Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Symposium and Mini-Symposium

Music and Brain Circuitry: Strategies for Strengthening Evidence-Based Research for Music-Based Interventions

Wen Grace Chen, John Rehner Iversen, Mimi H. Kao, Psyche Loui, Aniruddh Dhiren Patel, Robert J. Zatorre and Emmeline Edwards
Journal of Neuroscience 9 November 2022, 42 (45) 8498-8507; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1135-22.2022
Wen Grace Chen
1Division of Extramural Research, National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland, 20892
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
John Rehner Iversen
2University of California-San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for John Rehner Iversen
Mimi H. Kao
3Tufts University, Medford, Massachusetts 02155
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Mimi H. Kao
Psyche Loui
4Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Psyche Loui
Aniruddh Dhiren Patel
3Tufts University, Medford, Massachusetts 02155
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Aniruddh Dhiren Patel
Robert J. Zatorre
5Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec H3A2B4, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Emmeline Edwards
1Division of Extramural Research, National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland, 20892
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Emmeline Edwards
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The neuroscience of music and music-based interventions (MBIs) is a fascinating but challenging research field. While music is a ubiquitous component of every human society, MBIs may encompass listening to music, performing music, music-based movement, undergoing music education and training, or receiving treatment from music therapists. Unraveling the brain circuits activated and influenced by MBIs may help us gain better understanding of the therapeutic and educational values of MBIs by gathering strong research evidence. However, the complexity and variety of MBIs impose unique research challenges. This article reviews the recent endeavor led by the National Institutes of Health to support evidence-based research of MBIs and their impact on health and diseases. It also highlights fundamental challenges and strategies of MBI research with emphases on the utilization of animal models, human brain imaging and stimulation technologies, behavior and motion capturing tools, and computational approaches. It concludes with suggestions of basic requirements when studying MBIs and promising future directions to further strengthen evidence-based research on MBIs in connections with brain circuitry.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Music and music-based interventions (MBI) engage a wide range of brain circuits and hold promising therapeutic potentials for a variety of health conditions. Comparative studies using animal models have helped in uncovering brain circuit activities involved in rhythm perception, while human imaging, brain stimulation, and motion capture technologies have enabled neural circuit analysis underlying the effects of MBIs on motor, affective/reward, and cognitive function. Combining computational analysis, such as prediction method, with mechanistic studies in animal models and humans may unravel the complexity of MBIs and their effects on health and disease.

  • musical components
  • music-based interventions
  • brain circuits
  • technologies
  • therapeutic effects

Introduction

Music is an integral part of every human society. Music can bring pleasure, calm anxiety, soothe sorrow, inspire and/or stimulate movement, and promote social connections. Musical experiences may also have the remarkable ability to enhance brain and cognitive development, improve function and well-being, optimize the quality of life, and possibly ameliorate the symptoms of a broad range of diseases and disorders.

Recognizing the untapped therapeutic potentials of music-based interventions (MBIs), the National Institutes of Health (NIH), John F. Kennedy Center for the Performing Arts, and National Endowment for the Arts formed a collaborative partnership, Sound Health, in 2016. The journey started with a jointly organized workshop, Music and the Brain: Research Across the Lifespan, which was held in January 2017, to evaluate the state of basic and applied music research. In this meeting, a diverse panel of experts discussed the impact of music on the brain across the lifespan (childhood, adulthood, and aging) and made recommendations for enhancing research in each of these domains (Cheever et al., 2018). In the 2018 Dialogues Between Neuroscience and Society lecture, musician Pat Metheny discussed with a panel of Society of Neuroscience members the impact of music on the brain and the role of music in healing. Soon after, NIH issued a series of special funding opportunities to promote basic, mechanistic, and clinical research on MBI (Chen et al., 2018, 2020; Riddle et al., 2018a,b, 2020a,b). In 2021, NIH organized three workshops focusing on Laying the Foundation: Defining the Building Blocks of Music-Based Interventions, Assessing and Measuring Target Engagement—Mechanistic and Clinical Outcome Measures for Brain Disorders of Aging, and Relating Target Engagement to Clinical Benefit—Biomarkers for Brain Disorders of Aging, respectively. Discussions at these workshops resulted in the development of the NIH Music-Based Interventions Toolkit (Edwards et al., 2022). NIH also intends to support the development of research networks on MBIs with a particular emphasis on developing compelling research frameworks; identifying consistent terminology and taxonomy to guide future clinical research; supporting interdisciplinary collaborations and pilot studies to test novel mechanistic hypotheses; and developing strong mechanistic measures, outcomes, and biomarkers, with a special emphasis on several brain diseases and disorders, such as pain, Alzheimer's disease, Parkinson's disease (PD), stroke, and/or aging.

A central thesis involved in all these endeavors is the question of how MBIs achieve their therapeutic potentials. The power of music to influence movement, emotion, learning, and behavior is enormous. One hypothesis is that music's impact is linked to its ability to engage multiple neural systems of the brain. But what is the support for such a conclusion? If we are ever to harness music's multitude of influences, we need a solid understanding of the neural circuitry involved and rigorous evidence about how it is engaged by music. From a brain circuitry perspective, the idea may seem straightforward: as musical sounds are first processed by auditory mechanisms in our CNS, therapeutic effects derived from MBIs would most likely require the engagement of brain circuits and other physiological systems that are directly or indirectly connected to the auditory neural circuitry involved in perceiving and processing elements of music.

To test this idea scientifically, we first need to have clear definitions or characterizations of what music and MBIs are. Basic constituents of music include melody, harmony, and rhythm (Vuust et al., 2022). Each of these three elements has countless sequences, tempos, and dynamics or loudness of sound, and they can be combined in numerous ways. This enormous heterogeneity in musical contents is then implemented on a variable target population of MBIs (Loui, 2020) (Fig. 1A). The mode of MBI delivery also varies (Fig. 1B). The content of music may be heard in a receptive mode (Hanser, 2016), or it may be performed or presented by an individual in an active engagement mode often requiring some degree of motor activities, such as singing, playing an instrument, dancing, or even composing. In addition, MBIs may have a social interaction component if they are delivered in a group setting, such as listening in a concert hall with an audience, performing music as a group, or interactions between the performers and the listeners; or delivered by a music therapist to a patient or a group of patients, for instance. In MBI research, clearly describing the intervention itself, including music content and mode of delivery, in addition to other common intervention parameters, such as duration and frequency, may be the first important step. Music and MBIs have been associated with a variety of brain functions and disorders. Therefore identifying and testing the neural network connections between the auditory neural pathways where the sounds of music are first processed and other brain networks, such as motor, affective/reward, cognitive, as well as other sensory circuits, including pain, vision, and interoception, which impacts other physiological systems (Chen et al., 2021), will be critical to help us understand how MBIs may exert their therapeutic effects (Loui, 2020) (Fig. 1C).

This review article highlights research findings presented at the 2022 Society of Neuroscience Symposium Music and Brain Circuitry: Strategies for Strengthening Evidence-Based Research. Specifically, we will begin with the complexity of MBIs and the importance of neuroscience approaches to help address fundamentals of interventions, such as dosages. Comparative studies across multiple species, including birds, rodents, nonhuman primates, and humans (Fig. 1D), also offer significant insights into neural circuits involved in MBIs with a high level of rigor, especially regarding perception of music rhythm and the auditory and motor neural systems involved. Multiple brain imaging tools, including EEG and MRI, brain stimulation approaches, such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), as well as innovative behavioral analysis using technologies, such as motion capture and prediction analysis methods (Fig. 1E), further allowed investigators to probe the neural mechanisms and explore the neural network connections underlying the effects of music and MBIs on a variety of brain function and behavioral disorders.

Fundamentals of music-based interventions

The myriad musical contents conferred by countless combinations of its constituents pose a major challenge for MBIs, especially in defining the intervention and maintaining consistency. In basic and mechanistic research, it may be possible to study musical constituents in a reductionist way by focusing on one or a few specific combinations or forms of constituents. In contrast, more holistic approaches by the music therapy community, for example, seem to share the general consensus that there is no one-size-fits-all program: while self-selected music confers the most therapeutic benefit for a variety of clinical applications, the music therapist consults with clients and caregivers to come up with the best available course of therapy with regard to content (musical components), mode of engagement (active or passive protocols), and duration and intensity (dosage) of the intervention (Wheeler, 2015).

Dosage is a fundamentally important aspect to consider when studying MBIs. Regarding the question of dosage, consider the analogous case of physical activity: the American Academy of Pediatrics recommends 60 min of activity per day in school-aged children (www.healthychildren.org), and the Global Council of Brain Health recommends to “strive for at least 150 min of weekly, moderate-intensity aerobic activity” for adults over age 50 to manage heart and brain health. Is there an equivalent “recommended dosage” for music-based interventions? The answer to this question is complex, as the experience of music itself is complex. Although every society has music, the musical cultures that societies around the world have evolved are diverse and variable (Savage et al., 2015). Even the same piece of music may elicit varying responses among individuals within the same culture, or for the same individual with repeated listening over time (Margulis, 2014). As such, music that has therapeutic benefits for one individual may not necessarily translate to another.

In this regard, neuroscience can inform the question of dosage in MBIs by quantifying the effects of receptive music (perception) and active music (production) interventions on the CNS. For instance, fMRI studies have shown that listening to self-selected music engages the auditory and reward systems more than music selected by the researcher (Pereira et al., 2011; Quinci et al., 2022), converging with the intuitions from music therapy. Longitudinal fMRI results in healthy older adults show that an 8 week receptive MBI increased functional connectivity from the auditory cortex to the reward system, specifically to the mPFC (Quinci et al., 2022). While these results remain to be further validated with control interventions that isolate the active ingredient of music listening, the idea that systematic engagement with music can change the connectivity of the auditory and reward systems is appealing because it offers a tractable method by which to quantify the impact of MBI dosage. While specific parameters of the dose–response relationship between music and health are too complex to be knowable at this time, the responsivity, sensitivity, and connectivity of the engaged brain circuits may serve as potentially viable quantitative measures for the dose–response relationship, thus offering a window of opportunity to dissect the complexity of MBIs and their impacts on brain and health in general.

Comparative studies of musical rhythm perception

Among musical constituents, rhythm and temporal periodicity (sonic patterns which repeat regularly in time) are widely seen across species and have been richly studied. In humans, musical rhythm perception involves detecting such periodicities and generating precise temporal predictions about upcoming events (Merchant et al., 2015). This ability to detect and predict auditory rhythms is central to music's positive effect on a variety of neurologic disorders involving motor functions, including normalizing gait in PD (Benoit et al., 2014; Ghai et al., 2018; Krotinger and Loui, 2021), enhancing language recovery after stroke (Schlaug et al., 2009; Zumbansen et al., 2014), and improving phonological processing in dyslexia (Flaugnacco et al., 2015). While much remains to be understood about the neural mechanisms of rhythm perception, progress on this front has been facilitated by cross-species studies of perception along with incorporation of quantitative assessment and manipulation of neural activity.

Recent work has begun to elucidate the neural circuits for recognizing rhythmic communication signals based on tempo. For example, female field crickets are attracted to male calling songs within a narrow range of pulse rates, and this selectivity is mediated by a network of interneurons that processes instantaneous pulse rate using a coincidence detection mechanism (Schoneich, 2020). While preference for pulse rate is hard-wired in many invertebrates, experience can shape neural responses to call rates in other species. For example, excitatory neurons in a mouse auditory cortex are innately sensitive to the most common rate of pup distress calls (∼5 syll/s), but their tuning can broaden to a wider range of rates following cohousing with pups producing a range of call rates (Schiavo et al., 2020).

Moving beyond tempo, several studies have shown that auditory responses can be modulated by the presence of rhythmic patterns. For example, in gerbils, responses of neurons in the inferior colliculus are greater for noise bursts that occur on the beat of complex rhythms compared with the same bursts off the beat (Rajendran et al., 2017). In mice, excitatory neurons in the auditory cortex integrate signals over longer timescales and distinguish between rhythmically structured and irregular sequences by adjusting spike timing (Asokan et al., 2021). Moreover, in monkeys, EEG recordings have shown that deviant sounds elicit a larger auditory mismatch negativity signal when they are embedded in isochronous versus randomly timed sequences (Honing et al., 2018).

While such studies demonstrate context-dependent modulation of neural activity in auditory regions, there is growing evidence that human rhythm perception relies on interactions between auditory and motor regions, even in the absence of movement. As discussed later in this review, neuroimaging studies have shown that activity in several motor planning regions, including the premotor cortex, supplementary motor area, and basal ganglia, is greater when a stimulus has a strong periodic pulse, or beat (Grahn and Brett, 2007; Kung et al., 2013; Kasdan et al., 2022). In addition, transient disruption of auditory-motor connections using TMS can disrupt beat perception in humans without affecting perception of the timing of absolute intervals (Ross et al., 2018). Together, these results support the hypothesis that perception of temporal regularity depends on the interaction of auditory and motor regions.

Investigation of the functional contribution of motor regions to auditory rhythm perception would benefit greatly from a small animal model that (1) possesses reciprocally connected auditory-motor circuitry; and (2) can recognize rhythmic patterns. Like humans, songbirds possess specialized auditory-motor circuits for learning and producing rhythmically patterned sequences (Norton and Scharff, 2016; Roeske et al., 2020). Anatomical, physiological, and histochemical studies have found remarkable similarities in the premotor, auditory, and basal ganglia circuitry of birds and mammals, including shared cell types, patterns of connectivity, electrophysiological properties, and laminar organization (Doupe et al., 2005; Goldberg and Fee, 2010; Goldberg et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2010). A recent study found that zebra finches, the most commonly studied songbird, can detect temporal regularities in auditory sequences and predict the timing of calls of a vocal partner, allowing them to adjust the timing of their own answers to avoid overlap (Benichov et al., 2016). This ability to predictively adjust call timing was disrupted by lesions of vocal motor regions, consistent with the idea that call timing plasticity depends on the interaction of forebrain motor and auditory regions. However, it remains unclear whether zebra finches can perceive rhythms holistically or whether they learned the specific time interval between the vocal partner's calls and their own.

To examine whether songbirds can perceive rhythms holistically, as humans do, a behavioral paradigm to test whether zebra finches can learn to recognize a fundamental rhythmic pattern, equal timing between events, or “isochrony,” has been developed (Rouse et al., 2021). Humans readily recognize isochrony across a wide range of rates (Espinoza-Monroy and de Lafuente, 2021), indicating a facility with perceiving the relative timing of events, not just absolute interval durations. Using a sequential training procedure, whether zebra finches could discriminate between isochronous and arrhythmic sequences of a repeated song element was probed. By varying sound element identity and tempo across stimuli, birds were incentivized to attend to the relative timing in auditory sequences, rather than to specific spectral features or interval durations (Rouse et al., 2021). Once birds reached a performance criterion for overall accuracy, they were tested for the ability to generalize the discrimination to stimuli at novel tempi. This study found that zebra finches, like humans, can robustly recognize isochrony across a broad range of rates, including rates 20% slower and 25% faster than the original training stimuli. Notably, birds that successfully discriminated isochronous from arrhythmic stimuli listened to more intervals before responding than birds that failed, suggesting that success at rhythm discrimination is related to attention to global temporal patterns. This aligns with evidence from neuropsychology studies showing that neural mechanisms underlying detection of relative timing are distinct from those involved in encoding absolute timing (Grube et al., 2010; Teki et al., 2011; Breska and Ivry, 2018).

The finding that zebra finches, like humans, can categorize rhythms based on global temporal patterns contrasts with prior work in vocal nonlearners. For example, rats can be trained to discriminate isochronous from arrhythmic rhythms but show weak generalization when tested with stimuli at novel tempi, suggesting a strong reliance on absolute timing for rhythm perception (Celma-Miralles and Toro, 2020). Thus, the combination of a well-defined auditory-motor circuit and the ability to recognize relative timing make songbirds a tractable small animal model to investigate the contributions of motor regions to detecting temporal periodicity and predicting the timing of upcoming events, two hallmarks of rhythm perception in humans. Future experiments manipulating neural activity can test for a causal role of forebrain motor regions in the perception of rhythmic patterns independent of rate, and neural recordings will help to reveal whether predictive activity emerges in motor regions as birds learn to discriminate isochronous from arrhythmic stimuli. More generally, such mechanistic studies of auditory-motor interactions during rhythm perception should help to inform music-based interventions for enhancing function in normal and disease states.

Music and motor circuits

Similar to the animal species discussed earlier, humans have a special way of perceptually and motorically interacting with rhythmic stimuli. Repeating patterns of beats, or meter, establish a temporal scaffolding that shapes future expectations, shapes the perceptual meaning of individual events, and enables behavioral synchronization among groups of people.

One of the more intriguing ideas emerging from the field is that the motor system may be important for the ordered perception of musical structure, even in the absence of overt movement (Repp, 2005; Schubotz, 2007; Zatorre et al., 2007; Arnal, 2012; Patel and Iversen, 2014; Ross et al., 2016; Rimmele et al., 2018). Many accounts, such as the Action Simulation for Auditory Perception (ASAP), emphasize the role of the motor system as a source for generating temporal expectations about upcoming events, a critical biological function, specifically hypothesizing motor to auditory connectivity (Patel and Iversen, 2014; Cannon and Patel, 2021). Recent work has specifically examined the motor system's involvement in shaping the perception and imagery of auditory rhythm in the absence of movement, directly testing the predictions of the ASAP and other motor hypotheses and providing insight into temporal perception using advanced EEG and Mobile Brain/Body Imaging (MoBI) methods. MoBI is a new imaging approach using mobile brain imaging methods, including the EEG and/or near infrared spectroscopy synchronized to body motion capture and other behavioral and psychophysiological data streams to investigate brain activity supporting participants actively interacting with their environment and/or with others (Makeig et al., 2009; Gramann et al., 2011, 2014).

EEG measures voltages present at the scalp as a result of dynamic electrical activity in the brain, but any single electrode measures the sum of activity from many regions of the cortex. One solution is independent component analysis (Makeig et al., 1996), a method to optimally unmix the scalp signals and identify putative cortical sources. A recent study used auditory and motor localizer trials and independent component analysis to identify the most unimodal auditory and motor independent components in human participants and then examined auditory-motor interactions during a meter imagery task (Cheng et al., 2022). Participants first heard an unaccented control series of drum strokes, followed by accented strokes that established a duple (1-2) or triple (1-2-3) meter, followed again by unaccented strokes with the instruction to continue imagining the previously established meter. To verify the imagery task, participants finally tapped the meter they had imagined. By comparing brain activity during the unaccented control and the meter imagery conditions, two predictions of ASAP were confirmed: (1) representation of meter was present in brain signals during meter imagery in both auditory and motor regions; and (2) robust, bidirectional motor to auditory connectivity (assessed using a directional measure of “causal” influence) (Korzeniewska et al., 2008) was present during imagery. A problematic potential confound for any work on imagery is the presence of possibly unintentional, subtle movements, which can be ruled out by using MoBI methods, including motion capture and the measurement of muscle potentials. The use of neurostimulation to causally manipulate brain activity makes it possible to directly test the importance of the motor system for auditory perception. ASAP proposes a specific pathway to mediate auditory/motor reciprocal interactions, the dorsal auditory stream linking auditory cortex to premotor cortex via parietal cortex (Rauschecker, 2011). One can predict that interruption of this pathway would disrupt beat perception. TMS is one method by which activity on localized cortical regions can be temporarily suppressed (e.g., using continuous theta-burst stimulation) (Huang et al., 2005). Continuous theta-burst stimulation over parietal cortex has been shown to impact aspects of beat perception but spare other forms of temporal processing, providing causal evidence in favor of ASAP (Ross et al., 2018).

While it is common to think of music as an auditory phenomenon, something we can thoroughly enjoy through headphones, music and movement are inseparable in several ways. Until the advent of recorded music, all music was created by movement. Many types of music strongly compel movement and dance, an aspect that has been used profitably in therapies for movement disorders. One example may be active MBIs that take advantage of musical groove, which is the pleasurable urge to move to music, possibly through connections to broader neural circuits, including the motor systems. The experience of groove is strongest for slight violations to rhythmic structure (Janata et al., 2012; Witek et al., 2014), and is causally linked to sensorimotor coupling as demonstrated by TMS studies (Stupacher et al., 2013). As such, the use of groovy music to motivate dance may be an important ingredient in active MBIs for movement disorders, such as PD, which is associated with a loss of internal cues for timing and movement, as evidenced by impaired rhythm discrimination in PD patients (Grahn and Brett, 2009). As music and dance engage sensorimotor coupling through rhythm and groove, this has inspired interventions, such as Dance for PD, which is a program that uses dance as an intervention for individuals with PD and their caregivers. Standardized neurologic pre-post testing showed a reduction of Parkinsonian symptoms following 4 months of Dance for PD, with better improvement observed for those who were more accurate at finger-tapping in rhythm to music, suggesting more accurate sensorimotor coupling (Krotinger and Loui, 2021). The presence of lifelong dance experience was also associated with greater reductions of both motor and nonmotor symptoms after dance intervention, suggesting that long-term training engages the predictive processes that may underlie more efficient sensorimotor coupling, which may have synergistic benefits for active MBIs.

In the past 15 years, the field of MoBI has emphasized the study of brain activity underlying active and naturalistic interactions with the environment and with others, using mobile neuroimaging methods, such as EEG, to measure brain dynamics synchronized to full-body motion capture and other behavioral and psychophysiological data streams (Makeig et al., 2009). The approach is particularly attractive in therapeutic and at-home contexts because of the emerging availability of low-cost and portable EEG systems, simple camera-based motion capture, and low-cost wrist-worn physiological sensors. The study of music is a natural fit to MoBI, which has been successfully applied to studies of individual and group musical behavior (Maidhof et al., 2014) toward understanding of interpersonal interactions among performers (Chang et al., 2018; Varlet et al., 2020), between performer and audience (Swarbrick et al., 2018), and among audience listeners and dancers. Cooperative musical and dance interactions involve developing trust (Stupacher et al., 2013; Trainor and Cirelli, 2015), are positively related to interpersonal empathy (Novembre et al., 2019), and can be effective for the communication of intentions and emotion through movement (Leslie et al., 2014). A recent study showed that cooperative musical interactions led to long-lasting changes in interbrain phase coherence (Khalil et al., 2022), perhaps indicative of a lasting cooperative set. These relationships underlie aesthetic interactions but also therapeutic ones; thus, this line of work may have broader implications for the understanding of how to create and evaluate effective therapeutic interactions in general (Foubert et al., 2021). By enabling low-cost and portable assessment of brain and body states, MoBI methods will improve understanding of the mechanisms by which ecologically complex MBIs achieve therapeutic goals. These methods will also open the way for exciting new modalities of real time brain/body feedback for rehabilitative and augmentative training (Blanco and Ramirez, 2019; Turner et al., 2021).

Music and reward circuits

The reward system consists of various neural structures, including the midbrain tegmentum, the striatum in the basal forebrain, the ventromedial and orbitofrontal cortex, and various other regions all interconnected in complex ways (Haber, 2017). It plays a role in many basic biological functions and is thought to underlie our experience of hedonic pleasure (Berridge and Kringelbach, 2015). Yet, until about two decades ago, it was not even known whether the pleasure generated by music was mediated by this same system; indeed, some philosophical traditions argued strongly against it (Skov and Nadal, 2020).

A series of neuroimaging studies has shown that the striatum and related structures become activated when people experience pleasure from music, and that these responses scale with the degree of musical pleasure experienced (Blood and Zatorre, 2001; Koelsch et al., 2006; Montag et al., 2011; Salimpoor et al., 2013; Matthews et al., 2020), as summarized in a recent a meta-analysis (Mas-Herrero et al., 2021b). Furthermore, psychophysiological indices of autonomic system engagement (heart rate, skin conductance, respiration, etc.) also increase with subjective reports of musical pleasure (Grewe et al., 2007; Salimpoor et al., 2009). In addition, the laboratory of R.J.Z. showed that the striatal response is dopaminergic in nature (Salimpoor et al., 2011; Ferreri et al., 2019), and that it is related to reward prediction mechanisms (Gold et al., 2019), thus linking musical engagement of the reward system to the extensive animal literature on dopaminergic mediation of reward prediction error (Schultz, 2016).

These findings were critical in setting the stage for a scientific understanding of music's effects but do not on their own provide a functional model of how music activates the reward system. Furthermore, imaging studies are necessarily correlational in nature, so causal evidence was required to really prove the point. Recent advances have addressed both these issues.

Several studies have shown that, as the subjective liking of music increases, the functional connectivity between auditory cortical systems and reward structures also increases (Salimpoor et al., 2013; Shany et al., 2019; Quinci et al., 2022). This idea is very important as it suggests that patterns of sound processed in the auditory system are assigned value within the reward system. More specifically, sensory prediction errors computed in auditory cortical networks are believed to be propagated to the reward system where they are assigned value according to a reward prediction mechanism (Zatorre, 2023). Thus, according to this model, musical pleasure would arise from the crosstalk between these two systems.

If this idea is correct, it leads to the prediction that people with little or no hedonic response to music should exhibit reduced interactions between the two systems. This is precisely what was observed in a series of experiments exploring specific musical anhedonia, defined as a condition in which individuals experience very little pleasure to music, yet have no perceptual deficit, nor any generalized depression or anhedonia (Mas-Herrero et al., 2014). When tested with functional imaging, people with musical anhedonia showed reduced functional connectivity between auditory and striatal areas compared with average listeners and also compared with “hyperhedonic” music lovers, who showed the greatest degree of functional interaction (Martinez-Molina et al., 2016). Furthermore, structural imaging of musically anhedonic people showed evidence of reduced anatomic connectivity in auditory-orbitofrontal white-matter tracts (Martinez-Molina et al., 2019).

These findings strongly support the auditory-reward interaction model of musical pleasure. But more direct causal evidence was still lacking. If musical pleasure arises from these interactions, modulations of this auditory-reward circuit ought to lead to modifications of the experience of pleasure. To do so requires a method that allows for stimulation of deep structures. TMS is typically used to modulate cortical structures but can also be used to influence the striatum via targeting of dorsolateral frontal areas that are connected to it (Strafella et al., 2001). Importantly, depending on the parameters of stimulation, it is possible both to upregulate dopamine activity in the striatum or downregulate it (Pogarell et al., 2007; Ko et al., 2008).

A recent study combined these brain stimulation methods with the music-induced pleasure measures (behavioral and psychophysiological) already validated in the neuroimaging studies described above (Mas-Herrero et al., 2018). The results clearly showed that, after receiving excitatory TMS (compared with sham) targeting dorsolateral frontal cortex, listeners reported higher subjective rankings of music-induced pleasure, as well as higher objective psychophysiological responses; conversely, after inhibitory TMS, both types of dependent variables were reduced. The finding that the degree of pleasure we feel can be modulated in either direction by transiently changing the excitability of certain brain structures fits the predictions of the model very well. But the final step in the logical chain would require that a direct link be shown between modulation of auditory-reward connectivity and modulation of music-induced pleasure.

To achieve this goal, Mas-Herrero et al. (2021a) repeated the TMS experiment with a new sample of volunteers and different musical excerpts, but this time fMRI was acquired immediately after the TMS session, to document the neural changes associated with the stimulation. The behavioral findings from this study mirrored those from the previous one, showing that the stimulation effects are robust and replicable. The most important finding from the fMRI data were that the functional connectivity between the right auditory cortex and the right ventral striatum was modulated in direct relationship with the degree to which the stimulation changed pleasure ratings. Thus, enhancement or decrement in pleasure following stimulation was related to upregulation or downregulation of the auditory-reward circuitry. This outcome thus provides definitive, causal evidence in favor of the hypothesis that these interactions underlie the experience of musical pleasure.

Although much remains to be discovered, these experimental findings, together, provide a mechanistic understanding of the neural basis of music-induced pleasure. Such basic-science knowledge is essential to move forward with potential applications of music to various disorders. Indeed, we know that affective states can be manipulated via music (including mood induction or emotion regulation, for example). It is likely that the reward system plays a key role in these functions, which is consistent with the view of music as a transformative technology of the mind (Patel, 2008, 2018; Loughridge, 2021) in the sense that music both emerges as a creative product of the mind and can shape the mind by affecting its function. This underlying mechanism may therefore explain why music can be used to improve mood, reduce anxiety, and enhance well-being in many different clinical groups, including psychiatric disorders (Gebhardt et al., 2014), depression (Maratos et al., 2011), stroke (Särkämö et al., 2008), heart disease (Bradt et al., 2013), and dementia (Guétin et al., 2011; for systematic reviews and meta-analysis, see Sihvonen et al., 2017; de Witte et al., 2020).

Music and cognitive and sensory circuits

The relationships between music and cognitive or sensory functions have been explored largely in human subjects. Longitudinal studies have shown that multiple years of music education or training are associated with enhanced executive functions, including inhibition, planning, and verbal intelligence in school-aged children (Jaschke et al., 2018; Hennessy et al., 2019). Similarly in older adults, musical practice has also been shown to benefit cognitive function (Roman-Caballero et al., 2018); while various forms of MBIs seem to benefit cognitive functions, including short-term and working memories, digit span, orientation, fluency, abstraction, and psychomotor speed, as well as reduce pain in people with dementia (Hofbauer et al., 2022). A substantial amount of literature can also be found to support music as an adjuvant pain treatment (Lunde et al., 2019). In contrast, relatively few studies focus on the brain mechanisms by which MBIs deliver their cognitive and sensory effects in humans (Chaddock-Heyman et al., 2021) or in an animal model (Zhou et al., 2022).

One concept for music to shape cognitive circuits lies in its ability to engender predictions (Vuust et al., 2022): as we become exposed to musical sounds throughout the lifespan, the brain continuously and automatically learns to form predictions for sounds that will likely come next, and the implicit learning of these predictions and minimization of prediction errors shapes the cognitive circuits that give rise to one's body of knowledge, including of music within the culture. As a concrete example of these cognitive circuits at work, most listeners within the Western culture show implicit knowledge of musical scale: in common-practice Western music, musical scales are based around the octave, which is a doubling of acoustic frequency. This knowledge is based on exposure to the environment through one's culture, and the ability to learn from the environment via statistical learning as a cognitive mechanism is key among the cognitive circuits that give rise to musical knowledge. To test the cognitive mechanism of statistical learning outside of Western culture, Loui and colleagues (Loui et al., 2010; Loui, 2022) used digital musical technology to create music in the Bohlen–Pierce scale, which is an alternative tuning system based on a tripling of acoustic frequency. Systematically manipulating predictions for music composed in this new scale affected liking of the new music: more frequently presented patterns were more preferred, suggesting a dose–response type relationship between familiarity and preference. The statistical learning of predictions was comparable across U.S. and Chinese populations, suggesting a relatively similar dose–response relationship across cultures. Furthermore, functional neuroimaging showed that statistical learning of predictions was tied to the activity and connectivity of the auditory and reward systems (Kathios et al., 2022). Together, these results underlie the idea that prediction and reward may be a cognitive mechanism that explains how musical sounds become rewarding. Future work is needed to relate prediction and reward learning to the transfer effects of musical experiences toward more domain-general cognitive functions, which may in turn underlie the success of MBIs for multiple clinical populations.

Discussion

Collectively, this review aims to highlight a few approaches and examples of studies on music and a variety of brain circuits, rather than attempt to be comprehensive in the entire literature covering music and neuroscience. As we study music in the context of brain and health, thus studying music as an intervention, it is important to clearly define and describe the basic contents of an MBI, either including the chosen melody, harmony, and rhythm at the minimum, or articulating the contents of the self-selected music at the onset of a study. Whether an MBI is delivered in a receptive mode to a study subject by passively listening to the musical content or in an engaging active mode with the study subject participating in producing the musical content, or both, should also be clearly specified, in addition to whether an MBI involves a group setting, either as a group listening or performing event or as an interaction between therapists, teachers, or performers and patients, students, or audience. Clear specifications of the musical content and delivery mode can enable a better design of the control interventions as often required in rigorously designed mechanistic and clinical studies. Like all other types of intervention studies, determining the dosages, including intensity of the music contents as well as the frequency and duration of the delivery, should be a fundamental requirement for MBI studies.

In terms of comparative studies of MBIs, this review has emphasized the power of animal models in vocal learning species, such as songbirds, in elucidating auditory-motor interactions in rhythm perception, which may ultimately help us understand how and why periodic auditory rhythms can help normalize motor function in disorders, such as stroke and PD. Animal models are also critical for understanding interactions between auditory regions and other neural circuits, including the reward system and pathways for detecting and coding noxious stimuli that cause pain. For example, work in songbirds has begun to elucidate auditory-reward interactions. In male birds, dopaminergic neurons are sensitive to the quality of the bird's own vocal performance, exhibiting differential firing rates when song performance is better or worse than expected (“reward prediction error”) (Gadagkar et al., 2016). Similarly, recent work in mice has begun to shed light on the neural mechanisms underlying the ability of music to attenuate pain intensity in humans (Garza-Villarreal et al., 2017). Using a mouse model for peripheral pain, Zhou et al. (2022) found that sound (and potentially music) presented at a level slightly above background noise can blunt behavioral signs of pain by modulating cortico-thalamic input from auditory regions to posterior and ventral posterior nuclei of the thalamus. This effect can last for several days after sound exposure, so it cannot be explained by sound's short-term effect on attention. Together, these studies highlight the utility of animal models for investigating the mechanistic underpinnings of both active and receptive MBIs. Future endeavors captalizing on the power of molecular genetics in combination with sophisticated behavioral assays to probe motor, affective, cognitive, and sensory systems, such as audition, pain, interoception, and vision, may facilitate the discovery of novel mechanistic insights into MBIs.

In human brain circuit studies, EEG, MEG, and fMRI have helped to elucidate neural networks involved in MBIs. Much of the brain imaging evidence has supported the engagement of auditory, motor, and reward/affective neural circuits, some of which is further enhanced evidence provided by brain stimulation studies using technologies, such as TMS and high-resolution behavioral data collected by cutting-edge motion capture technologies. The evidence for engagement of cognitive and other sensory brain circuits unique to MBIs has, however, been relatively scant. Future studies incorporating powerful brain imaging and stimulation technologies and novel behavioral assessments may be needed to ascertain whether cognitive and other sensory circuits are also engaged during MBIs in human populations.

In conclusion, the complexity of MBIs and their potential impact on multiple brain circuits may require sophisticated computational approaches to further mechanistic understandings. Predication analysis, already applied for studying the relationships between music and motor, reward, and cognitive circuits, is a well-tested example of how a computational approach may enhance mechanistic insights. Development of cutting-edge computational tools, including machine learning methods, may help inform evidence-based research of MBIs in the future.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Evidence-based research on MBIs. A, Illustrative examples of components of music, including melody, harmony, and rhythm. B, Examples of the modes of delivery of MBIs. Receptive modes: when a subject passively listens to musical components. Active modes: when a subject actively performs musical components. Solo modes: when a subject is passively listening to (receptively) or actively performing musical components. Group/Social modes: when a subject is receiving or performing music in a group setting, or when a subject is, or subjects are, interacting with a music therapist or therapists. C, Brain circuits engaged in potential therapeutic effects by MBIs. Musical components are first processed through the auditory pathway. Evidence has emerged to support neural network connections between auditory and motor or affective/motivational systems, which may underlie MBI's therapeutic effects on related diseases, such as PD, stroke, stress, anxiety, and addition. The neural network connections between the auditory pathway and cognitive or other sensory systems, such as interoception, somatosensation, nociception, and vision, remain to be explored for implications on diseases, such as Alzheimer's disease, dementia, cardiovascular diseases, and pain. D, Examples of biological/model systems studied in MBI research include birds, rodents, nonhuman primates, and humans. E, Examples of technologies used to study MBIs. Examples of brain imaging technologies include MEG, EEG, and fMRI. An example of brain stimulation technology is TMS. An example of behavior capturing technology is a motion-capture and tracking system.

Footnotes

  • We thank Karen Kaplan and Catherine Law for help in overall edits to improve this manuscript; and Bryan Ewsichek for creating Figure 1.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Emmeline Edwards at emmeline.edwards{at}nih.gov

SfN exclusive license.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Arnal LH
    (2012) Predicting 'when' using the motor system's beta-band oscillations. Front Hum Neurosci 6:225. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00225 pmid:22876228
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Asokan MM,
    2. Williamson RS,
    3. Hancock KE,
    4. Polley DB
    (2021) Inverted central auditory hierarchies for encoding local intervals and global temporal patterns. Curr Biol 31:1762–1770.e4. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2021.01.076 pmid:33609455
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Benichov JI,
    2. Benezra SE,
    3. Vallentin D,
    4. Globerson E,
    5. Long MA,
    6. Tchernichovski O
    (2016) The forebrain song system mediates predictive call timing in female and male zebra finches. Curr Biol 26:309–318. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.037 pmid:26774786
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Benoit CE,
    2. Dalla Bella S,
    3. Farrugia N,
    4. Obrig H,
    5. Mainka S,
    6. Kotz SA
    (2014) Musically cued gait-training improves both perceptual and motor timing in Parkinson's disease. Front Hum Neurosci 8:494. pmid:25071522
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Berridge KC,
    2. Kringelbach ML
    (2015) Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron 86:646–664. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.018 pmid:25950633
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Blanco AD,
    2. Ramirez R
    (2019) Evaluation of a sound quality visual feedback system for bow learning technique in violin beginners: an EEG study. Front Psychol 10:165. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00165 pmid:30809163
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Blood AJ,
    2. Zatorre RJ
    (2001) Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:11818–11823. doi:10.1073/pnas.191355898 pmid:11573015
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Bradt J,
    2. Dileo C,
    3. Potvin N
    (2013) Music for stress and anxiety reduction in coronary heart disease patients. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 12: CD006577.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Breska A,
    2. Ivry RB
    (2018) Double dissociation of single-interval and rhythmic temporal prediction in cerebellar degeneration and Parkinson's disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 115:12283–12288. doi:10.1073/pnas.1810596115 pmid:30425170
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Cannon JJ,
    2. Patel AD
    (2021) How beat perception co-opts motor neurophysiology. Trends Cogn Sci 25:137–150. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2020.11.002 pmid:33353800
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Celma-Miralles A,
    2. Toro JM
    (2020) Discrimination of temporal regularity in rats (Rattus norvegicus) and humans (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol 134:3–10. doi:10.1037/com0000202 pmid:31589060
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Chaddock-Heyman L,
    2. Loui P,
    3. Weng TB,
    4. Weisshappel R,
    5. McAuley E,
    6. Kramer AF
    (2021) Musical training and brain volume in older adults. Brain Sci 11:50.
    OpenUrl
  13. ↵
    1. Chang A,
    2. Bosnyak DJ,
    3. Trainor LJ
    (2018) Beta oscillatory power modulation reflects the predictability of pitch change. Cortex 106:248–260. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2018.06.008 pmid:30053731
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Cheever T,
    2. Taylor A,
    3. Finkelstein R,
    4. Edwards E,
    5. Thomas L,
    6. Bradt J,
    7. Holochwost SJ,
    8. Johnson JK,
    9. Limb C,
    10. Patel AD,
    11. Tottenham N,
    12. Iyengar S,
    13. Rutter D,
    14. Fleming R,
    15. Collins FS
    (2018) NIH/Kennedy Center Workshop on Music and the Brain: Finding Harmony. Neuron 97:1214–1218. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2018.02.004 pmid:29566791
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Chen WG,
    2. Riddle R,
    3. Onken L,
    4. Cui C,
    5. Poremba A,
    6. Hillefors M,
    7. Scholoesser DM
    (2018) Promoting research on music and health: phased innovation award for music interventions (R61/R33, Clinical Trial Optional). NIH Funding Opportunity Announcement RFA-AT-19-001.
  16. ↵
    1. Chen WG,
    2. St. Hillaire-Clarke C,
    3. Onken L,
    4. Bakos A,
    5. Xu B,
    6. Leitman DI
    (2020) Promoting research on music and health: phased innovation award for music interventions (R61/R33 Clinical Trial Optional). NIH Funding Opportunity Announcement PAR-20-266.
  17. ↵
    1. Chen WG,
    2. Schloesser D,
    3. Arensdorf AM,
    4. Simmons JM,
    5. Cui C,
    6. Valentino R,
    7. Gnadt JW,
    8. Nielsen L,
    9. Hillaire-Clarke CS,
    10. Spruance V,
    11. Horowitz TS,
    12. Vallejo YF,
    13. Langevin HM
    (2021) The emerging science of interoception: sensing, integrating, interpreting, and regulating signals within the self. Trends Neurosci 44:3–16. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2020.10.007 pmid:33378655
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Cheng TH,
    2. Creel SC,
    3. Iversen JR
    (2022) How do you feel the rhythm? Dynamic motor-auditory interactions are involved in the imagination of hierarchical timing. J Neurosci 42:500–512. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1121-21.2021 pmid:34848500
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. de Witte M,
    2. Spruit A,
    3. van Hooren S,
    4. Moonen X,
    5. Stams GJ
    (2020) Effects of music interventions on stress-related outcomes: a systematic review and two meta-analyses. Health Psychol Rev 14:294–324. doi:10.1080/17437199.2019.1627897 pmid:31167611
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Doupe AJ,
    2. Perkel DJ,
    3. Reiner A,
    4. Stern EA
    (2005) Birdbrains could teach basal ganglia research a new song. Trends Neurosci 28:353–363. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2005.05.005 pmid:15935486
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Edwards E,
    2. St. Hilliaire-Clarke C.,
    3. Frank DW,
    4. Finkelstein R,
    5. Cheever T,
    6. Chen WG,
    7. Onken L,
    8. Poremba A,
    9. Riddle R,
    10. Schloesser DM,
    11. Burgdort C,
    12. Wells N,
    13. Fleming R,
    14. Collins FS
    (2022) The NIH Music-Based Interventions Toolkit. Neurology, in press.
  22. ↵
    1. Espinoza-Monroy M,
    2. de Lafuente V
    (2021) Discrimination of regular and irregular rhythms explained by a time difference accumulation model. Neuroscience 459:16–26. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2021.01.035 pmid:33549694
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Ferreri L,
    2. Mas-Herrero E,
    3. Zatorre RJ,
    4. Ripolles P,
    5. Gomez-Andres A,
    6. Alicart H,
    7. Olive G,
    8. Marco-Pallares J,
    9. Antonijoan RM,
    10. Valle M,
    11. Riba J,
    12. Rodriguez-Fornells A
    (2019) Dopamine modulates the reward experiences elicited by music. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 116:3793–3798. doi:10.1073/pnas.1811878116 pmid:30670642
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  24. ↵
    1. Flaugnacco E,
    2. Lopez L,
    3. Terribili C,
    4. Montico M,
    5. Zoia S,
    6. Schon D
    (2015) Music training increases phonological awareness and reading skills in developmental dyslexia: a randomized control trial. PLoS One 10:e0138715. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0138715 pmid:26407242
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Foubert K,
    2. Gill SP,
    3. De Backer J
    (2021) A musical improvisation framework for Shaping Interpersonal Trust. Nordic J Music Ther 30:79–96. doi:10.1080/08098131.2020.1788627
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  26. ↵
    1. Gadagkar V,
    2. Puzerey PA,
    3. Chen R,
    4. Baird-Daniel E,
    5. Farhang AR,
    6. Goldberg JH
    (2016) Dopamine neurons encode performance error in singing birds. Science 354:1278–1282. doi:10.1126/science.aah6837 pmid:27940871
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. ↵
    1. Garza-Villarreal EA,
    2. Chakravarty MM,
    3. Hansen B,
    4. Eskildsen SF,
    5. Devenyi GA,
    6. Castillo-Padilla D,
    7. Balducci T,
    8. Reyes-Zamorano E,
    9. Jespersen SN,
    10. Perez-Palacios P,
    11. Patel R,
    12. Gonzalez-Olvera JJ
    (2017) The effect of crack cocaine addiction and age on the microstructure and morphology of the human striatum and thalamus using shape analysis and fast diffusion kurtosis imaging. Transl Psychiatry 7:e1122. doi:10.1038/tp.2017.92 pmid:28485734
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Gebhardt S,
    2. Kunkel M,
    3. Georgi R
    (2014) Emotion modulation in psychiatric patients through music. Music Percept 31:485–493. doi:10.1525/mp.2014.31.5.485
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Ghai S,
    2. Ghai I,
    3. Schmitz G,
    4. Effenberg AO
    (2018) Effect of rhythmic auditory cueing on parkinsonian gait: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sci Rep 8:506. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-16232-5 pmid:29323122
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Gold BP,
    2. Mas-Herrero E,
    3. Zeighami Y,
    4. Benovoy M,
    5. Dagher A,
    6. Zatorre RJ
    (2019) Musical reward prediction errors engage the nucleus accumbens and motivate learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 116:3310–3315. doi:10.1073/pnas.1809855116 pmid:30728301
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    1. Goldberg JH,
    2. Fee MS
    (2010) Singing-related neural activity distinguishes four classes of putative striatal neurons in the songbird basal ganglia. J Neurophysiol 103:2002–2014. doi:10.1152/jn.01038.2009 pmid:20107125
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Goldberg JH,
    2. Adler A,
    3. Bergman H,
    4. Fee MS
    (2010) Singing-related neural activity distinguishes two putative pallidal cell types in the songbird basal ganglia: comparison to the primate internal and external pallidal segments. J Neurosci 30:7088–7098. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0168-10.2010 pmid:20484651
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    1. Grahn JA,
    2. Brett M
    (2007) Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain. J Cogn Neurosci 19:893–906. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.893 pmid:17488212
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Grahn JA,
    2. Brett M
    (2009) Impairment of beat-based rhythm discrimination in Parkinson's disease. Cortex 45:54–61. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2008.01.005 pmid:19027895
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Gramann K,
    2. Ferris DP,
    3. Gwin J,
    4. Makeig S
    (2014) Imaging natural cognition in action. Int J Psychophysiol 91:22–29. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.09.003 pmid:24076470
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Gramann K,
    2. Gwin JT,
    3. Ferris DP,
    4. Oie K,
    5. Jung TP,
    6. Lin CT,
    7. Liao LD,
    8. Makeig S
    (2011) Cognition in action: imaging brain/body dynamics in mobile humans. Rev Neurosci 22:593–608. doi:10.1515/RNS.2011.047 pmid:22070621
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Grewe O,
    2. Nagel F,
    3. Kopiez R,
    4. Altenmüller E
    (2007) Listening to music as a re-creative process: physiological, psychological, and psychoacoustical correlates of chills and strong emotions. Music Percept 24:297–314. doi:10.1525/mp.2007.24.3.297
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Grube M,
    2. Cooper FE,
    3. Chinnery PF,
    4. Griffiths TD
    (2010) Dissociation of duration-based and beat-based auditory timing in cerebellar degeneration. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:11597–11601. doi:10.1073/pnas.0910473107 pmid:20534501
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. ↵
    1. Guétin S,
    2. Florence P,
    3. Gabelle A,
    4. Touchon J,
    5. Bonté F
    (2011) Effects of music therapy on anxiety and depression in patients with Alzheimer's disease: a randomized controlled trial. Alzheimers Dement 7:e49.
    OpenUrl
  40. ↵
    1. Haber SN
    (2017) Anatomy and connectivity of the reward circuit. In: Decision Neuroscience (Dreher JC, Tremblay L, eds), pp 3–19. San Diego: Academic.
  41. ↵
    1. Hanser SB
    (2016) Integrative health through music therapy: accompanying the journey from illness to wellness. New York: Springer.
  42. ↵
    1. Hennessy SL,
    2. Sachs ME,
    3. Ilari B,
    4. Habibi A
    (2019) Effects of music training on inhibitory control and associated neural networks in school-aged children: a longitudinal study. Front Neurosci 13:1080. doi:10.3389/fnins.2019.01080 pmid:31680820
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Hofbauer LM,
    2. Ross SD,
    3. Rodriguez FS
    (2022) Music-based interventions for community-dwelling people with dementia: a systematic review. Health Soc Care Community. Advance online publication. Retrieved Jun 30, 2022. doi: 10.1111/hsc.13895.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  44. ↵
    1. Honing H,
    2. Bouwer FL,
    3. Prado L,
    4. Merchant H
    (2018) Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) sense isochrony in rhythm, but not the beat: additional support for the gradual audiomotor evolution hypothesis. Front Neurosci 12:475.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Huang YZ,
    2. Edwards MJ,
    3. Rounis E,
    4. Bhatia KP,
    5. Rothwell JC
    (2005) Theta burst stimulation of the human motor cortex. Neuron 45:201–206. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.033 pmid:15664172
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Janata P,
    2. Tomic ST,
    3. Haberman JM
    (2012) Sensorimotor coupling in music and the psychology of the groove. J Exp Psychol Gen 141:54–75. doi:10.1037/a0024208 pmid:21767048
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Jaschke AC,
    2. Honing H,
    3. Scherder EJA
    (2018) Longitudinal analysis of music education on executive functions in primary school children. Front Neurosci 12:103. doi:10.3389/fnins.2018.00103 pmid:29541017
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Kasdan AV,
    2. Burgess AN,
    3. Pizzagalli F,
    4. Scartozzi A,
    5. Chern A,
    6. Kotz SA,
    7. Wilson SM,
    8. Gordon RL
    (2022) Identifying a brain network for musical rhythm: a functional neuroimaging meta-analysis and systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 136:104588. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104588 pmid:35259422
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Kathios N,
    2. Sachs ME,
    3. Zhang E,
    4. Ou Y,
    5. Loui P
    (2022) Generating new musical preferences from hierarchical mapping of predictions to reward. Submitted.
  50. ↵
    1. Khalil A,
    2. Musacchia G,
    3. Iversen JR
    (2022) It takes two: interpersonal neural synchrony is increased after musical interaction. Brain Sci 12:409. doi:10.3390/brainsci12030409
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  51. ↵
    1. Ko JH,
    2. Monchi O,
    3. Ptito A,
    4. Petrides M,
    5. Strafella AP
    (2008) Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex affects performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task during provision of feedback. Int J Biomed Imaging 2008:143238. pmid:18350118
    OpenUrlPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Koelsch S,
    2. Fritz T,
    3. V Cramon DY,
    4. Müller K,
    5. Friederici AD
    (2006) Investigating emotion with music: an fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 27:239–250. doi:10.1002/hbm.20180 pmid:16078183
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Korzeniewska A,
    2. Crainiceanu CM,
    3. Kuś R,
    4. Franaszczuk PJ,
    5. Crone NE
    (2008) Dynamics of event-related causality in brain electrical activity. Hum Brain Mapp 29:1170–1192. doi:10.1002/hbm.20458 pmid:17712784
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Krotinger A,
    2. Loui P
    (2021) Rhythm and groove as cognitive mechanisms of dance intervention in Parkinson's disease. PLoS One 16:e0249933. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0249933
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  55. ↵
    1. Kung SJ,
    2. Chen JL,
    3. Zatorre RJ,
    4. Penhune VB
    (2013) Interacting cortical and basal ganglia networks underlying finding and tapping to the musical beat. J Cogn Neurosci 25:401–420. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00325 pmid:23163420
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Leslie G,
    2. Ojeda A,
    3. Makeig SD
    (2014) Measuring musical engagement using expressive movement and EEG brain dynamics. Psychomusicology 24:75–91. doi:10.1037/pmu0000031
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  57. ↵
    1. Loughridge D
    (2021) 'Always already technological': new views of music and the human in musicology and the cognitive sciences. Music Res Annu 2:1–22.
    OpenUrl
  58. ↵
    1. Loui P
    (2020) Neuroscientific insights for improved outcomes in music-based interventions. Music Sci 3:205920432096506. doi:10.1177/2059204320965065
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  59. ↵
    1. Loui P
    (2022) New music system reveals spectral contribution to statistical learning. Cognition 224:105071. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105071 pmid:35227982
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Loui P,
    2. Wessel DL,
    3. Hudson Kam CL
    (2010) Humans rapidly learn grammatical structure in a new musical scale. Music Percept 27:377–388. doi:10.1525/mp.2010.27.5.377 pmid:20740059
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  61. ↵
    1. Lunde SJ,
    2. Vuust P,
    3. Garza-Villarreal EA,
    4. Vase L
    (2019) Music-induced analgesia: how does music relieve pain? Pain 160:989–993. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001452 pmid:30507782
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Maidhof C,
    2. Kästner T,
    3. Makkonen T
    (2014) Combining EEG, MIDI, and motion capture techniques for investigating musical performance. Behav Res Methods 46:185–195. doi:10.3758/s13428-013-0363-9 pmid:23943580
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Makeig SD,
    2. Bell AJ,
    3. Jung TP
    (1996) Independent component analysis of electroencephalographic data. Adv Neural Information Process Syst 8:145–151.
    OpenUrl
  64. ↵
    1. Makeig SD,
    2. Gramann K,
    3. Jung TP,
    4. Sejnowski TJ,
    5. Poizner H
    (2009) Linking brain, mind and behavior. Int J Psychophysiol 73:95–100. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.11.008 pmid:19414039
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Maratos A,
    2. Crawford MJ,
    3. Procter S
    (2011) Music therapy for depression: it seems to work, but how? Br J Psychiatry 199:92–93. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.110.087494 pmid:21804144
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  66. ↵
    1. Margulis EH
    (2014) On repeat: how music plays the mind. Oxford: Oxford UP.
  67. ↵
    1. Martinez-Molina N,
    2. Mas-Herrero E,
    3. Rodriguez-Fornells A,
    4. Zatorre RJ,
    5. Marco-Pallares J
    (2016) Neural correlates of specific musical anhedonia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113:E7337–E7345.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  68. ↵
    1. Martinez-Molina N,
    2. Mas-Herrero E,
    3. Rodriguez-Fornells A,
    4. Zatorre RJ,
    5. Marco-Pallares J
    (2019) White matter microstructure reflects individual differences in music reward sensitivity. J Neurosci 39:5018–5027. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2020-18.2019 pmid:31000588
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  69. ↵
    1. Mas-Herrero E,
    2. Zatorre RJ,
    3. Rodriguez-Fornells A,
    4. Marco-Pallares J
    (2014) Dissociation between musical and monetary reward responses in specific musical anhedonia. Curr Biol 24:699–704. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.01.068 pmid:24613311
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Mas-Herrero E,
    2. Dagher A,
    3. Zatorre RJ
    (2018) Modulating musical reward sensitivity up and down with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Nat Hum Behav 2:27–32. doi:10.1038/s41562-017-0241-z pmid:30980048
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Mas-Herrero E,
    2. Dagher A,
    3. Farres-Franch M,
    4. Zatorre RJ
    (2021a) Unraveling the temporal dynamics of reward signals in music-induced pleasure with TMS. J Neurosci 41:3889–3899. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0727-20.2020 pmid:33782048
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  72. ↵
    1. Mas-Herrero E,
    2. Maini L,
    3. Sescousse G,
    4. Zatorre RJ
    (2021b) Common and distinct neural correlates of music and food-induced pleasure: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 123:61–71. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.12.008 pmid:33440196
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Matthews TE,
    2. Witek MA,
    3. Lund T,
    4. Vuust P,
    5. Penhune VB
    (2020) The sensation of groove engages motor and reward networks. Neuroimage 214:116768. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116768 pmid:32217163
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Merchant H,
    2. Grahn J,
    3. Trainor L,
    4. Rohrmeier M,
    5. Fitch WT
    (2015) Finding the beat: a neural perspective across humans and nonhuman primates. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 370:20140093. doi:10.1098/rstb.2014.0093 pmid:25646516
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Montag C,
    2. Reuter M,
    3. Axmacher N
    (2011) How one's favorite song activates the reward circuitry of the brain: personality matters! Behav Brain Res 225:511–514. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.08.012 pmid:21856337
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Norton P,
    2. Scharff C
    (2016) 'Bird song metronomics': isochronous organization of zebra finch song rhythm. Front Neurosci 10:309. pmid:27458334
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Novembre G,
    2. Mitsopoulos Z,
    3. Keller PE
    (2019) Empathic perspective taking promotes interpersonal coordination through music. Sci Rep 9:12255. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-48556-9 pmid:31439866
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. Patel AD
    (2008) Music as a transformative technology of the mind. In: Music: its evolution, cognitive basis, and spiritual dimensions, pp 18–20. Oxford University Press.
  79. ↵
    1. Patel AD
    (2018) Music as a transformative technology of the mind: an update. The evolution of musicality (Honing H, ed) Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
  80. ↵
    1. Patel AD,
    2. Iversen JR
    (2014) The evolutionary neuroscience of musical beat perception: the Action Simulation for Auditory Prediction (ASAP) hypothesis. Front Syst Neurosci 8:57. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2014.00057 pmid:24860439
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    1. Pereira CS,
    2. Teixeira J,
    3. Figueiredo P,
    4. Xavier J,
    5. Castro SL,
    6. Brattico E
    (2011) Music and emotions in the brain: familiarity matters. PLoS One 6:e27241. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0027241 pmid:22110619
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    1. Pogarell O,
    2. Koch W,
    3. Popperl G,
    4. Tatsch K,
    5. Jakob F,
    6. Mulert C,
    7. Grossheinrich N,
    8. Rupprecht R,
    9. Moller HJ,
    10. Hegerl U,
    11. Padberg F
    (2007) Acute prefrontal rTMS increases striatal dopamine to a similar degree as D-amphetamine. Psychiatry Res 156:251–255. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.05.002 pmid:17993266
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  83. ↵
    1. Quinci MA,
    2. Belden A,
    3. Goutama V,
    4. Gong D,
    5. Hanser S,
    6. Donovan NJ,
    7. Geddes M,
    8. Loui P
    (2022) Longitudinal changes in auditory and reward systems following receptive music-based intervention in older adults. Sci Rep 12:11517. doi:10.1038/s41598-022-15687-5
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  84. ↵
    1. Rajendran VG,
    2. Harper NS,
    3. Garcia-Lazaro JA,
    4. Lesica NA,
    5. Schnupp JW
    (2017) Midbrain adaptation may set the stage for the perception of musical beat. Proc Biol Sci 284:20171455.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  85. ↵
    1. Rauschecker JP
    (2011) An expanded role for the dorsal auditory pathway in sensorimotor control and integration. Hear Res 271:16–25. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2010.09.001 pmid:20850511
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  86. ↵
    1. Repp BH
    (2005) Rate limits of on-beat and off-beat tapping with simple auditory rhythms: 2. The roles of different kinds of accent. Music Percept 23:165–188. doi:10.1525/mp.2005.23.2.165
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  87. ↵
    1. Riddle R,
    2. Chen WG,
    3. Wiggs C,
    4. St. Hillaire-Clark C,
    5. Cui C,
    6. Poremba A,
    7. Vallejo Y,
    8. Adams L,
    9. Hillefors M,
    10. Scholoesser DM
    (2018a) Promoting research on music and health: fundamentals and applications (R01 Clinical Trials Optional). NIH Funding Opportunity Announcement RFA-NS-19-008.
  88. ↵
    1. Riddle R,
    2. Chen WG,
    3. Wiggs C,
    4. St. Hillaire-Clark C,
    5. Cui C,
    6. Poremba A,
    7. Vallejo Y,
    8. Adams L,
    9. Hillefors M,
    10. Scholoesser DM
    (2018b) Promoting research on music and health: fundamentals and applications (R21 Clinical Trials Optional). NIH Funding Opportunity Announcement RFA-NS-19-009.
  89. ↵
    1. Riddle R,
    2. Wiggs C,
    3. St. Hillaire-Clark C,
    4. Onken L,
    5. Xu B,
    6. Chen WG,
    7. Bakos A,
    8. Hoffman EA,
    9. Crisius M,
    10. Hillefors M,
    11. Mujuru P,
    12. Adams L,
    13. Scholoesser DM,
    14. Iyengar S
    (2020a) Music and health: understanding and developing music medicine (R01 Clinical Trial Optional). NIH Funding Opportunity Announcement PAR-21-100.
  90. ↵
    1. Riddle R,
    2. Wiggs C,
    3. St. Hillaire-Clark C,
    4. Onken L,
    5. Xu B,
    6. Chen WG,
    7. Bakos A,
    8. Hoffman EA,
    9. Crisius M,
    10. Hillefors M,
    11. Mujuru P,
    12. Adams L,
    13. Scholoesser DM,
    14. Iyengar S
    (2020b) Music and health: understanding and developing music medicine (R21 Clinical Trial Optional). NIH Funding Opportunity Announcement PAR-21-099.
  91. ↵
    1. Rimmele JM,
    2. Morillon B,
    3. Poeppel D,
    4. Arnal LH
    (2018) Proactive sensing of periodic and aperiodic auditory patterns. Trends Cogn Sci 22:870–882. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2018.08.003 pmid:30266147
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  92. ↵
    1. Roeske TC,
    2. Tchernichovski O,
    3. Poeppel D,
    4. Jacoby N
    (2020) Categorical rhythms are shared between songbirds and humans. Curr Biol 30:3544–3555.e6. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2020.06.072 pmid:32707062
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  93. ↵
    1. Roman-Caballero R,
    2. Arnedo M,
    3. Trivino M,
    4. Lupianez J
    (2018) Musical practice as an enhancer of cognitive function in healthy aging: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One 13:e0207957. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0207957 pmid:30481227
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  94. ↵
    1. Ross JM,
    2. Iversen JR,
    3. Balasubramaniam R
    (2016) Motor simulation theories of musical beat perception. Neurocase 22:558–565. doi:10.1080/13554794.2016.1242756
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  95. ↵
    1. Ross JM,
    2. Iversen JR,
    3. Balasubramaniam R
    (2018) The role of posterior parietal cortex in beat-based timing perception: a continuous theta burst stimulation study. J Cogn Neurosci 30:634–643. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_01237 pmid:29346017
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  96. ↵
    1. Rouse AA,
    2. Patel AD,
    3. Kao MH
    (2021) Vocal learning and flexible rhythm pattern perception are linked: evidence from songbirds. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 118:e2026130118.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  97. ↵
    1. Salimpoor VN,
    2. Benovoy M,
    3. Longo G,
    4. Cooperstock JR,
    5. Zatorre RJ
    (2009) The rewarding aspects of music listening are related to degree of emotional arousal. PLoS One 4:e7487. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007487 pmid:19834599
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  98. ↵
    1. Salimpoor VN,
    2. Benovoy M,
    3. Larcher K,
    4. Dagher A,
    5. Zatorre RJ
    (2011) Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music. Nat Neurosci 14:257–262. doi:10.1038/nn.2726 pmid:21217764
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  99. ↵
    1. Salimpoor VN,
    2. van den Bosch I,
    3. Kovacevic N,
    4. McIntosh AR,
    5. Dagher A,
    6. Zatorre RJ
    (2013) Interactions between the nucleus accumbens and auditory cortices predict music reward value. Science 340:216–219. doi:10.1126/science.1231059 pmid:23580531
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  100. ↵
    1. Särkämö T,
    2. Tervaniemi M,
    3. Laitinen S,
    4. Forsblom A,
    5. Soinila S,
    6. Mikkonen M,
    7. Autti T,
    8. Silvennoinen HM,
    9. Erkkilä J,
    10. Laine M,
    11. Peretz I,
    12. Hietanen M
    (2008) Music listening enhances cognitive recovery and mood after middle cerebral artery stroke. Brain 131:866–876. doi:10.1093/brain/awn013 pmid:18287122
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  101. ↵
    1. Savage PE,
    2. Brown S,
    3. Sakai E,
    4. Currie TE
    (2015) Statistical universals reveal the structures and functions of human music. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 112:8987–8992. doi:10.1073/pnas.1414495112 pmid:26124105
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  102. ↵
    1. Schiavo JK,
    2. Valtcheva S,
    3. Bair-Marshall CJ,
    4. Song SC,
    5. Martin KA,
    6. Froemke RC
    (2020) Innate and plastic mechanisms for maternal behaviour in auditory cortex. Nature 587:426–431. doi:10.1038/s41586-020-2807-6 pmid:33029014
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  103. ↵
    1. Schlaug G,
    2. Marchina S,
    3. Norton A
    (2009) Evidence for plasticity in white-matter tracts of patients with chronic Broca's aphasia undergoing intense intonation-based speech therapy. Ann NY Acad Sci 1169:385–394. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04587.x pmid:19673813
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  104. ↵
    1. Schoneich S
    (2020) Neuroethology of acoustic communication in field crickets: from signal generation to song recognition in an insect brain. Prog Neurobiol 194:101882.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  105. ↵
    1. Schubotz RI
    (2007) Prediction of external events with our motor system: towards a new framework. Trends Cogn Sci 11:211–218. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.006 pmid:17383218
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  106. ↵
    1. Schultz W
    (2016) Dopamine reward prediction-error signalling: a two-component response. Nat Rev Neurosci 17:183–195. doi:10.1038/nrn.2015.26 pmid:26865020
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  107. ↵
    1. Shany O,
    2. Singer N,
    3. Gold BP,
    4. Jacoby N,
    5. Tarrasch R,
    6. Hendler T,
    7. Granot R
    (2019) Surprise-related activation in the nucleus accumbens interacts with music-induced pleasantness. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 14:459–470. doi:10.1093/scan/nsz019 pmid:30892654
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  108. ↵
    1. Sihvonen AJ,
    2. Särkämö T,
    3. Leo V,
    4. Tervaniemi M,
    5. Altenmüller E,
    6. Soinila S
    (2017) Music-based interventions in neurological rehabilitation. Lancet Neurol 16:648–660. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(17)30168-0
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  109. ↵
    1. Skov M,
    2. Nadal M
    (2020) A farewell to art: aesthetics as a topic in psychology and neuroscience. Perspect Psychol Sci 15:630–642. doi:10.1177/1745691619897963 pmid:32027577
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  110. ↵
    1. Strafella AP,
    2. Paus T,
    3. Barrett J,
    4. Dagher A
    (2001) Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human prefrontal cortex induces dopamine release in the caudate nucleus. J Neurosci 21:RC157. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-15-j0003.2001 pmid:11459878
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  111. ↵
    1. Stupacher J,
    2. Hove MJ,
    3. Novembre G,
    4. Schutz-Bosbach S,
    5. Keller PE
    (2013) Musical groove modulates motor cortex excitability: aTMS investigation. Brain Cogn 82:127–136. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2013.03.003 pmid:23660433
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  112. ↵
    1. Swarbrick D,
    2. Bosnyak D,
    3. Livingstone SR,
    4. Bansal J,
    5. Marsh-Rollo S,
    6. Woolhouse MH,
    7. Trainor LJ
    (2018) How live music moves us: head movement differences in audiences to live versus recorded music. Front Psychol 9:2682. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02682 pmid:30687158
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  113. ↵
    1. Teki S,
    2. Grube M,
    3. Kumar S,
    4. Griffiths TD
    (2011) Distinct neural substrates of duration-based and beat-based auditory timing. J Neurosci 31:3805–3812. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5561-10.2011 pmid:21389235
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  114. ↵
    1. Trainor LJ,
    2. Cirelli L
    (2015) Rhythm and interpersonal synchrony in early social development. Ann NY Acad Sci 1337:45–52. doi:10.1111/nyas.12649 pmid:25773616
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  115. ↵
    1. Turner C,
    2. Visentin P,
    3. Oye D,
    4. Rathwell S,
    5. Shan G
    (2021) Pursuing artful movement science in music performance: single subject motor analysis with two elite pianists. Percept Mot Skills 128:1252–1274. doi:10.1177/00315125211003493 pmid:33775176
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  116. ↵
    1. Varlet M,
    2. Nozaradan S,
    3. Nijhuis P,
    4. Keller PE
    (2020) Neural tracking and integration of 'self' and 'other' in improvised interpersonal coordination. Neuroimage 206:116303. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116303
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  117. ↵
    1. Vuust P,
    2. Heggli OA,
    3. Friston KJ,
    4. Kringelbach ML
    (2022) Music in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 23:287–305. doi:10.1038/s41583-022-00578-5
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  118. ↵
    1. Wang Y,
    2. Brzozowska-Prechtl A,
    3. Karten HJ
    (2010) Laminar and columnar auditory cortex in avian brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:12676–12681. doi:10.1073/pnas.1006645107 pmid:20616034
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  119. ↵
    1. Wheeler BL
    (ed) (2015) Music therapy handbook: creative arts and play therapy. New York: Guildford.
  120. ↵
    1. Witek MA,
    2. Clarke EF,
    3. Wallentin M,
    4. Kringelbach ML,
    5. Vuust P
    (2014) Syncopation, body-movement and pleasure in groove music. PLoS One 9:e94446. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0094446 pmid:24740381
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  121. ↵
    1. Zatorre RJ
    (2023) From perception to pleasure: the neuroscience of music and why we love it. New York: Oxford UP.
  122. ↵
    1. Zatorre RJ,
    2. Chen JL,
    3. Penhune VB
    (2007) When the brain plays music: auditory–motor interactions in music perception and production. Nat Rev Neurosci 8:547–558. doi:10.1038/nrn2152 pmid:17585307
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  123. ↵
    1. Zhou W,
    2. Ye C,
    3. Wang H,
    4. Mao Y,
    5. Zhang W,
    6. Liu A,
    7. Yang CL,
    8. Li T,
    9. Hayashi L,
    10. Zhao W,
    11. Chen L,
    12. Liu Y,
    13. Tao W,
    14. Zhang Z
    (2022) Sound induces analgesia through corticothalamic circuits. Science 377:198–204. doi:10.1126/science.abn4663 pmid:35857536
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  124. ↵
    1. Zumbansen A,
    2. Peretz I,
    3. Hebert S
    (2014) The combination of rhythm and pitch can account for the beneficial effect of melodic intonation therapy on connected speech improvements in Broca's aphasia. Front Hum Neurosci 8:592.
    OpenUrl
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 42 (45)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 42, Issue 45
9 Nov 2022
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Music and Brain Circuitry: Strategies for Strengthening Evidence-Based Research for Music-Based Interventions
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Music and Brain Circuitry: Strategies for Strengthening Evidence-Based Research for Music-Based Interventions
Wen Grace Chen, John Rehner Iversen, Mimi H. Kao, Psyche Loui, Aniruddh Dhiren Patel, Robert J. Zatorre, Emmeline Edwards
Journal of Neuroscience 9 November 2022, 42 (45) 8498-8507; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1135-22.2022

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Music and Brain Circuitry: Strategies for Strengthening Evidence-Based Research for Music-Based Interventions
Wen Grace Chen, John Rehner Iversen, Mimi H. Kao, Psyche Loui, Aniruddh Dhiren Patel, Robert J. Zatorre, Emmeline Edwards
Journal of Neuroscience 9 November 2022, 42 (45) 8498-8507; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1135-22.2022
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • musical components
  • music-based interventions
  • brain circuits
  • technologies
  • therapeutic effects

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Advances in Deep Brain Stimulation: From Mechanisms to Applications
  • Beyond the 5-HT2A Receptor: Classic and Nonclassic Targets in Psychedelic Drug Action
  • Astrocytes as Drivers and Disruptors of Behavior: New Advances in Basic Mechanisms and Therapeutic Targeting
Show more Symposium and Mini-Symposium
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.