Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Interdigitated Columnar Representation of Personal Space and Visual Space in Human Parietal Cortex

Roger B.H. Tootell, Zahra Nasiriavanaki, Baktash Babadi, Douglas N. Greve, Shahin Nasr and Daphne J. Holt
Journal of Neuroscience 30 November 2022, 42 (48) 9011-9029; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0516-22.2022
Roger B.H. Tootell
2Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
3Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Brigham Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts 02129
4Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, Massachusetts 02129
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Zahra Nasiriavanaki
1Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, Massachusetts 02129
2Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Baktash Babadi
1Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, Massachusetts 02129
2Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Douglas N. Greve
2Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
3Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Brigham Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts 02129
4Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, Massachusetts 02129
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Shahin Nasr
2Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
3Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Brigham Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts 02129
4Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, Massachusetts 02129
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Daphne J. Holt
1Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, Massachusetts 02129
2Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
3Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Brigham Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts 02129
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Personal space (PS) is the space around the body that people prefer to maintain between themselves and unfamiliar others. Intrusion into personal space evokes discomfort and an urge to move away. Physiologic studies in nonhuman primates suggest that defensive responses to intruding stimuli involve the parietal cortex. We hypothesized that the spatial encoding of interpersonal distance is initially transformed from purely sensory to more egocentric mapping within human parietal cortex. This hypothesis was tested using 7 Tesla (7T) fMRI at high spatial resolution (1.1 mm isotropic), in seven subjects (four females, three males). In response to visual stimuli presented at a range of virtual distances, we found two categories of distance encoding in two corresponding radially-extending columns of activity within parietal cortex. One set of columns (P columns) responded selectively to moving and stationary face images presented at virtual distances that were nearer (but not farther) than each subject's behaviorally-defined personal space boundary. In most P columns, BOLD response amplitudes increased monotonically and nonlinearly with increasing virtual face proximity. In the remaining P columns, BOLD responses decreased with increasing proximity. A second set of parietal columns (D columns) responded selectively to disparity-based distance cues (near or far) in random dot stimuli, similar to disparity-selective columns described previously in occipital cortex. Critically, in parietal cortex, P columns were topographically interdigitated (nonoverlapping) with D columns. These results suggest that visual spatial information is transformed from visual to body-centered (or person-centered) dimensions in multiple local sites within human parietal cortex.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Recent COVID-related social distancing practices highlight the need to better understand brain mechanisms which regulate “personal space” (PS), which is defined by the closest interpersonal distance that is comfortable for an individual. Using high spatial resolution brain imaging, we tested whether a map of external space is transformed from purely visual (3D-based) information to a more egocentric map (related to personal space) in human parietal cortex. We confirmed this transformation and further showed that it was mediated by two mutually segregated sets of columns: one which encoded interpersonal distance and another that encoded visual distance. These results suggest that the cortical transformation of sensory-centered to person-centered encoding of space near the body involves short-range communication across interdigitated columns within parietal cortex.

  • 7T
  • cortical columns
  • high-resolution fMRI
  • personal space

Introduction

The importance of personal space (PS) regulation has been highlighted recently by the widespread adoption of “social distancing” practices, designed to reduce COVID-19 transmission (Cartaud et al., 2020, Iachini et al., 2021; Welsch et al., 2020, 2021; Holt et al., 2022). However, well before the introduction of this deliberate social distancing, numerous behavioral studies have described a fundamental discomfort that people experience when an unfamiliar person becomes “too close,” intruding into their PS (Hayduk, 1978, 1983). This interpersonal distancing between conspecifics may be related to the survival-based distancing that occurs between prey and predator species (Hediger, 1955; Ardrey, 1966).

Most models of PS processing distinguish between an inner (personal) zone, and the distance beyond that. This inner zone extends from the body surface to the PS boundary, typically 60–100 cm away (Hayduk, 1983). Repeated measurements of the distance to the PS boundary are highly reliable within a given individual but vary substantially between individuals (Hayduk, 1983; Bar-Haim et al., 2002; Cléry and Hamed, 2018; Tootell et al., 2021; Welsch et al., 2021). Within this inner zone, discomfort increases gradually with closer interpersonal proximity (Hayduk, 1981a; Welsch et al., 2019; Tootell et al., 2021). Beyond the PS boundary, discomfort is typically not elicited by the presence of another person (but see Welsch et al., 2019). Similarly, skin conductance (a measure of sympathetic nervous system arousal) increases when an experimental confederate is positioned close to a subject within (but not beyond) the PS boundary (Candini et al., 2021; Tootell et al., 2021). The concept of PS (as defined by the discomfort and/or defensive response evoked by interpersonal intrusion) is closely related to the concepts of peri-personal space (PPS; Bogdanova et al., 2021), body ownership (BO), and body schema.

Relatively little is known about the brain mechanisms that underlie PS processing. Several human fMRI studies suggest a role for the parietal cortex in the processing of PS and/or BO (Lloyd et al., 2006; Schienle et al., 2015; Grivaz et al., 2017). One study (Holt et al., 2014) described higher fMRI activity in response to images of approaching (compared with withdrawing) faces, in two specific parietal regions. Because these responses were stronger to faces than objects, these results were interpreted as a selective response to a social stimulus, rather than to approaching stimuli per se.

Some involvement of parietal cortex in PS processing is not unexpected because: (1) PS intrusion is a specific type of threatening event, and (2) in monkeys, threat-defensive responses have been reported in parietal cortex (and in premotor areas F4/5, which are interconnected with parietal cortex; Luppino et al., 1999). For instance, defensive behavior was repeatedly elicited by electrical micro-stimulation in inferior parietal cortex (Cooke et al., 2003) and (at lower threshold) in areas F4/5 (Graziano et al., 2002).

Threats in the visuo-spatial environment might lead to transformations in distance encoding within the brain, to enhance the representation of the threatening stimuli. Thus, here we investigated whether an eye-centered spatial representation (from occipital cortex) is transformed into a body-centered (more egocentric, or PS-related) representation in inferior parietal cortex, as previously suggested (Hadjidimitrakis et al., 2014; Cléry et al., 2017).

Such a hypothetical transformation from eye-centered to body-centered spatial representations could be topographically organized in different ways, including: (1) a large-scale gradient distributed across the parietal surface, (2) in multiple smaller-scale local networks (e.g., cortical columns), or (3) randomly, without systematic topographic organization. Such alternative organizations imply correspondingly differing computational schemes. The current findings generally supported the second model. Our results demonstrated multiple, local transformations of spatial information in parietal cortex, mediated via two types of BOLD-defined radial groupings (“columns”). These two column types differed in the nature of their spatial encoding properties, from visual to more egocentric spatial scales. To our knowledge, no type of cortical column has been described previously in human parietal cortex.

Materials and Methods

Experimental design and statistical analysis

Participants

Seven human subjects (four females; age range: 22–29 years; mean: 24.6 years) participated in this study. This sample size was based on those of similar, prior studies of 7 Tesla (7T) fMRI data analyzed initially at the single subject level (Nasr et al., 2016; Tootell and Nasr, 2017; Nasr and Tootell, 2018a,b, 2020).

All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity (Snellen test) and radiologically normal brains, and were without any history of neuropsychiatric disorders. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects before enrollment, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All experimental procedures were conducted according to Massachusetts General Hospital protocols, approved by the Massachusetts General Brigham Institutional Review Board. All data for this study were collected between February through October 2019, before the onset of the COVID pandemic in the greater Boston area.

Measurements of personal space

In each subject, the size of PS was measured in two ways. First, we used the standard stop distance procedure (SDP; Williams, 1971; Hayduk, 1978, 1983), in which each subject identified their preferred, comfortable distance from an “intruder” (an experimental confederate), in a neutral laboratory setting. During this procedure, subjects were asked to stand still, facing the intruder, who initially stood 3 m away from the subject. Subjects were instructed as follows: “Please stand still while my colleague walks slowly toward you. Say 'okay' when you start to feel slightly uncomfortable. When you say 'okay,' they will pause. Please make sure to maintain eye contact with my colleague throughout the procedure.” The distance between the subject and the intruder was measured after each trial, defined as the distance between the tips of each pair of shoes (the “stop distance”).

The second set of measurements was acquired while each subject lay prone in the MRI scanner, in the absence of scanning. Each subject performed a computerized version of the SDP, in which subjects were presented with face images which appeared to move toward them. A small central fixation target was superimposed on the bridge of the nose of each face image. The subjects were instructed to maintain fixation and to press a button on a button box when the face reached a virtual distance at which the subject began to feel uncomfortable.

The outside-the-scanner SDP values were generated as a standard for comparison to values in the literature, and to the within-scanner measurements. The within-scanner values were used to precalculate stimulus dimensions, and for the data analysis, and as a within-subject comparison to the outside-the-scanner values. In analyses of both types of PS measurement, the first trial was excluded as a “warm up” trial, as described previously (Hayduk, 1981b).

Our use of face images as stimuli here is consistent with previous studies that have also presented human-like stimuli, including mannequins, virtual reality-based avatars, and faces images (Bailenson et al., 2003; Mccall et al., 2009; Llobera et al., 2010; Rinck et al., 2010; Wieser et al., 2010; Iachini et al., 2014, 2016; Schienle et al., 2015; Ruggiero et al., 2016; Hecht et al., 2019; Welsch et al., 2019; Tootell et al., 2021), which can elicit PS-related responses analogous to those evoked by real human subjects (Tootell et al., 2021).

Visual stimuli

Stimuli were presented via an LCD projector (1024 × 768-pixel resolution, 60-Hz refresh rate) onto a rear-projection screen, viewed through a mirror mounted on the receive coil array. MATLAB 2018b (MathWorks) and the Psychophysics Toolbox were used for stimulus presentation (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997). Details of the experiments are described below, and schematized in Figure 1.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Schematic of the experimental paradigms used in the three experiments. A, Experiment 1. Subjects were shown continuously approaching faces (in one 15-second block) or continuously withdrawing faces (in other 15 sec blocks), presented in pseudorandom order. Variations in virtual stimulus distance were calculated as co-variations in the size and disparity cues (disparity cues not shown here), calculated to match those in real life. Thus, the approaching and withdrawing stimuli were identical to each other, other than the motion direction. BOLD responses were corrected for the known hemodynamic delay, then subtracted from each other in subsequent analyses to identify the cortical sites in which stimuli were significantly biased toward the approaching or withdrawing stimuli. B, Experiment 2. Subjects were shown stationary 3D faces at a range of virtual distances, using an event-related design (average stimulus duration = 3 seconds and ITI = 3 to 18 seconds) for each face distance. The virtual face distances ranged from 37% to 524% of each subject's individual personal space size, based on prior behavioral measurements. Presentation order was pseudorandomized. C, Experiment 3. Subjects were presented with random dot stimuli (RDS), to test for (and localize) disparity selective columns. The top row shows the shape of the 3D stimuli evoked during successful stereoscopic fusion of the RDS stimuli. The surface topography of the cuboids is illustrated as in Tsao et al., 2003. The shapes evoked selective activation by 24-second blocks of dynamic disparity-based 'near' or 'far' distance, or (as a control condition) zero disparity in the center of gaze (i.e., a fronto-parallel plane). An example of the monocular stimuli (which were binocularly fused during the experiment) are shown. Binocular fusion was achieved using color anaglyphs. The central white dot is a fixation target.

Experiment 1: approaching versus withdrawing 3D face stimuli

A series of sixteen face images were presented, in four different conditions: Female Approach, Female Withdrawal, Male Approach, and Male Withdrawal. These face stimuli had emotionally neutral (no valence) expressions. All faces were generated using a commercially available program (FaceGen), based on a customizable 3D face mesh. Multiple facial features, including skin pigmentation and face morphology, were customizable on the mesh surfaces, and varied across the set of faces. The face stimuli included 6 Caucasian, 2 Asian, 4 African-American, and 4 Multi-Racial -appearing face images. Examples of these face images are shown in Figure 1A,B. Each approaching or withdrawing face series was presented throughout a single block (15 s in duration), in a pseudorandom order, using eighteen blocks/run, and eight runs/session. To define baseline BOLD levels, two 15-s displays of uniform gray (including a small central fixation target) were also presented, one at the beginning and one at the end of each run.

The experimental manipulation of virtual distance was based on differences in face size and binocular disparity, presented within a wide field of view, to match the corresponding visual changes that would occur during the perception of a real person, either approaching or withdrawing, across a range of 39–300 cm from the viewer, at a constant walking speed (1.12 m/s). Consistent with published values, we assumed an average real-world face width of 16 cm. Within the scanner, these synthetic 3D faces were viewed at a real distance of 114 cm from the screen (i.e., the sum of distances from eye to mirror, plus from the mirror to the stimulus screen), thus 32 cm on the stimulus screen. The mean (real and virtual) PS size (i.e., 100% of the average virtual PS size) were both ∼60 cm from the viewer, thus ∼33.6 cm on the stimulus screen. (Because the size of PS varied between individuals, these values are only illustrative.)

The stimuli were similar to those used previously (Holt et al., 2014), except that 3D cues were added to the face images, based on real-world differences in binocular disparity at different viewing distances. Subjects viewed all disparity-varying stimuli using red-cyan anaglyph glasses [Kodak Wratten filters no. 25 (red) and no. 44A (cyan)] that were mounted on the top of the receive head coil, inside the scanner. Otherwise, virtual face distance was made as realistic as possible. For instance, in real life, binocularly viewed variations in face distance include small 3D face rotations, in opposed directions, in each of the component monocular views. Such realistic image changes were incorporated in the 3D face images used here. We also assumed an interpupillary distance of 6 cm. Thus, when viewed at 57 cm, an interpupillary disparity of 6 degrees in the 3D mesh would be matched by a rotation of the face mesh by three degrees in opposite rotation directions, for each eye. At different viewing distances, these values differed accordingly.

Subjects performed a dummy attention task during presentation of the face stimuli, in which a colored dot was presented repeatedly within each block at a random location on the computer screen (three presentations/block, each with a duration of 400 ms, timed unpredictably relative to the main stimuli). Subjects were instructed to maintain fixation on a small target located at the center of the images (specifically, at the junction between the bottom of the forehead and the upper terminus of the nose), and to press a button (located on the button box inside the scanner) whenever they saw the dot presented on the screen. The instructions were repeated before each run.

Experiment 2: stationary 3D face stimuli

In experiment 2, subjects viewed stationary face images, using a subset of the images that were presented during experiment 1. Here, each stationary face was presented at different virtual distances, in a pseudorandom order, in an event-related design. Each face was presented for 3 s followed by an intertrial interval (a spatially uniform gray screen) varying between 3 and 12 s. Virtual face distances were individually precalculated for each subject, based on each subject's PS boundary as measured previously within the scanner. Each face was presented at nine systematically varied virtual distances: four distances inside the subject's PS boundary, four distances outside the subject's PS boundary, and one distance corresponding to the subject's PS boundary (i.e., at 37%, 57%, 76%, 91%, 100%, 182%, 295%, 419%, and 524% of each subject's PS boundary).

Experiment 3: binocular disparity (stereoscopic) stimuli

In this experiment, disparity-varying stimuli were presented in sparse (5%) random dot stereograms based on small red or green squares (each 0.09° × 0.09°) presented against a black background, which extended 20° × 20° in the visual field. Two random dot stimuli (each either red or green) were overlaid on the display screen, and binocularly fused during BOLD acquisitions. In the main condition, stimuli formed a stereoscopic percept of a regular array of cuboids that varied sinusoidally in-depth, i.e., within 0.2–0.4° of horizontal binocular disparity, stereoscopically either “near” or “far,” relative to the fixation plane, with independent phase, similar to stimuli described earlier (Tsao et al., 2003; Nasr et al., 2016; see Fig. 1C). In the control condition, the fused percept spanned a fronto-parallel plane intersecting the fixation target (i.e., zero depth at the point of ocular convergence). Each block-designed experimental run included eight stimulus blocks (24 s/block), beginning and ending with an additional 12 s of uniform gray (“blank”). When viewed monocularly, or when the binocular images were identical, these control stimuli instead appeared as an array of dots, moving coherently and slowly from left to right (and vice versa) in the zero disparity plane (uncorrected for variation in the arc tangent).

To measure and control the level of attention during the 3D disparity scans, subjects were required to report changes in the shape (identifying the longer axis) of a small (average length/width = 0.1–0.2°) rectangular fixation target, by pressing a button located on the button box inside the scanner.

Imaging

All MRI experiments were conducted in a 7T Siemens whole-body scanner equipped with SC72 body gradients (maximum gradient strength, 70 mT/m; maximum slew rate = 200 T/m/s) using a 32-channel helmet receive coil array and a birdcage volume transmit coil. The anatomic data collected from each subject was based on a high-resolution multi-echo T1-weighted magnetization-prepared gradient-echo image [MPRAGE, 0.80-mm isotropic voxels, with a repetition time (TR) of 2530 ms, an inversion time of 1100 ms, an echo time (TE) of 1.76 ms, a flip angle (FA) of 7° and a field of view (FOV) of 240 mm]. The MR sequence used in this study did not include any parallel or partial Fourier imaging techniques. Echo spacing was 0.79 ms (no free echo spacing). Functional images were acquired using a single-shot gradient-echo EPI with 1.1 mm (isotropic) voxel size, TR = 3000 ms; TE = 26 ms; flip angle = 90°; FOV = 192 mm, number of slices = 51; EPI factor = 174. Each run included 90 TRs in experiments 1 and 2, and 80 TRs in experiment 3. In all scans, the coverage included the dorsal and posterior occipital cortex, and dorsal parietal cortex [see region of interest (ROI)]. In each subject, the number of functional volumes acquired was 1440 for experiment 1, 720 for experiment 2, and 1280 for experiment 3.

General imaging data analysis

All structural and functional MRI data analyses were performed using the FreeSurfer Functional Analysis Stream (FSFAST) v.6 (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/FsFast). In four main steps, this included: (1) bias field correction, (2) cortical reconstruction, (3) preprocessing of the functional data, and (4) first level and group statistical analyses (Dale and Sereno, 1993; Dale et al., 1999; Fischl and Dale, 2000; Fischl et al., 2001, 2002, 2004a, b). Details of these steps are as follows.

Bias field correction

To correct the anatomic scan for signal intensity variation across the brain volume caused by inhomogeneity of the B1 field at 7T, a bias field correction was performed using SPM 8. A Bayesian model estimated a smooth function that was multiplied with the image using prior knowledge about the field distributions likely to be encountered (Ashburner and Friston, 2005). The model assumed that the field inhomogeneity (a type of “noise”) is multiplicative and because of variations of the tissue properties in each voxel, rather than because of noise from the scanner. The full-width-half-maximum (FWHM) for the Gaussian smoothness used a bias of 18 mm, with a very light (0.0001) bias regularization (Zaretskaya et al., 2018).

Cortical reconstruction

The automated reconstruction of the structural data (FreeSurfer; https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu) included the following steps: image realignment and motion correction, skull stripping (removing the nonbrain tissue), gray-white matter segmentation, reconstruction of cortical surface models, and labeling of the regions on the cortical surfaces based on probabilistic information, derived from a manually labeled training set.

fMRI preprocessing

BOLD data were registered to the same-subject anatomic data using boundary-based registration (Greve and Fischl, 2009) and corrected for head motion, then sampled onto the cortical surface, where all further analysis was performed. Data were not spatially smoothed (i.e., 0 mm FWHM). The hemodynamic response amplitudes for each condition were estimated using a general linear model in FSFAST. Regressors were constructed by convolving the stimulus box car with the SPM canonical hemodynamic response function. Head motion parameters were treated as nuisance regressors; polynomial regressors were included to account for low-frequency drift. Voxel-wise statistical tests were conducted by computing functional contrasts.

During this analytical step, we measured and corrected for head motion and EPI distortion using two programs in FreeSurfer. First, the tkregister2 program allowed us to visually check for possible deviations in the otherwise-automated registration of functional to anatomic alignment. In very few cases, we found minor deviations in the automated registration, which were corrected manually.

Second, we used “tkregister-sess” to check the registration cost values. These cost values increased between 0 and 1. For this study, values below 0.7 were considered acceptable (https://www.freesurfer.net/pub/dist/freesurfer/tutorial_versions/freesurfer/fsfast/bin/tkregister-sess). In our experimental total of 264 runs (eight runs/session, and 33 sessions), the registration cost values across all/each of these runs were significantly lower than 0.7 (mean cost value = 0.25, SD = 0.13).

Quality control

All brain images were visually inspected for brain coverage, registration quality and anatomic defects. The FSL motion outlier algorithm was used to eliminate time points showing excessive head motion across volumes (Power et al., 2012), by using a compound matrix of outlier timepoints as a regressor in the general linear model (GLM). Among the several metrics available for defining outlier time points (Power et al., 2012; Bastiani et al., 2019), we used the DVARS metric (D referring to temporal derivative of time courses, VARS referring to RMS variance over voxels; Smyser et al., 2010), which reflects the rate of change of the BOLD signal across the entire brain in comparison to the previous time point (Power et al., 2012). The threshold used to define an outlier was set to the default value suggested by Power et al. (2012; 75th percentile + 1.5 times the interquartile range of the DVARS metric). Any time point with a value larger than this threshold was eliminated from further analysis, as an outlier. Very few timepoints (0.08% of the total) were rejected based on this criterion.

In addition to time point motion correction, the six rigid-body motion parameters within each volume were used as regressors, to account for absolute motion. Based on this threshold criterion, 9.9% of the timepoints were excluded, across all the runs (n = 264) from all three experiments.

In the responses to the dummy attention task, outliers were defined as a score (in any of the experiments) of less than two standard deviations from the mean. No data met this criterion (experiment 1: M = 92.15%, SD = 3.3; experiment 3: M = 93.48%, SD = 7.09).

Region of interest (ROI)

In prior group-averaged, conventional-resolution fMRI studies, preferential activity to both approaching/withdrawing (Holt et al., 2014) and disparity-selective (Tsao et al., 2003) stimuli were found within the dorsal medial and lateral banks of the intraparietal sulcus (IPS). Accordingly, the ROI used here was defined based on anatomic (sulcal/gyral) features bounding this region, specifically including the dorsal half of the dorsal parietal-occipital sulcus (dPOS) on the medial bank, continuing to the lateral bank of the IPS, then crossing perpendicularly to the small sulci on the lateral bank of the IPS, to intersect the starting point near dPOS. In the single-subject data, this ROI was defined on the cortical surface of each subject. In the group averaged data, this ROI was defined based on the averaged gyri and sulci in the group-averaged cortical surface (Fischl et al., 1999). This ROI includes areas POIPS, VIPS, hLIP, and VIPS as previously described (Orban et al., 2006), and perhaps areas SPL 7A and 7P (likelihood 45% and 32%, respectively) of Julich and colleagues (Amunts et al., 2020).

Specific analysis of 7T data

Consistency across sessions

To quantify the level of consistency between approach-biased and withdrawal-biased activity across different scan sessions, we measured the fMRI signal change evoked by the functional contrasts of interest, at a threshold of p < 0.05 (Face Approach > Face Withdrawal and Face Withdrawal > Face Approach), across the first two scan sessions of each experiment, for each vertex within the ROI, and in the average of the two scan sessions. The level of overlap in the resultant maps was used to quantify the consistency across sessions, when aligned (100% overlap = perfect consistency), compared with when systematically nonaligned. These control measurements of nonaligned maps were calculated by pseudo-randomizing the alignment of maps from session 1 relative to session 2.

Consistency across depth

To assess the radial (“columnar”) consistency in BOLD-activated sites, we subdivided and compared fMRI activity centered at multiple cortical depths, as described previously (Polimeni et al., 2010). For each subject, one surface was generated at the interface between the gray matter and the white matter (depth = 1.0), and a second surface was generated at the superficial boundary between the gray matter and the pia (depth = 0.0), based on each subject's high-resolution structural scans using FreeSurfer (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 2002). In addition to these two bounding surfaces, intermediate “mid-gray” surfaces were also generated, e.g., at 50% of the depth of the local gray matter (depth = 0.5, including cortical layer 4), and at 80% of the depth of the local gray matter (depth = 0.8, including layers 5 and 6; Dale et al., 1999).

Activity maps from experiment 1 (approach/withdrawal)

To create group-averaged maps, cortical surfaces from all hemispheres and subjects (14 hemispheres) were registered to an average reference surface for each hemisphere, and functional data were registered onto the average anatomic data, corrected for head motion and normalized without spatial smoothing (FWHM = 0; Fischl et al., 1999). Statistical analyses were performed by fitting a univariate general linear model (GLM) with a one-sample group mean design (OSGM), using a weighted least square to the fMRI data. The significance maps were created based on the predefined conditions, at a threshold of p < 0.01.

Columnar organization

To examine the BOLD maps from a viewpoint perpendicular to the cortical topography, the BOLD signal change evoked by the functional contrast of interest (face approach vs face withdrawal) was sampled at multiple equally-spaced points across the cortical depth. That activity was then illustrated along a line drawn along each subject's cortical surface map, which crossed patches of high and low activity.

Next, a more comprehensive analysis was conducted to test for a columnar organization across all subjects, within all parietal ROIs, by calculating the correlation between the selective activity in vertices distributed along each of the three cortical axes in the flattened cortical maps: X and Y within the surface-parallel cortical map, and Z along the perpendicular (radial) axis. As a control, we also measured BOLD activity at two different cortical depths: superficial (depth = 0.1) and deep (depth = 0.9). Given the cortical thickness within the ROI (mean = 2.37 mm), corresponding points on these two surface maps were centered 1.9 mm apart in depth, on average. For each of the three axes, correlation values were then calculated independently for each subject, and compared using paired t tests following transformation of the Pearson r values to z values using Fisher's transformation formula.

BOLD responses from experiment 2 (stationary faces)

For each subject, the average BOLD responses within the approach-selective and withdrawal-selective columns were calculated for each virtual stimulus distance. These distances were calculated separately for each subject, as percentages of the same subject's mean PS boundary, measured in the scanner.

Overlap measurements of the approach/withdrawal versus disparity-based activity maps

In several analyses, we measured the extent of overlap in evoked activity in different functional contrasts. For instance, we measured the overlap in the maps from each of four functional contrasts (approach-biased, withdrawal-biased, disparity near-biased and disparity far-biased), independently in the approach/withdrawal and disparity-selective columns, across a wide range of thresholds [log (p) 1.3 −10]. The overlap was calculated as a percentage of the surface area, in two ways: (1) when the maps were correctly aligned, and (2) (as a control condition) when one of the maps was randomly misaligned relative to the other (i.e., spatially shuffled) 1000 times.

Additional details of these experiments are equivalent to those published previously (Nasr et al., 2016; Tootell and Nasr, 2017, 2021; Nasr and Tootell, 2018a,b, 2020).

Data/codes/software

Behavioral and brain data (available on request).

MATLAB (RRID: SCR_001622; https://www.mathworks.com).

FreeSurfer (RRID:SCR_001847; https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/FsFast).

SPM (RRID:SCR_007037; https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/).

SPSS (RRID:SCR_007037; https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/).

Psychophysics Toolbox (RRID:SCR_002881; http://psychtoolbox.org/docs/Psychtoolbox).

Results

Behavioral measurements of personal space size

Figure 2 shows the PS size measurements for each subject using: (1) the standard stop distance procedure (SDP; Williams, 1971; Hayduk, 1981a, 1983), in response to an approaching human confederate outside of the scanner, and (2) in a within-scanner SDP, in response to the virtually-approaching 3D face images (see Materials and Methods). A random effect repeated measures ANOVA showed no significant differences among the SDP measurements collected for each subject (Greenhouse–Geisser F(2.5,16.1) = 0.95, p = 0.45; mean = 57 cm, SD = 11.3; Fig. 2A). Similarly, in the within-scanner PS data, a repeated measures ANOVA showed statistically equivalent mean values over repeated measurements (Fig. 2B), i.e., without evidence for habituation across trials (Greenhouse–Geisser F(2.9,17.8) = 1.85, p = 0.17; mean = 60.72 cm, SD = 15.01).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Personal space measurements differed between subjects but remained consistent across repeated measurements within each subject. A, Measurements acquired outside the scanner, using the standard passive stop distance procedure, in response to real human confederates (“intruders”). B, Analogous measurements to intruding face images, acquired while subjects were in the 7T scanner, in the absence of MR imaging. After a practice trial, personal space was measured in 5 (A) or 15 (B) trials, in each subject (n = 7) who was subsequently scanned in the main experiments. In both panels, data from the same subject are shown in a different, arbitrarily assigned color. The means are superimposed as a dashed white line. Error bars represent 1 SEM.

These behavioral results are consistent with results from prior studies which reported high within-subject reliability over time in the standard SDP (Hayduk, 1981a, 1983; Wormith, 1984; Tootell et al., 2021; Welsch et al., 2021). This demonstration of high reliability in the PS measurements was an important prerequisite for the subsequent analyses, insofar as our statistical sensitivity relied on extensive signal averaging over repeated measurements.

Within-scanner task response accuracy

Mean accuracy during the dummy attention task (all subjects, all sessions) was 92.2% in response to the face stimuli, and 93.5% in response to the random dot stimuli.

Experiment 1: approach-biased activity

In a prior study based on group-averaged (n = 22) conventional fMRI results (3T, 3 mm isotropic), presentation of approaching versus withdrawing face images consistently produced two patches of approach-biased activity in dorsal parietal cortex, one located posterior to the other (Holt et al., 2014). Using similar (but 3D) face stimuli here (see Materials and Methods), we tested for comparable maps of activity using a 7T scanner, in both group-averaged (n = 7) and in single subject analyses, at high (1.1 mm isotropic) spatial resolution.

Consistent with the earlier 3T results, the activity evoked here included two regions of approach-dominated activity, in anterior and posterior sites in the parietal cortex (Fig. 3). In the subsequent analyses, we focused on the posterior site, which was located in the posterior parietal cortex immediately anterior to (and known to share connections with) dorsal occipital (“visual”) cortex. Based on its proximity to visual cortex, we hypothesized that this posterior parietal ROI would be likely to show evidence for a transformation from eye-based spatial encoding (from visual cortex) to body-based spatial encoding (in parietal cortex).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Group-averaged (n = 7) cortical activity in response to virtually approaching (compared with withdrawing) face images, scanned at 7T, 1.1 mm isotropic. Data are shown from left and right hemispheres (L and R, top) in the corresponding cortical surfaces, viewed from a posterior-dorsal viewpoint in inflated cortical format (A), and in a cortically flattened view (B). The significance scale on the bottom indicates -log10(p), where p is chance probability (yellow/red = approach > withdrawal; cyan/blue = withdrawal > approach). The ROI here (see white outlines, in posterior parietal cortex) was based on the averaged gyral/sulcal anatomy in the group-averaged cortical surface. In B, the white arrows indicate clusters of activity within the dorsal parietal-occipital sulcus (dPOS). The spatial scale bar (bottom right) applies to the flattened view, subject to the expected distortion (±15%) due to cortical flattening.

A prior 3T study (Quinlan and Culham, 2007) found that a discrete site within the dorsal parietal-occipital sulcus (dPOS) responded selectively to “near” (compared with “far”) visual objects. Here, the corresponding parietal cortex site (Fig. 3B) also showed a strong bias for approaching (compared with withdrawing) stimuli, evident in the group-averaged activity, and in 11 (of 14) hemispheres tested.

Single subject maps

Two individual subject maps are shown in Figure 4. Maps from all subjects showed similar, approach-biased patches the parietal ROI (Fig. 5). These maps appeared highly consistent within each subject/hemisphere when compared across sessions (Fig. 6A–F). These observations were supported by comparisons within a given subject, when the maps of the two sessions were topographically aligned (as in vivo), compared with the nonaligned (systematically shuffled) map comparisons (p < 0.0001; Fig. 6G,H). Thus, the approach-versus-withdrawal maps did not appear to be dominated by statistical noise, which should vary across sessions.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

High-resolution approach vs. withdrawal activity maps from single subjects. Panels show flattened maps of cortical activity, in both hemispheres, in each of two individual subjects (1 and 3) in A and B, respectively. The maps display approach-biased versus withdrawal-biased activity, in red-yellow and blue-cyan, respectively (scale bar on the right). The medial-lateral border (where the cortex folds) is shown as a dashed green line, and the parietal ROI is indicated by a white outline. Ant, anterior; Post, posterior; Med, medial; Lat, lateral; L, left hemisphere; R, right hemisphere. The significance scale indicates -log10(p).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Response to approaching versus withdrawing face stimuli in each subject. Approach-biased versus withdrawal-biased activity is displayed in red-yellow and blue-cyan, respectively. The significance scale indicates -log10(p). All maps were averaged across both scan sessions, in each individual subject, in both right and left hemispheres (A–G), in the parietal cortex ROI (indicated by white outlines). H, Average across all subjects.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Consistency of evoked activity maps across sessions. Left panel, Consistency of BOLD activity maps in flattened cortex of the ROI, in response to the approaching versus withdrawing stimuli, across each of two sessions (A–D), and their average across both sessions (E, F), from subjects 3 and 2 (left and right panels, respectively). In panels A–F, yellow/red = approach > withdrawal; cyan/blue = withdrawal > approach. The corresponding scale on the right indicates -log10(p). Right panel, Overlap analysis confirms the consistency of the within-subject maps across scanning sessions. G, Group-averaged (n = 7) level of overlap in within-subject maps of approach > withdrawal activity, when generated in session 1 compared to session 2, when the two maps were aligned correctly relative to each other and to the cortical topography (left bar, saturated red), and when pseudo-randomly misaligned in topography (“shuffled,” averaged over 1000 iterations; right bar, light red; t test: p < 0.0001). H, Analogous levels of overlap in maps of withdrawal > approach activity, when aligned (left bar, saturated blue) and when misaligned (right bar, light blue; t test: p < 0.0001). Brackets represent 1 SEM.

Within the parietal ROI, the specific topography of the evoked patches varied between subjects (see Fig. 5), akin to the idiosyncratic (fingerprint-like) mapping of cortical columns in macaque visual cortex, which also varies widely between individuals (Tootell et al., 1988; Horton and Hocking, 1996). Presumably, the idiosyncratically arranged patches in each individual subject were effectively “averaged out” in the group averaged maps here (Fig. 3) and previously (Holt et al., 2014), because the patches do not align with each other across subjects.

In addition to patches that showed approach-biased activity, the single subject maps often included patches that were more strongly activated by the withdrawing stimuli (Figs. 4, 5). Like the approach-biased patches, the locations of the withdrawal-biased patches remained largely consistent across scan sessions within a given subject (Fig. 6). However, in the parietal ROI, the number of these withdrawal-biased patches was significantly lower than the number of the approach-biased patches, above all thresholds between 10−2 through 10−5 (all p < 0.01).

BOLD evidence for a columnar organization

These data raise the possibility that these approaching and withdrawing 3D face stimuli differentially activated a previously unknown set of functionally distinct cortical columns in posterior parietal cortex. If the evoked “patches” are indeed “columns,” and to the extent that BOLD activity faithfully reflects the 3D shape of neural activity, then these BOLD-based sites should be elongated along the radial axis (i.e., perpendicular to the cortical surface) within the gray matter. This radial consistency of a given functional property is a defining feature of cortical columns (Mountcastle, 1957; Hubel and Wiesel, 1962, 1968; Horton and Adams, 2005). To test for such a radial elongation, we first examined the single subject data qualitatively, then conducted a formal statistical analysis.

First, we observed that the flattened activity maps that were sampled preferentially from the upper cortical layers were strikingly similar to those sampled through middle and lower cortical layers (Fig. 7A–D), as in the well-established columns within visual area V2 (Nasr et al., 2016). In 3D, this observation suggests a radial elongation of the BOLD-evoked patches.

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

The patchy topography in the approach versus withdrawal maps remained consistent across variations in gray matter depth. Maps of approach versus withdrawal responses of the right and left hemispheres of subject 1 are shown across variations in cortical depth in flattened cortex, within the parietal ROI. A, Activity map sampled from vertices centered on the cortical surface (depth 0 mm), which included activity in cortical layers 1 and 2 (and perhaps the pial vasculature). The activity in panel B was (on average) centered midway through the gray matter (depth = 50% gray matter), including cortical layer 4. The activity map in panel C was centered at a depth of 80% of the gray matter, including cortical layers 5 and 6. Panel D was located at the gray/white matter boundary (depth 100%), including activity in layer 6. The activation amplitude in panel D was reduced relative to panels A–C, presumably because of partial volume inclusion of the white matter (which shows negligible BOLD variation). The significance scale on the right indicates -log10(p), yellow/red = approach > withdrawal; cyan/blue = withdrawal > approach. The boundary of the parietal ROI is indicated in each map by a white outline.

Next, we sampled the evoked activity differences from a viewpoint perpendicular to the cortical surface, as in classic views of “columns” across the cortical layers (see Materials and Methods). These results (see Fig. 8) further supported the presence of a radial elongation of the evoked BOLD activity, in response to both approaching and withdrawing stimuli.

Figure 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 8.

Activity evoked by the approaching (vs withdrawing) face stimuli is radially elongated. Flattened activity maps in the parietal ROI were sampled when centered at each of 20 equally spaced cortical depths, which were then radially aligned and reconstructed into a 3D map of the original cortex. Activity was then sampled along a line drawn across the flattened cortex (white line, A–D). The activity in panels A–D was averaged across cortical depths (i.e., 10 through 90%), and shown in views analogous to classical views of cortical columns (E, F). C, D, Same data as in A, B, except that values in the activity map were re-scaled to match the broader range of pseudo-color activity scaling in E, F. G, H and I, J, Additional examples of the BOLD activity across variations in cortical depth, sampled from a different subject. Other details are as in A, B and E, F, respectively. Generally, BOLD activity was elongated along the radial axis. Given the averaged gray matter thickness in this region (2.37 mm), and the voxel size (1.1 mm3, iso), the activity maps were nominally independent at the two depth extremes (10% and 90%). Activity at other depths may partially overlap, reflecting the relative depth levels and the local cortical thickness, plus the voxel size and the known spread of the BOLD signal. Panels A, C, and E are showing data from subject 5 (left hemisphere, thickness = 2.21 mm). Panels B, D, and F are showing data from subject 2 (right hemisphere, thickness = 2.46 mm). Panels I and G are showing data from subject 5 (right hemisphere, thickness = 2.28 mm). Panels H and J are showing data from subject 3 (right hemisphere, thickness = 2.41 mm). Yellow/red = approach > withdrawal; cyan/blue = withdrawal > approach.

Finally, we measured the correlation between stimulus-driven BOLD activity (approach vs withdrawal) in the surface-normal (i.e., radial) versus surface-parallel (i.e., within-laminar) axes (see Materials and Methods). As a further control, two surface-parallel maps were tested: one at shallow cortical depths, and one at deeper depths (Fig. 9). As shown in Figure 9 and Table 1, all seven subjects showed correlation coefficients measured along the radial axis (perpendicular to the cortical surface) that were approximately twice the magnitude of the coefficients measured along the orthogonal plane (i.e., parallel with the cortical surface), in planes centered in both the upper and lower cortical depths/layers (n = 7, t = 16.82, p < 0.001; within a column vs across columns in superficial layers: n = 7, t = 11.38, p < 0.001).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Correlations of functional activity along radial-parallel versus surface-parallel axes, in each subject

Figure 9.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 9.

Correlations of evoked approach-bias versus withdrawal-bias BOLD responses sampled along radial-parallel versus surface-parallel axes in the flattened cortical surfaces within the parietal ROI. Evoked activity was sampled from all vertices within the gray matter, from all (n = 14) hemispheres. The sampling planes are schematized in panels A and C. B, Correlation of activity when sampled along an axis perpendicular (radial) to the cortical surface. D, E, Correlation values when sampled across a comparable distance in the perpendicular cortical plane, i.e., parallel with the cortical surfaces and layers, deep (depth = 0.9) in panel D, and superficial (depth = 0.1) in panel E. Functional response variations were more strongly correlated with each other when sampled along the radial axis, compared with the surface-parallel axis.

In fact, the differences in within-column versus across-column correlation strengths suggested an even stronger radial bias here in parietal cortex, compared with that shown previously in the well-established columns in visual area V2 (cf. Nasr et al., 2016, V2 in Fig. 7, compared with Fig. 9 and Table 1 here). Specifically, the average z scores in the current parietal cortex data measured in radial versus surface-parallel axes were 0.84 versus 0.26, respectively, whereas comparable z scores in the prior V2 data were 0.81 versus 0.51.

It is widely accepted that hemodynamic changes arising from sites located immediately “above” the gray matter (including the larger diameter vessels in the pia) contribute significantly to BOLD signal changes in scans using conventional gradient echo sequences at 3T (Yacoub et al., 2003, 2005; Duong et al., 2003). Although the techniques used here (e.g., 7T and small voxels) reduce such concerns (Merboldt et al., 2000; Triantafyllou et al., 2005; Polimeni et al., 2010; De Zwart et al., 2002), we nevertheless evaluated the extent to which the radially elongated BOLD responses might reflect hemodynamic effects arising outside the gray matter (e.g., in pia and neighboring spaces).

We measured BOLD activity in cortex across a range of cortical depths, both (1) within the gray matter (at conventionally-defined depths, “below” the cortical surface), and (2) at equal distances from the cortical surfaces in flattened planes, sampled “above” the cortical surface, as described elsewhere (J.E. Chen et al., 2021). Resultant surface-parallel BOLD variation was compared at “below,” and “above” the flattened cortical surface (respectively, in steps of 0, −1.2, −1.9, −2.4 mm depths below, and +1.2, +1.9, and +2.4 mm above) the cortical surface. Consistent with the maps in Figure 7, Table 2 showed significant topographical correlations between all four maps that were acquired within the gray matter (all Pearson r > 0.73, p < 0.001). However, we found significantly weaker correlations between any combination of the maps that were sampled below-the-surface versus above-the-surface (all t > 16.53, p < 0.0001). These results suggest that the topographical organization of the BOLD maps described in Figures 4–9 were not unduly dominated by BOLD signal from pial vessels. Given these data, and for the sake of brevity, below we refer to both the approach-selective and withdrawal-selective “patches” as “columns,” specifically termed P columns (i.e., presumptively related to PS).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Correlation of functional activity below the cortical surface versus above the cortical surface

Experiment 2: responses to stationary 3D face stimuli across a range of interpersonal distances

In the conventional SDP measurements [in both the classic version (Williams, 1971; Hayduk, 1983) and as adapted here for use in the scanner (in experiment 1)], the stimuli move continuously, either approaching or withdrawing. Prior studies in humans (Cléry et al., 2015; Candini et al., 2021) and nonhuman primates (Cléry et al., 2017) raise the possibility that these dynamic PS measurements reflect contributions from visual motion cues in addition to PS requirements per se, e.g., based on anticipation of the oncoming trajectory of the moving stimuli. On the other hand, it is also known that the discomfort evoked by PS intrusion at a given distance is largely time-insensitive, i.e., relatively constant over a wide range of intervals (from seconds to months) and over extensive repetitions (Hayduk, 1981a; Wormith, 1984; Tootell et al., 2021; Welsch et al., 2021; see also Fig. 2).

Thus, in experiment 1, it is unclear whether stimulus motion confounds conventional SDP measurements of PS. To address this question, experiment 2 measured fMRI responses to stationary face stimuli. The stationary faces (Fig. 1B) were presented at several virtual distances from the observer, spanning both near and far distances relative to each subject's PS boundary (9 distances total, including each subject's PS boundary), in pseudo-randomized order using an event-related design (see Materials and Methods). BOLD responses to the stationary stimuli were measured independently in the approach-biased and withdrawal-biased columns.

The approach-biased columns showed a consistent pattern of BOLD responses to face variations across corresponding variations in virtual distance (Fig. 10A). The mean BOLD response to faces presented at the four largest distances (i.e., “further” than the PS boundary) showed a negligible (baseline) response (all p-values > 0.16), i.e., essentially nonresponsive to the virtually “far” face stimuli. In contrast, BOLD responses to faces presented closer than the PS boundary increased steeply and monotonically, such that the highest response was evoked by the closest face. The withdrawal-biased columns showed a less dramatic response function to the stationary faces, which was nevertheless largely inverted in sign relative to baseline, and negligible at distances further than the PS boundary (Fig. 10B).

Figure 10.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 10.

BOLD responses to variations in stationary interpersonal distance. In the scanner, subjects were presented with range of virtual face stimuli, of differing binocular disparity and size. The combination of size and disparity differences in the virtual faces (e.g., A) matched those present in real-world faces at comparable viewing distances. B, Average BOLD peak amplitude in response to each virtual distance. The response functions were measured independently in the approach-biased and withdrawal-biased columns. The vertical dashed white line indicates each subject's personal space boundary (100%); other distances were normalized, precalculated, and presented as percentages of that value, for each subject (see Materials and Methods). Error bars represent one standard deviation. C, One possible computational scheme to generate such parietal responses (bottom) from visual cortical inputs (top). Results from experiments 1 and 2 suggested that the input corresponding to physically closer faces was larger in size, hence stronger. In this scheme, the average firing rates of the excitatory and inhibitory populations in inferior parietal cortex were represented by rate equations (Wilson and Cowan, 1972), in which each population received weighted inputs from the other population and itself, through a sigmoid nonlinearity (Table 3). For simplicity, this model assumed that BOLD levels represent the excitatory postsynaptic response, although it is known that BOLD signals reflect a combination of both presynaptic and postsynaptic activity (Logothetis et al., 2001; Logothetis, 2003). The synaptic weights between and within the two populations were considered free parameters, which were tuned to approximate the response profiles of approach-biased and withdrawal-biased columns (Table 3). In the approach-biased columns, the model suggested that a strong recurrent excitation and a weak recurrent inhibition could account for the activity profile of the excitatory population in response to the upstream visual input (C, top, orange and purple, respectively). Because of the weak recurrent inhibition, the response of the excitatory population increased in response to the input. When the input was strong enough to activate the excitatory population beyond its intrinsic soft threshold, there was a rapid increase in the excitatory firing rate because of the strong recurrent excitation (C, bottom, red). In the withdrawal-biased columns, a weak recurrent excitation and a strong recurrent inhibition could account for the response of the excitatory population to the input. For weaker inputs, inhibition dominated over excitation; hence the excitatory firing decreased as a function of the input. However, as the input became even stronger, the activity of the inhibitory population saturated and excitation overcame the inhibition, which eventually caused the excitatory firing rate to plateau (C, bottom, cyan).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Parameters of the computational model

Figure 11.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 11.

Individual maps of near- and far-selective disparity columns (red-yellow and blue-cyan, respectively, scale bar on the right), in response to visual presentation of random dot stereograms in Experiment 3. Panels show flattened maps of cortical activity, in both hemispheres, in each of two individual subjects (1 and 3) in A and B, respectively. In both A and B, the medial-lateral border (where the cortex folds) is shown as a dashed green line, and the parietal ROI is indicated by a white outline. As expected, disparity columns were also evident within neighboring visual cortical area V3A, indicated here with black Xs. Ant, anterior; Post, posterior; Med, medial; Lat, lateral; L, left hemisphere; R, right hemisphere. The significance scale on the right indicates -log10(p).

Modeling of approach and withdrawal biased columns

It has been reported that intrusion into PS evokes responses in the amygdala (Kennedy et al., 2009; Todd and Anderson, 2009; Wabnegger et al., 2016; see also Nacewicz et al., 2006), consistent with long-standing evidence that the amygdala is involved in the processing of social threat and avoidance (Klüver and Bucy, 1937; Breiter et al., 1996; Emery et al., 2001; Zald, 2003; Mason et al., 2006; Adolphs, 2008; Adolphs, 2010). Thus, it is possible that input from the amygdala influences the parietal cortical responses to stimulus distances that are closer (but not further) than each subject's PS boundary, as in the function shown in Figure 10B.

Alternatively, we hypothesized that a differential weighting of purely visual inputs could instead account for these response differences in parietal cortex. To evaluate the computational feasibility of this idea, we tested a scheme in which both approach-selective and withdrawal-selective population responses resulted from ascending (“upstream”) input from visual cortex. This model matched the experimental results well (Fig. 10B,C). Thus, the differing response profiles of the approach-biased and withdrawal-biased columns observed here could reflect correspondingly differing strengths of the local excitatory and inhibitory synaptic signals from visual cortex to each column type in parietal cortex, without requiring input from the amygdala. Further testing is necessary to validate this model.

Experiment 3: near-selective and far-selective columns based on binocular disparity

As in real life conditions, the binocular disparities across the face stimuli of experiments 1 and 2 varied with distance from the viewer. Thus, it might be asked whether the approach-biased and withdrawal-biased columns involve disparity-selective neurons and columns similar to those described previously in occipital visual cortex in macaques (Felleman and Van Essen, 1987; Poggio et al., 1988; Roy and Narahashi, 1992; Deangelis et al., 1998; Deangelis and Newsome, 1999; Adams and Zeki, 2001; Tanabe et al., 2005; G. Chen et al., 2008) and in humans (Goncalves et al., 2015; Nasr et al., 2016; Tootell and Nasr, 2017). However, this hypothesis presumes that “near” and “far” disparity-selective columns extend beyond occipital cortex, anteriorly into parietal cortex, and no such disparity columns have been reported previously beyond occipital (visual) cortex.

Accordingly, we next tested for the presence of disparity columns in the posterior parietal ROI, using conventional random dot stereogram stimuli that selectively activated near-distance and far-distance disparity-selective columns in previous fMRI studies of occipital cortex (Tsao et al., 2003; Nasr et al., 2016; Nasr and Tootell, 2018a, 2020). When disparities in the two monocular images were binocularly fused, this random dot arrays produced a stereoscopic percept of multiple 3D cuboids, moving continuously through near or far visual depths (see Fig. 1C; and Materials and Methods). All subjects confirmed this stereoscopic percept during the scan session. The corresponding near-disparity and far-disparity patches extended bilaterally into posterior parietal cortex (Figs. 11, 12), but few disparity patches were found further anterior in parietal cortex. This is consistent with our overall hypothesis that posterior parietal cortex functions as a “transition” area, including both sensory and egocentric spatial encoding.

Figure 12.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 12.

Maps of binocularly fused disparity-selective activity in each subject, in both hemispheres, evoked by binocularly viewed random dot arrays. Higher activity to visually near and far distances is shown in red-yellow and blue-cyan, respectively. All maps were averaged across both scan sessions, in each individual subject, in both right and left hemispheres (panels A–G), in the parietal cortex region of interest (see white boundary in each map). Panel H shows the average activity across all subjects. The significance scale indicates –log10(p).

Comparisons across maps sampled at different cortical depths suggested that these disparity-selective BOLD sites in parietal cortex are radially elongated (i.e., columnar), consistent with the organization of disparity-selective columns identified in macaque monkeys and in high field fMRI studies in human occipital cortex. Accordingly, we refer to these disparity-sensitive patches in parietal cortex as D (disparity) columns.

In the parietal ROI, the near-selective disparity columns were more numerous than the far-selective disparity columns, across the testable range of thresholds (above all thresholds between 10−2 through 10−5; t test: all p-values < 0.01). This bias for near- (relative to far-) selective columns in parietal cortex may be related to a near-disparity versus far-disparity bias reported in neighboring occipital cortex (Nasr and Tootell, 2020). Overall, we conclude that posterior parietal cortex includes visually driven, stereo-selective columns, similar to those previously described in occipital cortex.

Topographical relationship between column types

Next, we investigated how the P and D column types are distributed relative to each other. Initial observation of the maps (compare Figs. 4, 5 and 11, 12) suggested that the two categories of columns (P and D), and/or the two functional poles in each of these categories (approaching vs withdrawing, and near vs far), might have a nonoverlapping relationship, analogous to the interdigitation of thin versus thick type columns demonstrated in human visual cortical areas V2 and V3 (Nasr et al., 2016; Dumoulin et al., 2017; Tootell and Nasr, 2017). However, alternative topographic relationships are possible, including: (1) a random organization of the two columnar categories and poles, or (2) an overlap of the two columnar categories and/or poles (i.e., only one set of columns, which respond to all of our test stimuli, to varying extents).

By definition, two of the possible stimulus contrasts (near vs far disparity, and approaching vs withdrawing) are nonoverlapping relative to each other, based on the nature of the fMRI subtraction per se. Thus, we examined the relationship between maps evoked by the four remaining functional contrasts (near disparity vs approach, far disparity vs withdrawal, near disparity vs withdrawal, and far disparity vs withdrawal). BOLD response levels were measured in each vertex within each hemisphere, in response to these four functional contrasts, when the maps were: (1) accurately aligned (as in vivo), compared with (2) computationally “shuffled” (pseudo-randomly misaligned).

The results confirmed that BOLD activity peaks in all four paired stimulus contrasts were preferentially nonoverlapping (i.e., interdigitated; Fig. 13). Based on these results, one possible cortical processing “unit” is illustrated in Figure 14.

Figure 13.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 13.

Topographic relationship of P and D columns. A–D, Maps of the cortical overlap (and nonoverlap) in the flattened activity maps of approach-biased and withdrawal-biased activity, plus the activity in response to near-biased and far-biased binocular disparity, based on a colorized overlay of individual activity maps. Panels A and B, and panels C and D, show the activity of the left and right hemispheres of subjects 7 and 2, respectively, within the parietal ROI (indicated by white outlines). In all four maps, vertices that showed significantly biased activity to any single one (but not two or more) of the four functional contrasts are color-coded as shown below panels C and D. Vertices that showed overlap (i.e., to two or more) of any of these functional contrasts are shown in black. The significance threshold for all contrasts was p < 0.05, uncorrected, to allow as much overlap as statistically possible. These examples suggest that there is relatively little overlap (black), implying a systematic segregation of function. In analyses across subjects, panels E–H show the extent of overlap in the maps, when averaged across all hemispheres (n = 14), within the parietal ROI, for all four pairwise contrasts of interest, across a range of activity thresholds (x-axis). The magenta line shows the extent of overlap when the maps were correctly aligned (as in vivo), across different thresholds (x-axis). As a control condition, the black line shows the level of overlap when the maps were aligned pseudo-randomly relative to each other, averaged across 1000 randomizations. Overlap is defined as the percentage of overlapped vertices (i.e., the number of vertices showing selectivity for a given functional contrast, divided by the number of vertices within the parietal ROI). Consistent with the examples in panel A–D, these group-averaged results confirm a strong tendency toward interdigitation (e.g., segregated functional sensitivity) between all four of the functional contrasts tested here.

Figure 14.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 14.

Hypothetical column-scale distance processing unit in parietal cortex. The cortical surface is represented at the top of the schematic and there are four types of radially-extending columns. ND = near disparity; FD = far disparity; NP = near personal; FP = far personal.

Discussion

Overall, our evidence suggests that parietal cortical responses to variations in stimulus distance from the body are organized into at least two types of functional columns. Thus, one fundamental organizational feature of early visual cortex (i.e., functional segregation within multiple types of columns) apparently also exists in human parietal cortex.

Encoding of interpersonal distance

More specifically, our results suggest that specific columns in the parietal cortex respond preferentially to variations in visually mediated virtual distance from an observer, along two stimulus dimensions: (1) purely visual (i.e., binocular disparity), and (2) partly nonvisual, presumptively related to interpersonal distance. In both dimensions, the activity maps differed significantly in response to presentations of visual stimuli at near versus far distance extremes.

However, the apparent congruence of “near” and “far” terms may be oversimplified, to the extent that the two underlying dimensions differ from each other. For instance, psychophysical sensitivity to stereoscopically-fused binocular visual stimuli ranges from a “near” limit roughly 10–20 cm from the eyes (depending on measurement technique; for review, see Wilcox and Allison, 2009) through a “far” limit at optical infinity. By comparison, the visual sensitivity to variations in personal space varies from a near limit close to the observer's skin, through a far limit averaging near 80 cm (for review, see Hayduk, 1983). The midpoint (zero-crossing) distance values between the near and far extremes of the two stimulus dimensions also differ from each other. In the dimension of personal space, those zero-crossing values (between near and far) are unrelated to where the subject is looking. However, based on visual binocular disparity, the zero-crossing distance depends on where the subject's eyes are converged, in each glance.

To simplify the experimental comparison, we studied both personal space cues and disparity cues by varying them along a common line of sight. However, sensitivity to personal space is not limited to visual stimuli. For instance, prior studies (Hayduk, 1981b; Wormith, 1984; Bogovic et al., 2016; Hecht et al., 2019) suggest that personal space surrounds the body, generating responses to intruders both visible and invisible. Auditory stimuli can also evoke discomfort when the source is perceived as near or within personal space (Lloyd et al., 2009; Noel et al., 2018). It has even been reported that proximity-driven discomfort can be evoked by simple statements that someone is within a subject's personal space (Wormith, 1984; Kennedy et al., 2009), without any manipulation of sensory cues. Consistent with this behavioral evidence, neurobiological studies have shown that sites in parietal cortex can be driven by proximity-related cues encoded by multiple senses, in addition to vision (Andersen, 1997; Holmes and Spence, 2004; Yang et al., 2011; Blanke, 2012; Ehrsson, 2012; Murray and Wallace, 2012; Sereno and Huang, 2014; Grivaz et al., 2017).

Similar to the overall behavioral response to personal space variation, the approach-biased columns in parietal cortex showed stronger BOLD responses to progressively nearer faces, even when such faces were stationary. Thus, based on their responses to both moving and stationary face stimuli, this category of columns could equally well be termed either (1) “approach-biased or withdrawal-biased,” or (2) “interpersonally-near or far-biased,” respectively. Our preferential use of the former term was largely arbitrary.

Potential relationship to vasculature-related responses

It might be argued that our BOLD-based maps reflect variations in oxygenation in the radially arranged “diving” veins and arteries, rather than a columnar organization of neural responses per se. However, prior studies that directly compared the location of the radial vessels with cortical functional maps found very little (Keller et al., 2011) or no (Adams et al., 2015) topographical relationship between the two radial features (but see Zheng et al., 1991; Shmuel et al., 2010).

In addition, this interpretation is unlikely because the radial vessels (both arteries and veins) vary in diameter (between 20 and 150 μm), partly as a function of maximum cortical depth (Duvernoy et al., 1981), whereas the topographic diameter of BOLD-defined columns described here and elsewhere is much larger than 150 μm. Thus, each BOLD-defined column likely contained multiple radial vessels (i.e., not just one radial vessel in the middle of each column, as was initially speculated). However, partly because the range of capillary diameters partially overlaps that of radial vessels (Lauwers et al., 2008), radial veins could not be unambiguously distinguished from capillaries using current techniques. Another complication is that the reperfusion territory of diving veins is conical in shape, rather than strictly radial (Duvernoy et al., 1981).

More conceptually, it seems unlikely that different sets of BOLD-based columns (e.g., a functional interdigitation, as found here and previously; Nasr et al., 2016; Dumoulin et al., 2017; Tootell and Nasr, 2017) could arise from any single common vascular substrate. Ultimately, we conclude that our BOLD-based “columns” likely include (but are not dominated by) radially aligned vessels.

Relationship to prior findings in macaques and humans

Our column-scale fMRI findings in human parietal cortex are consistent with findings of some single neuron recordings in macaque parietal cortex. For instance, a bias for approaching stimuli has been reported in electrophysiological responses in macaque parietal cortex (Colby et al., 1993; Graziano and Cooke, 2006), and in areas that provide input to parietal cortex (Albright, 1989). Additional studies reported a preference for “near” visual stimuli in monkey parietal cortex (Colby et al., 1993; Duhamel et al., 1998; Hadjidimitrakis et al., 2011; Bremmer et al., 2013; Cléry et al., 2017).

Here, a discrete site within parietal cortex (dorsal parietal-occipital sulcus, dPOS) showed particularly strong BOLD responses to approaching and near faces. Consistent with this, Quinlan and Culham (2007) identified dPOS as an area that responded strongly to physically near (vs far) visual objects in humans. However, those authors ultimately concluded that dPOS responds selectively to inward vergence, rather than to near visual objects per se. However, that interpretation cannot fully explain the current results, in which the attention task required subjects to maintain a single vergence angle on the stimulus screen throughout the functional scans. Further research is necessary to fully understand the functions of this area.

Columns in parietal cortex

More than 25 sets of functional columns have been reported in multiple areas in primate visual cortex. However, previously there has been little evidence for the existence of functional columns beyond visual cortex (but see Bruce et al., 1985). Insofar as the current P columns encode interpersonal social influences, the function of these parietal columns may be considered associative, rather than purely sensory. It is possible that earlier BOLD experiments evoked activity in higher order cortex that was radial in 3D shape, but not identified as columnar at that time.

Advanced fMRI techniques have revealed a coarse retinotopic organization in human parietal cortex (Swisher et al., 2007; Arcaro et al., 2011; Huang et al., 2017). Here, we could not identify any consistent topographical relationship between the current results and the retinotopy.

It is known that parts of parietal cortex (and cortical areas F4/5) can contribute to responses to approaching stimuli which are potentially physically threatening. The current data highlight that this role in threat processing may extend to stimuli that are socially threatening, or at least those that elicit arousal and/or attention. Additional brain areas that might be engaged by such stimuli include: (1) visual cortical sites involved in recognition of objects, and (2) parietal and premotor cortical areas involved in preparation of defensive actions.

Clinical and public health implications

Abnormalities in personal space regulation have been observed in several neuropsychiatric disorders, including schizophrenia (Horowitz et al., 1964; Duke and Mullens, 1973; Srivastava and Mandal, 1990; Nechamkin et al., 2003; Park et al., 2009; De La Asuncion et al., 2015; Holt et al., 2015; Zapetis et al., 2022) and autism (Gessaroli et al., 2013; Kennedy and Adolphs, 2014; Lough et al., 2015; Perry et al., 2015; Asada et al., 2016; Candini et al., 2017; Noel et al., 2017; Mul et al., 2019). Given our findings, it is possible that early changes in the organization of the parietal cortex (perhaps including column-scale effects) are associated with the abnormalities in personal space regulation observed in these neurodevelopmental conditions. Future studies can test this possibility.

Although personal space preferences tend to be highly stable over time within individuals, some plasticity of personal space has also been observed. For example, tool use may lead to temporary expansion of personal space boundaries (Iriki et al., 1996; Berti and Frassinetti, 2000; Maravita and Iriki, 2004; Cardinali et al., 2009; Iachini et al., 2014; Biggio et al., 2017; Quesque et al., 2017; but see Patané et al., 2016). Also, recent studies raise the possibility that preferred interpersonal distances changed during the COVID-19 pandemic, perhaps because of social distancing mandates and/or fear of infection (Iachini et al., 2021; Serino et al., 2021; Welsch et al., 2021; Holt et al., 2022). It is not yet known whether such environmental factors affect personal space processing at the spatial scale of cortical columns.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health Grant RO1MH109562 (to D.J.H.). We thank Dr. Jonathan Polimeni for help with the MR sequences.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Roger B.H. Tootell at tootell{at}mgh.harvard.edu

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Adams DL,
    2. Zeki S
    (2001) Functional organization of macaque V3 for stereoscopic depth. J Neurophysiol 86:2195–2203. pmid:11698511
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Adams DL,
    2. Piserchia V,
    3. Economides JR,
    4. Horton JC
    (2015) Vascular supply of the cerebral cortex is specialized for cell layers but not columns. Cereb Cortex 25:3673–3681. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu221
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Adolphs R
    (2008) Fear, faces, and the human amygdala. Curr Opin Neurobiol 18:166–172. pmid:18655833
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Adolphs R
    (2010) What does the amygdala contribute to social cognition? Ann N Y Acad Sci 1191:42–61. pmid:20392275
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Albright T
    (1989) Centrifugal directional bias in the middle temporal visual area (MT) of the macaque. Vis Neurosci 2:177–188. pmid:2487646
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Amunts K,
    2. Mohlberg H,
    3. Bludau S,
    4. Zilles K
    (2020) Julich-brain: a 3D probabilistic atlas of the human brain's cytoarchitecture. Science 369:988–992. doi:10.1126/science.abb4588 pmid:32732281
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    1. Andersen RA
    (1997) Multimodal integration for the representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 352:1421–1428. doi:10.1098/rstb.1997.0128
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Arcaro MJ,
    2. Pinsk MA,
    3. Li X,
    4. Kastner S
    (2011) Visuotopic organization of macaque posterior parietal cortex: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 31:2064–2078. pmid:21307244
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Ardrey R
    (1966) The territorial imperative. New York: Atheneum.
  10. ↵
    1. Asada K,
    2. Tojo Y,
    3. Osanai H,
    4. Saito A,
    5. Hasegawa T,
    6. Kumagaya S
    (2016) Reduced personal space in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. PLoS One 11:e0146306.
    OpenUrl
  11. ↵
    1. Ashburner J,
    2. Friston KJ
    (2005) Unified segmentation. Neuroimage 26:839–851. pmid:15955494
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Bailenson JN,
    2. Blascovich J,
    3. Beall AC,
    4. Loomis JM
    (2003) Interpersonal distance in immersive virtual environments. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 29:819–833. pmid:15018671
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Bar-Haim Y,
    2. Aviezer O,
    3. Berson Y,
    4. Sagi A
    (2002) Attachment in infancy and personal space regulation in early adolescence. Attach Hum Dev 4:68–83. pmid:12065031
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Bastiani M,
    2. Cottaar M,
    3. Fitzgibbon SP,
    4. Suri S,
    5. Alfaro-Almagro F,
    6. Sotiropoulos SN,
    7. Jbabdi S,
    8. Andersson JL
    (2019) Automated quality control for within and between studies diffusion MRI data using a non-parametric framework for movement and distortion correction. Neuroimage 184:801–812. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.073 pmid:30267859
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Berti L,
    2. Frassinetti F
    (2000) When far becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use. J Cogn Neurosci 12:415–420.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Biggio M,
    2. Bisio A,
    3. Avanzino L,
    4. Ruggeri P,
    5. Bove M
    (2017) This racket is not mine: The influence of the tool-use on peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia 103:54–58.
    OpenUrl
  17. ↵
    1. Blanke O
    (2012) Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nat Rev Neurosci 13:556–571. pmid:22805909
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Bogdanova OV,
    2. Bogdanov VB,
    3. Dureux A,
    4. Farnè A,
    5. Hadj-Bouziane F
    (2021) The Peripersonal Space in a social world. Cortex 142:28–46. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2021.05.005
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  19. ↵
    1. Bogovic A,
    2. Ivezic E,
    3. Filipcic I
    (2016) Personal space of war veterans with PTSD-SOME characteristics and comparison with healthy individuals. Psychiatr Danub 28:77–81.
    OpenUrl
  20. ↵
    1. Brainard DH
    (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis 10:433–436. pmid:9176952
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Breiter HC,
    2. Etcoff NL,
    3. Whalen PJ,
    4. Kennedy WA,
    5. Rauch SL,
    6. Buckner RL,
    7. Strauss MM,
    8. Hyman SE,
    9. Rosen BR
    (1996) Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron 17:875–887. pmid:8938120
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Bremmer F,
    2. Schlack A,
    3. Kaminiarz A,
    4. Hoffmann KP
    (2013) Encoding of movement in near extrapersonal space in primate area VIP. Front Behav Neurosci 7:8. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Bruce CJ,
    2. Goldberg ME,
    3. Bushnell MC,
    4. Stanton G
    (1985) Primate frontal eye fields. II. Physiological and anatomical correlates of electrically evoked eye movements. J Neurophysiol 54:714–734. pmid:4045546
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Candini M,
    2. Giuberti V,
    3. Manattini A,
    4. Grittani S,
    5. Di Pellegrino G,
    6. Frassinetti F
    (2017) Personal space regulation in childhood autism: Effects of social interaction and person's perspective. Autism Research 10:144–154.
    OpenUrl
  25. ↵
    1. Candini M,
    2. Battaglia S,
    3. Benassi M,
    4. Di Pellegrino G,
    5. Frassinetti F
    (2021) The physiological correlates of interpersonal space. Sci Rep 11:1–8. doi:10.1038/s41598-021-82223-2
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Cardinali L,
    2. Frassinetti F,
    3. Brozzoli C,
    4. Urquizar C,
    5. Roy AC,
    6. Farnè A
    (2009) Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema. Curr Biol 19:R478–R479.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Cartaud A,
    2. Quesque F,
    3. Coello Y
    (2020) Wearing a face mask against Covid-19 results in a reduction of social distancing. PLoS One 15:e0243023. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0243023
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  28. ↵
    1. Chen G,
    2. Lu HD,
    3. Roe AW
    (2008) A map for horizontal disparity in monkey V2. Neuron 58:442–450. pmid:18466753
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Chen JE,
    2. Glover GH,
    3. Fultz NE,
    4. Rosen BR,
    5. Polimeni JR,
    6. Lewis LD
    (2021) Investigating mechanisms of fast BOLD responses: the effects of stimulus intensity and of spatial heterogeneity of hemodynamics. Neuroimage 245:118658. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118658 pmid:34656783
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Cléry J,
    2. Hamed SB
    (2018) Frontier of self and impact prediction. Front Psychol 9:1073. pmid:29997556
    OpenUrlPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Cléry J,
    2. Guipponi O,
    3. Odouard S,
    4. Wardak C,
    5. Hamed SB
    (2015) Impact prediction by looming visual stimuli enhances tactile detection. J Neurosci 35:4179–4189. pmid:25762665
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Cléry J,
    2. Guipponi O,
    3. Odouard S,
    4. Pinède S,
    5. Wardak C,
    6. Hamed SB
    (2017) The prediction of impact of a looming stimulus onto the body is subserved by multisensory integration mechanisms. J Neurosci 37:10656–10670. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0610-17.2017
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    1. Colby CL,
    2. Duhamel JR,
    3. Goldberg ME
    (1993) Ventral intraparietal area of the macaque: anatomic location and visual response properties. J Neurophysiol 69:902–914. pmid:8385201
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Cooke DF,
    2. Taylor CS,
    3. Moore T,
    4. Graziano MS
    (2003) Complex movements evoked by microstimulation of the ventral intraparietal area. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100:6163–6168. doi:10.1073/pnas.1031751100
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    1. Dale AM,
    2. Sereno MI
    (1993) Improved localizadon of cortical activity by combining EEG and MEG with MRI cortical surface reconstruction: a linear approach. J Cogn Neurosci 5:162–176. pmid:23972151
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Dale AM,
    2. Fischl B,
    3. Sereno MI
    (1999) Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9:179–194. pmid:9931268
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Deangelis GC,
    2. Newsome WT
    (1999) Organization of disparity-selective neurons in macaque area MT. J Neurosci 19:1398–1415. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-04-01398.1999
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Deangelis GC,
    2. Cumming BG,
    3. Newsome WT
    (1998) Cortical area MT and the perception of stereoscopic depth. Nature 394:677–680. pmid:9716130
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. De La Asuncion J,
    2. Docx L,
    3. Sabbe B,
    4. Morrens M,
    5. De Bruijn ER
    (2015) Converging evidence of social avoidant behavior in schizophrenia from two approach-avoidance tasks. J Psychiatr Res 69:135–141. pmid:26343605
    OpenUrlPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. De Zwart JA,
    2. Van Gelderen P,
    3. Kellman P,
    4. Duyn JH
    (2002) Application of sensitivity-encoded echo-planar imaging for blood oxygen level-dependent functional brain imaging. Magn Reson Med 48:1011–1020. doi:10.1002/mrm.10303
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Duhamel JR,
    2. Colby CL,
    3. Goldberg ME
    (1998) Ventral intraparietal area of the macaque: congruent visual and somatic response properties. J Neurophysiol 79:126–136. pmid:9425183
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Duke MP,
    2. Mullens C
    (1973) Preferred interpersonal distance as a function of locus of control orientation in chronic schizophrenics, nonschizophrenic patients, and normals. J Consult Clin Psychol 41:230–234. doi:10.1037/h0035141
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Dumoulin SO,
    2. Harvey BM,
    3. Fracasso A,
    4. Zuiderbaan W,
    5. Luijten PR,
    6. Wandell BA,
    7. Petridou N
    (2017) In vivo evidence of functional and anatomical stripe-based subdivisions in human V2 and V3. Sci Rep 7:1–12. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-00634-6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Duong TQ,
    2. Yacoub E,
    3. Adriany G,
    4. Hu X,
    5. Uǧurbil K,
    6. Kim SG
    (2003) Microvascular BOLD contribution at 4 and 7 T in the human brain: gradient-echo and spin-echo fMRI with suppression of blood effects. Magn Reson Med 49:1019–1027. doi:10.1002/mrm.10472
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Duvernoy HM,
    2. Delon S,
    3. Vannson J
    (1981) Cortical blood vessels of the human brain. Brain Res Bull 7:519–579. pmid:7317796
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Ehrsson HH
    (2012) The concept of body ownership and its relation to multisensory integration. In: The new handbook of multisensory process. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
  47. ↵
    1. Emery NJ,
    2. Capitanio JP,
    3. Mason WA,
    4. Machado CJ,
    5. Mendoza SP,
    6. Amaral DG
    (2001) The effects of bilateral lesions of the amygdala on dyadic social interactions in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behav Neurosci 115:515–544. pmid:11439444
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Felleman DJ,
    2. Van Essen DC
    (1987) Receptive field properties of neurons in area V3 of macaque monkey extrastriate cortex. J Neurophysiol 57:889–920. pmid:3585463
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Fischl B,
    2. Dale AM
    (2000) Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:11050–11055. doi:10.1073/pnas.200033797
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  50. ↵
    1. Fischl B,
    2. Sereno MI,
    3. Tootell RB,
    4. Dale AM
    (1999) High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum Brain Mapp 8:272–284. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4<272::AID-HBM10>3.0.CO;2-4
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Fischl B,
    2. Liu A,
    3. Dale AM
    (2001) Automated manifold surgery: constructing geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral cortex. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 20:70–80. pmid:11293693
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Fischl B,
    2. Salat DH,
    3. Busa E,
    4. Albert M,
    5. Dieterich M,
    6. Haselgrove C,
    7. van der Kouwe A,
    8. Killiany R,
    9. Kennedy D,
    10. Klaveness S,
    11. Montillo A,
    12. Makris N,
    13. Rosen B,
    14. Dale AM
    (2002) Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33:341–355. pmid:11832223
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Fischl B,
    2. Salat DH,
    3. Van Der Kouwe AJ,
    4. Makris N,
    5. Ségonne F,
    6. Quinn BT,
    7. Dale AM
    (2004a) Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images. Neuroimage 23:S69–S84. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.016
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Fischl B,
    2. van der Kouwe A,
    3. Destrieux C,
    4. Halgren E,
    5. Ségonne F,
    6. Salat DH,
    7. Busa E,
    8. Seidman LJ,
    9. Goldstein J,
    10. Kennedy D,
    11. Caviness V,
    12. Makris N,
    13. Rosen B,
    14. Dale AM
    (2004b) Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 14:11–22. pmid:14654453
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Gessaroli E,
    2. Santelli E,
    3. Di Pellegrino G,
    4. Frassinetti F
    (2013) Personal space regulation in childhood autism spectrum disorders. PloS one 8:e74959.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Goncalves NR,
    2. Ban H,
    3. Sánchez-Panchuelo RM,
    4. Francis ST,
    5. Schluppeck D,
    6. Welchman AE
    (2015) 7 tesla FMRI reveals systematic functional organization for binocular disparity in dorsal visual cortex. J Neurosci 35:3056–3072. pmid:25698743
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    1. Graziano MS,
    2. Cooke DF
    (2006) Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior. Neuropsychologia 44:845–859. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.009 pmid:16277998
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Graziano MS,
    2. Taylor CS,
    3. Moore T
    (2002) Complex movements evoked by microstimulation of precentral cortex. Neuron 34:841–851. pmid:12062029
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Greve DN,
    2. Fischl B
    (2009) Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. Neuroimage 48:63–72. pmid:19573611
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Grivaz P,
    2. Blanke O,
    3. Serino A
    (2017) Common and distinct brain regions processing multisensory bodily signals for peripersonal space and body ownership. Neuroimage 147:602–618. pmid:28017920
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Hadjidimitrakis K,
    2. Breveglieri R,
    3. Placenti G,
    4. Bosco A,
    5. Sabatini SP,
    6. Fattori P
    (2011) Fix your eyes in the space you could reach: neurons in the macaque medial parietal cortex prefer gaze positions in peripersonal space. PLoS One 6:e23335. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023335 pmid:21858075
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Hadjidimitrakis K,
    2. Bertozzi F,
    3. Breveglieri R,
    4. Fattori P,
    5. Galletti C
    (2014) Body-centered, mixed, but not hand-centered coding of visual targets in the medial posterior parietal cortex during reaches in 3D space. Cereb Cortex 24:3209–3220. doi:10.1093/cercor/bht181 pmid:23853212
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Hayduk LA
    (1978) Personal space: an evaluative and orienting overview. Psychol Bull 85:117–134. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.85.1.117
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  64. ↵
    1. Hayduk LA
    (1981a) The permeability of personal space. Can J Behav Sci 13:274–287. doi:10.1037/h0081182
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  65. ↵
    1. Hayduk LA
    (1981b) The shape of personal space: an experimental investigation. Can J Behav Sci 13:87–93. doi:10.1037/h0081114
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  66. ↵
    1. Hayduk LA
    (1983) Personal space: where we now stand. Psychol Bull 94:293–335. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.94.2.293
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  67. ↵
    1. Hecht H,
    2. Welsch R,
    3. Viehoff J,
    4. Longo MR
    (2019) The shape of personal space. Acta Psychol (Amst) 193:113–122. pmid:30622020
    OpenUrlPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Hediger H
    (1955) Studies of the psychology and behavior of captive animals in zoos and circuses. London: Butterworth and Co.
  69. ↵
    1. Holmes NP,
    2. Spence C
    (2004) The body schema and multisensory representation (s) of peripersonal space. Cogn Process 5:94–105. pmid:16467906
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Holt DJ,
    2. Cassidy BS,
    3. Yue X,
    4. Rauch SL,
    5. Boeke EA,
    6. Nasr S,
    7. Tootell RB,
    8. Coombs G
    (2014) Neural correlates of personal space intrusion. J Neurosci 34:4123–4134. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0686-13.2014
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  71. ↵
    1. Holt DJ,
    2. Boeke EA,
    3. Coombs Iii G,
    4. Decross SN,
    5. Cassidy BS,
    6. Stufflebeam S,
    7. Rauch SL,
    8. Tootell RB
    (2015) Abnormalities in personal space and parietal–frontal function in schizophrenia. Neuroimage Clin 9:233–243. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2015.07.008
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  72. ↵
    1. Holt DJ,
    2. Zapetis SL,
    3. Babadi B,
    4. Zimmerman J,
    5. Tootell RBH
    (2022) Personal space increases during the COVID-19 pandemic in response to real and virtual humans. Front Psychol 13:2021.06.09.21258234. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2022.952998
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  73. ↵
    1. Horowitz MJ,
    2. Duff DF,
    3. Stratton LO
    (1964) Body-buffer zone: exploration of personal space. Arch Gen Psychiatry 11:651–656. pmid:14209746
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Horton JC,
    2. Hocking DR
    (1996) Intrinsic variability of ocular dominance column periodicity in normal macaque monkeys. J Neurosci 16:7228–7339. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-22-07228.1996
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  75. ↵
    1. Horton JC,
    2. Adams DL
    (2005) The cortical column: a structure without a function. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 360:837–862. doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1623
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Huang RS,
    2. Chen CF,
    3. Sereno MI
    (2017) Mapping the complex topological organization of the human parietal face area. Neuroimage 163:459–470. pmid:28889002
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Hubel DH,
    2. Wiesel TN
    (1962) Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. J Physiol 160:106–154. pmid:14449617
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. Hubel DH,
    2. Wiesel TN
    (1968) Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. J Physiol 195:215–243. pmid:4966457
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Iachini T,
    2. Coello Y,
    3. Frassinetti F,
    4. Ruggiero G
    (2014) Body space in social interactions: a comparison of reaching and comfort distance in immersive virtual reality. PLoS One 9:e111511. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0111511 pmid:25405344
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Iachini T,
    2. Coello Y,
    3. Frassinetti F,
    4. Senese VP,
    5. Galante F,
    6. Ruggiero G
    (2016) Peripersonal and interpersonal space in virtual and real environments: effects of gender and age. J of Environmental Psychology 45:154–164. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.01.004
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  81. ↵
    1. Iachini T,
    2. Frassinetti F,
    3. Ruotolo F,
    4. Sbordone FL,
    5. Ferrara A,
    6. Arioli M,
    7. Pazzaglia F,
    8. Bosco A,
    9. Candini M,
    10. Lopez A,
    11. Caffò AO,
    12. Cattaneo Z,
    13. Fornara F,
    14. Ruggiero G
    (2021) Social distance during the COVID-19 pandemic reflects perceived rather than actual risk. Int J Environ Res Public Health 8:5504.
    OpenUrl
  82. ↵
    1. Iriki A,
    2. Tanaka M,
    3. Iwamura Y
    (1996) Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. Neuroreport 7:2325–2330.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  83. ↵
    1. Keller AL,
    2. Schüz A,
    3. Logothetis NK,
    4. Weber B
    (2011) Vascularization of cytochrome oxidase-rich blobs in the primary visual cortex of squirrel and macaque monkeys. J Neurosci 31:1246–1253. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2765-10.2011
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  84. ↵
    1. Kennedy DP,
    2. Gläscher J,
    3. Tyszka JM,
    4. Adolphs R
    (2009) Personal space regulation by the human amygdala. Nat Neurosci 12:1226–1227. pmid:19718035
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  85. ↵
    1. Kennedy DP,
    2. Adolphs R
    (2014) Violations of personal space by individuals with autism spectrum disorder. PloS one 9:e103369.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  86. ↵
    1. Klüver H,
    2. Bucy PC
    (1937) “Psychic blindness” and other symptoms following bilateral temporal lobectomy in rhesus monkeys. Am J Physiol 119:352–353.
    OpenUrl
  87. ↵
    1. Lauwers F,
    2. Cassot F,
    3. Lauwers-Cances V,
    4. Puwanarajah P,
    5. Duvernoy H
    (2008) Morphometry of the human cerebral cortex microcirculation: general characteristics and space-related profiles. Neuroimage 39:936–948. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.024 pmid:17997329
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  88. ↵
    1. Llobera J,
    2. Spanlang B,
    3. Ruffini G,
    4. Slater M
    (2010) Proxemics with multiple dynamic characters in an immersive virtual environment. ACM Trans Appl Percept 8:1–12. doi:10.1145/1857893.1857896
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  89. ↵
    1. Lloyd D,
    2. Morrison I,
    3. Roberts N
    (2006) Role for human posterior parietal cortex in visual processing of aversive objects in peripersonal space. J Neurophysiol 95:205–214. pmid:16162829
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  90. ↵
    1. Lloyd DM,
    2. Coates A,
    3. Knopp J,
    4. Oram S,
    5. Rowbotham S
    (2009) Don't stand so close to me: the effect of auditory input on interpersonal space. Perception 38:617–620. pmid:19522329
    OpenUrlPubMed
  91. ↵
    1. Logothetis NK
    (2003) The underpinnings of the BOLD functional magnetic resonance imaging signal. J Neurosci 23:3963–3971. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-10-03963.2003
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  92. ↵
    1. Logothetis NK,
    2. Pauls J,
    3. Augath M,
    4. Trinath T,
    5. Oeltermann A
    (2001) Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature 412:150–157. doi:10.1038/35084005
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  93. ↵
    1. Lough E,
    2. Hanley M,
    3. Rodgers J,
    4. South M,
    5. Kirk H,
    6. Kennedy DP,
    7. Riby DM
    (2015) Violations of personal space in young people with autism spectrum disorders and Williams syndrome: Insights from the Social Responsiveness Scale. J Autism Dev Disord 45:4101–4108.
    OpenUrl
  94. ↵
    1. Luppino G,
    2. Murata A,
    3. Govoni P,
    4. Matelli M
    (1999) Largely segregated parietofrontal connections linking rostral intraparietal cortex (areas AIP and VIP) and the ventral premotor cortex (areas F5 and F4). Exp Brain Res 128:181–187. pmid:10473756
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  95. ↵
    1. Maravita A,
    2. Iriki A
    (2004) Tools for the body (schema). Trends Cogn Sci 8:79–86.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  96. ↵
    1. Mason WA,
    2. Capitanio JP,
    3. Machado CJ,
    4. Mendoza SP,
    5. Amaral DG
    (2006) Amygdalectomy and responsiveness to novelty in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): generality and individual consistency of effects. Emotion 6:73–81. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.73 pmid:16637751
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  97. ↵
    1. Mccall C,
    2. Blascovich J,
    3. Young A,
    4. Persky S
    (2009) Proxemic behaviors as predictors of aggression towards Black (but not White) males in an immersive virtual environment. Soc Influence 4:138–154. doi:10.1080/15534510802517418
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  98. ↵
    1. Merboldt KD,
    2. Finsterbusch J,
    3. Frahm J
    (2000) Reducing inhomogeneity artifacts in functional MRI of human brain activation—thin sections vs gradient compensation. J Magn Reson 145:184–191. pmid:10910686
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  99. ↵
    1. Mountcastle VB
    (1957) Modality and topographic properties of single neurons of cat's somatic sensory cortex. J Neurophysiol 20:408–434. pmid:13439410
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  100. ↵
    1. Mul C-L,
    2. Cardini F,
    3. Stagg SD,
    4. Sadeghi Esfahlani S,
    5. Kiourtsoglou D,
    6. Cardellicchio P,
    7. Aspell JE
    (2019) Altered bodily self-consciousness and peripersonal space in autism. Autism 23:2055–2067.
    OpenUrl
  101. ↵
    1. Murray MM,
    2. Wallace MT
    (2012) The neural bases of multisensory processes. Boca Raton: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis.
  102. ↵
    1. Nacewicz BM,
    2. Dalton KM,
    3. Johnstone T,
    4. Long MT,
    5. Mcauliff EM,
    6. Oakes TR,
    7. Alexander AL,
    8. Davidson RJ
    (2006) Amygdala volume and nonverbal social impairment in adolescent and adult males with autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63:1417–1428. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.63.12.1417
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  103. ↵
    1. Nasr S,
    2. Tootell RBH
    (2018a) Visual field biases for near and far stimuli in disparity selective columns in human visual cortex. Neuroimage 168:358–365. pmid:27622398
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  104. ↵
    1. Nasr S,
    2. Tootell RBH
    (2018b) Columnar organization of mid-spectral and end-spectral hue preferences in human visual cortex. Neuroimage 181:748–759. pmid:30053514
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  105. ↵
    1. Nasr S,
    2. Tootell RB
    (2020) Asymmetries in global perception are represented in near-versus far-preferring clusters in human visual cortex. J Neurosci 40:355–368. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2124-19.2019
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  106. ↵
    1. Nasr S,
    2. Polimeni JR,
    3. Tootell RB
    (2016) Interdigitated color-and disparity-selective columns within human visual cortical areas V2 and V3. J Neurosci 36:1841–1857. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3518-15.2016
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  107. ↵
    1. Nechamkin Y,
    2. Salganik I,
    3. Modai I,
    4. Ponizovsky AM
    (2003) Interpersonal distance in schizophrenic patients: relationship to negative syndrome. Int J Soc Psychiatry 49:166–174. pmid:14626359
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  108. ↵
    1. Noel J-P,
    2. Cascio CJ,
    3. Wallace MT,
    4. Park S
    (2017) The spatial self in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorder. Schizophrenia research 179:8–12.
    OpenUrl
  109. ↵
    1. Noel JP,
    2. Samad M,
    3. Doxon A,
    4. Clark J,
    5. Keller S,
    6. Di Luca M
    (2018) Peri-personal space as a prior in coupling visual and proprioceptive signals. Sci Rep 8:1–15. doi:10.1038/s41598-018-33961-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  110. ↵
    1. Orban GA,
    2. Claeys K,
    3. Nelissen K,
    4. Smans R,
    5. Sunaert S,
    6. Todd JT,
    7. Wardak C,
    8. Durand JB,
    9. Vanduffel W
    (2006) Mapping the parietal cortex of human and non-human primates. Neuropsychologia 44:2647–2667. pmid:16343560
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  111. ↵
    1. Park SH,
    2. Ku J,
    3. Kim JJ,
    4. Jang HJ,
    5. Kim SY,
    6. Kim SH,
    7. Kim CH,
    8. Lee H,
    9. Kim IY,
    10. Kim SI
    (2009) Increased personal space of patients with schizophrenia in a virtual social environment. Psychiatry Res 169:197–202. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2008.06.039
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  112. ↵
    1. Patané I,
    2. Iachini T,
    3. Farnè A,
    4. Frassinetti F
    (2016) Disentangling action from social space: tool-use differently shapes the space around us. PloS one 11:e0154247.
    OpenUrl
  113. ↵
    1. Pelli DG
    (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis 10:437–442. pmid:9176953
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  114. ↵
    1. Perry A,
    2. Levy-Gigi E,
    3. Richter-Levin G,
    4. Shamay-Tsoory SG
    (2015) Interpersonal distance and social anxiety in autistic spectrum disorders: A behavioral and ERP study. Social Neuroscience 10:354–365.
    OpenUrl
  115. ↵
    1. Poggio GF,
    2. Gonzalez F,
    3. Krause F
    (1988) Stereoscopic mechanisms in monkey visual cortex: binocular correlation and disparity selectivity. J Neurosci 8:4531–4550. pmid:3199191
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  116. ↵
    1. Polimeni JR,
    2. Fischl B,
    3. Greve DN,
    4. Wald LL
    (2010) Laminar analysis of 7 T BOLD using an imposed spatial activation pattern in human V1. Neuroimage 52:1334–1346. pmid:20460157
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  117. ↵
    1. Power JD,
    2. Barnes KA,
    3. Snyder AZ,
    4. Schlaggar BL,
    5. Petersen SE
    (2012) Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage 59:2142–2154. pmid:22019881
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  118. ↵
    1. Quesque F,
    2. Ruggiero G,
    3. Mouta S,
    4. Santos J,
    5. Iachini T,
    6. Coello Y
    (2017) Keeping you at arm's length: modifying peripersonal space influences interpersonal distance. Psychological Research 81:709–720.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  119. ↵
    1. Quinlan D,
    2. Culham JC
    (2007) fMRI reveals a preference for near viewing in the human parieto-occipital cortex. Neuroimage 36:167–187. pmid:17398117
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  120. ↵
    1. Rinck M,
    2. Rörtgen T,
    3. Lange WG,
    4. Dotsch R,
    5. Wigboldus DH,
    6. Becker ES
    (2010) Social anxiety predicts avoidance behaviour in virtual encounters. Cogn Emot 24:1269–1276. doi:10.1080/02699930903309268
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  121. ↵
    1. Roy ML,
    2. Narahashi T
    (1992) Differential properties of tetrodotoxin-sensitive and tetrodotoxin-resistant sodium channels in rat dorsal root ganglion neurons. J Neurosci 12:2104–2111. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-06-02104.1992
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  122. ↵
    1. Ruggiero G,
    2. D'errico O,
    3. Iachini T
    (2016) Development of egocentric and allocentric spatial representations from childhood to elderly age. Psychol Res 80:259–272. pmid:25805435
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  123. ↵
    1. Schienle A,
    2. Wabnegger A,
    3. Schöngassner F,
    4. Leutgeb V
    (2015) Effects of personal space intrusion in affective contexts: an fMRI investigation with women suffering from borderline personality disorder. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 10:1424–1428. pmid:25809402
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  124. ↵
    1. Sereno MI,
    2. Huang RS
    (2014) Multisensory maps in parietal cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 24:39–46. pmid:24492077
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  125. ↵
    1. Serino S,
    2. Trabanelli S,
    3. Jandus C,
    4. Fellrath J,
    5. Grivaz P,
    6. Paladino MP,
    7. Serino A
    (2021) Sharpening of peripersonal space during the COVID-19 pandemic. Curr Biol 31:R889–R890. pmid:34314711
    OpenUrlPubMed
  126. ↵
    1. Shmuel A,
    2. Chaimow D,
    3. Raddatz G,
    4. Ugurbil K,
    5. Yacoub E
    (2010) Mechanisms underlying decoding at 7 T: ocular dominance columns, broad structures, and macroscopic blood vessels in V1 convey information on the stimulated eye. Neuroimage 49:1957–1964. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.040 pmid:19715765
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  127. ↵
    1. Smyser CD,
    2. Inder TE,
    3. Shimony JS,
    4. Hill JE,
    5. Degnan AJ,
    6. Snyder AZ,
    7. Neil JJ
    (2010) Longitudinal analysis of neural network development in preterm infants. Cereb Cortex 20:2852–2862. pmid:20237243
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  128. ↵
    1. Srivastava P,
    2. Mandal MK
    (1990) Proximal spacing to facial affect expressions in schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry 31:119–124. pmid:2311379
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  129. ↵
    1. Swisher JD,
    2. Halko MA,
    3. Merabet LB,
    4. Mcmains SA,
    5. Somers DC
    (2007) Visual topography of human intraparietal sulcus. J Neurosci 27:5326–5337. pmid:17507555
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  130. ↵
    1. Tanabe S,
    2. Doi T,
    3. Umeda K,
    4. Fujita I
    (2005) Disparity-tuning characteristics of neuronal responses to dynamic random-dot stereograms in macaque visual area V4. J Neurophysiol 94:2683–2699. pmid:16000525
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  131. ↵
    1. Todd RM,
    2. Anderson AK
    (2009) Six degrees of separation: the amygdala regulates social behavior and perception. Nat Neurosci 12:1217–1218. pmid:19783979
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  132. ↵
    1. Tootell RB,
    2. Nasr S
    (2017) Columnar segregation of magnocellular and parvocellular streams in human extrastriate cortex. J Neurosci 37:8014–8032. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0690-17.2017
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  133. ↵
    1. Tootell RB,
    2. Nasr S
    (2021) Scotopic vision is selectively processed in thick-type columns in human extrastriate cortex. Cereb Cortex 31:1163–1181. pmid:33073288
    OpenUrlPubMed
  134. ↵
    1. Tootell R,
    2. Hamilton S,
    3. Silverman M,
    4. Switkes E
    (1988) Functional anatomy of macaque striate cortex. I. Ocular dominance, binocular interactions, and baseline conditions. J Neurosci 8:1500–1530. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-05-01500.1988
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  135. ↵
    1. Tootell RB,
    2. Zapetis SL,
    3. Babadi B,
    4. Nasiriavanaki Z,
    5. Hughes DE,
    6. Mueser K,
    7. Otto M,
    8. Pace-Schott E,
    9. Holt DJ
    (2021) Psychological and physiological evidence for an initial 'rough sketch' calculation of personal space. Sci Rep 11:1–15. doi:10.1038/s41598-021-99578-1
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  136. ↵
    1. Triantafyllou C,
    2. Hoge RD,
    3. Krueger G,
    4. Wiggins CJ,
    5. Potthast A,
    6. Wiggins GC,
    7. Wald LL
    (2005) Comparison of physiological noise at 1.5 T, 3 T and 7 T and optimization of fMRI acquisition parameters. Neuroimage 26:243–250. pmid:15862224
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  137. ↵
    1. Tsao DY,
    2. Vanduffel W,
    3. Sasaki Y,
    4. Fize D,
    5. Knutsen TA,
    6. Mandeville JB,
    7. Wald LL,
    8. Dale AM,
    9. Rosen BR,
    10. Van Essen DC,
    11. Livingstone MS,
    12. Orban GA,
    13. Tootell RBH
    (2003) Stereopsis activates V3A and caudal intraparietal areas in macaques and humans. Neuron 39:555–568. pmid:12895427
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  138. ↵
    1. Wabnegger A,
    2. Leutgeb V,
    3. Schienle A
    (2016) Differential amygdala activation during simulated personal space intrusion by men and women. Neuroscience 330:12–16. pmid:27246442
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  139. ↵
    1. Welsch R,
    2. Von Castell C,
    3. Hecht H
    (2019) The anisotropy of personal space. PLoS One 14:e0217587. pmid:31163063
    OpenUrlPubMed
  140. ↵
    1. Welsch R,
    2. Hecht H,
    3. Chuang L,
    4. Von Castell C
    (2020) Interpersonal distance in the SARS-CoV-2 crisis. Hum Factors 62:1095–1101. pmid:32902338
    OpenUrlPubMed
  141. ↵
    1. Welsch R,
    2. Wessels M,
    3. Bernhard C,
    4. Thönes S,
    5. V Castell C
    (2021) Physical distancing and the perception of interpersonal distance in the COVID-19 crisis. Sci Rep 11:1–9. doi:10.1038/s41598-021-90714-5
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  142. ↵
    1. Wieser MJ,
    2. Pauli P,
    3. Grosseibl M,
    4. Molzow I,
    5. Mühlberger A
    (2010) Virtual social interactions in social anxiety—the impact of sex, gaze, and interpersonal distance. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 13:547–554. doi:10.1089/cyber.2009.0432
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  143. ↵
    1. Wilcox LM,
    2. Allison RS
    (2009) Coarse-fine dichotomies in human stereopsis. Vision Res 49:2653–2665. pmid:19520102
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  144. ↵
    1. Williams JL
    (1971) Personal space and its relation to extraversion-introversion. Can J Behav Sci 3:156–160. doi:10.1037/h0082257
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  145. ↵
    1. Wilson HR,
    2. Cowan JD
    (1972) Excitatory and inhibitory interactions in localized populations of model neurons. Biophys J 12:1–24. pmid:4332108
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  146. ↵
    1. Wormith J
    (1984) Personal space of incarcerated offenders. J Clin Psychol 40:815–827. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(198405)40:3<815::AID-JCLP2270400332>3.0.CO;2-G
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  147. ↵
    1. Yacoub E,
    2. Duong TQ,
    3. Van De Moortele PF,
    4. Lindquist M,
    5. Adriany G,
    6. Kim SG,
    7. Uğurbil K,
    8. Hu X
    (2003) Spin-echo fMRI in humans using high spatial resolutions and high magnetic fields. Magn. Reson. Med 49:655–664. doi:10.1002/mrm.10433
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  148. ↵
    1. Yacoub E,
    2. Van De Moortele PF,
    3. Shmuel A,
    4. Uğurbil K
    (2005) Signal and noise characteristics of Hahn SE and GE BOLD fMRI at 7 T in humans. Neuroimage 24:738–750. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.002
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  149. ↵
    1. Yang Y,
    2. Liu S,
    3. Chowdhury SA,
    4. Deangelis GC,
    5. Angelaki DE
    (2011) Binocular disparity tuning and visual–vestibular congruency of multisensory neurons in macaque parietal cortex. J Neurosci 31:17905–17916. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4032-11.2011
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  150. ↵
    1. Zald DH
    (2003) The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 41:88–123. pmid:12505650
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  151. ↵
    1. Zapetis SL,
    2. Nasiriavanaki Z,
    3. Luther L,
    4. Holt DJ
    (2022) Neural correlates of variation in personal space and social functioning in schizophrenia and healthy individuals. Schizophr Bull 48:1075–1084. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbac052
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  152. ↵
    1. Zaretskaya N,
    2. Fischl B,
    3. Reuter M,
    4. Renvall V,
    5. Polimeni JR
    (2018) Advantages of cortical surface reconstruction using submillimeter 7 T MEMPRAGE. Neuroimage 165:11–26. pmid:28970143
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  153. ↵
    1. Zheng D,
    2. Lamantia A,
    3. Purves D
    (1991) Specialized vascularization of the primate visual cortex. J Neurosci 11:2622–2629. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-08-02622.1991
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 42 (48)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 42, Issue 48
30 Nov 2022
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Interdigitated Columnar Representation of Personal Space and Visual Space in Human Parietal Cortex
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Interdigitated Columnar Representation of Personal Space and Visual Space in Human Parietal Cortex
Roger B.H. Tootell, Zahra Nasiriavanaki, Baktash Babadi, Douglas N. Greve, Shahin Nasr, Daphne J. Holt
Journal of Neuroscience 30 November 2022, 42 (48) 9011-9029; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0516-22.2022

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Interdigitated Columnar Representation of Personal Space and Visual Space in Human Parietal Cortex
Roger B.H. Tootell, Zahra Nasiriavanaki, Baktash Babadi, Douglas N. Greve, Shahin Nasr, Daphne J. Holt
Journal of Neuroscience 30 November 2022, 42 (48) 9011-9029; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0516-22.2022
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • 7T
  • cortical columns
  • high-resolution fMRI
  • personal space

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Identification of the Acid-Sensitive Site Critical for Chloral Hydrate (CH) Activation of the Proton-Activated Chloride Channel
  • Disentangling Object Category Representations Driven by Dynamic and Static Visual Input
  • Irrelevant Threats Linger and Affect Behavior in High Anxiety
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Disentangling Object Category Representations Driven by Dynamic and Static Visual Input
  • Irrelevant Threats Linger and Affect Behavior in High Anxiety
  • Multisession Anodal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Enhances Adult Hippocampal Neurogenesis and Context Discrimination in Mice
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.