to autism spectrum disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 107:388–398. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.09.034 pmid:31560922
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Dicks D,
    2. Myers RE,
    3. Kling A
    (1968) Uncus and amygdala lesions: effects on social behavior in the free-ranging rhesus monkey. Science 165:69–71.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Dinh HT,
    2. Nishimaru H,
    3. Matsumoto J,
    4. Takamura Y,
    5. Le QV,
    6. Hori E,
    7. Maior RS,
    8. Tomaz C,
    9. Tran AH,
    10. Ono T,
    11. Nishijo H
    (2018) Superior neuronal detection of snakes and conspecific faces in the macaque medial prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex 28:2131–2145.
    OpenUrl
  36. ↵
    1. Emery NJ,
    2. Capitanio JP,
    3. Mason WA,
    4. Machado CJ,
    5. Mendoza SP,
    6. Amaral DG
    (2001) The effects of bilateral lesions of the amygdala on dyadic social interactions in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behavioral Neuroscience 115:515–544.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Eslinger PJ,
    2. Damasio AR
    (1985) Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: patient EVR. Neurology 35:1731–1741. doi:10.1212/wnl.35.12.1731 pmid:4069365
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Fellows LK,
    2. Farah MJ
    (2003) Ventromedial frontal cortex mediates affective shifting in humans: evidence from a reversal learning paradigm. Brain 126:1830–1837. doi:10.1093/brain/awg180 pmid:12821528
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Fiuzat EC,
    2. Rhodes SE,
    3. Murray EA
    (2017) The role of orbitofrontal-amygdala interactions in updating action-outcome valuations in macaques. J Neurosci 37:2463–2470. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1839-16.2017 pmid:28148725
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    1. Gangopadhyay P,
    2. Chawla M,
    3. Dal Monte O,
    4. Chang SWC
    (2021) Prefrontal-amygdala circuits in social decision-making. Nat Neurosci 24:5–18. doi:10.1038/s41593-020-00738-9 pmid:33169032
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Ghashghaei HT,
    2. Barbas H
    (2002) Pathways for emotion: interactions of prefrontal and anterior temporal pathways in the amygdala of the rhesus monkey. Neuroscience 115:1261–1279. doi:10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00446-3 pmid:12453496
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Gillan CM,
    2. Robbins TW
    (2014) Goal-directed learning and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 369:20130475.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Goursaud AS,
    2. Bachevalier J
    (2020) Altered face scanning and arousal after orbitofrontal cortex lesion in adult rhesus monkeys. Behav Neurosci 134:45–58. doi:10.1037/bne0000342 pmid:31904252
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Grabenhorst F,
    2. Baez-Mendoza R,
    3. Genest W,
    4. Deco G,
    5. Schultz W
    (2019) Primate amygdala neurons simulate decision processes of social partners. Cell 177:986–998.e15. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2019.02.042 pmid:30982599
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Hadland KA,
    2. Rushworth MF,
    3. Gaffan D,
    4. Passingham RE
    (2003) The effect of cingulate lesions on social behaviour and emotion. Neuropsychologia 41:919–931. doi:10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00325-1 pmid:12667528
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Hiser J,
    2. Koenigs M
    (2018) The multifaceted role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion, decision making, social cognition, and psychopathology. Biol Psychiatry 83:638–647. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.10.030 pmid:29275839
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Hornak J,
    2. Bramham J,
    3. Rolls ET,
    4. Morris RG,
    5. O'Doherty J,
    6. Bullock PR,
    7. Polkey CE
    (2003) Changes in emotion after circumscribed surgical lesions of the orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices. Brain 126:1691–1712. doi:10.1093/brain/awg168 pmid:12805109
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Izquierdo A,
    2. Murray EA
    (2004) Combined unilateral lesions of the amygdala and orbital prefrontal cortex impair affective processing in rhesus monkeys. J Neurophysiol 91:2023–2039. doi:10.1152/jn.00968.2003 pmid:14711973
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Izquierdo A,
    2. Murray EA
    (2007) Selective bilateral amygdala lesions in rhesus monkeys fail to disrupt object reversal learning. J Neurosci 27:1054–1062. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3616-06.2007 pmid:17267559
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  50. ↵
    1. Izquierdo A,
    2. Suda RK,
    3. Murray EA
    (2004) Bilateral orbital prefrontal cortex lesions in rhesus monkeys disrupt choices guided by both reward value and reward contingency. J Neurosci 24:7540–7548. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1921-04.2004 pmid:15329401
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  51. ↵
    1. Izquierdo A,
    2. Suda RK,
    3. Murray EA
    (2005) Comparison of the effects of bilateral orbital prefrontal cortex lesions and amygdala lesions on emotional responses in rhesus monkeys. J Neurosci 25:8534–8542. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1232-05.2005 pmid:16162935
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  52. ↵
    1. Jenkins LM,
    2. Andrewes DG,
    3. Nicholas CL,
    4. Drummond KJ,
    5. Moffat BA,
    6. Phal P,
    7. Desmond P,
    8. Kessels RP
    (2014) Social cognition in patients following surgery to the prefrontal cortex. Psychiatry Res 224:192–203. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.08.007 pmid:25284626
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Jung B,
    2. Taylor PA,
    3. Seidlitz J,
    4. Sponheim C,
    5. Perkins P,
    6. Ungerleider LG,
    7. Glen D,
    8. Messinger A
    (2020) A comprehensive macaque fMRI pipeline and hierarchical atlas. Neuroimage 235:117997.
    OpenUrl
  54. ↵
    1. Kling A
    (1972) Effects of amygdalectomy on social-affective behavior in non-human primates. In: The neurobiology of the amygdala. Advances in behavioral biology, vol 2. Boston, MA: Springer.
  55. ↵
    1. Koenigs M,
    2. Tranel D
    (2007) Irrational economic decision-making after ventromedial prefrontal damage: evidence from the Ultimatum Game. J Neurosci 27:951–956. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4606-06.2007 pmid:17251437
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. ↵
    1. Koob GF,
    2. Volkow ND
    (2016) Neurobiology of addiction: a neurocircuitry analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 3:760–773. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(16)00104-8 pmid:27475769
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Lichtenberg NT,
    2. Pennington ZT,
    3. Holley SM,
    4. Greenfield VY,
    5. Cepeda C,
    6. Levine MS,
    7. Wassum KM
    (2017) Basolateral amygdala to orbitofrontal cortex projections enable cue-triggered reward expectations. J Neurosci 37:8374–8384. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0486-17.2017 pmid:28743727
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. ↵
    1. Lockwood PL,
    2. Apps MAJ,
    3. Chang SWC
    (2020) Is there a 'social' brain? Implementations and algorithms. Trends Cogn Sci 24:802–813.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  59. ↵
    1. Machado CJ,
    2. Bachevalier J
    (2007) The effects of selective amygdala, orbital frontal cortex or hippocampal formation lesions on reward assessment in nonhuman primates. Eur J Neurosci 25:2885–2904. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05525.x pmid:17561849
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Machado CJ,
    2. Kazama AM,
    3. Bachevalier J
    (2009) Impact of amygdala, orbital frontal, or hippocampal lesions on threat avoidance and emotional reactivity in nonhuman primates. Emotion 9:147–163. doi:10.1037/a0014539 pmid:19348528
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Mah L,
    2. Arnold MC,
    3. Grafman J
    (2004) Impairment of social perception associated with lesions of the prefrontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry 161:1247–1255. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.161.7.1247 pmid:15229058
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Málková L,
    2. Gaffan D,
    3. Murray EA
    (1997) Excitotoxic lesions of the amygdala fail to produce impairment in visual learning for auditory secondary reinforcement but interfere with reinforcer devaluation effects in rhesus monkeys. J Neurosci 17:6011–6020. pmid:9221797
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  63. ↵
    1. Málková L,
    2. Lex CK,
    3. Mishkin M,
    4. Saunders RC
    (2001) MRI-based evaluation of locus and extent of neurotoxic lesions in monkeys. Hippocampus 11:361–370. doi:10.1002/hipo.1050
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Martin AL,
    2. Franklin AN,
    3. Perlman JE,
    4. Bloomsmith MA
    (2018) Systematic assessment of food item preference and reinforcer effectiveness: enhancements in training laboratory-housed rhesus macaques. Behav Processes 157:445–452. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2018.07.002
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  65. ↵
    1. Medina A,
    2. Torres J,
    3. Kazama AM,
    4. Bachevalier J,
    5. Raper J
    (2020) Emotional responses in monkeys differ depending on the stimulus type, sex, and neonatal amygdala lesion status. Behav Neurosci 134:153–165. doi:10.1037/bne0000360 pmid:32175761
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Meunier M,
    2. Bachevalier J,
    3. Murray EA,
    4. Málková L,
    5. Mishkin M
    (1999) Effects of aspiration versus neurotoxic lesions of the amygdala on emotional responses in monkeys. Eur J Neurosci 11:4403–4418. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00854.x pmid:10594668
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Munuera J,
    2. Rigotti M,
    3. Salzman CD
    (2018) Shared neural coding for social hierarchy and reward value in primate amygdala. Nat Neurosci 21:415–423. doi:10.1038/s41593-018-0082-8 pmid:29459764
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Murray EA,
    2. Rudebeck PH
    (2018) Specializations for reward-guided decision-making in the primate ventral prefrontal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 19:404–417. doi:10.1038/s41583-018-0013-4
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Murray EA,
    2. Moylan EJ,
    3. Saleem KS,
    4. Basile BM,
    5. Turchi J
    (2015) Specialized areas for value updating and goal selection in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. Elife 4:e11695. doi:10.7554/eLife.11695
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Noonan MP,
    2. Sallet J,
    3. Rudebeck PH,
    4. Buckley MJ,
    5. Rushworth MF
    (2010a) Does the medial orbitofrontal cortex have a role in social valuation? Eur J Neurosci 31:2341–2351. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07271.x pmid:20550569
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Noonan MP,
    2. Walton ME,
    3. Behrens TE,
    4. Sallet J,
    5. Buckley MJ,
    6. Rushworth MF
    (2010b) Separate value comparison and learning mechanisms in macaque medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:20547–20552. doi:10.1073/pnas.1012246107 pmid:21059901
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  72. ↵
    1. Noonan MP,
    2. Mars RB,
    3. Neubert FX,
    4. Ahmed B,
    5. Smith J,
    6. Krug K,
    7. Sallet J
    (2017) Organization of the social brain in macaques and humans. In: Decision neuroscience (Dreher J-C, Tremblay L, eds), pp 189–198. San Diego: Academic.
  73. ↵
    1. Padoa-Schioppa C
    (2011) Neurobiology of economic choice: a good-based model. Annu Rev Neurosci 34:333–359. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113648 pmid:21456961
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Padoa-Schioppa C,
    2. Assad JA
    (2006) Neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex encode economic value. Nature 441:223–226. doi:10.1038/nature04676 pmid:16633341
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Padoa-Schioppa C,
    2. Jandolo L,
    3. Visalberghi E
    (2006) Multi-stage mental process for economic choice in capuchins. Cognition 99:B1–B13. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.04.008 pmid:16043168
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Pujara MS,
    2. Wolf RC,
    3. Baskaya MK,
    4. Koenigs M
    (2015) Ventromedial prefrontal cortex damage alters relative risk tolerance for prospective gains and losses. Neuropsychologia 79:70–75. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.10.026 pmid:26597003
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Pujara MS,
    2. Rudebeck PH,
    3. Ciesinski NK,
    4. Murray EA
    (2019) Heightened defensive responses following subtotal lesions of macaque orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurosci 39:4133–4141.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  78. ↵
    1. Reber J,
    2. Feinstein JS,
    3. O'Doherty JP,
    4. Liljeholm M,
    5. Adolphs R,
    6. Tranel D
    (2017) Selective impairment of goal-directed decision-making following lesions to the human ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Brain 140:1743–1756. doi:10.1093/brain/awx105 pmid:28549132
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Rhodes SE,
    2. Murray EA
    (2013) Differential effects of amygdala, orbital prefrontal cortex, and prelimbic cortex lesions on goal-directed behavior in rhesus macaques. J Neurosci 33:3380–3389. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4374-12.2013 pmid:23426666
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  80. ↵
    1. Rosenberger LA,
    2. Eisenegger C,
    3. Naef M,
    4. Terburg D,
    5. Fourie J,
    6. Stein DJ,
    7. van Honk J
    (2019) The human basolateral amygdala is indispensable for social experiential learning. Curr Biol 29:3532–3537.e3. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2019.08.078 pmid:31607530
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    1. Rudebeck PH,
    2. Buckley MJ,
    3. Walton ME,
    4. Rushworth MF
    (2006) A role for the macaque anterior cingulate gyrus in social valuation. Science 313:1310–1312. doi:10.1126/science.1128197 pmid:16946075
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  82. ↵
    1. Rudebeck PH,
    2. Saunders RC,
    3. Prescott AT,
    4. Chau LS,
    5. Murray EA
    (2013a) Prefrontal mechanisms of behavioral flexibility, emotion regulation and value updating. Nat Neurosci 16:1140–1145. doi:10.1038/nn.3440 pmid:23792944
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  83. ↵
    1. Rudebeck PH,
    2. Mitz AR,
    3. Chacko RV,
    4. Murray EA
    (2013b) Effects of amygdala lesions on reward-value coding in orbital and medial prefrontal cortex. Neuron 80:1519–1531. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.036 pmid:24360550
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  84. ↵
    1. Rudebeck PH,
    2. Saunders RC,
    3. Lundgren DA,
    4. Murray EA
    (2017a) Specialized representations of value in the orbital and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex: desirability versus availability of outcomes. Neuron 95:1208–1220. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.07.042 pmid:28858621
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  85. ↵
    1. Rudebeck PH,
    2. Ripple JA,
    3. Mitz AR,
    4. Averbeck BB,
    5. Murray EA
    (2017b) Amygdala contributions to stimulus-reward encoding in the macaque medial and orbital frontal cortex during learning. J Neurosci 37:2186–2202. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0933-16.2017 pmid:28123082
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  86. ↵
    1. Saad ZS,
    2. Glen DR,
    3. Chen G,
    4. Beauchamp MS,
    5. Desai R,
    6. Cox RW
    (2009) A new method for improving functional-to-structural MRI alignment using local Pearson correlation. Neuroimage 44:839–848. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.037
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  87. ↵
    1. Saleem KS,
    2. Kondo H,
    3. Price JL
    (2008) Complementary circuits connecting the orbital and medial prefrontal networks with the temporal, insular, and opercular cortex in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol 506:659–693. doi:10.1002/cne.21577 pmid:18067141
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  88. ↵
    1. Seidlitz J,
    2. Sponheim C,
    3. Glen D,
    4. Ye FQ,
    5. Saleem KS,
    6. Leopold DA,
    7. Ungerleider L,
    8. Messinger A
    (2018) A population MRI brain template and analysis tools for the macaque. Neuroimage 170:121–131. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.063 pmid:28461058
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  89. ↵
    1. Sharpe MJ,
    2. Schoenbaum G
    (2016) Back to basics: making predictions in the orbitofrontal-amygdala circuit. Neurobiol Learn Mem 131:201–206. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2016.04.009 pmid:27112314
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  90. ↵
    1. Talmi D,
    2. Hurlemann R,
    3. Patin A,
    4. Dolan RJ
    (2010) Framing effect following bilateral amygdala lesion. Neuropsychologia 48:1823–1827. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.03.005 pmid:20227427
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  91. ↵
    1. Thakkar KN,
    2. Polli FE,
    3. Joseph RM,
    4. Tuch DS,
    5. Hadjikhani N,
    6. Barton JJ,
    7. Manoach DS
    (2008) Response monitoring, repetitive behaviour and anterior cingulate abnormalities in autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Brain 131:2464–2478. doi:10.1093/brain/awn099 pmid:18550622
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  92. ↵
    1. Tremblay L,
    2. Schultz W
    (1999) Relative reward preference in primate orbitofrontal cortex. Nature 398:704–708. doi:10.1038/19525 pmid:10227292
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  93. ↵
    1. Troiani V,
    2. Dougherty CC,
    3. Michael AM,
    4. Olson IR
    (2016) Characterization of face-selective patches in orbitofrontal cortex. Front Hum Neurosci 10:279. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2016.00279 pmid:27378880
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  94. ↵
    1. Tsao DY,
    2. Schweers N,
    3. Moeller S,
    4. Freiwald WA
    (2008) Patches of face-selective cortex in the macaque frontal lobe. Nat Neurosci 11:877–879. doi:10.1038/nn.2158 pmid:18622399
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  95. ↵
    1. Vaidya AR,
    2. Pujara MS,
    3. Petrides M,
    4. Murray EA,
    5. Fellows LK
    (2019) Lesion studies in contemporary neuroscience. Trends Cogn Sci 23:653–671. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2019.05.009 pmid:31279672
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  96. ↵
    1. Viding E,
    2. McCrory E
    (2019) Towards understanding atypical social affiliation in psychopathy. Lancet Psychiatry 6:437–444. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30049-5 pmid:31006435
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  97. ↵
    1. Walker AE
    (1940) A cytoarchitectural study of the prefrontal area of the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol 73:59–86. doi:10.1002/cne.900730106
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  98. ↵
    1. Wallis JD
    (2011) Cross-species studies of orbitofrontal cortex and value-based decision-making. Nat Neurosci 15:13–19. doi:10.1038/nn.2956 pmid:22101646
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  99. ↵
    1. Watson KK,
    2. Platt ML
    (2012) Social signals in primate orbitofrontal cortex. Curr Biol 22:2268–2273. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.10.016 pmid:23122847
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  100. ↵
    1. Wellman LL,
    2. Gale K,
    3. Malkova L
    (2005) GABAA-mediated inhibition of basolateral amygdala blocks reward devaluation in macaques. J Neurosci 25:4577–4586. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2257-04.2005 pmid:15872105
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  101. ↵
    1. Wheeler EZ,
    2. Fellows LK
    (2008) The human ventromedial frontal lobe is critical for learning from negative feedback. Brain 131:1323–1331. doi:10.1093/brain/awn041 pmid:18344561
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 42 (28)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 42, Issue 28
13 Jul 2022
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Selective Prefrontal–Amygdala Circuit Interactions Underlie Social and Nonsocial Valuation in Rhesus Macaques
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Selective Prefrontal–Amygdala Circuit Interactions Underlie Social and Nonsocial Valuation in Rhesus Macaques
Maia S. Pujara, Nicole K. Ciesinski, Joseph F. Reyelts, Sarah E.V. Rhodes, Elisabeth A. Murray
Journal of Neuroscience 13 July 2022, 42 (28) 5593-5604; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0794-21.2022

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Selective Prefrontal–Amygdala Circuit Interactions Underlie Social and Nonsocial Valuation in Rhesus Macaques
Maia S. Pujara, Nicole K. Ciesinski, Joseph F. Reyelts, Sarah E.V. Rhodes, Elisabeth A. Murray
Journal of Neuroscience 13 July 2022, 42 (28) 5593-5604; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0794-21.2022
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Conclusion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • amygdala
  • decision-making
  • lesions
  • medial frontal cortex
  • orbitofrontal cortex
  • social cognition

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Rapid and chronic ethanol tolerance are composed of distinct memory-like states in Drosophila
  • The orienting reflex reveals behavioral states set by demanding contexts: role of the superior colliculus
  • Individual variability in performance reflects selectivity of the multiple demand network among children and adults
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • The orienting reflex reveals behavioral states set by demanding contexts: role of the superior colliculus
  • Individual variability in performance reflects selectivity of the multiple demand network among children and adults
  • Language experience during infancy predicts white matter myelination at age 2 years
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.